Ghost From The Past
Ghost From The Past
Lies Van Asschea, Luc Van de Vena, Mathieu Vandenbulckea, Patrick Luytenb,c
a
Section of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, University Hospitals Leuven,
KUL, Belgium
b
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Leuven, Belgium
c
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College
London, UK
Corresponding author:
Lies Van Assche
University Hospitals Leuven
Herestraat 49
3000 Leuven
+3216341300
[email protected]
1
Abstract
Objectives. Research suggests that vulnerability for anxiety and depression in late life results
context, there is growing evidence for the role of childhood trauma on vulnerability for both
anxiety and depression throughout the course of life, mainly through its effects on attachment
childhood trauma on depression and anxiety in late life specifically remains unclear. The
current study therefore aims to investigate the association between retrospectively reported
childhood interpersonal trauma, attachment dimensions and levels of anxiety and depression
in late life. Method. A sample of 81 community dwelling older adults completed measures of
early and current adversity, attachment dimensions, and levels of anxiety and depression.
Results. The occurrence and frequency of childhood trauma, but not later negative adult life
events, was associated with late life anxiety and depression. Both attachment anxiety and
avoidance were related to anxiety and depression. Only attachment anxiety affected the
association between childhood trauma, and emotional neglect in particular, and late life
anxiety and depression. Conclusion. Childhood trauma may be associated with anxiety and
depression in late life. Part of this association is probably indirect, via the effect of insecure
Disclosure statement
2
Introduction
Depression and anxiety are highly prevalent in late life. Indeed, estimates suggest that
depression occurs in approximately 25% of the elderly (Forlani et al., 2014), whereas the
prevalence of anxiety disorders ranges from 10 to 20% in older adults (Cassidy & Rector,
2008; Kirmizioglu, Dogan, Kugu, & Akyüz, 2009). Moreover, comorbidity between
depression and anxiety in late life is substantial (Beekman et al., 2002) and is related to poor
clinical outcome (Beekman et al., 2002; Flint, 1994; Cassidy & Rector, 2008). Studies have
depression and anxiety in early adulthood (Anderson, Fields, & Dobb, 2011; Vink, Aartsen,
& Schoevers, 2008) and many older adults have been confronted with childhood abuse or
neglect. Specifically, estimated prevalence rates of childhood trauma in American adults older
than 55 years were 13.5% for verbal abuse, 9.6% for physical abuse and 9.3% for sexual
abuse (Bynum et al., 2010). Yet, there is still a paucity of studies in this area in older adults,
and relatively little is understood about the mechanisms explaining the relationship between
understand putative links between early adversity and vulnerability for anxiety and depression
in late life. This model essentially suggests that disorders that have their onset in late life
might reflect ‘delayed’ manifestation effects of exposure to early life stressors (Lupien,
McEwen, Gunnar, & Heim, 2009). Specifically, repeated exposure to severe distress has been
suggested to produce enduring effects on the brain through activation of the hypothalamus-
pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis which leads to the production of glucocorticoids that exert
harmful effects on brain tissue, particularly during so-called ‘critical time windows’ of
3
increased neuronal plasticity in brain areas involved in the regulation of stress, such as the
hippocampus, the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex (Giedd et al., 1996; Pruessner et al.,
2010). Whilst these ‘programming’ effects may manifest in early childhood or adulthood, in a
substantial proportion of individuals these effects may not manifest until late life (Clark,
Caldwell, Power, & Stansfield, 2010). Both age-related brain changes and age-specific
challenges (and particularly experiences of loss of attachment figures) have been argued to
play a key role in this context. Indeed, old age is associated with several challenges
concerning physical health, sensory loss, retirement with associated changes in income,
narrowing social networks, cognitive declines, and increasing awareness of mortality (Charles
& Carstensen, 2010) that increase frailty which may lead to the onset of psychopathology in
late life in individuals who have been exposed to early life trauma but have shown a resilience
to the development of mental health issues until then. For instance, in a group of 567
community dwelling adults older than 60 years, an increased frailty as assessed using the
Fried biological syndrome model was associated with more anxiety and depression compared
with robust older adults (Ni Mhaolain et al., 2012). More broadly, in social sciences, there has
domains in explaining the impact of aging on wellbeing (Ferraro & Shippee, 2009).
