MEMORY Notes
MEMORY Notes
Memory is an active system that receives information from the senses, puts that information
into a usable form, organizes it as it stores it away, and then retrieves the information from
storage (Baddeley, 1996, 2003). Memory is the center of all our cognitive processes such as
reasoning, decision making etc. Memory also provides the sense of continuity in life because
it establishes a link between past and present, it helps in acquiring one’s identity as a person
and it enables us to store a large amount of information. Such capacity of storing enables a man
to remember large many things. However, with the lapse of time, we also forget.
Memory is a higher-level mental (cognitive) process. There are various organs of memory such
The word memory derives its origin from Latin term ‘memoria’ which means historical account
refers to the fact that remembering is retaining the past events or experiences with or without
awareness. Many of our learned acts are performed automatically, i.e., without being aware of
Although there are several different models of how memory works, all of them involve the
same three processes: getting the information into the memory system, storing it there, and
ENCODING: - The first process in the memory system is to get sensory information (sight,
sound, etc.) into a form that the brain can use. This is called encoding. Encoding is the set of
mental operations that people perform on sensory information to convert that information into
a form that is usable in the brain’s storage systems. For example, when people hear a sound,
their ears turn the vibrations in the air into neural messages from the auditory nerve
(transduction), which make it possible for the brain to interpret that sound. We use memory
codes to translate information from the senses into mental representations of that information.
Acoustic codes represent information as sequences of sounds, such as a tune or a rhyme. Visual
codes represent information as pictures, such as the image of your best friend’s face. Semantic
codes represent the general meaning of an experience. Encoding is not limited to turning
sensory information into signals for the brain. Encoding is accomplished differently in each of
three different storage systems of memory. In one system, encoding may involve rehearsing
information over and over to keep it in memory, whereas in another system, encoding involves
STORAGE: - The second basic memory process is storage. It refers to the holding of
information in your memory over time. The period of time will actually be of different lengths,
depending on the system of memory being used. For example, in one system of memory, people
hold on to information just long enough to work with it, about 20 seconds or so. In another
RETRIEVAL: - retrieval, occurs when you find information stored in memory and bring it
into consciousness. Retrieving stored information such as your address or telephone number is
usually so fast and effortless that it seems automatic. Retrieval involves both recall and
recognition. To recall information, you have to retrieve it from memory without much help;
this is what is required when you answer an essay test question. In recognition, retrieval is
aided by clues, such as the response alternatives given on multiple-choice tests. Accordingly,
A classic basis for the distinction between different memories corresponding to different time
intervals was formalized by Richard Atkinson and Richard Shiffrin in 1968. It is the multi store
model of memory. According to stage model (Atkinson and Shiffrin, 1971), there are three
memory stores with a buffer or central processor. They proposed that memory consisted of
three stores: a sensory register, short-term memory (STM) and long-term memory (LTM).
Sensory memory: Sensory input, i.e., the incoming information first enters the sensory
memory (SM), where the information is registered. It is the first system in the process of
memory, the point at which information enters the nervous system through the sensory
systems—eyes, ears, and so on. There are two kinds of sensory memory that have been studied
extensively. They are the iconic (visual) and echoic (auditory) sensory systems.
consciousness, that message will move from sensory memory to the next process of memory,
called short-term memory (STM). Unlike sensory memory, short-term memories may be held
for up to 30 seconds and possibly longer through maintenance rehearsal. The sensory registers
allow our memory system to develop a representation of a stimulus. However, they can’t
perform the more thorough analysis needed if the information is going to be used in some way.
memory (STM) is the part of our memory system that stores limited amounts of information
for up to about eighteen seconds. Working memory is the part of the memory system that allows
us to mentally work with, or manipulate, the information being held in short-term memory.
Long-term memory: The third stage of memory is long-term memory (LTM), the system into
which all the information is placed to be kept more or less permanently. In terms of capacity,
LTM seems to be unlimited for all practical purposes (Bahrick, 1984; Barnyard & Grayson,
1996). Long-term memory is involved when information has to be retained for intervals as brief
as a few minutes (such as a point made earlier in a conversation) or as long as a lifetime (such
as an adult’s childhood memories). Long-term memory (LTM) is the part of the memory
system whose encoding and storage capabilities can produce memories that last a lifetime.
