1990 - Functional Analysis in Behavior Therapy
1990 - Functional Analysis in Behavior Therapy
oo
Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. Copyright 0 1990 Pergamon Press plc
Stephen N. Haynes
University of Hawaii
William H. O’Brien
The authors would like to thank Linda Gannon for her comments on an earlier draft of this
manuscript.
Correspondence should be addressed to Stephen N. Haynes, Department of Psychology,
University of Hawaii at Manoa, 2430 Campus Road, Honolulu, HI 96822.
649
650 S. N. Haynes and W. H. O’Brien
‘Client is a generic term referring to the object of behavioral assessment or therapy. A client
may include a family, classroom, inpatient adult, institution, teacher, etc.
‘Behuavior analyst is a generic term referring to a behavioral scientist interested in the
understanding of human behavior and the treatment of behavior disorders; it is not
restricted to individuals with any specific theoretical orientation.
FunctionalAnalysis 651
A functional relationship exists when two or more variables have shared variance:
Some parameter (e.g., rate, magnitude, length, age) of one variable is associated
with some parameter of another. In an alternative language, variables are func-
tionally related when they demonstrate a mathematical relationship. A simple
functional relationship is described by the functional equation Y = f(aX, + bX, +
e) in which the dependent (criterion) variable “Y” is a function of two independent
(predictor, functional) variables, “Xi, Xs” each one with a specific weight (“u” or
“V’) and an error term “e” which indicates that Y is also a function of unmeasured
variables and measurement error.
A focus on functional relationships avoids most of the controversies surround-
ing casual relationships, primarily because functional relationships need not imply
causality. The familiar functional equation E = mc* suggests a functional relation-
ship between energy, “E,” and the speed of light, “c,” without implying that the
speed of light “causes” energy. Also, the temporal and directional relationships
among functionally related variables need not be specified (Bunge, 1963).
Because the functional analysis in behavior therapy is based on identification of
functional relationships, it is affected by the following characteristics:
‘A “causal” variable is a functional variable whose modification leads (or would lead) to a
change in a parameter (e.g., probability, rate, magnitude) of a designated dependent
variable. Causal variables may be original or maintaining and need be neither necessary,
sufficient, exclusive, nor modifiable. Thus, all causal variables are functional variables but
not all functional variables are causal. Extended discussions of the concept of causality can
be found in Asher (1976), Blalock (1971), Bunge (1963), Cook and Campbell (1979),
McCormick (1937), Russell (1948), Sellitz, Wrightsman, and Cook (1976), and Simon
(1977).
652 S. N. Haynes and W. H. O’Brien
There are frequent connotative and denotative inconsistencies in use of the term
“functional analysis” in the behavior therapy literature. These are primarily
attributable to differences in its definitional components and in the degree to
which the definitions reflect the principles of functional relationships outlined
above. Definitions of “functional analysis” have included: (a) a specification of
target behaviors (e.g., Craighead et al., 198 l), (b) demonstrations of “control”
through the manipulation of hypothesized controlling (causal) variables, usually
within a single-subject design (e.g., Nelson, 1983; Nelson & Hayes, 1986; Peterson,
Homer, & Wonderlich, 1982), (c) specification of controlling factors for a class of
behavior problems rather than for an individual case (e.g., Boyd & Levis, 1980),
(d) identification of situational or setting factors (Bootzin, 1975; Maisto, 1985), (e)
stimulus-response or response-response relationships (Hawkins, 1986), (f) motiva-
tional and developmental factors (Kanfer & Philips, 1970), (g) identification of
@~&al functional relationships alternative to those operating for a client (Hawk-
ins, 1986), (h) predictions about a client’s behavior (Turkat & Maisto, 1985), (i)
specification of response components (Bernstein, Borkovec, 8c Coles, 1986), and (j)
an overall conceptual integration of behavior problems, causal and mediating
variables, resources, and so on (Correa & Sutker, 1986). Additionally, a functional
analysis has been defined as a process by some (e.g., Hake & Olvera, 1978) and as
a product by others (e.g., Ferster, 1965).
Semantic imprecision has also resulted from interchangeable use of the terms
“functional analysis, ” “behavioral analysis” (Lanyon & Lanyon, 1976), “functional
behavioral analysis” (Wincze, 1982), “behavioral assessment” (Kalish, 1981), and
“behavioral case formulation” (Wolpe & Turkat, 1985). Other authors seem to
have restricted the domain of functional relationships to those involving response
contingencies (Voeltz & Evans, 1982). Within-author inconsistency in definition of
the term “functional analysis” has also occurred (e.g., Keefe, Kopel, & Gordon,
1978).