Consistent with these concepts and assumptions, a number of studies have indeed found an
late life, even after controlling for early adulthood psychiatric symptoms (Clark, Caldwell,
Power, & Stansfield, 2010; Falk, Hersen, & van Hasselt, 1994; Gershon, Sudheimer,
Tirouvanziam, Williams, & O'Hara, 2013). Yet, more research in this area is clearly needed,
particularly on the role of attachment in these relationships as the attachment system has been
shown to be a key modulator of stress and adversity throughout life (Nolte et al., 2011).
4
The Role of Attachment
take into account that the development of stress and affect regulation largely occurs in the
formulations indeed converge to suggest that the attachment system may be thought of as a
behavioural system that is activated when faced with threat. It involves the coordination of
different subsystems aimed at reducing distress through seeking actual or imagined proximity
to an attachment figure (Fonagy & Luyten, 2009; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007; Sbarra &
Hazan, 2008).
child fosters the development of adaptive affect regulation strategies typical of secure
attachment in adulthood. Individuals who are securely attached generally have a strong sense
intentioned, and thus have the ability to turn to others in times of stress and adversity.
secondary attachment strategies develop that function as habitual responses to stress in later
system. Individuals with high levels of attachment anxiety often view themselves as
incompetent in dealing with life’s challenges and they are hypervigilant for signs of rejection
primarily use hyperactivating strategies are therefore typically associated with an excessive
need for others (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007), but paradoxically they are not easily comforted
in times of distress because of their underlying belief that others will not be there for them.
5
A second compensatory attachment strategy that is observed in individuals with avoidant
attachment features, entails attempts to deactivate the attachment system and avoid contact
with attachment figures during stressful times. When distressed, these individuals have a
tendency towards compulsive autonomy (i.e., the conviction that they have to be able to deal
with distress without the help of others), and consequently they tend to suppress negative
emotions. As there is a relative stability in the quality of attachment throughout life, early
Indeed, there is considerable research that has found a relationship between adult
attachment insecurity and vulnerability for psychopathology in adulthood and late life
(Mikulicer & Shaver, 2012; Van Assche et al., 2013). Additionally, childhood trauma as
opposed to adulthood trauma has been linked to the development of insecure attachment
patterns (Ogle, Rubin & Siegler, 2015), without consistent evidence for the differential impact
of different types of trauma on attachment (e.g. Erozkan, 2016). Insecure attachment patterns,
in their turn, affect the quality of relationships and psychological function in an ongoing
fashion (Fowler, Allen, Oldham, & Frueh, 2013). Coincidently, studies also converge to
suggest that higher levels of attachment anxiety are negatively related to self-reported
wellbeing in older adults (Andersson & Stevens, 1993; Bodner & Cohen-Fridel, 2010;
Cicirelli, 1989; Cookman, 2005; Kafetsios & Sideridis, 2006; Park & Vandenberg, 1994;
Webster, 1997, 1998; Wensauer & Grossmann, 1995). By contrast, the use of attachment
psychological and physical wellbeing in old age in some studies (Jain & Labouvie-Vief,
reporting bias typical of individuals that primarily rely on attachment deactivating strategies.
Jain and Labouvie-Vief (2010), for instance, found that attachment avoidance in community
6
dwelling older adults was at the same time associated with relatively high self-reported
wellbeing, but also with elevated heart rate and blood pressure which suggests that there still
The present study aims to further explore the nature of the relationship between childhood
interpersonal trauma and late life anxiety or depression. Specifically, we hypothesized that
attachment anxiety and avoidance in adulthood would mediate the association between
childhood trauma and anxiety or depression in older adults. Based on the findings reviewed
above, we hypothesized that childhood trauma in the elderly will be associated with higher
levels of anxiety and depression. We also predicted a dose-response effect, i.e., that levels of
self-reported anxiety and depression would increase as the number of trauma’s increases.