SENSORY MEMORY
The information initially acquired from the environment via the sense organs is placed into a
short-lasting memory called sensory memory. That is the sensory input, which is the incoming
information, first enters the sensory memory (SM), where the information is registered. This
retains the unprocessed signal images of the physical energies received from sensory channel.
Though, this system has a large capacity, but it is of very short duration, i.e., less than a second.
This system registers information from each of the senses with reasonable accuracy.
Information is encoded into sensory memory as neural messages in the nervous system. As
long as those neural messages are traveling through the system, it can be said that people have
a “memory” for that information that can be accessed if needed. sensory inputs are briefly held
transferred into short-term memory or decays and is lost forever. Often this system is referred
to as sensory memories or sensory registers, because information from all the senses are
registered here as exact replica of the stimulus. For example, the transitory visual images (such
as trail of light that stays after the bulb is switched off), flash of letters on the screen or a
momentary auditory image of first spoken words etc. are retained in for a brief period (for a
fraction of second). These visual and auditory memories are also called iconic memory and
Iconic memory: Iconic memory is the visual sensory memory, which lasts only a
fraction of a second. This kind of memory is most widely known as pictorial memory (or visual
sensory memory). When a visual stimulus impinges on retina an image of the stimulus is
formed which we call retinal image. This image persists on the retina for a fraction of seconds
even after removal of the visual stimulus. Icon is the Greek word for “image.” In real life,
information that has just entered iconic memory will be pushed out very quickly by new
information, a process called masking (Cowan, 1988). Although it is rare, some people do have
what is properly called eidetic imagery, or the ability to access a visual sensory memory over
a long period of time. Although the popular term photographic memory is often used to mean
this rare ability, some people claiming to have photographic memory actually mean that they
have an extremely good memory. Iconic memory actually serves a very important function in
the visual system. Iconic memory helps the visual system view surroundings as continuous and
stable in spite of these saccadic movements. It also allows enough time for the brain stem to
Echoic memory: It is the auditory sensory memory which lasts only 2–4 seconds.
Echoic memory refers o persistence or lingering of sound impressions for sometimes even
when the sound ceases. It does persist or linger on for a very brief period. It is the echoic
memory that accounts for listening the sounds in organized form of words or sentences. The
iconic memory). The sounds are stored in raw form. Echoic memory’s capacity is limited to
what can be heard at any one moment and is smaller than the capacity of iconic memory,
SHORT-TERM MEMORY
If an incoming sensory message is important enough to enter consciousness, that message will
move from sensory memory to the next process of memory, called short-term memory (STM).
Unlike sensory memory, short-term memories may be held for up to 30 seconds and possibly
may last weeks, months, or years. However, there is a way that you can maintain information
short-term memory is lost in 10 to 20 seconds. Short-term memory is also limited in the number
of items it can hold. The small capacity of STM was pointed out by George Miller (1956) in a
famous paper called “The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two. When short-term
memory is filled to capacity, the insertion of new information “bumps out” some of the
information currently in STM. The limited capacity of STM constrains people’s ability to
perform tasks in which they need to mentally juggle various pieces of information. Despite the
severe limitations of short-term memory, individuals are able to enhance the functioning of
basis of similarity or some other organizing principle, or by combining them into larger
patterns. A chunk is a meaningful unit of information. A chunk can be a single letter or number,
a group of letters or other items, or even a group of words or an entire sentence. For example,
the sequence 1–9–8–4 consists of four digits that could exhaust your STM capacity. However,
if you see the digits as a year or the title of George Orwell’s book 1984, they constitute only
one chunk, leaving you much more capacity for other chunks of information.
short-term memory.
be remembered over and over in one’s head in order to maintain it in short-term memory. l.