In sum, there has been disagreement about the definition of functional analysis,
its underlying assumptions, methods of derivation, relevant components, and
domain of utility. These inconsistencies hinder communication among behavior
analysts about the characteristics of the functional analysis and its role in behavior
therapy. Additionally, many definitions do not incorporate the important princi-
ples of functional relationships they are meant to reflect. Consequently, the
elegance, communicability, cohesiveness, and amenability to empirical refinements
of behavioral construct systems are compromised.
Perhaps more importantly, a number of the definitions of functional analysis
include restraints on the methods considered appropriate for its derivation (e.g.,
functional analysis as “manipulation”), or on the types of variables (e.g., response
contingencies) considered appropriate for inclusion. These constraints limit the
power of our methods of identifying functional relationships and of the conse-
quent causal models of behavior disorders. Indeed, identification of important
functional relationships requires diverse methods, and many classes of variables
may have important causal functions.
To address some of these methodological and conceptual inconsistencies, a
definition of functional analysis is presented which reflects the axioms of func-
tional relationships presented earlier. It is also congruent with the tenets of
behavioral construct systems, is clinically useful, and avoids unnecessary restraints
on the methods for its derivation or on the types of variables that may be included.
Functional analysis is defined as:
There are several inferences implicit in this apparently simple definition. First,
only some variables functionally related to a target behavior will be causal,
controllable, and important. For example, a history of exposure to a major
Functional Analysis 655
analytic procedure is used, causal inferences must be held more tentatively because
temporal precedence is difficult to establish. Causal inferences can be tesded more
powerfully through longitudinal designs such as those used in time-series or
sequential analysis. Finally, systematic manipulation of hypothesized controlling
variables (e.g., ABAB designs), can be used to test causal relationships by demon-
strating reliable control of a dependent variable.
These methods differ dramatically in the confidence engendered in the result-
ing causal inferences. It is important to note here that those methods that are the
most poweful (e.g., manipulation, time-series) tend to also be the most costly and
the most cumbersome in clinical situations. Consequently, most functional analyses
and intervention decisions tend to be based on the less objective analytic proce-
dures resulting many times in a specious understanding of the relationships
between the hypothesized independent and dependent variables (Nelson, 1988).
Most traditional behavioral assessment methods are based on unvalidated
assumptions that the data derived serve as valid marker variables for causal
relationships that occur in the natural environment.5 For example, we often
presume that interview-derived parent reports of how they respond to their child’s
tantrums accurately reflect (i.e., serve as a marker for) responses to their child’s
tantrums in the natural environment. However, validation of marker variables
requires extensive investigation of the degree to which they correlate with
behaviors they are intended to predict - an empirical process seldom pursued.
It should be reemphasized that development of a functional analysis is not tied
to any specific method. Imposed methodological limitations for establishing causal
relationships can also impose constraints on the adequacy of our functional
analyses. If, for example, the functional analysis is methodologically defined as the
manipulation of temporally contiguous response contingencies, then behavior
analysts must confine their search for important causal relationships to this class of
variables (e.g., Baer, 1978). Any construct system which includes methodologically
mandated conceptual restraints will be significantly limited in power, applicability
across populations and behavior problems, and evolution, because only a very
limited set of causal variables will be subject to inclusion. Hence, the explanatory
power of the construct system will be limited to a few persons, situations, behaviors
or variables.
In summary, there are several methods of identifying possible causal relation-
ships for a functional analysis of clients’ behavior disorders, and there is an inverse
relationship between the power of those methods and their clinical utility. The
most powerful, time-series analysis and manipulation, can be prohibitively time-
consuming. Contemporaneously administered behavioral assessment instruments
are a more efficient method of deriving causal hypotheses, but inferences must be
held more tentatively because causal and noncausal functional relationships cannot
be readily differentiated. The use of marker variables can increase the efficiency
and validity of measures of causal relationships but require extensive validation.
One goal of this manuscript is to examine the role and impact of functional
analyses in the design of behavioral intervention programs. In order to estimate
the degree to which intervention designs for clients are based on preintervention
functional analyses in published case studies, a review was conducted of 156 case
studies, published in the years 1985-1988, appearing in Behavior Therupr (N = 21),
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis (N = 78), Behavior Modification (N = 31), and
Behaviour Research and Therapy (N = 26). Each study was coded as to whether or
not the intervention was based on preintervention data collected on functional
relationships. For example, a study in which contingency management or self-
instruction was used to reduce classroom “hyperactivity” would be coded as “based
on a functional analysis” if preintervention data was collected which suggested a
causal role for those factors.