Finally, we expected that the relationship between childhood trauma and late life anxiety and
depression would be indirect, through the effect of associations with attachment dimensions.
attachment avoidance has been shown to be associated with a tendency to suppress negative
emotions and underreport distressing events, which can be expected to attenuate the
relationship between childhood trauma and feelings of anxiety and depression in late life.
Method
A sample of community dwelling older adults were recruited from organisations for seniors.
The main researcher visited several meetings from senior citizen organisations, provided
information about the current study and prepared leaflets with practical information about
participation in the study (timing, fee, …). Older adults who were present at the time of the
7
visits received a leaflet with information. Those willing to participate filled in a form
containing demographic information such as date of birth, formal education, marital status
and number of children. The candidates for participation in the study also provided a
telephone number or email address. The main researcher collected all the leaflets and
participants were contacted by telephone or email within two months after recruitment. Extra
leaflets were given to the persons responsible for the organisations and older adults who were
not present during the visits but who read the leaflet and were interested in participation,
could send an email or call the main researcher. The study was approved by the ethics
committee of the University Hospitals of Leuven and all subjects signed an informed consent
older adults aged 74.90 (SD: 6.64, range: 62-90) agreed to participate in the study. There were
29 male (36%) and 52 female volunteers (64%). Most were married (58%) or widowed
(27%). A minority was single (9%) or divorced (6%). The average number of children was 2
(SD = 1.34; range 0-5). The mean number of years in formal education was 12.85 (SD = 3.66,
range 6-16). A brief screening using the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein,
Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) showed intact cognitive functioning in all of the participants with
Measures
Information about childhood adverse life events was acquired using the Childhood Trauma
Questionnaire – Short Form (CTQ-SF) (Bernstein et al., 2003). The CTQ-SF contains 25
questions that need to be scored on a five point Likert scale ranging from 1 = ‘Never true’ to 5
= ‘Very often true’, pertaining to five subscales: emotional neglect, physical neglect,
emotional abuse, physical abuse, sexual abuse. Besides dimensional scores, cut-off scores
8
based on the CTQ manual were used to assess the presence or absence of different types of
trauma (emotional abuse cut-off 12, physical abuse cut-off 9, sexual abuse cut-off 7, physical
neglect cut-off 9, emotional neglect cut-off 14) (Bernstein & Fink, 1998). We also divided
participants into two groups based on the presence of any type of trauma as opposed to none.
Finally, we calculated a score based on the total number of traumatic events, resulting in a
score ranging from 0 ( = no trauma) to 5 ( = 5 different types of trauma). The CTQ-SF has
shown good reliability and validity as a measure for retrospective report of childhood trauma
(Bernstein et al., 2003). Specifically, there was measurement invariance across different
significant factor loadings of the individual items and the scales with alpha coefficients
ranging from .84 to .89 across groups on emotional abuse, .81 to .86 for physical abuse, .92 to
.95 for sexual abuse, .85 to .91 for emotional neglect and slightly lower factor loadings
ranging from .61 to .78 for physical neglect. Furthermore, the questionnaire has shown good
criterion related validity in a subsample of 179 adolescents for whom corroborative data on
trauma were available. Specifically, the CTQ latent constructs that were obtained through
neglect using the child maltreatment ascertainment interview (CMAI), with an excellent fit in
a path model that allowed covariance among predictor variables χ2 (361, n = 179) = 550.08; p
< .001; comparative fit index (CFI) = .93; root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA)
= .05 (Bernstein et al., 2003). A validation study of the Dutch CTQ-SF showed good
reliability for physical abuse (α = .91), emotional abuse (α = .89), sexual abuse (α = .95) and
emotional neglect (α = .91). There was a slightly lower reliability for physical neglect (α =
.63) (Thombs, Brett, Bernstein, Lobbestael & Arntz, 2009). In the current sample of older
community dwelling adults, internal consistency reliability of the CTQ subscales was good in
9
the subscales emotional abuse (λ-2 = .83; α = .83), sexual abuse (λ-2 = .92; α = .91), physical
abuse (λ-2 = .85; α = .80) and emotional neglect (λ-2 = .81; α = .80). In line with prior
research, reliability was slightly lower in the physical neglect subscale (λ-2 = .62; α = .59).
