Hegarty 2008
Hegarty 2008
Hegarty 2008
1
This work was supported by an ESRC grant (RES-000-22-0288) to the first author. The
authors thank Lynsey Mahony for research assistance, and Mick Finlay and Thomas Morton
for comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript.
2
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Peter Hegarty, University of
Surrey, Guildford GU2 7XH. E-mail: [email protected]
1023
The attributional theory of stigma was inspired by findings that people are
more likely to help a person whose distress originates in an uncontrollable
cause, rather than in a controllable cause (e.g., Piliavin, Rodin, & Piliavin,
1969). This effect is mediated by emotional reactions: Uncontrollable causes
of distress elicit pity and sympathy, while controllable causes of distress elicit
anger and hostility (Reizenstein, 1986). Weiner et al. (1988) argued that
attributions similarly determine reactions to stigmatized individuals and
groups, such that uncontrollable stigmata elicit pity, sympathy, and helping
behavior; while controllable stigmata elicit anger and a refusal to extend aid.
Attributions of controllability are said to affect the degree to which stigma-
tized targets are blamed for their own fate (Weiner, 1993, 1995, 1996). For
example, addicted, obese, gay/lesbian, or mentally ill persons are predicted to
be treated better wherever their stigma is understood to originate in uncon-
trollable biological factors rather than in personal weaknesses or personal
choices.
Several studies of emotional reactions to individual stigmatized targets
support attribution theory. Targets with uncontrollable stigmata elicit more
sympathy and pity, are more likely to receive aid, and elicit less anger (Menec
& Perry, 1998; Rush, 1998; Weiner et al., 1988), regardless of whether their
stigmatized trait is HIV/AIDS (Cobb & deChabert, 2002; Dooley, 1995;
Graham, Weiner, Giuliano, & Williams, 1993; Steins & Weiner, 1999),
mental illness (Corrigan et al., 2001), physical illness (Crandall & Moriarty,
1995), obesity (DeJong, 1980), poverty (Zucker & Weiner, 1993), homosexu-
ality (Armesto & Weisman, 2001), or a failure to seek genetic testing for an
inheritable medical condition (Menec & Weiner, 2000). However, the
hypothesized causal link between anger reactions and refusals to extend aid
has failed to materialize in several studies (see Dooley, 1995; Menec & Perry,
1998; Steins & Weiner, 1999).
ATTRIBUTIONS AND STIGMA 1025
Method
Participants
Study participants were 166 individuals (97 female, 69 male) who were
recruited from a major university in the United Kingdom through flyers and
classroom announcements (age range = 18 to 56 years; M age = 23.2 years).
All of the participants were students or workers at the university, but none of
them were psychology students. The participants received £5 (approx. $8 US
at the time of the study; approx. $10 US today) in return for their participa-
tion, and a raffle ticket for an additional prize of £50 (approx. $80 US at the
time of the study; approx. $100 US today).
1028 HEGARTY AND GOLDEN
Design
Materials
There were 12 numbered blank lines that followed. This task afforded a
measure of the degree to which participants’ attributional thinking alighted
on controllable and uncontrollable causes of the relevant stigmatized trait.
We used these thoughts both as a manipulation check and as a means of
testing the justification–suppression model.
Second, participants’ comprehension of the texts was assessed using 12
True–False forced-choice items. Some of these items referred to information
that was common across both forms of the texts (4 items in the homosexual
orientation condition; 5 in the alcoholism condition; and 6 in both the obesity
and depression conditions). Other items referred to distinct controllability-
specific information, and the correct answer to these items depended on the
manipulation to which the participant had been exposed (8 in the homo-
sexual orientation conditions; 7 in the alcoholism condition; and 6 in both the
obesity and depression conditions). (See Appendix B for the terms used in the
obesity conditions. All of the others are available from the first author upon
request.)