Intervention decisions (i.e., which independent variables were selected for
manipulation) were based on preintervention data collection on functional rela-
tionships in only 20% of the 156 cases. In contrast, intervention decisions in 80%
of the cases were based only on a presumed, but untested, functional relationship
between the designated independent and target variables. These results are
consistent with those reported by Haynes (1986a) and Lennox, Miltenberger,
Spengler, and Efanian (1988), in which preintervention assessment was found to
be infrequently used in the design of behavioral intervention programs. There-
fore, despite the emphasis in behavioral construct systems on individual differ-
ences in the causes of behavior problems and on individualized interventions,
preintervention functional analyses were infrequently conducted; intervention
decisions were usually based on a nomothetic model of the behavior problem.
Although the infrequent use of idiographic preintervention functional analyses
to design behavioral intervention programs may be a result of several factors (see
review in Haynes, 1986a), it is reasonable to suppose that the lack of efficient and
clinically useful methods of identifying, conceptualizing, and organizing func-
tional relationships is an important contributor. Before proposing a clinically
useful schema for constructing functional analyses, a discussion of complex causal
relationships and their impact on functional analysis strategies is warranted.
Areas of needed research on the functional analysis have been well articulated and
will not be reviewed in detail here. In brief, the functional analysis has been
infrequently subjected to empirical investigation. Sufficient data are unavailable
on: (a) its reliability across behavior analysts; (b) its criterion-related or treatment
validity; (c) the variables affecting its reliability and validity; (d) the assessment
instruments which are optimally suited for its derivation and under what condi-
tions they are useful; (e) the clincial decision-making processes involved in its
derivation; (f) markers of complex functional relationships; (g) the best methods
of testing hypothesized functional relationships; (h) its generalizability across
target behaviors and clients; and (i) its impact on intervention efficacy.
The empirical study of the functional analysis would be facilitated by the
adoption of a consistent definition and, especially, by the delineation of a
conceptual model for its construction. The last section of this paper attempts to
660 S. N. Haynes and W. H. O’Brien
integrate the conceptual and methodological issues into a conceptual schema for
the construction of functional analyses.
The central role of the functional analysis in the design of behavioral intervention
programs, their infrequent, informal and unsystematic construction by behavior
analysts, and the complexity of causal relationships which must be incorporated,
suggest that a more structured approach to the development of a functional
analysis in clinical settings is needed. A more systematic method of organizing the
behavior problems and hypothesized causal relationships of a client would facili-
tate selection of variables for intervention and design of intervention programs.
Causal modeling provides the conceptual basis for such an organizational scheme.
Causal modeling has been used for many years to derive and test hypotheses
about causal relationships among sets of variables for diverse targets, such as
coronary heart disease (Pepping 8c Vaitl, 1981), substance abuse (Huba & Bentler,
1982), political violence (Kritzer, 1977), and adolescent antisocial behavior (Patter-
son, 1986). Other discussions of causal model applications have been provided by
Evans (1985), Snyder (1987), and Taylor and Agras (1981). Basically, causal
modeling is a statistical technique for estimating patterns of relationships among a
set of variables, and the underlying organization of that pattern, given an
hypothesized causal structure. Usually, it is conducted with large samples, involves
multiple measures of each variable, and involves cross-sectional data collection.
Importantly, causal modeling provides estimates of shared variance among multi-
ple variables.
We do propose to review the theory or statistical basis of causal modeling here.
That has been done very well by many others (Asher, 1976; Bentler, 1980; Biddle
SCMarlin, 1987; Joreskog, 1978), and it is not the focus of this manuscript. Nor do
we suggest that the statistical techniques of causal modeling are adaptable to the
functional analysis of individual clients. We propose that some of the concepts and
662 S. N. Haynes and W. H. O’Brien
“5
Attribution - Strong Relationship
&direction Causal
vector systems of causal modeling can help systematize the functional analysis of
individual clients.
Used for purposes of a functional analysis, functional analytic causal models are
vector diagrams (e.g., a conceptual schema) of all important, controllable, causal
variables associated with a particular clients’ target behaviors. These models also
include estimates of the strength and form of hypothesized causal relationships
and, therefore, conform to the definition of the functional analysis.