Negative life events in adulthood (from age 18 onwards) were assessed using the
The participants were first asked whether they had experienced specific events pertaining to
several domains (work, family, friends, romantic relationships, health, crime, finance). Next,
they had to indicate how distressing each event was on a Likert scale ranging from 1 to 7. The
total number of events was used as a measure of adulthood adversity and the total sum of
subjective distress associated with the events was used as a measure for the distress associated
Attachment Dimensions
Waller, & Brennan, 2000) was used to assess levels of attachment avoidance and attachment
anxiety in adulthood. The ECR-R consists of 36 items which have to be scored on a 6-point
Likert type scale, with 18 items measuring the level of attachment anxiety and the other 18
items assessing the level of attachment avoidance. Participants were invited to respond to the
questions keeping in mind their relationship with one important attachment figure: partner,
(adult) child, sibling, friend or other. In this study, scores for attachment anxiety and
avoidance were averaged. Higher scores are suggestive of more attachment anxiety or
avoidance. The ECR-R has demonstrated good psychometric qualities (Sibley, Fischer, & Liu,
2005) with a Cronbach’s alfa of .91 for attachment avoidance and .94 for attachment anxiety.
Additionally, confirmatory factor analysis showed that a two factor model exhibited an
excellent fit to the data, with χ2(53, n = 478) = 142.26, CFI = .98, RMSEA = .06. A Dutch
10
version of the ECR-R demonstrated good reliability for attachment avoidance (α = .94) and
attachment anxiety (α = .92). Moreover, confirmatory factor analysis showed that the
hypothesized two-factor model of the ECR-R, showed a good fit with the data, χ2(53, n =
262) = 169.650, CFI = .95, RMSEA = .092 (Kooiman, Klaassens, van Heloma Lugt &
Kamperman, 2013). Internal consistency reliability was also good in our sample for
attachment anxiety (λ-2 = .90; α = .90) and avoidance (λ-2 = .86; α = .85).
The anxiety subscale of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS-A) (Zigmond &
Snaith, 1983) and the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) (Yesavage et al., 1982) were used to
assess current levels of anxiety and depression, respectively. Both instruments are brief, easy
to administer and have been shown to have good psychometric properties with a Cronbach α
of .83 for the HADS-A (Herrmann, 1997; Bjelland, Dahl, Haug & Neckelmann, 2002) and .82
for the Dutch translation in a group of adults aged more than 65 years (Spinhoven, Ormel,
Sloekers, Kempen, Speckens & van Hemert, 1997). There was a Cronbach α of .94 for the
English version of the GDS (Yesavage et al., 1982) and a Cronbach α of .87 for the Dutch
version of the GDS (van Marwijk, Wallace, de Bock, Hermans, Kaptein & Mulder, 1985).
Internal consistency reliability in our sample is good for the HADS-A (λ-2 = .85; α = .84) and
GDS (λ-2 = .82; α = .81). Scores of 7 or higher on the HADS-A are considered clinically
significant. The GDS distinguishes between mild depression (scores between 11 and 13),
moderate depression (scores ranging from 14-20) and severe depression (scores > 20).
Cognition
Finally, a standardized Dutch version of the Mini Mental Status Examination (MMSE)
(Folstein et al., 1975; Kok, Verhey, & Schmand, 2004) was used as a tool for the assessment
11
of cognitive abilities. It measures orientation skills, memory function, concentration, language
and praxis. The maximum score is 30, with 23 considered as an appropriate cut-off value with
a sensitivity of 0.82 (Bour, Rasquin, Boreas, Limburg, & Verhey, 2010). Scores below this
cut-off are suggestive of cognitive dysfunction that is more pronounced than normal.
Data Analysis
Associations between the study variables were explored based on bivariate Pearson
correlations and biserial correlations for the association between continuous and dichotomized
approach, because of the relatively small sample size, to investigate the hypothesized
mediation models involving adult attachment, dimensional measures of childhood trauma and
depression and anxiety in late life (Preacher & Hayes, 2004). Analyses were performed using
SPSS Version 24. Results were considered statistically significant at the p < .05 level.