Third, participants were presented with an inventory assessing their
evaluation of the texts. The inventory contained eight items that were pre-
sented in 7-point Likert-type format. The obesity items are as follows:
1. The article extract was informative about obesity.
2. The article was easy to read.
3. The article extract was difficult to understand. (reverse-scored)
4. The article extract seems to have come from a science textbook or
journal.
5. The argument about the cause of obesity was convincing.
6. The article had insufficient detail about obesity. (reverse-scored)
7. The article has increased my knowledge about people who are
obese.
8. From your reading of the article extract, how favorable would you
consider the author be toward people who are obese as a group?
Equivalent items about homosexuality, alcoholism, and depression were pre-
sented in the relevant conditions.
Fourth, post-manipulation attitudes were assessed using three types of
measures. A thermometer item was presented about the relevant stigmatized
group. Next, a personal-stereotype item was presented (Eagly, Mladinic, &
Otto, 1991; Esses, Haddock & Zanna, 1994). The name of the relevant
stigmatized group was presented. Participants were asked to write down as
many as three terms that characterize the group, to describe the percentage of
the group that is described by each term, and to rate the valence of each term
on a 5-point scale ranging from -2 (extremely negative) to +2 (extremely
positive). Then, attitudes were assessed using standardized scales.
1030 HEGARTY AND GOLDEN
Procedure
3
The short form of Herek’s (1994) ATLG usually contains 10 items. One item (“State laws
prohibiting private, consenting lesbian behavior should be loosened”) was not included, as it is
not relevant to the British legal context.
4
British students take A-level exams at the end of their secondary education in various
chosen subjects, including psychology.
ATTRIBUTIONS AND STIGMA 1031
Results
Overview
Pre-Manipulation Attitudes
and 51.95, respectively). Attitudes toward obese people and depressed people
were intermediate and were not significantly different from the means
for either alcoholics or lesbians and gay men (Ms = 46.88 and 50.50,
respectively).
The 168 participants produced 1,049 thoughts in total, which were coded
independently by two coders in two stages. First, all thoughts were coded as
attributional if they referred to the cause or to the controllability of the
stigma; or non-attributional if they did not refer to these characteristics of the
stigma. The coders agreed initially on the coding of 93% of cases and resolved
disagreements easily through discussion. In all, 35.7% of the thoughts were
coded as attributional. A 2 ¥ 4 ANOVA using stigma group and controlla-
bility manipulation as independent variables reveals no main effects or inter-
actions on either the total number of thoughts produced (M = 6.33), or the
total number of attributional thoughts produced (M = 2.24; all Fs < 2.10, all
ps > .10).
Next, all attributional thoughts were coded as implying controllability,
uncontrollability, or as ambiguous thoughts. Coders agreed initially on 92%
of cases, and disagreements were easily resolved, as before. Ambiguous
thoughts accounted for 10.5% of all attributional thoughts. For each partici-
pant, we calculated the total number of thoughts that implied controllability
or uncontrollability (Ms = 1.18 and 0.83, respectively).
To assess whether attributional thinking was manipulated effectively by
the texts, a 4 ¥ 2 ¥ 2 ANOVA was conducted, with stigma trait (alcoholism,
depression, homosexuality, or obesity) and attributional belief manipulation
(controllable vs. uncontrollable) as between-subjects factors, and type of
attributional thought (controllability implied vs. uncontrollability implied)
as a within-subjects factor. Participants’ thoughts implied that the stigma-
tized traits were controllable more often than uncontrollable, F(1,
158) = 6.80, p = .01. This effect was moderated by a significant interaction
involving the controllability manipulation, F(1, 158) = 20.49, p < .001. Sig-
nificantly more thoughts implying controllability were produced by those
who read that the stigmatized traits were controllable (Ms = 1.45 and 0.92,
respectively), but significantly more thoughts implying uncontrollability were
produced by participants who read that the stigmatized traits were uncon-
trollable (Ms = 1.19 and 0.46, respectively). In other words, the texts effec-
tively manipulated participants’ causal thinking.
