Act Como Terapia Basada en Procesos

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 29

Running head: BEYOND ACT: PBT 1

Beyond Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Process-Based Therapy

Clarissa W. Ong, M.S.

Michael E. Levin, Ph.D.

Michael P. Twohig, Ph.D.

Department of Psychology

Utah State University

Corresponding author:
Clarissa W. Ong
Department of Psychology
Utah State University
2810 Old Main Hill
Logan, UT 84322-2810
(435) 797-8303
[email protected]
BEYOND ACT: PBT 2

Abstract

This article describes process-based therapy (PBT) as a natural evolution toward more effective

and efficient mental healthcare. Using acceptance and commitment therapy as an example of an

early prototype of PBT, this paper explicates the broader features of PBT and the shift in mindset

researchers and clinicians will need to take to fully embrace PBT with respect to assessment,

conceptualization, and intervention. In addition, the paper enumerates challenges to

implementing the PBT model and proposes recommendations for circumventing these challenges

in the areas of theory development, research methodology, and clinical practice. Finally, we

make the argument shifting to PBT is the logical next step for our field.

Keywords: process-based therapy, acceptance and commitment therapy, research

methodology, treatment planning, case conceptualization


BEYOND ACT: PBT 3

Beyond Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Process-Based Therapy

Introduction to Process-Based Therapy (PBT)

Definition of PBT

Process-based cognitive-behavioral therapy or process-based therapy (PBT) is defined as

the “contextually specific use of evidence-based processes linked to evidence-based procedures

to help solve the problems and promote the prosperity of particular people” (Hofmann & Hayes,

2018, p. 2). There are three key parts of this definition. First, intervention occurs in a specific

context, which means use of therapeutic processes and procedures cannot be applied in a rote

fashion. Rather, selection of processes and procedures must be sensitive to contextual variables

including presenting concern, individual history, and situational stressors. Second, evidence-

based processes must be linked to evidence-based procedures and vice versa. In other words,

processes must be manipulable by available procedures and effective procedures must be able to

shift processes of change. Without these links, processes of change and procedures are rendered

useless because they cannot be moved or have no impact respectively. Finally, PBT has a

specific goal: to solve problems and promote prosperity. Thus, its objective is not merely to find

empirical links between processes and symptoms, predict trajectories of processes and

symptoms, or even to operationalize and classify these events; it is to have a meaningful impact

on quality of life.

PBT and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy

Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) is a cognitive-behavioral therapy that aims

to foster psychological flexibility through increasing acceptance, defusion, present moment

awareness, self-as-context, committed action, and values clarification (Hayes, Luoma, Bond,

Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). In the ACT model, psychological flexibility is defined as “the process
BEYOND ACT: PBT 4

of contacting the present moment fully as a conscious human being and persisting or changing

behavior in the service of chosen values” (Hayes et al., 2006, p. 9). Hence, psychological

flexibility is inextricably tied to observable behavior and entails consistency between behavior

and self-determined values. Psychological flexibility is the hypothesized process of change in

ACT; the therapeutic procedures linked to it are varied and include experiential exercises,

metaphors, exposures, and skills training (Hayes et al., 2006; Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 2011).

PBT and ACT share overlapping features as does PBT with many other therapies given

its inclusive stance. Similar to PBT, ACT has its own explicit goal against which its

effectiveness should be evaluated: valued living. Furthermore, both approaches are concerned

with improving wellbeing beyond other philosophical goals. ACT and PBT are also process-

based models by design. That is, they are grounded in empirically supported change processes

and any discussion of intervention theory and application necessarily involves these processes. In

these ways, ACT can be seen as a nascent prototype of PBT: it posits its own theoretical

framework and chosen philosophy of science, it uses empirically sound methods to test

procedures and evaluate predetermined outcomes, and it advocates focusing on processes over

presentation.

Still, in other ways, ACT is a rudimentary iteration of PBT mainly because it is more

exclusive than what PBT strives for. The ACT model specifies its own change process

(comprised of six subprocesses) that may not perfectly encompass all possible empirically

supported change processes. For example, ACT tends to focus on altering the function of verbal

stimuli (e.g., thoughts, feelings, memories) rather than their form or frequency, which can be

accomplished through practicing acceptance, defusion, present moment awareness, or self-as-

context. In contrast, the PBT model is more inclusive with respect to procedures and change
BEYOND ACT: PBT 5

processes. For instance, cognitive restructuring (procedure) aims to change the content of

thoughts through cognitive reappraisal (process; Hofmann & Asmundson, 2008) and is not

formally used in ACT. Still, cognitive restructuring have been found to be effective for

decreasing subjective distress (Hofmann, Heering, Sawyer, & Asnaani, 2009; Wolgast, Lundh, &

Viborg, 2011), making cognitive restructuring an empirically supported therapeutic procedure.

Furthermore, cognitive restructuring shifts dysfunctional thinking (Cristea et al., 2015), which

has been found to influence symptom outcomes (Wilhelm, Berman, Keshaviah, Schwartz, &

Steketee, 2015). There is also evidence cognitive reappraisal influences positive affect

(Brockman, Ciarrochi, Parker, & Kashdan, 2017), making it a relevant process of change with

respect to emotional wellbeing. Yet, most ACT interventions do not make room for cognitive

reappraisal. Thus, while ACT is a step toward PBT, there are still differences between the two.