Figure 1 presents an illustrative functional analytic causal model of a client
whose major complaint was sleep maintenance isomnia. Note that the model
illustrates: (a) bidirectional causal relationships (e.g., between Y, and Ye; (b)
multiple “dependent” and “independent” variables and causal paths (although the
differentiation between independent and dependent variables is more conven-
tional than real when bidirectional relationships are postulated); (c) the relative
strengths (weights) of causal relationships; (d) mediated relationships (e.g., X,
---)X6--->Y2); (e) causal relationships between target behaviors; (f) functional
but noncausal relationships (X4<--->XS).
The structure of a causal model (or a subset of it) can be expressed more
specifically in functional equations (Duncan, 1975; Evans, 1985; James et al.,
1982). For example, for a subset of the model in Figure 1, the behavior analyst
may hypothesize that Y, = log(X,) + 2(X,)’ + R; indicating that the sleep
maintenance problem is a weak logarithmic function of causal attributions and a
stronger quadratic function of presleep ruminations (e.g., the relative impact
presleep ruminations would tend to rapidly increase as parameters (e.g., rate,
intensity) of both increased.
Functional Analysis 663
broken down into subsets of lower level causal models. Thus, for Figure 1, the
behavior analyst may construct a separate causal model for aversive marital
interactions which more specifically dictates the intervention target within this
higher order variable (e.g., more microlevel variables might include “criticism in
front of family members” or “sarcastic comments” during disagreements). At any
level, the goal of the functional analytic causal model is to provide a clinically
useful causal schema relevant to a client, and the most useful level may be
expected to vary across disciplines and across persons within disciplines.
Construction of functional analytic causal models is proposed as a method of
objectifying and systematizing case conceptualizations and clinical decision-making
processes, thereby rendering them more amenable to empirical investigation.
Causal model construction for individual clients can facilitate evaluations of the
following aspects of functional analysis: (a) the temporal stability and criterion
validity of hypothesized causal relationships and (b) between-behavior analyst
agreement, treatment utility and validity, social acceptability, and contribution of
various assessment methods in functional analyses. In short, increased objectifica-
tion of functional analyses in the form of functional analytic causal models may
help clarify an important clinical decision-making process, may help communicate
those processes to other professionals and students, and may open them to
empirical investigation and, therefore, refinement.
The authors have found preparation and discussions of functional analytic
causal models for clients to be an excellent vehicle for training graduate students
in many aspects of clinical decision-making. They promote a more organized
approach to understanding the behavior problems of individual clients and
designing intervention programs and help integrate the assessment and treatment
phases of behavior therapy. Furthermore, they are useful in consulting with
medical staff about patients. Functional analytic causal models provide a parsimo-
nious, graphic representation of the client which can usually be readily interpreted
by nonpsychologists.
SUMMARY
parameters of causal relationships, and their domains were reviewed and their
impact on functional analyses noted.
Finally, functional analytic causal modeling was proposed as a method of
systematizing clinical intervention decisions involving complex causal relation-
ships. The generation of functional analytic causal models facilitates the system-
ization and empirical study of assessment and treatment decisions.
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association (1987). LXagnostk and statisticalmanual of mental disorders (3rd ed. rev.).
Washington, DC: Author.
Aristotle (1947). Metaphysics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Asher, H. B. (1976). Causal modeling. Beverly Hills: Sage.
Arnold, H. J. (1982). Moderator variables: A clarification of conceptual, analytic and psychometric
issues. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 29, 143-174.
Baer, D. M. (1978). On the relation between basic and applied research. In A. C. Catania & T. A.
Brigham (Eds.), Handbook of applied behavior analysis; social and instructional processes (pp. 11-16). New
York: Halsted Press.
Baer, D. M. (1986). Advances and gaps in a behavioral methodology of pediatric medicine. In N. A.
Krasnegor, J. D. Arasteh, & M. F. Cataldo (Eds.), Child health behavior: A behavioral pediatrics perspective
(pp. 54-69). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Barlow, D. H. (1986). Causes of sexual dysfunction: The role of anxiety and cognitive interference.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 140-148.
Barnett, P. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (1988). Psychosocial functioning and depression: distinguishing among
antecedents, concomitants, and consequences. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 97-126.
Beck, A. T., & Young, J. E. (1985). Depression. In D. Barlow (Ed.), Clinical handbook of psychological
disorders (pp. 206-244). New York: Guilford.