Next, we used the thoughts to test the justification–suppression hypoth-
esis that prejudiced persons spontaneously attribute stigmatized traits to
ATTRIBUTIONS AND STIGMA 1033
Comprehension
Evaluation of Texts
The eight items used to assess evaluation of the texts were not sufficiently
correlated to form a single measure (Cronbach’s a = .49) and were analyzed
separately. Stigma group affected responses to three items. Responses to Item
1 show that the sexual orientation texts were viewed as significantly less
1034 HEGARTY AND GOLDEN
informative (M = 5.11) than were the obesity and depression texts (Ms = 5.85
and 5.78, respectively), F(3, 158) = 3.61, p < .05. Responses to Item 2 show
that the sexual orientation texts were viewed as significantly harder to read
than were the obesity texts (M = 5.64 and 6.25, respectively), F(3, 158) = 2.77,
p < .05. Finally, a significant effect was observed with regard to Item 7, F(3,
158) = 2.70, p < .05, but post hoc tests reveal no clear significant differences
between conditions.
The controllability manipulation affected only responses to Item 7. Par-
ticipants in the uncontrollable conditions perceived that they learned more
than did participants in the controllable conditions, F(1, 158) = 7.69, p < .01
(Ms = 5.14 and 4.46, respectively). This finding, along with the tendency to
spontaneously produce thoughts implying controllability, suggests that attri-
butions of stigma to controllable factors were the default among these par-
ticipants. No other main effects of controllability approached significance (all
Fs < 2.70, all ps > .10). None of the interaction effects were significant (all
Fs < 1.47, all ps < .22).
Thus, the participants both understood information that the texts com-
municated about controllability, and spontaneously produced attributional
thoughts consistent with our predictions. In other words, the texts were
effective. Under such conditions, attribution theory predicts that attitudes
toward stigmatized groups should be affected. We next examined if this was
the case by examining participants’ attitudes as assessed by thermometer
scales, personal stereotypes, and standardized measures.
Post-Manipulation Thermometers
5
We also examined change scores on the thermometer measure. Attitudes toward the stig-
matized groups were more positive after reading the texts than before, t(164) = 4.32, p < .001. A
2 ¥ 4 ANOVA using change scores as a dependent variable reveals no significant effects of
stigma group, controllability manipulation, or interaction between them (all Fs < 1.70, all
ps > .17).
ATTRIBUTIONS AND STIGMA 1035
Personal Stereotypes
Personal stereotypes for each participant for each stigma were computed
according to the formula
∑ ( p × v) n
where p is the percentage of the group judged to share the trait (i.e., 0 to 100),
v is the valence of the trait (i.e., -2 to 2), and n is the total number of traits
described. Thus, personal stereotype scores ranged from -200 to +200. A
2 ¥ 4 ANOVA including pre-manipulation thermometer scores as a covariate
was conducted. Once again, the effect of the covariate was significant, F(3,
157) = 9.91, p < .01. In addition, there was a significant main effect of stig-
matized group, F(3, 157) = 10.08, p < .001. Post hoc tests show that partici-
pants had significantly more positive stereotypes of gay men and lesbians
(M = 21.40) than of obese people and depressed people (Ms = -36.07 and
-48.01, respectively). Stereotypes of alcoholics were significantly more nega-
tive than were stereotypes of all other groups (M = -91.38). However, there
was no effect of the attributional belief manipulation, nor any interaction
between stigma group and attributional belief manipulation (both Fs < 1).
Personal stereotypes were equally negative after reading that the stigmatized
traits were controllable or uncontrollable (Ms = -36.54 and -37.25, respec-
tively). This result also failed to confirm attribution theory.
Discussion
6
None of the statistical conclusions about the lack of an effect of the manipulation on
attitudes were affected by running the analysis again without including pre-manipulation atti-
tudes as a covariate, with one exception. A marginally significant effect of the manipulation on
AFA scores was observed, t(38) = 1.76, p < .09.