PBT and Cognitive-Behavioral Therapies

PBT can be more easily differentiated from manual-based cognitive-behavioral therapies

(CBT) given their discrepant intervention goals and overarching philosophies. As mentioned

earlier, the explicit goal of PBT is to solve problems and enhance wellbeing, the form of which

depends on what is meaningful to the individual. In contrast, CBT tends to be more concerned

with nomothetic outcomes that can be targeted and assessed with group-validated measures,

permitting comparison of such generic indices across studies and populations. Moreover, these

outcomes tend to focus on symptoms rather than wellbeing. In addition, CBT is primarily

developed and tested in the form of standardized manuals on a topographical level of analysis

(Chambless & Hollon, 1998), whereas PBT is designed to be developed and tested on a process-

based or functional level of analysis, jettisoning formulaic protocols for principle-guided

flexibility.
BEYOND ACT: PBT 6

Orienting Toward the PBT Model

A Paradigm Shift

Moving toward PBT warrants a fundamental change in how psychologists understand

and conceptualize clinically relevant behaviors and effective interventions. Although it is easy to

call for such changes, envisioning and planning exactly what our next steps as a field is

complicated because such a paradigm shift likely entails a steep learning curve for

everyone⎯from graduate students to experienced clinicians and tenured professors.

Furthermore, the PBT framework forces us to rethink the very purpose of our work. Whereas the

field of clinical psychology has historically been an outcome-focused endeavor (i.e., “What

treatment packages work best to reduce symptoms?”), PBT demands process-focused efforts

wherein the key question becomes, “Which processes should treatments target to improve

wellbeing?”

Changing the questions our field seeks to answer has practical ramifications because

scientific and clinical methods and attention have to shift correspondingly. On a broader scale,

there is a need for a functional taxonomy more suited to the complexity and challenges of diverse

clinical conditions and individual goals. This means reorganizing and even reformulating

psychological ideas and constructs in a way that clearly aligns with the stated goals and

principles of PBT. Thus, even the constructs we are used to studying and treating may change.

Philosophical Underpinnings of PBT

It seems prudent to preface the following discussion on research and clinical work in PBT

with an explicit description of what we see as the philosophical stance of PBT. Understanding

the underlying philosophical assumptions of PBT will clarify how PBT decides which change
BEYOND ACT: PBT 7

processes are worth analyzing, which theories are useful, or which principles should guide

therapeutic decision making.

One possible philosophical approach to clinical epistemology is ontological⎯like much

of science⎯and concerned with coherence with what we perceive as reality. In this approach, the

goal is to model all the parts, relations, and forces operating in a given case as they occur in the

“real world.” Although the unit of analysis is clearer in such a mechanistic approach, this degree

of precision might require an insurmountable amount of research that would ultimately result in

complex models with limited clinical utility.

The functional contextual assumptions underlying contextual behavioral science offers an

alternative in which truth is defined as what works to enable prediction and influence of behavior

with precision, scope and depth (Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & Wilson, 2012). From this

perspective, clinical science is not simply about identifying processes that locally (in a limited

set of currently relevant circumstances) permit prediction and influence; instead, it strives to

identify processes that support progressive knowledge building, allowing us to make consistent

steps toward our stated analytic goals of prediction and influence across people and settings. This

a-ontological stance can provide selection criteria for deciding which processes to study, which

levels of analysis to use, and how to address conflicting or overlapping processes without getting

mired in concerns about coherence with “reality.”

Acknowledging the plethora of theoretical frameworks in clinical psychology, PBT takes

a universal stance in the sense that it does not pledge allegiance or disavow any one treatment

model and instead accepts coexistence of discrete sets of philosophical assumptions on the

condition that they share an end goal (Hofmann & Hayes, 2018). Thus, PBT welcomes useful
BEYOND ACT: PBT 8

elements from various orientations so long as they serve the explicit objective of enhancing

human wellbeing.

Still, PBT itself has a core epistemology underlying its methods: empiricism. This means

PBT relies on theory-driven, testable hypotheses and methodologically sound means of data

collection and interpretation to advance its scientific agenda. These investigations may be

performed on various levels of analysis (e.g., neurological, physiological, behavioral), with

nomothetic or ideographic methods (e.g., randomized controlled trials, single-subject designs),

and in basic or applied settings. The parameters matter less than the scientific rationale

underlying queries. With respect to elements to include in this taxonomy, we may emphasize

mid-level maladaptive (e.g., clinical perfectionism, rumination) and adaptive processes (e.g.,

perspective taking, cognitive reappraisal) given their utility in research, clinical, and translational

work. Sticking to processes that are too narrowly defined or too general can end up being

unhelpful as they provide imprecise psychological targets that are difficult to generalize or apply

to specific contexts.

Research in the PBT Model

Although PBT has clear advantages conceptually, it poses practical challenges for

research that need to be overcome to fully meet the promise of PBT. PBT requires identifying a

set of evidence-based processes with adequate precision, scope, and depth that can be (1)

systematically applied to conceptualize relevant cases and (2) reliably linked to procedures to

treat such cases. In other words, we need processes that can do the work required in PBT as a

more flexible, idiographic model of evidence-based therapy. This requires developing a

systematic, progressive knowledge base with these processes and associated procedures that can

broadly, reliably and efficiently answer the clinical decision-making question of “What core
BEYOND ACT: PBT 9

biopsychosocial processes should be targeted with this client given this goal in this situation, and

how can they most efficiently and effectively be changed?” (Hofmann & Hayes, 2018, p. 47).

