Neuroscience Assesment Paper
Neuroscience Assesment Paper
Neuroscience Assesment Paper
Neurobiology of Disease
7Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, Columbia University, New York, New York 10032, 8Foundation for Molecular
Medicine, Università Politecnica delle Marche, Ancona 60020, Italy, and 9Oasi Research Institute-IRCCS, Troina, 94018, Italy
Failure of anti-amyloid- peptide (A) therapies against Alzheimer’s disease (AD), a neurodegenerative disorder characterized by high
amounts of the peptide in the brain, raised the question of the physiological role of A released at low concentrations in the healthy brain.
To address this question, we studied the presynaptic and postsynaptic mechanisms underlying the neuromodulatory action of picomolar
amounts of oligomeric A42 (oA42 ) on synaptic glutamatergic function in male and female mice. We found that 200 pM oA42 induces
an increase of frequency of miniature EPSCs and a decrease of paired pulse facilitation, associated with an increase in docked vesicle
number, indicating that it augments neurotransmitter release at presynaptic level. oA42 also produced postsynaptic changes as shown
by an increased length of postsynaptic density, accompanied by an increased expression of plasticity-related proteins such as cAMP-
responsive element binding protein phosphorylated at Ser133, calcium-calmodulin-dependent kinase II phosphorylated at Thr286, and
brain-derived neurotrophic factor, suggesting a role for A in synaptic tagging. These changes resulted in the conversion of early into late
long-term potentiation through the nitric oxide/cGMP/protein kinase G intracellular cascade consistent with a cGMP-dependent switch
from short- to long-term memory observed in vivo after intrahippocampal administration of picomolar amounts of oA42. These effects
were present upon extracellular but not intracellular application of the peptide and involved ␣7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. These
observations clarified the physiological role of oA42 in synaptic function and memory formation providing solid fundamentals for
investigating the pathological effects of high A levels in the AD brains.
Key words: amyloid precursor protein; amyloid-beta oligomers; neurotransmitter release; nicotinic receptors; synaptic plasticity; syn-
aptic transmission
Significance Statement
High levels of oligomeric amyloid-42 (oA42 ) induce synaptic dysfunction leading to memory impairment in Alzheimer’s disease
(AD). However, at picomolar concentrations, the peptide is needed to ensure long-term potentiation (LTP) and memory. Here, we
show that extracellular 200 pM oA42 concentrations increase neurotransmitter release, number of docked vesicles, postsynaptic
density length, and expression of plasticity-related proteins leading to the conversion of early LTP into late LTP and of short-term
memory into long-term memory. These effects require ␣7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors and are mediated through the nitric
oxide/cGMP/protein kinase G pathway. The knowledge of A function in the healthy brain might be useful to understand the
causes leading to its increase and detrimental effect in AD.
Gulisano et al. • Physiological Role of A Oligomers at the Synapse J. Neurosci., July 24, 2019 • 39(30):5986 – 6000 • 5987
phosphonovaleric acid (APV, Tocris Bioscience, 50 M) (Puzzo et al., for 1 h before equilibration at room temperature. During recordings,
2008), and methyllycaconitine (MLA, Sigma-Aldrich, 10 M based on slices were placed in a recording chamber perfused with heated ACSF
DR curves obtained in our laboratory) were dissolved in DMSO, ali- (32°C) and bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2. All recordings were made
quoted, and stored at ⫺20°C. All drugs were diluted in ACSF or saline with the GABAA receptor antagonist picrotoxin (50 M) added to the
solution (0.9% NaCl) to the desired final concentration right before ACSF. Whole-cell recording pipettes (3–5 M⍀) were filled with the same
electrophysiological or behavioral experiments. 6E10 (Covance, catalog internal solution used for dual-patch recordings in organotypic hip-
#SIG-39320, RRID:AB_662798, 1:300) and M3.2 (Covance, catalog pocampal slices. Whole-cell recordings were performed with a Multi-
#SIG-39155, RRID:AB_2028758, 2 g/ml) antibodies used for electro- clamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices). A Digidata 1440A series
physiological experiments were directly diluted in ACSF according to interface and pClamp 10 software were used for data acquisition and
previous studies (Ripoli et al., 2014; Palmeri et al., 2017). stimulation protocols. Data were filtered at 1 kHz, digitized at 10 kHz,
and analyzed online and offline.