ATTRIBUTIONS AND STIGMA 1037
The present study has many limitations, most of which are typical of
research in this area. First, we relied on a college sample, but we conscien-
tiously avoided recruiting psychology students. Second, our studies were
based in the United Kingdom, while most studies in this domain have been
conducted with samples in the United States. Previous research, at least in the
domain of sexual orientation, has shown correlations between attitudes and
attributional beliefs to be stronger in the U.S. than in the UK, and we
recommend that researchers located in the U.S. explore the effects of preju-
dice on attributional thinking (Hegarty, 2002). Third, like other researchers
(e.g., Crandall, 1994), we only examined immediate effects of our manipula-
tions, and long-term effects ought to be examined in future studies, too.
Finally, as we observed no effects of attributional belief manipulations on
attitudes, we were unable to answer questions about possible moderators or
mediators of those effects.
We encourage other researchers in this area to employ diverse samples
from more than one country to test theories across multiple stigmatized
groups. Research should explore the when, how, and why of the reciprocal
relationship between thinking about the cause of a stigmatized trait and
evaluations of the group that is characterized by that trait.
ATTRIBUTIONS AND STIGMA 1039
References
Aguero, J. E., Bloch, L., & Byrne, D. (1984). The relationships among sexual
beliefs, attitudes, experience, and homophobia. Journal of Homosexuality,
10, 95–107.
Anderson, C. A., Krull, D. S., & Weiner, B. (1996). Explanations: Processes
and consequences. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social
psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 271–296). New York:
Guilford.
Armesto, J. C., & Weisman, A. G. (2001). Attributions and emotional reac-
tions to the identity disclosure (“coming out”) of a homosexual child.
Family Process, 40, 145–161.
Campbell, A. (1971). White attitudes toward Black people. Ann Arbor, MI:
Institute for Social Research.
Cobb, M., & deChabert, J. T. (2002). HIV/AIDS and care provider attribu-
tions: Who’s to blame? AIDS Care, 14, 545–548.
Corrigan, P. W. (2000). Mental health stigma as social attribution: Implica-
tions for research methods and attitude change. Clinical Psychology:
Research and Practice, 7, 48–67.
Corrigan, P. W., Markowitz, F. E., Watson, A., Rowan, D., & Kubiak,
M. A. (2003). An attribution model of public discrimination towards
persons with mental illness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 44,
162–179.
Corrigan, P. W., River, L. P., Lundin, R. K., Penn, D. L., Uphoff-Wasowski,
K., Campion, J., et al. (2001). Three strategies for changing attributions
about severe mental illness, Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27, 187–195.
Crandall, C. S. (1994). Prejudice against fat people: Ideology and self-
interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 882–894.
Crandall, C. S., D’Anello, S., Sakalli, N., Lazarus, E., Nejtardt, G. W., &
Feather, N. T. (2001). An attribution-value model of prejudice: Anti-fat
attitudes in six nations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27,
30–37.
Crandall, C. S., & Eshleman, A. (2003). A justification–suppression model of
the expression and experience of prejudice. Psychological Bulletin, 129,
414–446.
Crandall, C. S., & Martinez, R. (1996). Culture, ideology, and anti-fat atti-
tudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1165–1176.
Crandall, C. S., & Moriarty, D. (1995). Physical illness stigma and social
rejection. British Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 67–83.
Crisp, A. H., Gelder, M. G., Rix, S., Meltzer, H. I., & Rowlands, O. J. (2000).
Stigmatisation of people with mental illnesses. British Journal of Psychia-
try, 177, 4–7.
1040 HEGARTY AND GOLDEN
Crocker, J., Major, B., & Steele, C. (1998). Social stigma. In D. T. Gilbert,
S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (Vol.
2, pp. 504–553). New York: Oxford University Press.
DeJong, W. (1980). The stigma of obesity: The consequences of naïve
assumptions concerning the causes of physical deviance. Journal of Health
and Social Behavior, 21, 75–87.
