Argument Structure of Transition and Tra 6e47bfc5
Argument Structure of Transition and Tra 6e47bfc5
Argument Structure of Transition and Tra 6e47bfc5
a
Udayana University, Denpasar, Indonesia
b
Udayana University, Denpasar, Indonesia
67
68 ISSN: 2455-8028
1 Introduction
Different predicates require different numbers of arguments. In general, there are some constructions of clauses in
English, SV, SVO, SV OBL etc. This sentence considerably belongs to SV, someone was laughing loudly in the next
room (Quirk et al., 1985). The verb of this sentence is laughing and someone is the subject. The following example is
SVO, my mother usually enjoys parties very much. The verb of this sentence is enjoys, and this verb requires two
core arguments, my mother as subject and parties as object. Other than the number of arguments, the meaning also
provides an essential rule in the appearance of an argument, and it could be related to the category of the verb,
transfer predicates within the view of semantics (Newton & Kennedy, 1996; Bickel & Yādava, 2000).
Movement can be expressed by the verbs like come and go. These verbs convey a movement or position
changing. According to Kreidler (2002), transition predicates express the going or coming from one place to another.
This type of verb or predicate must or might have its own characteristics, either the types of arguments or its number.
This type of predicate might be interrelated with transfer verbs as having similar notions of movement. Transfer
predicate is one of the subjects in semantics (Clifton Jr et al., 1965). According to Kreidler (2002), verbs and other
predicates determine the meaning of the sentence expresses and what roles assigned by the arguments. Mostly,
transfer predicates express transition, movement from one place to another, respectively the source and the goal.
Another function of transfer predicate shows the change of position of a track, represented by focusing the verbs in
path. This study concerned on the argument structure of transfer and transition verbs (Marantz, 2013; Gropen et al.,
1991). It was decided to discuss a particular class of verb; slide verbs based on Levin (1993), which consists of verbs
bounce, float, move, roll and slide in order to provide an accurate understanding.
Argument Structure
According to Kroger (2005), arguments are those elements which are “selected” by the verb; they are required or
permitted by certain predicates, but not by others. It means certain arguments might be obligatory for certain verbs.
Dealing with argument structure, it is necessary to involve the discussion of grammatical relation and the semantic
role since it is the elements that mapped within the argument structure (Lidz & Gleitman, 2004).
Grammatical Relation
According to Kroger (2005), in order to express grammaticality, arguments must be assigned a grammatical relation
within the clause. In addition, it is determined by the syntactic and morphological properties. Van Valin (2001),
stated that there are strong tendencies for certain phenomena to involve a particular relation and examples of the
most likely constructions to pick out subjects, direct objects or indirect objects are presented. However, Greenbaum
& Nelson (2009), stated that regular sentences consist of a subject and a predicate, and the predicate contains at least
a verb. Within this understanding, it classifies the basic sentence structure, namely SV, SVO, SVOO, SVA, SVC,
etc.
The term “subject” in English grammar refers to a person or thing (noun or pronoun) that serves as one of the
primary components of a clause and performs the action (or verb). According to Kroeger (2005), here are the
following properties of subject in English:
a) Word order: In basic English sentences, the subject normally comes before the verb, and object and other
elements come after the verb. Van Valin (2001), stated that word order is a common means of signalling
grammatical relations, and it is also relevant in many languages to the relation between adpositions and their
objects, since the object must immediately follow (preposition) or precede (postposition) the adposition.
However, postposition is not concerned in this study as English does not have postposition.
b) Pronoun forms: Pronouns have a special form when they appear in a certain position that indicates whether
they are subject or object pronouns.
c) Agreement with verb: In the simple present tense, a morphological marking, a suffix -s, is added to the verb
when a third person subject is singular. However, the number and person of the object or any other element in
the sentence does not give any effect to the form of the verb. According to Van Valin (2001), the primary
coding properties are verb agreement, case marking and (in languages with very rigid word order) the position
of an argument in the sentence, which may serve to express a particular grammatical relation.
d) Content questions: If the subject is replaced by a question word (who or what), the rest of the sentence
remains unchanged. However, if the object is replaced by a question word, there must be an auxiliary before
the subject.
e) Tag questions: A tag question is used to seek confirmation of a statement. It always contains a pronoun which
refers back to the subject, and never to any other element in the sentence.
Kroeger (2005), stated that the term “indirect object” in traditional grammar is used to refer to the semantic role of
recipient (or sometimes beneficiary), rather than to a specific Grammatical Relation. Van Valin (2001), stated that
the direct– indirect object contrast is not appropriate for languages of this kind and that a different distinction is
required, namely primary object (the recipient of ditransitive verbs or the usual direct object of plain transitive verbs)
versus secondary object (the theme of ditransitive verbs). On the other hand, Dryer (1986), stated that A Primary
Object is an Indirect Object in a ditransitive clause or a Direct Object in a monotransitive clause, while a Secondary
Object is a Direct Object in a ditransitive clause. However, a different view is adopted in this study. Primary object is
regarded as the first object that comes after the verb; on the other hand, the secondary object is regarded as the object
that comes after the primary object.