Dual whole-cell recordings Experimental design. To study LTP in CA1 pyramidal cells, the stimu-
Hippocampal organotypic slice cultures were prepared from P4 –P7 rats lation intensity that elicited one-third of the maximal response ampli-
through a McIllwain tissue chopper and placed on semiporous mem- tude of AMPAR EPSC was used for delivering test pulses every 20 s. CA1
branes (Millipore) for 5–7 d before recordings, as described previously pyramidal cells were held at ⫺60 mV. LTP was induced by two trains of
(Spinelli et al., 2017). Hippocampal subfields and electrode positions HFS (100 Hz, 1 s) separated by 20 s, with the patched cells depolarized to
were identified with the aid of 4⫻ and 40⫻ water-immersion objectives 0 mV. This induction protocol was always applied within 5–7 min of
on an upright microscope equipped with differential interference con- achieving whole-cell configuration, to avoid “washout” of LTP. Re-
trast optics under infrared illumination (BX51WI; Olympus) and video sponses to test pulse were recorded for 30 min to assess LTP. The mag-
observation (BTE-B050-U CMOS camera; Mightex). Neighboring pairs nitude of LTP was calculated on basis of the averaged EPSC values during
of pyramidal cells were recorded simultaneously in CA1 by single stim- the last 5 min of post-HFS recordings (from minute 25 to minute 30).
ulating bipolar tungsten electrode (FHC) placed on the Schaffer collat- LTP magnitude was expressed as the percentage change in the mean
eral fibers, as described previously (Barone et al., 2019). Slices were EPSC peak amplitude normalized to baseline values, taken as 100% (i.e.,
incubated in artificial CSF (ACSF) containing the following (in mM): 119 mean values for the 5 min of recording before HFS).
NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 4 CaCl2, 4 MgCl2, 1 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 11 D-glucose,
and 0.005 2-chloroadenosine, gassed with 95% O2/5% CO2. Whole-cell Electrophysiological field recordings
recording pipettes (3– 4 M⍀) were filled with a solution containing the Extracellular electrophysiological field recordings were performed on
following (in mM): 135 CsMeSO3, 8 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 0.25 EGTA, 2 400 m transverse hippocampal slices as described previously (Puzzo et
Mg2ATP, 0.3 Na3GTP, 0.1 spermine, 7 phosphocreatine, and 5 QX-314, al., 2008). After a cutting procedure using a manual tissue chopper, slices
pH 7.25–7.30 (osmolarity 300 mOsm). Data were collected with a Mul- were transferred to a recording chamber and perfused (1–2 ml/min) with
tiClamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices), digitized at 10 kHz using ACSF containing the following (in mM): 124 NaCl, 4.4 KCl, 1 Na2HPO4,
the Digidata 1440A data acquisition system (Molecular Devices), and 25 NaHCO3, 2 CaCl2, 2 MgCl2, 10 glucose kept at 29°C and continuously
analyzed offline using pClamp 10 software (RRID:SCR_011323; Molec- bubbled with an O2/CO2 mixture at 95% and 5%. After 120 min recov-
ular Devices). ery, field EPSPs (fEPSPs) were recorded in CA1 stratum radiatum by a
Experimental design. The AMPA/NMDA ratio was calculated as the glass electrode filled with ACSF in response to Schaffer collateral stimu-
peak averaged AMPAR-mediated EPSC at holding potential of ⫺70 mV lation by a bipolar tungsten electrode.
divided by the averaged NMDAR-mediated EPSC at holding potential of Experimental design. Recordings were performed and analyzed in
⫹40 mV at a latency at which AMPAR-mediated EPSC responses were pClamp 10. We first measured basal synaptic transmission (BST) by
fully decayed (50 ms after stimulation). Miniature EPSC (mEPSC) am- stimulating with a series of increasing voltage pulses (from 5 to 35 V).
