Life Span Development Week 4
Life Span Development Week 4
Life Span Development Week 4
Theorists do not all agree about how to define or characterize emotions. They do agree that
emotions have survival value. Emotions are powerful motivators of behavior, and spontaneous
emotional expressions help us to communicate with one another. For infants, this communicative
function helps initiate reciprocal interaction with a caregiver.
Emotions enhance cognitive functioning like planning. They play a role in mental health and
wellness. Positive life outcomes are associated with higher levels of emotional intelligence.
Researchers agree that basic emotions are similar across cultures.
Izard argues that even infants’ expressive behaviors reveal these emotions
(happiness, interest, sadness, anger, disgust, and fear), that they are innate, and
that each has its own neural circuitry, although more elaborate emotion schemas
emerge as a function of experience.
LeDoux found that there is an “early warning emotional system.” Some sensory
information bypasses the cortex, causing fear and allowing a rapid response to
stimuli that may pose a threat.
Depressed individuals show lower levels of left sided activation compared to right-sided
activation.
Synchrony
Interactions between mothers and
infants exhibiting a repetitive-
rhythmic organization.
When synchrony breaks down, infants
tend to show distress and mothers tend to initiate repairs that regulate the infants’
emotions, encouraging longer periods of positive affect.
The still-face paradigm is used to study synchrony, along with its breakdown and repair.
Sensitive adults adjust their emotion management behaviors to the needs and abilities of infants
as they grow. And as infants get older, they engage in social referencing, basing their own
emotional reactions to ambiguous situations, on their caregivers’ behaviors.
Theorists such as Bowlby and Erikson have proposed that the infant’s first relationships with
caregivers provide them with working models of the self, of others, and of relationships.
Erikson - Argued that consistent, sensitive care helps the infant establish basic trust in others
and feelings of worthiness, which contribute to optimism about future relationships.
Bowlby’s Attachment Theory - Biologically prepared behaviors bring infant and mother into a
close relationship, or attachment system, that emerges in stages. By about 8 months the infant
has a strong affectional bond to the primary caregiver, which leads to both separation anxiety and
stranger anxiety. The attachment system serves the purpose of proximity maintenance and
provides a secure base and haven.
Both infants and parents display expressive and behavioral characteristics that foster attachment.
Orienting behaviors (e.g., caregiver stays in infant’s field of vision)
Recognition (e.g., infant’s early recognition of mother’s voice)
Intuitive parenting (e.g., caregiver ascribes meaning to infant behavior)
Researchers have identified some of the neurological processes and structures involved in this
bonding system. Oxytocin is associated with more caregiving behavior in both mothers and
fathers, and for most adults’ close contact with infants promotes increases in oxytocin levels.
Maternal caregiving through the first year of life is correlated with infants’ attachment quality at
12 months.
Mothers of securely attached babies provide consistent, sensitive care to their infants.
Mothers of insecurely attached babies are more likely to be insensitive in one way or
another.
Mothers of disorganized-disoriented babies can be abusive.
Babies form attachments to both their mothers and fathers, as well as other frequent caregivers.
Each parent’s sensitivity influences the quality of the infant’s attachment to that parent, although
the primary caregiver may have the most influence on a baby’s attachments overall. Across
cultures the connection between sensitive caregiving (responsiveness to distress) and security of
attachment has been corroborated.
It is possible for children with reactive attachment disorder to later form secure attachments.
Early attachment quality can predict later psychosocial functioning, including dependency, self-
confidence, and social skills. If children have secure attachments with both parents, their later
functioning is more positive than if they have a secure attachment with only one parent. It is
unclear whether the early attachment quality is the cause of later psychosocial adjustment or
whether the ongoing quality of the parents’ caregiving is the key predictor.
Once a pattern is established between parent and child, it tends to be repeated. However, it can
be changed, and if it is, the child’s working model of relationships, of self, and of others can also
change. In some studies, parents’ own attachment quality has been predictive of the quality of
attachment they establish with their infants. Focusing on the quality of infant–caregiver relations,
rather than the specific parenting practices, seems to provide a better understanding of later
socioemotional development.
Attachment in Context
Attachment contexts
Parents’ relationships
Extended family relationships
Neighborhoods. Etc.
All contexts can affect infants’ emotional well-being and attachment security. Children under 5
are especially likely to be exposed to negative experiences in these contexts, including
traumatizing events such as maltreatment, accidents, and marital conflict. Attachment security
can be a protective factor, but children who have such experiences are also at increased risk of
forming insecure attachments.
The Self-System
William James
Viewed the self-system as multidimensional
The self as subject
The self as active agent or “I”
o Is continuously experienced, is distinguished from others
The self-concept
o The object of our own observations and evaluations
The “Me”
o Consists of personal attributes, the material (or physical), social, and spiritual
characteristics of self
Self-esteem
o The evaluation of one’s attributes, can be good, bad, or neutral.
Charles Cooley
Introduced a developmental perspective into theorizing about the self
Self-representations are constructed from our interactions with others,
especially caregivers
We build a looking-glass self that reflects our view of how others see us
Self-regulation involves both emotional and behavioral control. Emotional control begins in
infancy when caregiver–infant interactions give babies outside assistance with affective control.
The caregiving relationship is the context in which the infant’s own capacity for emotion
regulation develops.
Behavior regulation begins in the second year when toddlers achieve objective self-recognition.
The simultaneous summary emergence of symbolic or representational thought and other
cognitive skills allow the child to begin learning and storing rules or standards of conduct. By
late in the second year, self-conscious emotions emerge, such as embarrassment or guilt,
emotions that require awareness of self and of others’ judgments. Between 2 and 3, children
show such emotions when they realize they have broken a rule or made a mistake. Once these
capacities are in place, the child can begin to control her own behavior based on others’
expectations.