With maintenance rehearsal, a person is simply continuing to pay attention to the information
to be held in memory, and since attention is how that information got into STM in the first
place, it works quite well (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968; Rundus, 1971). With this type of
rehearsal, information will stay in short-term memory until rehearsal stops. When rehearsal
stops, the memory rapidly decays and is forgotten. If anything interferes with maintenance
rehearsal, memories are also likely to be lost. For example, if someone is trying to count items
by reciting each number out loud while counting, and someone else asks that person the time
and interferes with the counting process, the person who is counting will probably forget what
the last number was and have to start all over again. Short-term memory helps people keep
Rote rehearsal: Learning by simple repetition. Rote is like “rotating” the information
retrieval cues (stimuli that aid in remembering) for information by connecting new information
with something that is already well known. It is the encoding that links new information with
existing memories and knowledge. It is the process in which you relate new material to
information you already have stored in memory. For example, instead of trying to remember a
new person’s name by simply repeating it to yourself, you could try thinking about how the
WORKING MEMORY: Short-term memory is often used for more than just storing
information. When STM is combined with other mental processes, it acts more like a sort of
“mental scratchpad,” or working memory. Working memory briefly holds the information we
need when we are thinking and solving problems. Some memory theorists use the term working
memory as another way of referring to short-term memory. Working memory is the part of the
memory system that allows us to mentally work with, or manipulate, the information being
held in short-term memory. When you mentally calculate what time you have to leave home in
order to have lunch on campus, return a library book, and still get to class on time, you are
using working memory. Short-term memory is actually a component of working memory, and
together these memory systems allow us to do many kinds of mental work (Baddeley, 2003;
Engle & Oransky, 1999). Working memory provides a foundation for the moment-by-moment
Alan Baddeley (2002, 2003) has provided evidence for four components of working memory:
The phonological loop overlaps most with short-term memory. When you rehearse a
telephone number by “listening” to it as you run it through your head, you are making
• A visuospatial sketchpad: This resource performs the same types of functions as the
phonological loop for visual and spatial information. If, for example, someone asked
you how many desks there are in your psychology classroom, you might use the
resources of the visuospatial sketchpad to form a mental picture of the classroom and
coordinating information from the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad.
Any time you carry out a task that requires a combination of mental processes—
imagine, for example, you are asked to describe a picture from memory—you rely on
the central executive function to apportion your mental resources to different aspects of
the task.
• The episodic buffer: It is a storage system with limited capacity that is controlled by the
central executive. The episodic buffer allows you to retrieve information from long-
term memory and combine it with information from the current situation. Most life
events include a complex array of sights, sounds, and so on. The episodic buffer
provides a resource to integrate those different types of perceptual stimulation with past
LONG-TERM MEMORY
long-term memory (LTM), is the system into which all the information is placed to be kept
more or less permanently. In terms of capacity, LTM seems to be unlimited for all practical
purposes. LTM can hold nearly limitless amounts of information. Some information is encoded
into long-term memory even if we make no conscious effort to memorize it (Ellis, 1991).
However, putting information into long-term memory is often the result of more elaborate and
conscious processing that usually involves semantic coding. More often an information in
memory. Rehearsing perhaps increases the likelihood of organizing the information and
relatively permanent. Many of the materials remain stored in LTM lifelong. It has been found
that many information and experiences gathered during primary school stage also remain stored
in LTM for a pretty longer duration. At the same time, it is also found that some of the remote
past experiences decay in strength and are not readily available for retrieval especially for
recalling and if recalled, these may not be always accurate or some of the information are
altered, some are condensed or shortened and some may include new additions at the time of
recall.