Difficulties with a PBT approach are likely to arise if we fail to better answer this

question over time, are only able to answer this question in a limited set of circumstances, have

wide variability across clinicians or researchers in how this question is answered, or require an

impractical amount of effort to answer this question. These challenges are, in many ways,

opportunities as they point to areas in which clinical psychology has stagnated and map out

directions to move forward differently. Addressing these challenges may mean reconsidering

how research is approached⎯from specific methods used and research questions asked to

researchers’ core scientific strategy and assumptions for knowledge generation.

Identifying an Adequate Set of Processes

PBT requires an organized set of processes that can be reliably and practically applied to

conceptualize cases seen in practice and to guide decision making with regard to therapeutic

procedures. Without such a system, we risk problems like lack of clear, evidence-based

guidelines for delivering PBT; high degree of variability in clinical practice that diverges from

existing research; and barriers to adoption of PBT (e.g., complexity to learn and implement,

perceived lack of applicability to cases).

One way to avoid these problems is to ensure PBT processes have high precision (i.e.,

avoiding excessive overlap among processes such that each accounts for distinct phenomena)

and scope (i.e., relevant to a range of cases and presentations such that the process is practically

useful to learn and apply within practice). If a system includes multiple overlapping processes

that account for the same clinical problem (e.g., experiential avoidance, anxiety sensitivity,

distress intolerance, emotion dysregulation) or treatment methods (e.g., cognitive defusion,


BEYOND ACT: PBT 10

decentering, mindfulness) then it becomes unclear which process to use when and how the

associated research is to be organized to guide evidence-based practice.

That said, it can be equally problematic when a set of precise processes are excessively

narrowly defined, especially in relation to topography rather than function (e.g., discomfort

intolerance, intolerance of uncertainty, tolerance of ambiguity; Leyro, Zvolensky, & Bernstein,

2010). The lack of parsimony associated with high-precision, narrow-scope processes can lead to

notable difficulty in creating a progressive knowledge base or practically useful clinical

guidelines. We need theoretical constructs that match the precision and scope of the clinical

decision-making framework for PBT especially if such an approach aims to integrate processes

and procedures across existing treatment models. With respect to integration across models,

basic levels of analysis may be critical as they provide a common language that is precise and

abstract enough to potentially encompass overlapping processes from different traditions.

Consistent with the reticulated approach to integrating basic and applied sciences in

contextual behavioral science, high-precision and wide-scope processes may be best achieved by

developing and refining processes at multiple levels of analysis, with developments in basic and

applied areas informing the other, and emphasizing coherence across levels (i.e., depth). Basic

research often focuses on highly abstracted and precise principles and processes that can account

for a range of phenomena (e.g., reinforcement, inhibitory control). In applied work, middle-level

terms are typically developed for targeted contexts in ways that guide clinical decision making,

which often have less precision and scope than the abstract principles on which they are based.

Ultimately, useful constructs have to be evaluated against our stated goal of supporting personal

growth and wellbeing. Varying constructs we study, selecting based on clearly defined

objectives, and retaining ones that work are all necessary steps of advancing clinical science.
BEYOND ACT: PBT 11

Developing Adequate Process Measures

Measurement is an obvious and critical challenge for PBT. Even a perfectly specified

theoretical model of processes is unlikely to be useful over time if we are not able to measure

these processes reliably and accurately. There are common, well-known measurement challenges

related to over-reliance on self-report, global recall insensitive to context, and group designs that

only consider aggregate data (e.g., Shull, 1999; Sidman, 1960; Trull & Ebner-Priemer, 2013). All

these issues reduce sensitivity to detecting more precise phenomena of interest when examining

which pathological processes are relevant for a given case and how procedures engage processes

to produce clinically meaningful change.

We need measures that can distinguish between highly correlated and overlapping but

distinct processes. Real-world decisions based on how processes and associated procedures

function in research are much more likely to be progressive if there is a reasonable degree of

confidence in the measures used to assess these constructs. These issues are reflected, for

example, in the observed challenges with measurement found in ACT. The Acceptance and

Action Questionnaire⎯II (AAQ-II; Bond et al., 2011) represents the most established process of

change measure for ACT. The AAQ-II has been found to predict a range of mental health

problems (Levin et al., 2014) and mediate treatment outcomes for ACT (e.g., Pots, Trompetter,

Schreurs, & Bohlmeijer, 2016; Yadavaia, Hayes, & Vilardaga, 2014). However, there are also

validity concerns with the AAQ-II such as a high overlap with psychological distress (Tyndall et

al., 2019; Wolgast, 2014), lack of precision with regards to measuring experiential avoidance or

some/all aspects of psychological inflexibility (Gámez, Chmielewski, Kotov, Ruggero, &

Watson, 2011), and notably high correlations with other ACT processes such as cognitive fusion

(Gillanders et al., 2014). The AAQ-II has also been found to be less sensitive to detecting effects
BEYOND ACT: PBT 12

than domain-specific measures of psychological inflexibility (Ong, Lee, Levin, & Twohig,

2019). These issues create challenges for developing a more precise model of clinical decision

making that could inform PBT as the role of psychological inflexibility in presenting problems

and the unique effects of ACT procedures designed to target specific aspects of psychological

inflexibility are obscure.

One potential solution is to use other sources of information beyond self-report. Yet,

multimethod assessment may introduce other auxiliaries and conditions that affect reliability and

validity because of methodological noise that is necessarily incurred when multiple means are

used to indirectly measure a construct (e.g., behavioral tasks, GPS data). Algorithm-based

methods could potentially overcome these challenges, particularly when used to combine across

data sources, but developing such algorithms depends on already having a reliable and valid

criterion, which returns to existing measurement challenges.