Kroeger (2005), defined that element which are not closely related to the meaning of the predicate but which are
important to help the hearer understand the flow of the story are called adjunct. Subjects and objects are often
referred to as terms, or direct arguments. Arguments which are not subjects or objects are called indirect or oblique
arguments (Suryasa, 2016; Chandio et al., 2019). In other word, an oblique argument is an argument of a relation that
is marked by a preposition. (Syntactically, oblique arguments aren't direct arguments; in other words, they aren't
subjects or direct objects or second objects). On the other hand, adjunct is an optional unit within the relation,
whereas oblique is semantically required by the verb. However, the terms oblique and adjunct may appear to be
similar (Mohamed & Oussalah, 2019; Osman et al., 2012). All oblique arguments are marked with prepositions,
whereas adjuncts are always optional. In the subcategorization, however, adjunct is not obligatory. In order to
provide better understanding related to oblique and adjunct, a data taken from Kroeger (2005), is presented. Henry
put the money into his pocket. My daughter swallowed a penny last night. The argument after the preposition “into”
is considered as oblique as it is required by the verb “put”. It is different from the second data presented, the italic,
considered as adjunct which can be omitted at any time without causing any sense of incompleteness.
Ariasih, N. L. P., & Sedeng, I. N. (2022). Argument structure of transition and transfer verbs. International Journal
of Linguistics, Literature and Culture, 8(3), 67-75.
https://doi.org/10.21744/ijllc.v8n3.2076
70 ISSN: 2455-8028
Semantic roles
According to Kroeger (2005), semantic roles are defined as helpful elements to classify arguments into broad
semantic categories according to the kind of role they play in the situations described by their predicates. While Van
Valin (2001), stated that each verb or other predicate has a certain number of arguments, each of which bears a
distinct semantic role; this will be referred to as a verb’s argument structure. Huddleston & Pullum (2005), stated
that, there is something in both of these that is relevant to a definition of the subject at the general level: many
languages have a function in the clause that is often associated with the semantic role of actor or with the topic and
that shows other signs of primary syntactic importance in the clause (though some languages seem to be organized
rather differently). Kroeger (2005), divided the semantic roles into some roles. These roles are presented as follows:
According to Kreidler (2002), verbs and other predicates determine what meaning a sentence expresses and, to a
large extent, they determine what roles the accompanying arguments have, and even what kinds of noun phrases
occur as arguments. Kreidler (2002), defined transition predicate as a verb or predicate that expresses the going or
coming of entities from one place to another. In order to provide better understanding related to transition verbs, a
data taken from Kreidler (2002), is presented.
This clause has verb “go” and this verb has 3 arguments as follows; the bus, Greenville and Stratford. In this data,
movement is shown from one place to another, Greenville and Stratford respectively. This verb belongs to transition
as it expresses the going and coming of an entity. However, there is no entity which causes the movement. Therefore,
verb “go” is regarded as transition verb.
According to Kreidler (2002), transfer verbs are the causative equivalent of the transition verbs. Furthermore,
transfer predicate has a role which causes the transition to occur. In order to provide better understanding related to
transfer verb, a data taken from Kreidler (2002), is presented.
This clause has verb “drives” and this verb has 4 arguments as follows; Fenwick, Greenville, Stratford and Compton.
In this data, the movement is shown from one place to another, Greenville and Stratford respectively. This verb
belongs to transfer as it expresses the going and coming of an entity. And this verb has causative argument.
Therefore, verb “drives” is regarded as transfer verb. Slide verbs involved to the verb of sending and carrying bears
Bounce
The possible constructions operated by the verb bounce involves subject-verb, subject-verb-object, subject-verb-
oblique, subject-verb-object-oblique, and subject-verb-oblique-oblique. However, it is also possible to operate
construction with subject-verb-object-oblique-oblique as oblique can be added to the verb depending on the
information required to be conveyed. The clause which involves this verb can assign causative argument, and it is
indicated based on the structure and the meaning. While the clause is intransitive, it does not require any causative
argument; on the other hand, the transitive clause requires causative argument. Moreover, based on the framework,
the transfer has a causative argument, and the transition does not.
Float
The possible constructions operated by the verb float involves subject-verb, subject-verb-object, subject-verb-
oblique, subject-verb-object-oblique, and subject-verb-oblique-oblique. However, it is also possible to operate
construction with subject-verb-object-oblique-oblique as oblique can be added to the verb depending on the
information required to be conveyed. The clause which involves this verb can assign causative argument, and it is
indicated based on the structure and the meaning. While the clause is intransitive, it does not require any causative
argument; on the other hand, the transitive clause requires causative argument. Moreover, based on the framework,
the transfer has a causative argument, and the transition does not.