Early Socialization
Authoritative parenting
Associated with the most positive outcomes in child development
Adaptability, competence, good social relations, low levels of antisocial behavior, high
self-esteem, and good self-regulation.
The correlations between parenting styles and outcomes are significant but moderate, indicating
that such outcomes are also influenced by other factors, not just parenting style.
Methods of control
Power assertion - Involves physical punishment or withdrawal of privileges
o Effective for immediate control of behavior, but may not have longer term
benefits
o Harsher forms can have some negative side effects, such as increased
aggressiveness in children.
Psychological control - Includes love withdrawal
o Generates high anxiety and elicits immediate compliance
o Has few effects on long term self-regulation.
Induction - Providing explanations and emphasizing benefits to the child and to others
o most effective for promoting internalization of rules and longer term self-
regulation.
Some parenting practices may have different meaning, and therefore different outcomes,
depending on ethnicity and culture. In some cultures, harsh practices are normative, and parents
may combine high levels of warmth with corporal punishment.
Children’s internalization of rules and values is associated with conscience, feeling distress when
one violates a rule. This is the beginning of moral development.
Children are more cooperative and compliant when they share a warm, responsive relationship
with a parent. Their compliance can also be related to anxious arousal. Hoffman argued that mild
arousal can help children pay attention to a rule but doesn’t make them so anxious that they pay
attention only to how afraid they are. This is consistent with the finding that mild power
assertion practices are more likely than more severe power assertion practices to lead to
children’s internalization of rules and their long-term compliance. Further, Hoffman’s ideas help
explain why children who have fearful temperaments respond best to gentle discipline, but
children who are fearless are unfazed by either gentle or harsh disciplinary practices. The most
important determiner of compliance for fearless children is the warmth of their relationship to the
parent.
Bibliography
Broderick, P. C., & Blewitt, P. (2019). The life span: Human development for helping
professionals. (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Allyn & Bacon.
LS 4&5 Vocabulary
Chapter 4
The function of emotions
Emotions have survival value
Emotions are powerful motivators of behavior
Emotional expressions help us to communicate
Emotions enhance cognitive functioning, including planning and social perspective
taking
Maclean argued
Emotions originate in evolutionarily old system (limbic circuit), serves survival function
for many species
Ledoux
Found an "early warning emotional system," with some sensory information bypassing
the cortex and rational thought
Emotion regulation
Is one of the cornerstones of emotional well-being and positive adjustment throughout the
life span
Emotion regulation
Encompasses the strategies and behaviors we use to moderate our emotional experiences
in order to meet the demands of different situations or achieve our goals
Adult caregivers
Serve the critical function of helping to manage the newborn's affect or to modulate
affective expression while scaffolding the infant's developing emotion regulation
Sychrony
Repetitive-rhythmic organization, a temporal coordination of nonverbal behaviors
Social referencing
Basing emotional reactions on caregivers' behaviors
Bolwby proposed
Relationships an infant has with one or a few caregivers during the 1st year of life
provide him with a working model of himself and others
Basic trust
Seeing others as dependable and trustworthy
Attachment theory
(Bowlby) infant and caregiver participate in and attachment system that has evolved to
serve the purpose of keeping the infant safe and assuring his survival
Stranger anxiety
Increased tendency to be wary of strangers and seek comfort and protection of the
primary caregiver
Securely attached
Show distress when separated for the mother, often crying and trying to go after her.
Greet her happily upon return
Anxious ambivalent
Often seemed stressed and are quite distressed when separated from their mothers. Upon
reuniting, they may act angry and resist the mother.
Avoidant
Fail to cry when separated from their mothers. Actively ignore her upon return
Disorganized - disoriented
Produce contradictory behaviors, showing both inclination to approach the mother when
stressed and a tendency to avoid her when she approaches
Ocytocin
Hormone released in the hypothalamus and modulates the transmission of impulses
Oxytocin promotes
Physical proximity, responsive caregiving, empathy, and affection
Oxytocin reduces
Stress and helps mothers deal with the physical and emotional challenges of childbirth
and childrearing
Chapter 5
William James
Distinguished self-as-subject ("i") and the self-as-object, or self-concept ("me")
"i" continuously experienced, distinct from others
"me" a collection of personal attributes
Self-esteem is evaluation of one's attributes
James Cooley
Added developmental perspective
Self-representation constructed in interactions with others over time
Self-system
Includes aspects related to the self, such as self-concept, self-regulation, and self-esteem
Self concept
A description of personal attributes
Self esteem
One's evaluation of personal attributes
Valence
Affective value of a characteristic either good, bad, or neutral
Self recognition
Is a clear signal the child has begun to formulate a conscious concept of self
Self-conscious emotions
Take place after objective self-awareness ex: shame, guilt, and pride
Child centered
Sidelining parental needs
Parent centered
Show little responsiveness to their children's concerns and are unlikely to do things to
meet those concerns
4 parenting styles
The authoritative style
- highly responsive and highly demanding
The authoritarian style
- low responsiveness but highly demanding
The permissive style
- moderately to highly responsive but low on demandingness
The neglecting-uninvolved style
- low on both dimensions
Other possible explanations for the correlation of parenting style with child outcomes
Shared inheritance and traits may influence parenting style and child behavior
Child's temperament influences parenting, outcomes, susceptibility to parenting strategies
Cultural context
Shapes parenting practices and effectiveness
Important not to conflate culture with geography, sex, or race
Wide variation both within and between groups
Authoritative parents
Seem to get the best results from their children
Differential susceptibility
When a physical make up makes and individual more likely to be affected by
environmental influences than other people.