Another solution is to more rigorously develop self-report measures designed to more

precisely measure specific change processes. Examples of such measures include the

Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Questionnaire (Gámez et al., 2011), Cognitive Fusion

Questionnaire (Gillanders et al., 2014), and Multidimensional Psychological Flexibility

Inventory (Rolffs, Rogge, & Wilson, 2018). However, these measures are still susceptible to the

inevitable disadvantages of self-report measures related to subjective perception and recall

ability.

As measure development progresses, the field will have to grapple with the challenge of

organizing and weeding through an increasing number of process measures. Similar to the

intellectual distillation of overlapping theories and procedures to empirically supported

components, a parallel process should occur with corresponding measures⎯bearing in the mind
BEYOND ACT: PBT 13

the overarching objective of promoting prosperity among individuals. This means measures have

to contribute to the development of a coherent and parsimonious knowledge base that clarify

procedures and processes linked to enhanced wellbeing. Furthermore, measures retained in the

field need to meet the demands of capturing context-sensitive, idiosyncratic data from which

treatment planning and clinical decision making can proceed. Otherwise, we risk forming a

fragmented knowledge base disconnected to our stated goals and the inability to synthesize

results across studies.

A final point to consider is whether we should revisit criteria used to determine reliability

and validity of our measures. A common method for developing measures is to rely on how self-

report items naturally relate to each other in samples outside the context of treatment. This may

fit with the cross-sectional use of such measures to identify relevant baseline pathological

processes in clinical samples that might inform case conceptualization. At the same time, it may

be less helpful with regard to using these process measures to assess and compare the effects of

different procedures designed to engage distinct processes.

An alternative approach might be to place criteria like treatment sensitivity, discriminant

validity, and incremental validity at the forefront of process measure validation such that

measures are created with the intent of clarifying distinct processes that may or may not apply to

a given client and distinct procedures that engage these processes differentially. For example, a

good measure of cognitive flexibility might not be the set of items that most highly relate to each

other and account for the largest amount of variance in an outcome but rather a measure that can

identify the unique effects of a procedure aimed to increase cognitive flexibility relative to other

procedures. These measurement issues are critical to developing a progressive knowledge base

about processes that can guide clinical decision making in PBT.


BEYOND ACT: PBT 14

Researching Procedures Linked to Processes

Assuming an adequate set of processes have been identified with an adequate set of

measures, the next task is to develop an adequate knowledge base to identify what procedures to

use that are effective and efficient in moving the processes that will achieve personally

meaningful gains for given clients and contexts. This means using methods that can answer the

relevant questions that will guide clinical decision making in PBT. In part, the challenge is to

integrate and organize our existing knowledge base across the range of evidence-based

interventions in such a way that guides a more comprehensive PBT model and clinical decision

making. This challenge is heightened due to overlapping processes developed in distinct

traditions and the need to build bridges to avoid replicating competing, branded therapy

packages.

We also need a wealth of additional research based on gaps identified in the literature.

For example, what therapeutic procedures are most effective and efficient for engaging targeted

change processes, what contexts and client characteristics moderate these effects, to what degree

are procedures and processes additive and overlapping in producing changes in processes, and

how do we combine these specific therapeutic procedures and processes into a broader PBT

model of care that integrates other biopsychosocial processes and procedures? The last

unanswered question represents a whole host of other questions: how procedures and processes

across therapies overlap and how are they distinct, when are particular biopsychosocial processes

more critical than others, etc. Potentially this can demand an unrealistic amount of research given

the potential of evaluating countless procedures, processes, clients, and contexts across levels

and types of empirical support. Therefore, we need to be strategic to maximize efficiency of the

research process and outputs that can be generalized to clinical decision making. Other
BEYOND ACT: PBT 15

publications have provided excellent primers on the range of promising methodologies that can

help meet the goals of PBT (Hayes et al., 2019). We want to emphasize one particularly critical

implication of PBT, which resonates with clinical behavior analysis and its roots: a need to return

to more idiographic analyses of individual subjects.

The numerous limitations of group designs studying aggregated data across individuals

has been explicated from behavior analytic viewpoints (Shull, 1999; Sidman, 1960). These issues

become especially prominent as the focus shifts from protocols for syndromes to processes for

individuals. The precision required from PBT in matching procedures to processes for individual

clients and contexts will continue to elude us if treatment effects are always aggregated into

groups assuming homogeneity among participants. This is particularly problematic if

homogeneity is based on overly simplistic categories such as topography of clinical presentations

(e.g., panic disorder, major depressive disorder). This group-level approach obfuscates the

important heterogeneity in treatment response in which we are interested for clinical decision

making in PBT (i.e., who did this work for and how did it work?). The “right” question is

unlikely to simply be: which collections of procedures are necessary and sufficient to produce

improvements among clients in general? Rather, the question is: which procedures are necessary

to engage which biopsychosocial processes for which clients? And this question warrants closer

inspection of individual patterns.

To ensure relevance to clinical work, there is a need to model the complexity of change

processes and contexts that moderate their effects. This fits with typical idiographic approaches

in which a much more precise and intensive assessment procedure over time is typically used to

support causal interpretations of effects rather than group randomization. This intensive

assessment approach is more likely to capture the complex, dynamic ways that procedures,
BEYOND ACT: PBT 16

processes, and contexts interact over time. It is also better suited to match the process of clinical

decision making, which is typically based on more data than those provided at baseline. Rarely is

the question a static one of “what set of procedures should I use for the whole course of

treatment?”