Results
Almost half of the participants reported having experienced at least one type of childhood
trauma. Emotional and physical neglect were reported most (see Table 1). Participants
reported on average 11 negative life events in adulthood (SD = 3.6; range 3-19), with most
events relating to the family (e.g. a severe illness or death of a family member), children or
friends (e.g. severe illness or death of a friend, falling out with a friend) and health (e.g.
surgery, reduced mobility). Work, financial adversity, crime or issues related to romantic
relationships accounted for only a small proportion of the negative life events reported (Table
1).
There was an average level of anxiety of 4.41, with a range of 0 to 18. Twenty individuals
reported elevated levels of anxiety (cut-off value of 7 or more). There was an average level of
12
5.56 for the GDS, with a range of 0 to 22. There were nine individuals who reported clinically
significant depressive feelings (cut-off value of 11 or more). Anxiety and depression were
Zero-order correlations between childhood traumatic events, adulthood negative life events
and anxiety and depression are shown in Table 2. First, the presence of any childhood trauma
was positively related to current levels of anxiety and depression. The total number of
childhood traumatic events reported, was significantly positively correlated with anxiety, but
not with depression. The number of negative life events experienced in adulthood was not
With respect to specific types of trauma, both emotional abuse and neglect showed a
positive association with current levels of anxiety. Emotional neglect was also significantly
and positively related to depression. Physical neglect, physical abuse, and sexual abuse,
however, were not significantly correlated with depression or anxiety (Table 2).
Physical abuse was strongly negatively correlated with attachment avoidance. Emotional
neglect, on the other hand, was significantly positively associated with attachment anxiety.
There were no significant correlations between attachment anxiety or avoidance and physical
neglect, emotional abuse or sexual abuse. Both attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance
showed a significant positive correlation with current levels of anxiety and depression (Table
2).
Hence, only for attachment anxiety and emotional neglect, conditions for mediation were
satisfied and we thus investigated whether emotional neglect was associated with late life
anxiety and depression through the effect of attachment anxiety. Results showed that there
was indeed an indirect effect of emotional neglect on late life anxiety through attachment
anxiety (Table 3). There was no evidence that emotional neglect influenced current levels of
anxiety directly (c’ = 1.85, p = .06). The indirect effect coefficient was .04 (confidence
13
intervals: .00-.15). Additionally, there was an indirect relationship between emotional neglect
and late life depression through the effect of attachment anxiety (Table 4). Again, there was
no evidence that emotional neglect influenced current levels of depression directly (c’ = 1.83,
p = .14). The indirect effect coefficient was .05 (confidence intervals: .00-.17).
Discussion
reported having been exposed to at least one type of childhood trauma, which slightly exceeds
prevalence rates usually reported in younger cohorts (Saunders & Adams, 2014). Prior
research indeed suggests that the occurrence of childhood maltreatment has declined in some
regions, particularly in Western countries (Finkelhor & Jones, 2006), leading to lower
prevalence rates in younger cohorts. Moreover, emotional and physical neglect was reported
more often than abuse, which may also reflect cohort effects, as pedagogic changes and
greater improvements in welfare have occurred over the past few decades.
Surprisingly, only childhood trauma, but not negative life events that occurred in
adulthood, were related to late life anxiety and depression. We also found a dose-response
effect as individuals who were exposed to different types of childhood trauma, experienced
higher levels of both anxiety and depression. These findings are consistent with prior research
on the enduring effects of childhood trauma (Lupien et al., 2009). Indeed, other researchers
have also looked into the differential impact of early versus adulthood trauma and they found
a significant interaction between the developmental timing of the trauma and attachment
anxiety, showing that attachment anxiety was stronger in older individuals with Post
Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) symptoms and childhood trauma compared with older
adults with PTSD symptoms and adulthood trauma (Ogle, Rubin & Siegler, 2015).