Behavioral Assessment and DSM-III-R [mini-series]. (1988). Behavioral Assessment, 10, 43-121.
Bentler, P. M. (1980). Multivariate analysis of latent variables: Causal modeling. Annual Rev& of
Psychology, 31,419-456.
Bernstein, D. A., Borkovec, T. D. & Coles, M. G. H. (1986). Assessment of anxiety. In A. R. Ciminero,
C. S. Calhoun, & H. E. Adams (Eds.), Handbook of Behavioral Assessment (pp. 353-403). New York:
John Wiley & Sons.
Biddle, B. J., & Martin, M. M. (1987). Causality, confirmation, credulity and structural equation
modeling. Child Development, 58, 4-17.
Blalock, H. M. (1964). Causal inferences in nonexperimental research. Chapel Hill, NC: The University of
North Carolina Press.
Bootzin, R. R. (1975). Behavior mod$ication and therapy, an introduction. Cambridge, MA: Winthrop
Publishers, Inc.
Boring, E. G. (1957). A history of experimental psycholology. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts.
Bornstein, P. H., Bornstein, M. T., & Dawson, D. (1984). Integrated assessment and treatment. In T.
Ollendick & M. Hersen (Eds.), Child behavioral assessment: Principles and procedures (pp. 223-243). New
York: Pergamon.
Boyd, T. L., & Levis, D. J. (1980). Depression. In R. J. Daitzman (Ed.), Clinical behavior therapy and
behavior modz$ation (pp. 301-350). New York: Garland STPM Press.
Bunge, M. (1963). Causality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior problems through functional communication
training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111-126.
Ciminero, A. R., Calhoun, K. S., & Adams, H. E. (1986). Handbook of behavior assessment (2nd ed.). New
York: John Wiley & Sons.
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Q u&-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for field settings.
Chicago: Rand McNally.
Correa, E. I., & Sutker, P.B. (1986). Assessment of alcohol and drug behaviors. In A. R. Ciminero, K.
S. Calhoun, & H. E. Adams (Eds.), Handbook of behavior assessment (2nd ed., pp. 446-495). New York:
John Wiley & Sons.
Craighead, W. E., Kazdin, A. E., & Mahoney, M. J. (1981). Behavior modification: Principles, issues and
applications. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co.
666 S. N. Haynes and W. H. O’Brien
Taylor, C. B., & Agras, W. S. (1981). Assessment of phobia. In D. H. Barlow (Ed.). Behavioral assessment
ofudt& di.sorders (pp. 181-208). New York: Guilford Press.
Tofte-Tipps, S., Mendonca, R., & Peach, R. V. (1982). Training and generalization of social skills: A
study with two developmentally handicapped, socially isolated children. Behavior Modz$ation, 6,
45-71.
Turkat, I. D., & Maisto, S. A. (1985). Application of the experimental method to the formulation and
modification of personality disorders. In D. H. Barlow (Ed.), Clinical handbook of psychological disorders
(pp. 502-570). New York: Guilford.
Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., & Costello, A. (1987). Psychopathology in the offspring of anxiety
disorders patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 229-235.
Ulrich, R. (1975). Toward experimental living, Phase II: “Have you ever heard of a man named
Frazier, Sir?” In E. Ramp & G. Semb (Eds.), Behavior analysis: Areas of research and application (pp.
45-60). Engelwood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Voeltz, L. M., & Evans, I. M. (1982). The assessment of behavioral interrelationships in child behavior
therapy. Behavioral Assessment, 4, 131-165.
Wahler, R. G., & Fox, J. J. (1981). Setting events in applied behavior analysis: Toward a conceptual and
methodological expansion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 327-338.
Wickens, T. D. (1982). Models for behavior, Stochastic processes in psychology. San Francisco: W. H.
Freeman.
Wincze, J. P. (1982). Assessment of sexual disorders. Behavioral Assessment, 4, 257-27 1.
Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychothmae by reciprocal inhibition. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Wolpe, J. (1977). Inadequate behavioral analysis: The Achiles heel of outcome research in behavior
therapy. Journal of Behavior therapy and Exprimentul Psychiatry, 8, 1-3.
Wolpe, J., & Turkat, I. D. (1985). Behavioral Formulation of clinical cases. In I. Turkat (Ed.), Behavioral
case fomulution (pp. 213-144). New York: Plenum Press.