Rather, clinical decision making evolves over time in response to client behavior and

response to intervention (e.g., “What procedure should I use at which point to alter what

process?”). The former is what is typically tested in a dismantling design where the effects of

procedures targeting specific processes are examined before and after treatment. In contrast,

more dynamic approaches might test the proximal effects of matching particular procedures to

engage particular processes based on in-the-moment variables that match routine clinical

decision making (e.g., "When is it more effective to target acceptance versus values?"; Levin,

Haeger, & Cruz, 2019). A greater focus on dynamic effects over time substantially increases

complexity, and this is needed to match the complexity of human experience to be addressed by

an effective PBT model.

Ultimately, idiographic findings must be scaled back up and generalized into models that

guide clinical decision making. These are unlikely to be based on the silos provided in diagnostic

manuals. Thus, we also need to find useful ways to organize sets of clients that will support

prediction and influence. One way to do this might be to work backwards from idiographic

analyses, inductively identifying characteristics and generalizable processes that guide clinical

decision making. A number of promising examples exist in the literature that orient to

pathological processes that span across presentations and guide responses to particular

procedures targeting particular processes (e.g., clinical perfectionism, impulsive decision


BEYOND ACT: PBT 17

making; Egan et al., 2014; Fairburn et al., 2015; Gros, Szafranski, & Shead, 2017; Morrison et

al., 2019; Newby, McKinnon, Kuyken, Gilbody, & Dalgleish, 2015).

Clinical Work in the PBT Model

Clinical Advantages of a Process-Based Intervention Model

Delivering PBT necessitates a different framework from the diagnosis-focused, manual-

based approach clinical psychology has been using in the past few decades. Although the

proliferation of empirically tested protocols has improved quality and accessibility of care

(Chorpita et al., 2002; Morgenstern, Morgan, McCrady, Keller, & Carroll, 2001; Muñoz &

Mendelson, 2005; Otto, Pollack, & Maki, 2000), the almost exclusive topographical analysis of

intervention and presentation has constrained our ability to perform functional case

conceptualization and design treatment plans accordingly. That is, the “how” of intervention has

been inadvertently sacrificed for the “what” of intervention. One limitation of a topographical or

symptom-based approach to therapy is the same diagnostic label can be assigned to vastly

different presentations. Conversely, behaviors that fall within the same diagnostic category can

have different functions and histories.

The inadequacy of straightforward diagnosis-protocol matching warrants a need to

ground intervention in theoretically consistent principles of change and corresponding

idiographic assessment. The shift from cookbook manuals to a context- and individual-sensitive

principles-informed approach to care is undoubtedly intimidating. However, the beauty of PBT is

it does not require clinicians to start from a blank slate. PBT is Bayesian in the sense that it

considers extant literature and uses available data to constantly shape and update its theoretical

scaffolding (Hofmann & Hayes, 2018). For example, procedures reliably found to affect change

processes like exposure and behavioral activation and change processes linked to valued
BEYOND ACT: PBT 18

outcomes like cognitive reappraisal and psychological acceptance already have a place in the

PBT model (Hofmann & Hayes, 2018).

Another notable draw of PBT is the number of overlapping change processes and

therapeutic procedures in empirically supported treatments is considerably smaller than the

number of identified disorders and various protocols designed for them. Thus, although process-

based treatment would involve stepping away from the familiarity of manualized interventions

for specific diagnoses, it may ultimately be simpler because there are fewer elements with which

to become familiar. Furthermore, because clients with the same diagnosis show significant

variability, clients present with comorbidities, and clients can be in need of clinical services even

without meeting diagnostic criteria, focusing on function or processes⎯as opposed to diagnostic

labels⎯may provide a more helpful means of case conceptualization and intervention planning.

Assessment and Outcome Monitoring

In line with the shift from diagnoses and manuals to functionally defined behaviors and

change processes, assessment and outcome monitoring practices need to be updated as well.

Specifically, clinicians have to: (1) identify relevant change processes and behavioral outcomes

to assess, (2) determine methods for assessing those change processes and behavioral outcomes,

(3) administer assessments, (4) design treatment plans based on data from assessments, (5)

continuously monitor change processes and behavioral outcomes to determine if treatment is

working as expected, and (6) adjust treatment as needed.

To date, behavioral, cognitive, emotional, and neurological change processes applicable

across diagnoses have been identified as logical targets in PBT (Hayes & Hofmann, 2017).

Clinicians may choose to measure these more global change processes in addition to outcomes

specific to client presentations. Furthermore, the need for idiographic assessment cannot be
BEYOND ACT: PBT 19

understated given therapeutic work frequently focuses on the individual. The key idea behind

idiographic assessment is to identify and accurately and reliably track change processes specific

to the client’s presentation, treatment goals, and perception of wellbeing. For example, when

working with a client who catastrophizes commonly encountered problems and avoids situations

that elicit anxiety, clinicians may choose to measure perceived power of cognitive distortions,

frequency of behavioral avoidance of anxiety, or even anxiety severity with a standardized

measure. In certain cases, a standardized measure will work well but, in other cases, a brief face-

valid question (e.g., “On a scale from 1 to 10, how much did you push the thought away today?”

or “On a scale from 1 to 10, how content are you with the way you are living your life right

now?”) will be the easiest way to conduct assessment. Similarly, behavior tracking can be useful

when the intervention target is overt and concrete (e.g., number of compulsions in obsessive-

compulsive disorder [OCD]).