14
In line with these findings, attachment anxiety was positively related to emotional neglect
in early youth in our sample of older adults and it was also related to current levels of anxiety
and depression. Moreover, the relationship between childhood emotional neglect and current
levels of anxiety and depression was indirect, via the effect of attachment anxiety. Cognitive-
affective schema’s in individuals with high levels of attachment anxiety are assumed to
encompass the belief that one is incompetent to solve problems oneself. This belief might lead
to higher levels of anxiety and depression in late life, when individuals are exposed to more
age-related losses and social networks typically decrease (Kafetsios & Sideridis, 2006).
However, at the same time, individuals with high levels of attachment anxiety may be
inclined to ‘over-report’ trauma, and emotional neglect in particular. Indeed, because of their
excessive need of reassurance, individuals with high levels of attachment anxiety might feel
neglected more easily when attachment figures have been unable to meet their emotional
Attachment avoidance, to the contrary, was associated with a smaller number of reported
childhood trauma’s. This was particularly the case for physical abuse. Although it is difficult
to draw strong conclusions based on the current cross-sectional data, this latter finding is
similar to findings from other studies suggesting that individuals high on attachment
avoidance tend to cope with distress through defensive attempts to deemphasize the
importance of negative emotions related to these events (Shaver, Collins, & Clark, 1996).
This may also result in a tendency to underreport distressing events. However, this
mechanism, which may be adaptive in the short term, might become counterproductive in the
longer term, and especially in late life, as it may increasingly fail, leading to higher levels of
self-reported anxiety and depression in individuals with high levels of attachment avoidance
(Jain & Labouvie-Vief, 2010). Indeed, Gross and Levenson (1993) have reported that
suppression of negative emotions actually puts individuals under constant and considerable
15
strain as it results in greater physiological arousal, which could lead to physical and emotional
‘wear and tear’ and ultimately a collapse of emotion regulation strategies resulting in higher
In summary, results suggest that childhood trauma may negatively impact on late life
systematic screening of early life trauma in older patients seems important and an awareness
of the importance of security in attachment to the therapist as well as for instance family
treating older patients who present with anxious or depressive symptoms and who have a
history of trauma (Mikulincer & Florian, 1998). Also, the clinician needs to be aware of the
attachment characteristics.
the mechanism underlying this association may be different in individuals that have high
levels of attachment avoidance versus those with high levels of attachment anxiety.
Other limitations of the current study include the relatively small sample size (n=81),
which possibly reduces statistical power. Power analysis for multiple correlations indeed
showed that a sample of 87 individuals would insure a power of .80 to detect correlations with
a coefficient of at least .30 and alpha set at .05 (Gatsonis & Sampson, 1989; Hulley,
Cummings, Browner, Grady & Newman, 2013). Still, the use of bootstrapping in the
mediation analyses mitigates this limitation somewhat (Preacher & Hayes, 2004). However,
because of the small sample size, we were not able to conduct analyses for men and women
separately, although gender has often been shown to play an important role in research on the
impact of childhood trauma (Varese et al., 2012). Second, the cross-sectional nature of the
study design limits the possibilities for causal interpretation. Retrospective reports of
16
childhood trauma may introduce an important bias. It is likely, as we have discussed, that the
quality of current attachment working models has an influence on the reporting and
to under-report such events, whereas attachment anxiety, to the contrary, may lead to the
limitation is the lack of data on PTSD symptoms in our group. Also, we did not assess
adulthood trauma, but adulthood negative life events that might not necessarily be traumatic
and thus less harsh. This may already explain a difference in the observed associations of
adulthood versus childhood adversity. Indeed, some prior researches have even demonstrated
an inverse relationship (Shrira, Shmotkin & Litwin, 2012). Specifically, when data concerning
potentially traumatic adulthood and childhood events were collected in 1130 older Israeli
participants, and these were correlated with measures of wellbeing, only adulthood adversity
was significantly and negatively related to wellbeing. Importantly, the authors refer to a
cumulative effect, suggesting that at a certain point throughout the lifespan individuals may
have reached a critical threshold and wellbeing starts to reduce with each additional adverse
event. This threshold for experiencing (possibly non-traumatic) adversity may not have been
reached in early youth, explaining the absence of a significant relationship between wellbeing
and childhood adversity in their sample. Finally, we have studied a sample of community
dwelling older adults and hence the current findings might not be generalizable to clinical
populations.