The storage capacity of LTM is practically infinite. It has been shown that once any information
enters the long-term memory store, it is never forgotten because it gets encoded semantically,
Procedural memory: Procedural memory includes basic conditioned responses and learned
actions, such as those involved in typing, driving, or swinging a golf club. Memories such as
these can be fully expressed only as actions (or “know-how”). It is likely that skill memories
register in “lower” brain areas, especially the cerebellum. They represent the more basic
Declarative memory: Declarative memory stores specific factual information, such as names,
faces, words, dates, and ideas. Declarative memories are expressed as words or symbols. For
example, knowing that Peter Jackson directed both the Lord of the Rings trilogy and the latest
remake of King Kong is a declarative memory. This is the type of memory that a person with
amnesia lacks and that most of us take for granted. Declarative memory can be further divided
immune to forgetting. The names of objects, the days of the week or months of the year, simple
math skills, the seasons, words and language, and other general facts are all quite lasting. Such
impersonal facts make up a part of LTM called semantic memory. Semantic memory serves as
Episodic Memory: Semantic memory has no connection to times or places. It would be rare,
for instance, to remember when and where you first learned the names of the seasons. In
life events (or “episodes”) day after day, year after year. More than a simple ability to store
information, they make it possible for us to mentally travel back in time and re-experience
events (Tulving, 2002). In fact, it is the forgetting of episodic information that results in the
formation of semantic memories. At first, you remembered when and where you were when
you learned the names of the seasons. Over time you forgot the episodic details but will likely
A flashbulb memory is an especially vivid image that seems to be frozen in memory at times
of personal tragedy, accident, or other emotionally significant events. It is the memories that
are created at times of high emotion that seem especially vivid. Powerful experiences activate
the limbic system, a part of the brain that processes emotions. Heightened activity in the limbic
tend to form at times of intense emotion. Flashbulb memories are often related to public
tragedies, but memories of both positive and negative events can have “flashbulb” clarity.
Those are memories of events that are very arousing or surprising. Such memories are very
detailed. They are like photographs taken with an advanced camera. Flash memories are like
images frozen in memories and tied to particular places, dates and times.
Often, we find situations in which one feels certain that he knows a specific name, word,
meaning et. yet, we are unable to reproduce the same immediately. In other words, one feels
the item lying just beneath awareness and hence, unable to recall immediately. This is called
tip of tongue (TOT) phenomenon. The word or name seems to be on the tip of our tongue, but
Implicit memory
Implicit memory refers to the memories about which the person is not aware. It is automatically
retrieved. Individuals are often able to retrieve such events which are not in the conscious
awareness of the individual. For example, a typist knows all the letters on the keyboard, but
can hardly recall verbally the blank eyes. However, while typing, his fingers press only the
correct keys as these keys are in his implicit memory. Implicit memory though lies outside the
Explicit memory
Explicit memories are past experiences that are consciously brought to mind. Recall,
recognition, and the tests you take in school rely on explicit memories. When you deliberately
try to remember something, such as where you went on your last vacation, you are relying on
explicit memory.
Priming
Priming is an automatic or unconscious process that can enhance the speed and accuracy of a
response as a result of past experience. different cues (see examples below) prompt the retrieval
of memory. Memories are stored as a series of connections that can be activated by different
kinds of cues; there is not any single location in the brain associated with a specific memory
trace. Priming helps trigger associated concepts or memories, making the retrieval process
more efficient. An example of priming is repetition priming: you are faster reading the word
Attention is critical to the encoding of memories, but not all attention is created equal. You can
attend to things in different ways, focusing on different aspects of the stimulus input. According
to some theorists, these qualitative differences in how people attend to information are
important factors influencing how much they remember. In an influential theoretical treatise,
Fergus Craik and Robert Lockhart (1972) proposed that incoming information can be
processed at different levels. The model proposed by Craik and Lockhart is known as the level
of processing model. Through this model Craik and Lockhart proposed that it is possible to
analyze the input (incoming information) at more than one level: structural, phonemic, and
semantic encoding.
Structural encoding is relatively shallow processing that emphasizes the physical structure of
the stimulus. For example, if words are flashed on a screen, structural encoding registers such
things as how they were printed (capital, lowercase, and so on) or the length of the words (how
many letters). Further analysis may result in phonemic encoding, which emphasizes what a
word sounds like. Phonemic encoding involves naming or saying (perhaps silently) the words.
Finally, semantic encoding emphasizes the meaning of verbal input; it involves thinking about
the objects and actions the words represent. Levels-of-processing theory proposes that deeper
MEASURING MEMORY
we can define memory as retention. Retention is storing or holding the information into
memory stores in the form of a structure. Thus, the output of remembering process, memory is
nothing. Retention is very important aspect of remembering because memory depends upon
how much and how well (organized) the previous experiences are retained.