plitude and frequency were evaluated in 60 s recordings in the presence of This allowed us to preliminarily select healthy slices to be used for elec-
tetrodotoxin (0.5 M) at ⫺70 mV. trophysiological recordings. For LTP, baseline was recorded every min-
We used two different experimental settings to evaluate whether ute by stimulating at a voltage able to evoke a response of 35% of the
oA42 exerted an extracellular and/or an intracellular effect. To study the maximum evoked response in BST. After 30 – 45 min, slices with a stable
extracellular effect, slices were perfused with extracellular 200 pM oA42 baseline (slope variation ⫾ 5%) were used. We recorded for 15 min, and
with one patch pipette filled with vehicle and the other with the 6E10 then perfused with vehicle or drugs for the appropriate time. LTP was
antibody that blocks the possible effect due to intracellular oA42. To induced by a theta-burst stimulation (TBS): trains of 10 ⫻ 100 Hz bursts
study the intracellular effect, slices were perfused with extracellular vehi- with five pulses per burst with a 200 ms interburst interval at the test pulse
cle with one patch pipette filled with vehicle and the other with 200 pM intensity. To elicit early LTP (E-LTP), we delivered a single TBS train
oA42. This design also allowed us to concurrently compare the effects of (weak tetanic stimulation), whereas for late LTP (L-LTP, we delivered
vehicle and 200 pM oA42 injected into adjacent neurons through patch three TBS trains with a 15 s intertrain interval (strong tetanic stimula-
pipettes. tion). Analysis of the fEPSP slope was performed offline and results were
To confirm the ability of 6E10 to block oA42 action, slices were per- expressed by normalizing on the first 15 min of baseline recordings. In
fused with extracellular 200 pM oA42 paired with 6E10, 6E10 alone, or another series of experiments, we evaluated PPF. After BST assessment,
200 pM oA42 alone; the patch pipette was filled with vehicle. slices were perfused with the NMDA receptor antagonist (2R)-amino-5-
phosphonovaleric acid (APV; 50 M) for 45 min, and then treated with
Whole-cell LTP recordings in hippocampal brain slices vehicle or drugs in APV for the appropriate time. Two paired pulses
Experiments examining LTP were performed from single CA1 pyramidal within a time interval of 10, 20, 30, 40, 50, 100, 200, 500, and 1000 ms
cells after stimulating the Schaffer collateral fibers by means of a bipolar were delivered. PPF was measured as percentage of the synaptic response
tungsten electrode (FHC) in acute hippocampal brain slices (300 m of the second against the first delivered stimulus.
thick) obtained by 21-d-old male C57BL/6 mice, as described previously
(Ripoli et al., 2013, 2014). Animals were anesthetized with isoflurane, Intrahippocampal injections of oA42
decapitated, and the brains were rapidly placed in ice-cold cutting solu- Mice underwent stereotaxic surgery for cannulas implantation. After
tion containing the following (in mM): 124 NaCl, 3.2 KCl, 1 NaH2PO4, 2 anesthesia with tiletamine ⫹ zolazepam (60 mg/kg) and medetomidine
MgCl2, 1 CaCl2, 26 NaHCO3, 10 glucose, 2 Na-pyruvate, and 0.6 ascorbic (40 g/kg), mice were implanted with a 26-gauge guide cannula into the
acid, pH 7.4, 95% O2/5% CO2. Slices were sectioned on a vibratome dorsal part of the hippocampi (coordinates from bregma: posterior ⫽
(VT1200S; Leica Microsystems) and rapidly transferred to an incubation 2.46 mm, lateral ⫽ 1.50 mm to a depth of 1.30 mm). The cannulas were
chamber filled with ACSF containing the following (in mM): 124 NaCl, fixed to the skull with acrylic dental cement (RelyX TM Unicem, 3M) and
3.2 KCl, 1 NaH2PO4, 1 MgCl2, 2 CaCl2, 26 NaHCO3, and 10 glucose, pH mice were allowed to recover for a minimum of 6 – 8 d. Twenty minutes
7.4, gassed with 95% O2/5% CO2. Slices were allowed to recover at 32°C before the training phase (T1), mice were bilaterally infused with oA42
Gulisano et al. • Physiological Role of A Oligomers at the Synapse J. Neurosci., July 24, 2019 • 39(30):5986 – 6000 • 5989
solution or vehicle or oA42 ⫹ Rp-8-Br-cGMPS in a final volume of 1 l vesicle pool, number of docked vesicles, area of spines, length of the
over 1 min with a microsyringe connected to the cannulas via polyethyl- postsynaptic density (PSD), and of the proportion of perforated synapses
ene tubing. During infusion, animals were handled gently to minimize (Shepherd and Harris, 1998; Pozzo-Miller et al., 1999; Geinisman, 2000;
stress. After infusion, the needle was left in place for another minute to Bourne et al., 2013; Babits et al., 2016) was performed. Randomly se-
allow diffusion. In some animals, after behavioral studies, a solution of lected electron microscopical fields of the stratum radiatum (vehicle, n ⫽
4% methylene blue was infused for localization of infusion cannulas. 87; vehicle ⫹ 1 TBS, n ⫽ 116; oA42 200 pM, n ⫽ 92; oA42 200 pM ⫹ 1
TBS, n ⫽ 113) with at least one identifiable axospinous synapse (Shep-
Novel object recognition (NOR) test herd and Harris, 1998) were acquired at original magnification of
The NOR test was performed as described previously (Bollen et al., 2014; 36,000⫻.