Clinics and clinicians will need to develop and refine methods to routinely perform these

assessments. Automating these assessments can improve usability, decrease risk of human error,

leading to much greater adoption. Furthermore, with technological advances, it may be easy to

incorporate client self-report data into treatment notes and to design systems that allow for

individualized assessment. For example, web- and app-based assessments can provide more

individualized and time-specific assessments. In our research, we have found mobile apps can

assess processes in the moment, which can be used to characterize changes in processes over

time (Levin, Navarro, Cruz, & Haeger, 2019; Levin, Pierce, & Schoendorff, 2017) or even to

guide individualized tailoring of what procedures to apply to clients in the moment based on

time-specific assessments (Levin, Haeger, et al., 2019).

Treatment Delivery
BEYOND ACT: PBT 20

As clinicians start to understand clinical presentations in terms of processes, they need to:

(1) clarify key change processes for clients, (2) identify procedures that will move relevant

change processes, and (3) explicate clinical decision-making rules based on potentially

unfamiliar theoretical frameworks and philosophies of science. The latter may be uniquely

challenging in the absence of manuals that sequentially organize steps within sessions or context-

sensitive data on the effectiveness of therapeutic procedures.

An example of treatment based on the PBT model follows. At baseline, the clinician

conducts a typical intake assessment that includes collecting data on demographic variables,

individual history, clinical presentation, diagnoses, and nomothetic assessment of likely change

processes. This information would be integrated with the client’s treatment goals. Specifically,

the clinician forms a case conceptualization of processes that need to change to increase

probability of behavioral change, which will, in turn, allow clients to achieve their therapeutic

goals. These choices should be influenced by client history and individual characteristics and

based on nomothetic research that suggests changes in particular processes will positively

influence changes in behavioral outcomes related to treatment goals. Then, using evidence-based

decision making, the clinician would present a treatment plan to the client. However, instead of

describing the manual they would use, the clinician would focus on skills that need to be

developed to address the presenting issue. Idiographic assessment would be used to track client

goals and key change processes. Movement in change processes and target behaviors will clarify

the effectiveness of treatment.

Twohig and colleagues (Crosby, Dehlin, Mitchell, & Twohig, 2012; Twohig & Crosby,

2010; Twohig, Hayes, & Masuda, 2006a, 2006b) have utilized some of these principles in their

work with obsessive-compulsive and related disorders. At baseline, clients complete a battery of
BEYOND ACT: PBT 21

standardized assessments that include change process (cognitive distortions and psychological

inflexibility) and disorder severity measures (specific disorder measures, depression, and quality

of life). Assessment continues with week-long self-monitoring between the intake and first

session. Self-monitoring is circumscribed to easily trackable behaviors (e.g., compulsions in

OCD, skin picking in excoriation disorder, or hairs pulled in trichotillomania) and change

processes (e.g., responses to internal events) that will be explicitly targeted in therapy. Daily self-

monitoring is maintained over the course of treatment. It can be completed on paper or via texts,

websites, or apps depending on what makes sense for the client.

These data are graphed and used to inform treatment decisions. Generally, we look for

relationships between change processes and target behaviors wherein a decrease in the process

predicts a decrease in the target behavior (or other relevant outcome). If the target behavior is

decreasing much faster than the target process of change, there is a disconnect. Such a pattern

indicates the hypothesized key change process is not contributing to behavioral

improvement⎯assuming measures used are reliable and valid⎯and the treatment plan should be

refined accordingly. The standardized measures are typically administered approximately every

four weeks. Collectively, these methods allow us to conceptualize the case in terms of change

processes, move processes using evidence-based procedures, and verify that we are shifting key

change processes that ultimately produce changes in the target behavior.

Pivoting away from protocol-to-disorder matching and familiar theoretical orientations

adds complexity to treatment delivery. Implementing process-based assessment and intervention

requires clinicians to build up “big picture” skills with respect to becoming fluent in developing

context-specific case conceptualizations and individualizing treatment based on evidence-based

models of psychopathology and intervention. Furthermore, relinquishing the safety blanket of


BEYOND ACT: PBT 22

clearly delineated manuals introduces potential for drift from evidence-based methods and loss

of benefits with actuarial decision making for those who stray from the PBT model. However,

this may be a training issue rather than an implementation issue. If clinicians receive solid

training in PBT, this vision can be readily realized. After all, applied behavior analysts have been

doing this type of work for decades. Our field will always struggle with adequately training

aspiring clinicians but that should not stop us from trying.

Conclusion

Ultimately, the goals of PBT with respect to increased theoretical and procedural

parsimony, broader applicability, and multidisciplinary coherence need to be met by appropriate

methods in research and clinical settings. The utility of pontification is limited if psychologists

fail to test falsifiable hypotheses with sound methodology or track change processes following

implementation of specific therapeutic techniques.

Much has been written on the advantages and recommendations of the PBT model (see

for e.g., Hayes et al., 2019; Hofmann & Hayes, 2018). This paper echoes the call for sincere

efforts to move toward practicing PBT. At the same time, it highlights realistic challenges that

may hinder the transition and provides concrete suggestions for possible next steps. As we have

discussed in this paper, the path ahead will be intellectually and pragmatically onerous.