How much do we remember means what we have retained. This can be known by employing
Recall: recall means ‘calling back again’. Thus, bringing into consciousness and producing the
same in absence of the original material which one has learnt or experienced in the past is called
recall or reproduction. In the process of recall, an individual revives his earlier experiences or
original learning. Recall describes retrieving a memory without any additional clues.
In the context of learning, materials are recalled immediately after each exposure. Hence it
called immediate recall. In the context of memory, there is delayed recall, i.e., recall occurs
Recognition: recognition refers to identification of the past images of familiar objects. In other
situations. Correct recognition of familiar and unfamiliar object reflects the storage or retention
of the past images and thus, measures memory (retention of visual images of the experienced
objects). In this method, originally learnt or familiar materials are mixed with unfamiliar or
unlearnt materials and subjects are asked to identify correctly (or sort out) the familiar ones
from amongst the unfamiliar materials. In this way, the correct recognitions indicate the amount
of memory.
Relearning: in relearning, the individual relearns the task to the level of original learning.
Relearning is sometimes called the savings method. Using this method, the subject first learns
some material and, after various lengths of time, relearns that same material to the same
criterion level. The number of trials to relearn the material is always fewer than the number
RETRIEVAL CUES
Retrieval cues are stimuli that help you retrieve information from long-term memory. Retrieval cues
are what make recognition tasks (such as multiple-choice tests) easier than recall tasks (such as essay
exams). The effectiveness of retrieval cues depends on the extent to which they tap into information
that was encoded at the time of learning (Tulving, 1983). This rule is known as the encoding specificity
principle. Because long-term memories are often encoded in terms of their general meaning, cues that
trigger the meaning of the stored information tend to work best. Imagine that you have learned a long
list of sentences. One of them was either (1) “The man lifted the piano” or (2) “The man tuned the
piano.” Now suppose that on a later recall test, you were given the retrieval cue “something heavy.”
This cue would probably help you to remember the first sentence (because you probably encoded
something about the weight of a piano as you read it) but not the second sentence (because it has nothing
to do with weight). Similarly, the cue “makes nice sounds” would probably help you recall the second
surroundings or physiological state) that is available when the memory is first formed is also
available when the memory is being retrieved. The concept introduced by Tulving, states that
the most effective cues at retrieval are those that match the cues present at encoding. Tulving
(1983) makes the case that much of our forgetting is due to problems in accessibility or the
failure to retrieve memories that still reside in long-term memory. (This contrasts with
forgetting due to problems in availability, where the memory is simply no longer stored in the
brain). More generally, there are many situations in which memories are available in long-term
memory; but we have difficulty accessing them (e.g., the name of a well-known actor, some of
the details of a vacation we took last year) until we are given a good cue (e.g., the first name
of the actor, a photograph of us playing miniature golf while on vacation), which, according to
the encoding specificity principle, is a cue that gets us to think about the event like we did when
we encoded it.
Context-dependent memories are those that are helped or hindered by similarities or differences
in environmental context. This context-dependency effect is not always strong (Smith, Vela, &
Williamson, 1988). Many people attending a reunion at their old high school find that being in
the building again provides context cues that help bring back memories of their school days.
The fact that information entered into memory in a particular context or setting is easier to
during learning versus recall. For example, if people learn new material while under the
influence of marijuana, they tend to recall it better if they are also tested under the influence of
marijuana
One of the interesting features of recall is that it is often subject to a kind of “prejudice” of
memory retrieval, in which information at the beginning and the end of a list, such as a poem
or song, tends to be remembered more easily and accurately. This is called the serial position
effect (Murdock, 1962). That is, the tendency of information at the beginning and end of a body
of information to be remembered more accurately than information in the middle of the body
of information.
Serial position effect is the effect an item’s position on a list has on how well it is recalled. For
example, when participants are given a long list of items to remember in an immediate memory
task, they tend to remember best the items listed first on the list (primacy effect) and the items
1. The primacy effect is the concept that the first items in a list receive a great deal of
rehearsal, and are, thus, more likely to be transferred into long-term memory. The primacy
effect is diminished when list items are presented at a fast rate. The primacy effect is not
affected by a distractor task (a task not related to the task currently taking place) presented after
a list first while those items are still in their working memory. The recency effect is not
affected by the rate of speed a list is presented. The recency effect can be eliminated if
a distractor task is presented immediately after the list is presented and before recall is
required.