Palmeri et al., 2017) in sex-balanced WT mice. Mice underwent 3 d of Axospinous synapses were identified by the presence of a presynaptic
habituation to the arena, objects, and intrahippocampal injections; 1 d of terminal with vesicles, including those nearby the presynaptic density
training (T1); and 1 d of testing (T2). The arena was a plastic white box (i.e., the active zone), a synaptic cleft displaying electrodense material,
(50 ⫻ 35 ⫻ 45 cm), and objects (e.g., pyramid, cube, truncated sphere, and a postsynaptic membrane associated with a prominent PSD within
etc.) were designed by a computer-aided design software (Solidworks) the postsynaptic spines (Peters et al., 1991; DeFelipe et al., 1999). The
and printed in polylactic acid with a Prusa-inspired 3D printer of our distinction between vesicle pools (which comprise the reserve vesicle
design. After each trial, the box and the objects were cleaned with 70% pool) and docked vesicles (which are thought to be part of the readily
ethanol and dried with absorbent paper. releasable pool), was made according to Shepherd and Harris 1998;
Experimental design. During the first day of habituation the mouse was Pozzo-Miller et al., 1999; Bourne et al., 2013. Briefly, vesicle pools were
put into the empty arena and allowed to explore it for 5 min. During the determined by counting the total number of small vesicles (⬃50 nm) per
second and the third day (familiarization with objects), the mouse was terminal; the docked vesicles pool by counting the vesicles touching the
put into the arena containing two different objects, randomly chosen membrane of the presynaptic active zone. Spines profile area and PSD
among our object collection and changed from day to day, for 5 min. length were measured by ImageJ software tools (Schneider et al., 2012;
During the fourth day, NOR training session (T1) was performed with Babits et al., 2016). Spine profiles and PSD were traced along the mem-
two different protocols: short T1 and long T1. The mouse was put into branes and measured; PSD length corresponded to the distance between
the arena and allowed to explore two identical objects placed in the the edges of PSD. Perforated synapses were identified based on the pres-
central part of the box, equally distant from the perimeter and the center, ence of a discontinuous PSD (Geinisman, 2000; Babits et al., 2016). Ul-
for 3 min (short T1) or 10 min (long T1). During the fifth day, the mouse trathin sections and microscopical features of axospinous synapses were
underwent the second trial (T2) to test memory retention for 10 min. The examined and analyzed in a blinded manner.
long delay interval of 24 h between T1 and T2 did not allow storage of
memory information (natural forgetting) in mice that were previously Western blot on hippocampal slices
exposed to a short T1 (no discrimination between the familiar and the Western blot (WB) analysis was performed as described previously
novel objects). Conversely, long-term memory (LTM) could be formed (Caraci et al., 2015). Tissues were homogenized in RIPA buffer (Thermo
in mice that were previously exposed to a long T1. In T2, mice were Fisher Scientific) in the presence of phosphatase and protease inhibitors
presented with two different objects, respectively a “familiar” (i.e., the (Thermo Fisher Scientific), and sonicated 3 times for 10 min. Protein
one used for T1) and a “novel” object. Animal exploration was defined as concentrations were determined by Pierce BCA protein assay kit
the mouse pointing its nose toward the object from a distance not ⬎2 cm (Thermo Fisher Scientific) and equal amounts of proteins (30 –50 g)
and was measured in T2 to analyze: (1) the discrimination index, explo- were then loaded onto 10% or 12% Tris-glycine polyacrylamide gels for
ration of novel object minus exploration of familiar object/total explo- electrophoretic separation. Membranes were blocked for 1 h, at room
ration time, and (2) total exploration time, the time spent exploring the temperature, in a solution of 5% nonfat dry milk in Tris-buffered saline
objects was scored using a personal computer by an experimenter who containing 0.1% Tween 20 or SEAblock (Thermo Fisher Scientific) be-