Dooley, P. A. (1995). Perceptions of the onset controllability of AIDS and
helping judgments: An attributional analysis. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 25, 858–869.
Eagly, A. H., Mladinic, A., & Otto, S. (1991). Are women evaluated more
favorably than men? An analysis of attitudes, beliefs, and emotions.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15, 203–216.
Ernulf, K. E., Innala, S. M., & Whitam, F. (1989). Biological explanation,
psychological explanation, and tolerance of homosexuals: A cross-
national analysis of beliefs and attitudes. Psychological Reports, 65, 1003–
1010.
Esses, V. M., Haddock, G., & Zanna, M. P. (1994). The role of mood in
the expression of intergroup stereotypes. The psychology of prejudice:
The Ontario Symposium (Vol. 7, pp. 77–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Fine, M., & Asch, A. (1988). Disability beyond stigma: Social interaction,
discrimination, and activism. Journal of Social Issues, 44, 3–21.
Finell, D. S. (2002). The case for teaching patients about the neurobiological
basis of addictions. Journal of Addictions Nursing, 12, 149–158.
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Graham, S., Weiner, B., Giuliano, T., & Williams, E. (1993). An attributional
analysis of reactions to Magic Johnson. Journal of Applied Social Psy-
chology, 23, 96–1010.
Haddock, G., & Zanna, M. P. (1998). On the use of open-ended measures to
assess attitude components. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 129–
149.
Haslam, N., Rotschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2000). Essentialist beliefs about
social categories. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 113–127.
Haslam, N., Rotschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2002). Are essentialist beliefs asso-
ciated with prejudice? British Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 87–100.
Hegarty, P. (2002). “It’s not a choice, it’s the way we’re built”: Symbolic
beliefs about sexual orientation in the U.S. and Britain. Journal of Com-
munity and Applied Social Psychology, 12, 153–166.
Hegarty, P., & Pratto, F. (2001). Sexual orientation beliefs: Their relationship
to anti-gay attitudes and biological determinist attitudes. Journal of
Homosexuality, 41, 121–135.
ATTRIBUTIONS AND STIGMA 1041
Appendix A
Weight gain is highly visible when it occurs. But why do some people
become fat, while others do not? Recent medical research in the UK has
shown that 8 out of 10 children with two obese parents will become obese
themselves, as compared with 2 out of 10 children with two lean parents. But
is this due to nature or to nurture? Studies of adopted children have revealed
that their weight patterns are similar to those of their biological [adoptive],
rather than their adoptive [biological] parents. In addition, scientists have
found differences [have consistently found no differences] in the brain activi-
ties of obese and non-obese people in response to food stimuli. Therefore,
most scientists now agree that genes and other biological factors play a large
part in the development of obesity [are largely irrelevant in obesity]. Life
experience seems to play a minor role, if any at all [a much larger role].
Almost everyone has used a diet or an exercise regime at some point or
other to control their weight. But why don’t these strategies work all of the
time? At a simple level, weight is only lost when net energy expenditure
exceeds net energy intake over a prolonged period of time. However, humans
are hardwired with a strong drive to eat, and as a direct result of our lifestyle,
we engage in much less physical activity than did our evolutionary ancestors.
In addition, as a person loses weight, that person’s metabolism slows down. One
effect of this is that people on diets extract more calories from food than they do
normally. This makes weight loss difficult. In fact, many people gain more
weight than they lose as a result of dieting. The balance between eating and
exercise, and changes in humans’ metabolism while dieting explain why obesity
is such a difficult condition to change. [Obesity often results from an imbalance
between diet and exercise and can be cured by addressing this balance.
Organizations like Weight Watchers® re-educate obese people about healthy
eating habits and exercise, and many obese people who join such programs
have managed to lose weight and to keep it off. In other words, when obese
people manage to consistently eat less and to consistently exercise more,
obesity can be successfully treated.]
Note. Controllable condition items appear in brackets.
1044 HEGARTY AND GOLDEN
Appendix B
Controllability-Specific Items