Taking into account these limitations, the current results suggests that early life trauma
shows a positive association with anxiety and depression in late life (Comijs et al., 2013),
especially in individuals with insecure attachment features. Still, even though findings in the
current study are in line with earlier findings from studies on childhood adversity and diverse
17
psychopathology throughout the lifespan, prospective research is needed to replicate and
18
References
Anderson, K. A., Fields, N. L., & Dobb, L. A. (2011). Understanding the impact of early-life
trauma in nursing home residents. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 54(8), 755-
767.
Andersson, L., & Stevens, N. (1993). Associations between early experiences with parents
10.1093/geronj/48.3.P109
Beekman, A. T., Penninx, W. J. H., Deeg, D. J. H., de Beurs, E., Geerlings, S. W., & Van
Tilburg, W. (2002). The impact of depression on the well-being, disability and use of
105(1), 20-27.
Bernstein, D. P., & Fink, L. (1998). Childhood trauma questionnaire: A retrospective self-
Bernstein, D. P., Stein, J. A., Newcomb, M. D., Walker, E., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T., . . .
Bjelland, I., Dahl, A. A., Haug, T. T., & Neckelmann, D. (2002). The validity of the Hospital
Bodner, E., & Cohen-Fridel, S. (2010). Relations between attachment styles, ageism and
10.1017/s1041610210001249
Bour, A., Rasquin, S., Boreas, A., Limburg, M., & Verhey, F. (2010). How predictive is the
MMSE for cognitive performance after stroke? Journal of Neurology, 257(4), 630-
637.
19
Bynum, L., Griffin, T., Riding, D. L., Wynkoop, K. S., Anda, R. F., Edwards, V. J., ... &
Cassidy, K., & Rector, N. A. (2008). The silent geriatric giant: Anxiety disorders in late life.
Charles, S., & Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Social and emotional aging. Annual Reviews in
Clark, C., Caldwell, T., Power, C., & Stansfield, C. A. (2010). Does the influence of
Comijs, H. C., van Exel, E., van der Mast, R. C., Paauw, A., Oude Voshaar, R., & Stek, M. L.
Erozkan, A. (2016). The link between types of attachment and childhood trauma. Universal
Falk, B., Hersen, M., & van Hasselt, V. (1994). Assessment of post-traumatic stress disorder
Ferraro, K. F., & Shippee, T. P. (2009). Aging and cumulative inequality: How does
20
Finkelhor, D., & Jones, L. (2006). Why have child maltreatment and victimization declined?
Flint, A. J. (1994). Epidemiology and comorbidity of anxiety disorders in the elderly. The
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). "Mini-mental state". A practical
method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of
Forlani, C., Morri, M., Ferrari, B., Dalmonte, E., Menchetti, M., De Ronchi, D., & Atti, A. R.
10.1016/j.jagp.2012.08.015
Fowler, J. C., Allen, J. G., Oldham, J. M., & Frueh, C. B. (2013). Exposure to interpersonal
149(1-3), 313-318.
Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item response theory analysis of
Gatsonis, C., & Sampson, A. R. (1989). Multiple correlation: Exact power and sample size
Gershon, A., Sudheimer, K., Tirouvanziam, R., Williams, L. M., & O'Hara, R. (2013). The
21
Giedd, J. N., Snell, J. W., Lange, N., Rajapakse, J. C., Casey, B. J., Kozuch, P. L., . . .
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiology, self-report, and
Herrmann, C. (1997). International experiences with the Hospital Anxiety and Depression
Hulley, S. B., Cummings, S. R., Browner, W. S., Grady, D., & Newman, T. B. (2013).
Jain, E., & Labouvie-Vief, G. (2010). Compensatory effects of emotion avoidance in adult
Kafetsios, K., & Sideridis, G. D. (2006). Attachment, social support and well-being in young
10.1177/1359105306069084
Kirmizioglu, Y., Dogan, O., Kugu, N., & Akyüz, G. (2009). Prevalence of anxiety disorders
1033.
