Definitions of Timbre
Definitions of Timbre
Sandel)
Helmholtz (1885)
"When we hear notes of the same force and same pitch sounded successively
on a piano-forte, a violin, clarinet, oboe, or trumpet, or by the human voice,
the character of the musical tone of each of these instruments,
notwithstanding the identify of force and pitch, is so different that by means
of it we recognize with the greatest of ease which of these instruments was
used." (p. 19) "...the amplitude of the vibration determines the force or
loudness, and the period of vibration the pitch. Quality of tone can therefore
depend upon neither of these. The only possible hypothesis, therefore; is that
the quality of tone should depend upon the manner in which the motion is
performed within the period of each single vibration." (p. 19) "...to what
extent can the differences of musical quality be reduced to the combination
of different partial tones with different intensities in different musical
tones?" (p. 65) "There has been a general inclination to credit quality with all
possible peculiarities of musical tones that were not evidently due to force
and pitch. This was correct to the extent that quality of tone was merely a
negative conception: But very slight consideration will suffice to show that
many of these peculiarities of musical tones depend upon the way in which
they begin and end: . . . differences in the quality of tone of struck
strings...partly depends on the rapidity with which the tone dies away." (p:
66) "When we speak in what follows of musical quality of tone, we shall
disregard these peculiarities of beginning and ending, and confine our
attention to the peculiarities of the musical tone which continues uniformly.
But even when a musical tone continues with uniform or variable intensity, it
is mixed up, in the general methods of excitement; with certain noises, which
express greater or less irregularities in the motion of the air." (p. 67) "Such
accompanying noises and little inequalities in the motion of the air, furnish
much that is characteristic in the tones of musical instruments...
Notwithstanding the absence of these noises, it is generally possible to
discriminate the-different musical instruments; although it must be
acknowledged that under such circumstances the tone of a French horn may
be occasionally mistaken for that of the singing voice; or a violoncello may be
confused with an harmonium: . . . In the present chapter we shall at first
disregard all irregular portions of the motion of the air; and the mode in
which sounds commence or terminate, directing our attention solely to the
musical part of the tone, properly so called, which corresponds to a
uniformly sustained and regularly period motion of the air..." (p: 68)
"...differences in musical quality of tone depend solely on the presence and
strength of partial tomes; and in -no respect on the differences in phase
under which these partial tones enter into composition. It must be here
observed that we are speaking only of musical quality as previously defined."
(p: 127)
Fletcher (1934)
Textbooks customarily believe that loudness, pitch and timbre correlate
directly with sound intensity, fundamental frequency and overtone
structure... "but these experiments show that a simple one-to-one
relationship does not exist." (p. 59) One might define timbre as "that
characteristic of sensation which enables the listeners to recognize the kind
of musical instrument producing the tone, that is, whether it is a cornet, a
flute or a violin." (p. 67) "...changes in loudness or pitch; without in any way
changing the overtone structures, will also produce changes in timbre." (p.
68) "...timbre depends principally upon the overtone structure; but large
changes in the intensity and the frequency also produce changes in the
timbre." (p. 68)
Seashore (1938)
"Tone quality has two fundamental aspects, namely, (1) timbre; which is the
simultaneous presence or fusion of the fundamental and its overtones at a
given moment, and (2) sonance, the successive presence or fusion of
changing timbre; pitch, and intensity in a tone as a whole. The first may be
called simultaneous fusion; the second; successive: Each of these may be
reduced to the constituent factors which are recordable and measurable and,
from the physical point of view, represent the structure of the tone." (p: 95)
"In general, we may say that, aside from accessory noises and inharmonic
elements, the timbre of atone depends upon (1) the number of harmonic
partials present, (2) the relative location or locations of these partials in the
range from the lowest to- the highest, and (3) the relative strength or
dominance of each partial." (pp. 96-97) ". . . we obtain a definition of timbre
as follows: Timbre is that characteristic of a tone which depends upon its
harmonic structure as modified by absolute pitch and total intensity. . . .we
must also take phase relations into account. Physically the timbre of the tone
is a cross section of the tone quality for the moment represented by the
duration of one vibration in the sound." (p. 97) Definition of sonance: he first
observes that when viewing a movie of any moving object; although we are
being presented a series of discrete snapshots of the object appearing
stationary, our experience is of actual motion of the object, because the
successive snapshots fuse. "The same principle of fusion appears in hearing.
In tonal hearing, successive waves come so fast that they cannot be heard as
individual waves; that is, we cannot separate the timbre or the pitch or the
intensity in one wave from that in the next wave by hearing. The result is that
they fuse and for a given period of time, the mean period of clear perception,
we hear a resultant pitch; intensity; or timbre which tends to be an average
for what is represented in the series of waves that can be grasped in one -
moment of perception: The timbre of atone corresponds to the single
instantaneous picture; sonance corresponds to the picture progression."
