Wing L. e Gould J. (1979) - Deficiências Graves de Interação Social e Anormalidades Associadas em Crianças Epidemiologia e Classificação.
Wing L. e Gould J. (1979) - Deficiências Graves de Interação Social e Anormalidades Associadas em Crianças Epidemiologia e Classificação.
Wing L. e Gould J. (1979) - Deficiências Graves de Interação Social e Anormalidades Associadas em Crianças Epidemiologia e Classificação.
1, 1979
INTRODUCTION
often enough to form specific syndromes. Examples are the "dementia pre-
cocissima" and "dementia precosissima catatonica" of De Sanctis (1906,
1908), Earl's (1934) "primitive catatonic psychosis of idiocy," the
"dementia" occurring between 3 and 5 years of age described by both
Heller and Weygandt (see Hulse, 1954), Mahler's (1952) "symbiotic psy-
chosis," the "autistic psychopathy" of Asperger (1944; Van Krevelen,
1971), and Kanner's (1943) "early infantile autism." These "syndromes,"
although thought by their proponents to be specific, have many features in
common. Individual children may show mixtures of items from more than
one syndrome, making diagnosis difficult. When discussing this subject,
Anthony (1958a) wrote, "The cult of names added chaos to an already con-
fused situation, since there did not seem to be a sufficiency of symptoms to
share out among the various prospectors, without a good deal of overlap."
Some attempts have been made to subclassify the whole range of the
clinical phenomenon being described here, rather than to select particular
subgroups and ignore the rest. Anthony (1958a,b, 1962), Kolvin (1971), and
Rutter (1972) divided on age of onset, suggesting that if the abnormal
behavior began before age 3, the cause, course, and prognosis were differ ent
from those associated with onset between 3 and 5 years. They also made the
important point that the conditions being discussed here should not be
classified as forms of schizophrenia. An illness resembling adult schizo-
phrenia can be seen rarely in childhood, but never before age 5 years.
Despite this work, classification remains in a most unsatisfactory
state. There are problems not only in subdividing within the group as a
whole but in relating it to other handicapping conditions of childhood. In
particular, abnormalities of social interaction and language development,
and stereotyped behavior can be found in some children administratively
categorized as mentally retarded (Haracopos & Kelstrup, 1978), especially
those with intelligence quotients below 50 (defined in the United Kingdom
as in the severely retarded range). Conversely, the majority of children who
show the abnormalities being considered perform as mildly or severely men-
tally retarded even on tests not involving language (DeMyer, 1976; Kolvin,
Humphrey, & McNay, 1971; Lotter, 1967; Rutter & Lockyer, 1967; Rutter,
Shaffer, & Sturge, 1975; Wing, Yeates, Brierley, & Gould, 1976), though
there are some with normal intelligence on nonverbal or even verbal tests
(Bartak & Rutter, 1976).
In order to investigate these problems of classification, the present
authors decided to carry out an epidemiological survey, within a defined
geographical area, of all children in a specified age range who showed one
or more of the impairments and abnormal behaviors discussed above,
whether or not there were associated organic conditions or additional
handicaps, such as deafness or blindness, and regardless of level of intel-
ligence or age of onset.
Impairments of Social Interaction 13
The aims of the study were to find (a) the prevalence and distribution
of the three types of abnormalities, and whether they tended to occur
together, (b) how the clinical pictures of which they formed a part could be
subgrouped, and (c) how they were related to mental retardation.
No prior assumptions were made as to the specificity of any of the
previously proposed syndromes that include these abnormalities.
METHOD
Selection of Subjects
The subjects were selected from children aged under 15 years on the
chosen census day, December 31, 1970, who had parents living in the
former southeast London borough of Camberwell. This mainly working-
class area had a total population of 155,000, of whom 35,000 were under 15
years old.
All children, whether at home or in residential care, who were known
to the local health, education, or social services for reasons of physical or
mental handicap or behavior disturbance were identified through the
Camberwell cumulative psychiatric and mental retardation register (Wing &
Halley, 1972) and the records kept by the local services. These 914 children
were then screened, as described in Wing et al. (1976) and 132 of them were
selected on one or both of the following criteria.
The first criterion was the presence of at least one of the following
items, regardless of level of intelligence: (a) absence or impairment of social
interaction, especially with peers; (b) absence or impairment of develop -
ment of verbal and nonverbal language; (c) repetitive, stereotyped activities
of any kind. (These are defined below, under "Behavioral Variables.")
The second criterion was a level of function on formal tests or on
educational achievement in the severely retarded range, regardless of the
pattern of behavior and impairments. The only exceptions were nonmobile
children, 28 in all, who were excluded because their inability to walk un-
aided limited the possibility of their showing abnormal behavior.
At the time of assessment, 108 of the 132 children selected were
known to the preschool or school-age services for children with severe re-
tardation, 10 were in schools for children with mild educational subnor -
mality and 6 in schools or classes for autistic children, 3 were attending
schools for the deaf or partially hearing, 2 were in units for deaf/blind
children, 1 was in a school for the partially sighted, 1 was at a school for
delicate children, and 1 was in a day nursery. None was in a school for nor-
mal children, though 1 autistic child was later transferred to such a school.
14 Wing and Gould
The final step in the study was the intensive investigation of the
children identified from the preliminary screening procedures. Their
patterns of handicaps, behavior, and skills were examined in detail. Psycho-
logical and medical data were collected in order to provide independent
criteria for evaluating possible methods of clinical classification.
At the time of the detailed interviews with parents and teachers, the
ages of the children ranged from 2 years 2 months to 18 years. The inves-
tigators have remained in touch with the children since the interviews and
tests were completed.