RECONSTRUCTIVE MEMORY
The process of putting information together based on general types of stored knowledge in the
absence of a specific memory representation. Memory reconstruction can occur at the time the
memory is originally formed via perceptual errors of various sorts. More often, memory
reconstruction occurs at varying times after its formation on the basis of various kinds of post-
event information. Memory reconstruction forms the basis for memories that, although
systematically incorrect, seem very real and are recounted with a great deal of confidence. This
is critical in various practical settings, notably the legal system, which often relies heavily on
eyewitness memory.
SOURCE MONITORING
Source monitoring is the process of making attributions about the origins of memories.
According to Johnson, source monitoring errors appear to be common and may explain why
people sometimes “recall” something that was only suggested to them or confuse their own
ideas with others’ ideas. Reality monitoring involves deciding whether memories are based on
In all kinds of criminal cases, the police and judiciary consider eyewitness memory as
oathfully describes the crime events that had occurred in his presence and solemnly affirms his
statement of the incident that it is exactly what he has witnessed. Therefore, the police and
judiciary assume that eyewitness’ account is true and reliable or dependable because the
detailed account of the crime occurrence is stored in his memory which is being produced in
the court. No doubt, normally the eyewitness account should be a true account of the event.
But, since the memory is constructive and reconstructive, the possibilities of errors cannot
outrightly be ruled out. Elizabeth Loftus (1979) had tested the accuracy of the eyewitness.
FALSE MEMORY
It refers to a pattern of thoughts and actions which are distorted and mistaken form of memory.
The person claims such memories as true which has previously been repressed. This is a sort
of syndrome which arouse questions or doubts about the accuracy of the recollected memories
in which the individual reports about the memories of early traumatic experiences. Owing to
distortion, people not only report false memory but also believe them as complete. However,
it does not mean that traumatic experiences are not repressed. The traumatic experiences do get
repressed. However, one needs to be cautious in accepting the claim of repressed experiences.
METAMEMORY
Metamemory refers to our implicit or explicit knowledge about memory abilities and effective
memory strategies; cognition about memory. It is our knowledge and awareness of our own
memory processes. Knowledge in this case means self-knowledge about our memory
processes. For example, when a person asserts that he or she is good at remembering faces, but
knowledge. Metamemory awareness refers to our feelings or experiences of our own memory.
For example, if a person feels certain that he or she will remember later something just learned
FORGETTING
Like memory or remembering, forgetting is also an important cognitive function of mind that
influences our behavior in a significant way. Forgetting refers to failure either to recall or to
retain, i.e., it is a failure to receive the learnt or acquired information into present consciousness.
Though, it is correct that our memory system has a very wide scope of retaining vast number
of information of various kinds for considerably longer period of time, it is also true that for
certain reasons, it is not possible to keep on remembering each and every bit of information.
We forget many of the acquired experiences or information. Many of these are forgotten
because of infrequent use owing to which the memory traces lose their strength and become
weak and gradually fade out. This causes forgetting. Sometimes, new information invades or
intrudes causing displacement or superimposition over the older traces by the newer ones.
Further, often we voluntarily forget some experiences which are unpleasant and cause distress.
Thus, the past experiences do not always remain fresh. Therefore, forgetting is a common event
of life.
As regards the nature of forgetting, there seems no agreement in the views held by different
psychologists. There are at least four different ways in which the phenomenon of forgetting
This theory is the outcome of extensive experimental work of Ebbinghaus (1885). This theory
can be better understood with the help of a curve called retention curve or forgetting curve.
A retention curve graphically shows the amount we remember as a function of passage of time.