Nonetheless, we believe the benefits to be accrued from embracing PBT will be worth the

journey. Namely, that PBT promises a single organizing framework in clinical psychology that

bridges theoretical factions, a core set of empirically tested procedures that move useful change

processes related to meaningful outcomes, theoretically informed principles to guide context-

sensitive clinical decision making, and, most important, advancement of quality and accessibility
BEYOND ACT: PBT 23

of care in the service of promoting client wellbeing. After all, is that not why we got into this

field in the first place?


BEYOND ACT: PBT 24

References

Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H. K., . . . Zettle,

R. D. (2011). Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance and Action

Questionnaire-II: A revised measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential

avoidance. Behavior Therapy, 42(4), 676-688. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2011.03.007

Brockman, R., Ciarrochi, J., Parker, P., & Kashdan, T. (2017). Emotion regulation strategies in

daily life: Mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression. Cognitive

Behaviour Therapy, 46(2), 91-113. doi:10.1080/16506073.2016.1218926

Chambless, D. L., & Hollon, S. D. (1998). Defining empirically supported therapies. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66(1), 7-18. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.66.1.7

Chorpita, B. F., Yim, L. M., Donkervoet, J. C., Arensdorf, A., Amundsen, M. J., McGee, C., . . .

Morelli, P. (2002). Toward large‐scale implementation of empirically supported

treatments for children: A review and observations by the Hawaii Empirical Basis to

Services Task Force. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 9(2), 165-190.

doi:10.1111/j.1468-2850.2002.tb00504.x

Cristea, I. A., Huibers, M. J., David, D., Hollon, S. D., Andersson, G., & Cuijpers, P. (2015). The

effects of cognitive behavior therapy for adult depression on dysfunctional thinking: A

meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 42, 62-71. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2015.08.003

Crosby, J. M., Dehlin, J. P., Mitchell, P. R., & Twohig, M. P. (2012). Acceptance and

commitment therapy and habit reversal training for the treatment of trichotillomania.

Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 19(4), 595-605. doi:10.1016/j.cbpra.2012.02.002

Egan, S. J., van Noort, E., Chee, A., Kane, R. T., Hoiles, K. J., Shafran, R., & Wade, T. D.

(2014). A randomised controlled trial of face to face versus pure online self-help
BEYOND ACT: PBT 25

cognitive behavioural treatment for perfectionism. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 63,

107-113. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2014.09.009

Fairburn, C. G., Bailey-Straebler, S., Basden, S., Doll, H. A., Jones, R., Murphy, R., . . . Cooper,

Z. (2015). A transdiagnostic comparison of enhanced cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT-

E) and interpersonal psychotherapy in the treatment of eating disorders. Behaviour

Research and Therapy, 70, 64-71. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2015.04.010

Gámez, W., Chmielewski, M., Kotov, R., Ruggero, C., & Watson, D. (2011). Development of a

measure of experiential avoidance: The Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance

Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 692-713. doi:10.1037/a0023242

Gillanders, D. T., Bolderston, H., Bond, F. W., Dempster, M., Flaxman, P. E., Campbell, L., . . .

Remington, B. (2014). The development and initial validation of the Cognitive Fusion

Questionnaire. Behavior Therapy, 45, 83-101. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2013.09.001

Gros, D. F., Szafranski, D. D., & Shead, S. D. (2017). A real world dissemination and

implementation of Transdiagnostic Behavior Therapy (TBT) for veterans with affective

disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 46, 72-77. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.04.010

Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Wilson, K. G. (2012). Contextual Behavioral Science:

Creating a science more adequate to the challenge of the human condition. Journal of

Contextual Behavioral Science, 1(1-2), 1-16. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2012.09.004

Hayes, S. C., & Hofmann, S. G. (Eds.). (2017). Process-based CBT: The science and core

clinical competencies of cognitive behavioral therapy. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.

Hayes, S. C., Hofmann, S. G., Stanton, C. E., Carpenter, J. K., Sanford, B. T., Curtiss, J. E., &

Ciarrochi, J. (2019). The role of the individual in the coming era of process-based

therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 117, 40-53. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2018.10.005


BEYOND ACT: PBT 26

Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and

commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy,

44(1), 1-25. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006

Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2011). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy,

Second Edition: The Process and Practice of Mindful Change. New York, NY: Guilford

Press.

Hofmann, S. G., & Asmundson, G. J. (2008). Acceptance and mindfulness-based therapy: New

wave or old hat? Clinical Psychology Review, 28(1), 1-16. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.09.003

Hofmann, S. G., & Hayes, S. C. (2018). The future of intervention science: Process-based

therapy. Clinical Psychological Science, 1-14. doi:10.1177/2167702618772296

Hofmann, S. G., Heering, S., Sawyer, A. T., & Asnaani, A. (2009). How to handle anxiety: The

effects of reappraisal, acceptance, and suppression strategies on anxious arousal.

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(5), 389-394. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.010

Levin, M. E., Haeger, J., & Cruz, R. A. (2019). Tailoring acceptance and commitment therapy

skill coaching in-the-moment through smartphones: Results from a randomized

controlled trial. Mindfulness, 10, 689-699. doi:10.1007/s12671-018-1004-2

Levin, M. E., MacLane, C., Daflos, S., Seeley, J. R., Hayes, S. C., Biglan, A., & Pistorello, J.