(pp.103-104).
Wood (19??)
Licklider (1951)
"The timbre of a complex sound has usually been defined as the subjective
quality that depends upon the complexity or overtone structure of the
physical sound. We have seen; however, that both the loudness and the pitch
of a complex tone are influenced to some extent by its overtone structure. We
must, therefore, not fall back upon the ill-defined notion that timbre has to
do with the distribution or pattern of pitch and loudness in the total
sensation Until careful scientific work has been done on the subject, it cam
hardly be possible to say more about timbre than that it is a
'multidimensional' dimension." (p. 1019)
Ellis (1885)
ANSI (1960)
"When a violin and oboe; of instance, play the same pitch with the loudness,
we can yet distinguish the two tones by their timbre. This property is often
ailed the 'tone quality' or the 'tone color.' I advocate ignoring these names,
because they tend to create confusion. Tone 'quality' is too general, for
logically pitch and loudness are also qualities. Tone 'color' is an optical term
which is out of place in acoustics." (p: 63) "...the indirect bearing of loudness
on timbre. For as the loudness of atone increases, overtones are aroused that
may have been negligible before; and as it wanes; some overtones are
dampened and lost before others. The result is a change of timbre
concomitant with a change in loudness." (pp: 63-64) "The point need not be
belabored that the smallest change in any agent participating in the creation
of a tone...evokes a change in the actually sounding overtone series and
hence a corresponding modification of the total timbre. . . . Any change of
pressure or position necessarily brings about a fresh overtone constellation;
and if a new pitch or a new degree of loudness is the primary purpose of such
a change, timbre can justifiably be thought of as an attribute that perpetually
characterizes the other properties of a tone. The singer Luisa Tetrazzini
claimed that every pitch has its appropriate 'natural' timbre, and many wind
players would agree:" (p: 13?)
Schouten (1968)
They cite the generally accepted definition of timbre as "the attribute that
enables the listener to identify the instrument producing the tone." (p. 23)
Plomp (1970)
Erickson {1975)
Grey {1975)
"Timbre may refer to the features of tone which serve to identify that a
musical sound originates from some particular instrument or family of
instruments, for example, that it is an oboe, or perhaps some sort of double-
reed instrument; or maybe just some woodwind instrument." {p. 1)
Roederer (1975)
"Timbre perception is, however, just a first stage of the operation of tone
source recognition---in music, the identification of the instrument. From this
point of view, tone quality perception is the mechanism by means of which
information is extracted from the auditory signal in such a way as to make it
suitable for: (1) storage in the memory with an adequate label of
identification, and (2) comparison with previously stored and identified
information. The first operation-involves learning or conditioning. A child
who learns to recognize a given musical instrument is presented repeatedly
with a melody played on that instrument and told: 'This is a clarinet.' Ibis
brain extracts suitable information from the succession of auditory stimuli,
labels this information with the qualification 'clarinet' and stores it in the
memory. The second operation represents the conditioned response to a
learned pattern: When the child hears a clarinet play after the learning
experience, his brain compares the information extracted from the incoming
signal (i.e. the timbre) with stored cues, and, if a successful- match is found;
conveys the response: 'a clarinet.' On the other hand; if we listen to anew'
sound, e.g., a series of tones concocted with an electronic synthesizer, our
information-extracting system will feed the cures into the matching
mechanism, which will then try desperately to compare the input with
previously stored information: If this matching process is unsuccessful; a
new storage 'file' will eventually be opened up for this new, now identified,
sound quality. If the process is only partly successful; we react with such
judgments as 'almost like a clarinet' or 'like a barking trombone."' (pp.
138-139)
Plomp (1976)
"Timbre is, after pitch and loudness, the third attribute of the subjective
experience of musical tones. Subjectively, timbre is often coded as the
function of the sound source or of the meaning of the sound: We talk about
the timbre of certain musical instruments, of vowels, and of sounds that
signify certain events in our environment (apparatus, sounds from nature;
footsteps, the slapping of a door; etc.)." (p: 12) "In a restricted sense timbre
may be considered the subjective counterpart of the spectral composition of
tones. Especially important is the relative amplitude of the harmonics. . . .