Kok, R. M., Verhey, F., & Schmand, B. (2004). Meetinstrumenten bij cognitieve stoornissen.
Kooiman, C. G., Klaassens, E. R., van Heloma Lugt, J. Q., & Kamperman, A. M. (2013).
22
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout
the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,
McEwen, B. S., & Gianaros, P. J. (2010). Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation:
Links to socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Annals of the New York Academy
Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1998). The relationship between adult attachment styles and
(Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 143-165). New York: Guilford.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics, and
Ni Mhaolain, A. M., Fan, C. W., Romero-Ortuno, R., Cogan, L., Cunningham, C., Kenny, R.
A., & Lawlor, B. (2012). Frailty, depression, and anxiety in later life. International
Nolte, T., Guiney, J., Fonagy, P., Mayes, L., & Luyten, P. (2011). Interpersonal stress
10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00055
Ogle, C. M., Rubin, D. C., & Siegler, I. C. (2014). Cumulative exposure to traumatic events in
Park, D., & Vandenberg, B. (1994). The influence of separation orientation on life satisfaction
in the elderly. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 39(2),
23
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect
Pruessner, J. C., Li, L. M., Serles, W., Pruessner, M., Collins, D. L., Kabani, N., . . . Evans, A.
Pruessner, J. C., Dedovic, K., Pruessner, M., Lord, C., Buss, C., Collins, L., . . . Lupien, S. J.
(2010). Stress regulation in the central nervous system: Evidence from structural and
10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.016
childhood. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 23(2), 167-
separation, loss, and recovery. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12(2), 141-
167.
Shaver, P. R., Collins, N. L., & Clark, C. (1996). Attachment styles and internal working
24
Shrira, A., Shmotkin, D., & Litwin, H. (2012). Potentially traumatic events at different points
in the life span and mental health: Findings from SHARE-Israel. The American
Sibley, C. G., Fischer, R., & Liu, J. H. (2005). Reliability and validity of the Revised
Spinhoven, P., Ormel, J., Sloekers, P. P. A., Kempen, G. J. M., Speckens, A. E. M & Van
Scale (HADS) in different groups of Dutch subjects. Psychological Medicine, 27, 363-
370.
10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12070957
Thombs, B. D., Bernstein, D. P., Lobbestael, J., & Arntz, A. (2009). A validation study of the
Tisserand, D. J., Pruessner, J. C., Sanz Arigita, E. J., van Boxtel, M. P., Evans, A. C., Jolles,
J., & Uylings, H. B. (2002). Regional frontal cortical volumes decrease differentially
Van Assche, L., Luyten, P., Bruffaerts, R., Persoons, P., van de Ven, L., & Vandenbulcke, M.
implications for clinical practice. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(1), 67-81. doi:
10.1016/j.cpr.2012.10.003
25
van Marwijk, H. W., Wallace, P., de Bock, G. H., Hermans, J., Kaptein, A. A., & Mulder, J.
45(393), 195-199.
Varese, F., Smeets, F., Drukker, M., Lieverse, R., Lataster, T., Viechtbauer, W., . . . Bentall,
Vink, D., Aartsen, M. J., & Schoevers, R. A. (2008). Risk factors for anxiety and depression
Webster, J. D. (1998). Attachment styles, reminiscence functions and happiness in young and
4065(98)90006-8
integration and use of social network resources in advanced age]. Zeitschrift fur
Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., & Leirer, V. O.
Zigmond, A. S., & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta
26
Table 1. Self-reported childhood trauma and adulthood negative life events in a sample of
(% of individuals)
Work 1 (9.09%)
Relationship 1 (9.09%)
Children 2 (18.18%)
Crime 1 (9.09%)
Finance 0 (0%)
Note. CTQ = Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; HADS-A = Hospital Anxiety and Depression
Anxiety Depression
Note. CTQ = Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; PERI = Psychiatric Epidemiology Research Interview; * = p <0.05, ** = p <0.01, *** = p
<0.001.
Table 3. Model coefficients for the mediation through attachment anxiety of the effect of
Consequent
R2 = 0.20 R2 = 0.19
Consequent
R2 = 0.20 R2 = 0.31