(2014). Examining psychological inflexibility as a transdiagnostic process across

psychological disorders. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 3, 155-163.

doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2014.06.003

Levin, M. E., Navarro, C., Cruz, R. A., & Haeger, J. (2019). Comparing in-the-moment skill

coaching effects from tailored versus non-tailored Acceptance and Commitment Therapy
BEYOND ACT: PBT 27

mobile apps in a non-clinical sample. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 48, 200-216.

doi:10.1080/16506073.2018.1503706

Levin, M. E., Pierce, B., & Schoendorff, B. (2017). The acceptance and commitment therapy

matrix mobile app: A pilot randomized trial on health behaviors. Journal of Contextual

Behavioral Science, 6, 268-275. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2017.05.003

Leyro, T. M., Zvolensky, M. J., & Bernstein, A. (2010). Distress tolerance and

psychopathological symptoms and disorders: A review of the empirical literature among

adults. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 576-600. doi:10.1037/a0019712

Morgenstern, J., Morgan, T. J., McCrady, B. S., Keller, D. S., & Carroll, K. M. (2001). Manual-

guided cognitive-behavioral therapy training: A promising method for disseminating

empirically supported substance abuse treatments to the practice community. Psychology

of Addictive Behaviors, 15(2), 83-88. doi:10.1037/0893-164x.15.2.83

Morrison, K. L., Smith, B. M., Ong, C. W., Lee, E. B., Friedel, J. E., Odum, A., . . . Twohig, M.

P. (2019). Effects of acceptance and commitment therapy on impulsive decision-making.

Behavior Modification. doi:10.1177/0145445519833041

Muñoz, R. F., & Mendelson, T. (2005). Toward evidence-based interventions for diverse

populations: The San Francisco General Hospital prevention and treatment manuals.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(5), 790-799. doi:10.1037/0022-

006X.73.5.790

Newby, J. M., McKinnon, A., Kuyken, W., Gilbody, S., & Dalgleish, T. (2015). Systematic

review and meta-analysis of transdiagnostic psychological treatments for anxiety and

depressive disorders in adulthood. Clinical Psychology Review, 40, 91-110.

doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2015.06.002
BEYOND ACT: PBT 28

Ong, C. W., Lee, E. B., Levin, M. E., & Twohig, M. P. (2019). A review of AAQ variants and

other context-specific measures of psychological flexibility. Journal of Contextual

Behavioral Science. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2019.02.007

Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., & Maki, K. M. (2000). Empirically supported treatments for panic

disorder: Costs, benefits, and stepped care. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 68(4), 556-563. doi:10.1037//0022-006X.68A556

Pots, W. T., Trompetter, H. R., Schreurs, K. M., & Bohlmeijer, E. T. (2016). How and for whom

does web-based acceptance and commitment therapy work? Mediation and moderation

analyses of web-based ACT for depressive symptoms. BioMedCentral Psychiatry, 16,

158. doi:10.1186/s12888-016-0841-6

Rolffs, J. L., Rogge, R. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2018). Disentangling components of flexibility via

the hexaflex model: Development and validation of the Multidimensional Psychological

Flexibility Inventory (MPFI). Assessment, 25(4), 458-482.

doi:10.1177/1073191116645905

Shull, R. L. (1999). Statistical inference in behavior analysis: Discussant’s remarks. The

Behavior Analyst, 22, 117-121. doi:10.1007/BF03391989

Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research: Evaluating experimental data in psychology.

Boston, MA: Authors Cooperative, Inc.

Trull, T. J., & Ebner-Priemer, U. (2013). Ambulatory assessment. Annual Review of Clinical

Psychology, 9, 151-176. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185510

Twohig, M. P., & Crosby, J. M. (2010). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy as a Treatment

for Problematic Internet Pornography Viewing. Behavior Therapy, 41(3), 285-295.

doi:10.1016/j.beth.2009.06.002
BEYOND ACT: PBT 29

Twohig, M. P., Hayes, S. C., & Masuda, A. (2006a). Increasing willingness to experience

obsessions: Acceptance and commitment therapy as a treatment for obsessive-compulsive

disorder. Behavior Therapy, 37(1), 3-13. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2005.02.001

Twohig, M. P., Hayes, S. C., & Masuda, A. (2006b). A preliminary investigation of acceptance

and commitment therapy as a treatment for chronic skin picking. Behaviour Research

and Therapy, 44(10), 1513-1522. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.002

Tyndall, I., Waldeck, D., Pancani, L., Whelan, R., Roche, B., & Dawson, D. L. (2019). The

Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II (AAQ-II) as a measure of experiential

avoidance: Concerns over discriminant validity. Journal of Contextual Behavioral

Science, 12, 278-284. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2018.09.005

Wilhelm, S., Berman, N. C., Keshaviah, A., Schwartz, R. A., & Steketee, G. (2015).

Mechanisms of change in cognitive therapy for obsessive compulsive disorder: role of

maladaptive beliefs and schemas. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 65, 5-10.

doi:10.1016/j.brat.2014.12.006

Wolgast, M. (2014). What does the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ-II) really

measure? Behavior Therapy, 45, 831-839. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2014.07.002

Wolgast, M., Lundh, L. G., & Viborg, G. (2011). Cognitive reappraisal and acceptance: An

experimental comparison of two emotion regulation strategies. Behaviour Research and

Therapy, 49(12), 858-866. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2011.09.011

Yadavaia, J. E., Hayes, S. C., & Vilardaga, R. (2014). Using acceptance and commitment

therapy to increase self-compassion: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Contextual

Behavioral Science, 3, 248-257. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2014.09.002

You might also like