Ariasih, N. L. P., & Sedeng, I. N. (2022). Argument structure of transition and transfer verbs. International Journal
of Linguistics, Literature and Culture, 8(3), 67-75.
https://doi.org/10.21744/ijllc.v8n3.2076
72 ISSN: 2455-8028
Move
The possible constructions operated by the verb move involves subject-verb, subject-verb-object, subject-verb-
oblique, subject-verb-object-oblique, subject-verb-oblique-oblique, and subject-verb-object-oblique-oblique. The
clause which involves this verb can assign causative argument, and it is indicated based on the structure and the
meaning. While the clause is intransitive, it does not require any causative argument; on the other hand, the transitive
clause requires causative argument. Moreover, based on the framework, the transfer has a causative argument, and
the transition does not.
Roll
The possible constructions operated by the verb roll involves subject-verb, subject-verb-object, subject-verb-oblique,
subject-verb-object-oblique, and subject-verb-oblique-oblique. However, it is also possible to operate construction
with subject-verb-object-oblique-oblique as oblique can be added to the verb depending on the information required
to be conveyed. The clause which involves this verb can assign causative argument, and it is indicated based on the
structure and the meaning. While the clause is intransitive, it does not require any causative argument; on the other
hand, the transitive clause requires causative argument. Moreover, based on the framework, the transfer has a
causative argument, and the transition does not.
The possible constructions operated by the verb slide involves subject-verb, subject-verb-object, subject-verb-
oblique, subject-verb-object-oblique, and subject-verb-oblique-oblique. However, it is also possible to operate
construction with subject-verb-object-oblique-oblique as oblique can be added to the verb depending on the
information required to be conveyed. The clause which involves this verb can assign causative argument, and it is
indicated based on the structure and the meaning. While the clause is intransitive, it does not require any causative
argument; on the other hand, the transitive clause requires causative argument. Moreover, based on the framework,
the transfer has a causative argument, and the transition does not.
4 Conclusion
Based on the data that have been analysed and presented in the previous chapter, the conclusions of both problems
are presented as follows. The grammatical relations operated within transition and transfer verbs with the class of
slide verb involve subject, object and oblique. Verb bounce, float, move, roll and slide can be constructed with
Subject-Verb, Subject-Verb-Object, Subject-Verb-Oblique, Subject-Verb-Object-Oblique and Subject-Verb-
Oblique-Oblique. However, the structure Subject-Verb-Object-Oblique-Oblique only appears in the verb move.
Related to the semantic roles of the arguments in the verb of bounce, float, move, roll and slide involved agent,
theme, location, source, path and goal. The semantic role played by subject arguments of the verb of bounce, float,
move, roll and slide in Subject-Verb construction is theme or agent. On the other hand, the subject arguments in
Subject-Verb-Object construction always play the role of agent. However, its object arguments always play the role
of theme. The role played by the subject arguments in Subject-Verb-Oblique is theme or agent. However, the oblique
plays the role of source or goal. The subject arguments in subject-verb-object-oblique construction always play the
role of agent. However, its object arguments always play the role of theme. Moreover, the oblique plays the role of
path,source, goal or location. The role played by the subject arguments in Subject-Verb-Oblique-Oblique is theme or
agent. However, the obliques always play the role of source and goal in sequence. On the other hand, the subject
arguments in Subject-Verb-Object-Oblique-Oblique construction always play the role of agent and its object
arguments always play the role of theme. However, the obliques always play the role of source and goal in sequence.
Each clause can be categorized as a transition or transfer verb. The clauses of which the construction are Subject-
Verb, Subject-Verb-Oblique, and Subject-Verb-Oblique-Oblique are considered as transition as providing
information about movement without showing an entity which becomes the causer of the movement. On the other
hand, the clauses of which the construction is Subject-Verb-Object, Subject-Verb-Object-Oblique, and Subject-Verb-
Object-Oblique-Oblique are considered as transfer because the argument has causative argument which becomes the
causer of the movement.
Ariasih, N. L. P., & Sedeng, I. N. (2022). Argument structure of transition and transfer verbs. International Journal
of Linguistics, Literature and Culture, 8(3), 67-75.
https://doi.org/10.21744/ijllc.v8n3.2076
74 ISSN: 2455-8028
Statement of authorship
The authors have a responsibility for the conception and design of the study. The authors have approved the final
article.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to two anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments on the earlier version of this paper.
Ariasih, N. L. P., & Sedeng, I. N. (2022). Argument structure of transition and transfer verbs. International Journal
of Linguistics, Literature and Culture, 8(3), 67-75.
https://doi.org/10.21744/ijllc.v8n3.2076