The first attempt to measure and graphically represent the amount of retention was made by
German psychologist Ebbinghaus (1885). He himself served as a subject and used the method
of saving. He did many experiments on memory. In one of his experiments he memorized a list
of nonsense syllables such as BEZ, XES, JEK, VAM, PUW etc. He waited for various lengths
of retention intervals (RI) ranging from 20 minutes to 31 days and then relearned the same
syllables. In this way, he was able to obtain saving scores for different intervals between
The Ebbinghaus’ curve further shows that forgetting is a passive process which is caused by
the time factor. The longer the experiences, the poorer is the memory. This hypothesis assumes
that learning leaves a trace in the brain. This trace is a kind of physical change which was not
present before learning. With the passage of time, the normal metabolic process of brain causes
fading or decaying of the memory traces formed in course of learning. Consequently, the traces
begin to disintegrate gradually which eventually lead to disappearance of the traces from
memory stores. If the learnt material is not kept in practice for long, the traces fade out due to
lack of rehearsal. Besides, new learning experiences superimpose the older one. Thus, older
REASONS OF FORGETTING
Measuring of forgetting is only the first step in the long journey toward explaining why
forgetting occurs. It can be explore by looking at factors that may affect encoding, storage, and
retrieval processes.
Ineffective encoding
A great deal of forgetting may only appear to be forgetting. The information in question may
never have been inserted into memory in the first place. Since you can’t really forget something
you never learned, this phenomenon is sometimes called pseudoforgetting. We opened the
chapter with an example of pseudoforgetting. People usually assume that they know what a
penny looks like, but most have actually failed to encode this information. Pseudoforgetting is
usually attributable to lack of attention. Even when memory codes are formed for new
(Brown & Craik, 2000). The research on levels of processing shows that some approaches to
encoding lead to more forgetting than others (Craik & Tulving, 1975). For example, if you’re
distracted while you read your textbooks, you may be doing little more than saying the words
to yourself. This is phonemic encoding, which is inferior to semantic encoding for retention of
verbal material. When you can’t remember the information that you’ve read, your forgetting
Decay
memory storage. Decay theory proposes that forgetting occurs because memory traces fade
with time. The implicit assumption is that decay occurs in the physiological mechanisms
responsible for memories. According to decay theory, the mere passage of time produces
As we noted earlier, evidence suggests that decay does contribute to the loss of information
from the sensory and short-term memory stores. However, the critical task for theories of
forgetting is to explain the loss of information from long-term memory. Researchers have not
Interference theory
Interference theory proposes that people forget information because of competition from other
hundreds of studies have shown that interference influences forgetting (Anderson & Neely,
1996; Bower, 2000). In many of these studies, researchers have controlled interference by
varying the similarity between the original material given to subjects (the test material) and the
intervening material is most similar to the test material. Decreasing the similarity should reduce
interference and cause less forgetting. This is exactly what McGeoch and McDonald (1931)
found in an influential study. They had subjects memorize test material that consisted of a list
of two-syllable adjectives. They varied the similarity of intervening learning by having subjects
then memorize one of five lists. In order of decreasing similarity to the test material, the lists
contained synonyms of the test words, antonyms of the test words, unrelated adjectives,
nonsense syllables, and numbers. Later, subjects’ recall of the test material was measured.
Figure 7.19 shows that as the similarity of the intervening material decreased, the amount of
interference: retroactive and proactive (Jacoby, Hessels, & Bopp, 2001). Retroactive
interference occurs when new information impairs the retention of previously learned
information. Retroactive interference occurs between the original learning and the retest on
that learning, during the retention interval. For example, the interference manipulated by
interference occurs when previously learned information interferes with the retention of new
information. Proactive interference is rooted in learning that comes before exposure to the test
material.
Retrieval failure
People often remember things that they were unable to recall at an earlier time. This
phenomenon may be obvious only during struggles with the tip-of-the tongue phenomenon,
but it happens frequently. In fact, a great deal of forgetting may be due to breakdowns in the
process of retrieval. One theory is that retrieval failures may be more likely when a mismatch
occurs between retrieval cues and the encoding of the information you’re searching for.