Recent research has shown that temporal characteristics of the tones may
have a profound influence on timbre as well, which has led to a broadening
of the concept of timbre... Both onset effects (rise time, presence of noise or
inharmonic partials during onset, unequal rise of partials, characteristic
shape of rise curve, etc.) and steady state effects (vibrato, amplitude
modulation, gradual swelling, pitch instability; etc:) are important factors in
the recognition and, therefore, in the timbre of tones." (pp. 13-14) "Sounds
cannot be -ordered on a single scale with respect to timbre. Timbre is a
multidimensional attribute of the perception of sounds." (p. 14)
Slawson (1985)
He chooses to dispense with the word timbre altogether; since the classical
definition is inadequate, yet widely accepted (p. 19) A theory of sound color
should be able "to specify how to preserve color under changes in the
loudness or the duration of a sound. We would like to know how to change
pitch without changing sound color: Moreover; if sound color itself is a
complex phenomenon made up a several different aspects or dimensions, we
must show how one aspect of color can be held invariant as other aspects are
varies." (p. 17) "There is no agreement, however, about what constitutes an
element of sound color. Is sound color to be associated with a specific
musical instrument, say, a particular Stradivarius violin? . . . Or should the
sound of all instruments of the same type be taken as the basic element of
color? When the violin, any violin, is said to have a different 'instrumental
color' from the oboe; we are using the term that way:" (p. 15)
"Timbre (or tone color) . . . refers to the differences of sound quality among
various musical instruments, as well as among the various syllables of speech
(hah vs. goo, for example). Sound vary in timbre along several dimensions;
just as the syllables pah, poh, tah and toh differ on at least two dimensions. .
. . timbre distinctions for speech sounds are fundamentally the same as those
required for musical sounds:" (p: 5) "Timbre has always been the
miscellaneous category for describing the psychological attributes of sound;
gathering into one bundle whatever was left over alter pitch loudness, and
duration had been accounted for. Unlike the psychophysical relationships
involved in the latter categories, which are relatively straightforward, the
relationship underlying timbre are complex and multidimensional . . . The
psychological attributes clustered under the heading timbre fall along more
than one psychological dimension; that is, sounds do not simply direr in how
much timbre they have. And there are several physical dimensions whose
variation causes changes in timbre that interact with each other in complex
ways." (p. 63)
Houtsma (1989)
Krumhansl (1989)
Bregman (1990)
On the ASA definition: "This is, of course; no definition at all. For example; it
implies that there are some sounds for which we cannot decide whether they
possess the quality of timbre or not. In order for the definition to apply; two
sounds need to be able to be presented at the same pitch, but there are some
sounds, such as the scarping of a shovel in a pile of gravel, that have no pitch
at all: We obviously have a problem: Either we must assert that only sounds
with pitch can have timbre, meaning that we cannot discuss the timbre of-a
tambourine or of the musical sounds of many African cultures, or there is
something terribly wrong with the definition." (p. 92) He points out that part
of the problem may stem from-the fact that pitch and loudness are
controlled in musical instruments in straightforward, consistent ways, but
timbre is not. The methods of varying timbre ''vary from instrument to
instrument: . . . When we do find a characteristic of sound that can be
obtained on different instruments, such as vibrato, the characteristic tends to
be given a label and no longer falls into the nameless wastebasket of
'timbre'." (p. 93) His requirements for a vocabulary for timbre include
having labels for salient dimensions that should (1) "act in psychologically
simple ways" and should "have straightforward physical definitions." (pp.
93-94) However; he also says: "Until such time as the dimensions of timbre
are clarified perhaps it is better to drop the term timbre" (p. 94)
Rossing (1990)
Handel (1995)
"We will use the term timbre to refer to the perceptual qualities of objects
and events; that is, 'what it sounds like.' Traditionally, timbre has been
thought of as related to one acoustically measurable property such as that
each note of an instrument or each spoken sound of one voice would be
characterized by a single value of that property. . . . due to the interactive
nature of sound production, there are many stable and time varying
acoustical properties. It is unlikely that any one property or combination of
properties uniquely determines timbre. The sense of timbre comes from the
emergent, interactive properties of the vibration pattern. . . . One possibility
is that timbre is perceived in terms of the actions required to generate the
event. . . . The perception of the production invariances would allow us to
hear the same object in spite of large changes in the acoustical signal:
Another possibility is that timbre is perceived simply in terms of the acoustic
properties and that the connection between the acoustic properties and the
object is learned by experience. In this view, the acoustic properties are used
to figure out what event was most likely to have produced that sound." (p.
426)
REFERENCES (incomplete)
. Cho, J.L., Hall, M.D. and Pastore, R.E. (1993). Normalization of musical
instrument timbre. Unpublished manuscript.
. Fletcher, H. (1934). Loudness, pitch and the timbre of musical tones and
their relation to the intensity, the frequency and the overtone structure.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 6, 59-69.