According to the encoding specificity principle, the value of a retrieval cue depends on how
well it corresponds to the memory code. This principle provides one explanation for the
inconsistent success of retrieval efforts (Tulving & Thomson, 1973). A related line of research
indicates that memory is influenced by the “fi t” between the processing during encoding and
similar to the type of processing required by the subsequent measure of retention. For example,
Morris, Bransford, and Franks (1977) gave subjects a list of words and a task that required
either semantic or phonemic processing. Retention was measured with recognition tests that
emphasized either the meaning or the sound of the words. Semantic processing yielded higher
retention when the testing emphasized semantic factors, while phonemic processing yielded
higher retention when the testing emphasized phonemic factors. Thus, retrieval failures are
more likely when a poor fi t occurs between the processing done during encoding and the
processing invoked by the measure of retention (Lockhart, 2002; Roediger & Guynn, 1996).
Motivated forgetting
Many years ago, Sigmund Freud (1901) came up with an entirely different explanation for
retrieval failures. Freud asserted that people often keep embarrassing, unpleasant, or painful
memories buried in their unconscious. For example, a person who was deeply wounded by
perceived slights at a childhood birthday party might suppress all recollection of that party. In
his therapeutic work with patients, Freud recovered many such buried memories. He theorized
that the memories were there all along, but their retrieval was blocked by unconscious
avoidance tendencies.
The tendency to forget things one doesn’t want to think about is called motivated forgetting,
demonstrate the operation of repression in laboratory studies (Holmes, 1995; Kihlstrom, 2002),
a number of experiments suggest that people don’t remember anxiety-laden material as readily
as emotionally neutral material, just as Freud proposed (Guenther, 1988; Reisner, 1998). Thus,
when you forget unpleasant things such as a dental appointment, a promise to help a friend
Rehearsal
Practice makes perfect, or so you’ve heard. In reality, practice is not likely to guarantee
perfection, but it usually leads to improved retention. Studies show that retention improves
with increased rehearsal (Greene, 1992). This improvement presumably occurs because
rehearsal helps to transfer information into long-term memory. Although the benefits of
practice are well known, people have a curious tendency to overestimate their knowledge of a
topic and how well they will perform on a subsequent memory test of this knowledge (Koriat
& Bjork, 2005). That’s why it is a good idea to informally test yourself on information that you
think you have mastered before confronting a real test. In addition to checking your mastery,
recent research suggests that testing actually enhances retention, a phenomenon dubbed the
testing effect (Roediger & Karpicke, 2006a). Theorists are not sure yet, but the key may be that
testing forces students to engage in deep processing of the material, as well as transfer
appropriate processing (Roediger & Karpicke, 2006b). In any event, self-testing appears to be
an excellent memory tool, which suggests that it would be prudent to take the Practice Tests in
this text or additional tests available on the website for the book. Another possible remedy for
overconfidence is trying to overlearn material (Driskell, Willis, & Copper, 1992). Overlearning
refers to continued rehearsal of material after you fi rst appear to have mastered it.
Elaboration
Elaboration enriches encoding by linking a stimulus to other information. Visual imagery may
work in much the same way, creating two memory codes rather than just one. Encoding that
Elaboration is linking a stimulus to other information at the time of encoding. For example,
let’s say you read that phobias are often caused by classical conditioning, and you apply this
idea to your own fear of spiders. In doing so, you are engaging in elaboration. The additional
Mnemonics
Mnemonics are strategies for putting information into an organized framework in order to
remember it more easily. To remember the names of the Great Lakes, for example, you could
use the acronym HOMES (for Huron, Ontario, Michigan, Erie, and Superior). Verbal
organization is the basis for many mnemonics. You can link items by weaving them into a
story, a sentence, or a rhyme. To help customers remember where they left their cars, some
large parking lots have replaced traditional section designations such as “A1” or “G8” with the
names of colors, months, or animals. Customers can then tie the location of their cars to
mother’s birthday.” One simple but powerful mnemonic is called the method of loci
(pronounced “LOWsigh”), or the “method of places.” To use this method, first think about a
set of familiar locations. Use your home, for example. You might imagine walking through the
front door, around all four corners of the living room, and through each of the other rooms.
Next, imagine that each item you want to remember is in one of these locations. Creating vivid
or unusual images of how the items appear in each location seems to be particularly effective
(Kline & Groninger, 1991). For example, tomatoes smashed against the front door or bananas
hanging from the bedroom ceiling might be helpful in recalling these items on a grocery list.
Whenever you want to remember a new list, you can create new images using the same
***********************