was blinded to the conditions tested. We excluded from the analyses mice fore incubation overnight at 4°C with the following primary antibodies:
with a total exploration time ⬍5 s. mouse anti-neuronal nitric oxide synthase (anti-nNOS) (Thermo Fisher
Scientific, catalog #37-2800, RRID:AB_2533308; 1:1000); rabbit anti-p-
Electron microscopy of hippocampal slices CREB (ser133) (Millipore, catalog #06-519, RRID:AB_310153; 1:1000);
Hippocampal slices (n ⫽ 2 for each conditions from 8 animals) were mouse anti-CREB (Cell Signaling Technology, catalog #9104, RRID:
quickly immersed (within ⬃30 s after 120 min of electrophysiological AB_490881; 1:500); rabbit anti-p-CaMKII (thr286) (Cell Signaling
recording) in a solution containing 4% paraformaldehyde and 0.5% glu- Technology, catalog #12716, RRID:AB_2713889; 1:1000); mouse anti-
taraldehyde in phosphate buffer (PB) and then stored for 6 weeks in the CaMKII␣ (Cell Signaling Technology, catalog #50049, RRID:
same fixative solution at 4°C. Subsequently, slices were exposed to an AB_2721906; 1:1000); rabbit anti-BDNF (Millipore, catalog #AB1534,
embedding procedure as described previously (Melone et al., 2011, RRID:AB_90746; 1:500). Mouse anti tubulin (Sigma Aldrich, catalog
2015). Briefly, they were postfixed in 1% osmium tetroxide in PB for 45 #T5293, RRID:AB_477580; 1:1000) was used as loading control. Molec-
min and contrasted with 1% uranyl acetate in maleate buffer, pH 6.0, for ular weights for immunoblot analysis were determined using precision
1 h. Dehydrated sections were immersed in propylene oxide, infiltrated Plus Protein Dual color Standards (Bio-Rad), PAGE-MASTER Protein
with a mixture of Epon/Spurr resins (Electron Microscopy Sciences) Standard (Genscript). Protein detection was performed by using a sec-
sandwiched between Aclar films and polymerized at 60°C for 48 h. A ondary infrared fluorescent dye conjugated antibody absorbing at 800 or
small block of tissue containing CA1 stratum radiatum was selected by 680 nm. The secondary antibody goat anti-rabbit IRDye 680 (Li-Cor
light microscopic inspection, glued to blank epoxy, and sectioned with an Biosciences, catalog #926-68021, RRID:AB_10706309) and goat anti-
ultramicrotome (MTX; Research and Manufacturing Company). Ultra- mouse IRDye 800CW (Li-Cor Biosciences, catalog #926-32210, RRID:
thin sections (⬃60 nm; for a total of 20 –25 ultrathin sections for each AB_621842) were used at 1:20,000 and 1:30,000, respectively. Blots were
small selected block) were mounted on 200 mesh copper grids, stained visualized using an Odyssey Infrared Imaging Scanner (Li-Cor). In other
with Sato’s lead, and examined with Philips EM 208 and/or CM10 elec- experiments, after incubation with appropriate secondary horseradish
tron microscopes coupled to a MegaView-II high resolution CCD cam- peroxidase-conjugated antibodies (1:2500; Cell Signaling Technology),
era (Soft Imaging System). visualization was performed with ECL plus (GE Healthcare) using
Experimental design. Electron microscopy was performed on hip- UVItec (Cambrige Alliance). Densitometric analysis was performed with
pocampal slices treated with vehicle, vehicle ⫹ 1 TBS, oA42 200 pM, or either Odyssey Infrared Imaging Scanner or UVItec or ImageJ software
A 200 pM ⫹ 1 TBS. These slices were randomly collected during elec- after normalization with loading controls.
trophysiological field recording experiments 120 min after tetanic stim- Experimental design. We collected slices from electrophysiological field
ulation and analysis of recordings was performed for each sample recordings experiments 120 min after tetanic stimulation. For each ex-
following the sample protocol described above. Quantitative analysis of perimental condition (vehicle, 1 TBS, A, A ⫹ 1 TBS; or vehicle, 3 TBS
5990 • J. Neurosci., July 24, 2019 • 39(30):5986 – 6000 Gulisano et al. • Physiological Role of A Oligomers at the Synapse
for nNOS), 4 slices from different animals were collected to obtain a confirmed that blocking oA42 with extracellular 6E10 prevented
sample; 2–3 samples for a total of 8 –12 slices were used to evaluate the oA42 to increase mEPSC frequency (Fig. 1H ), whereas 6E10 did
expression of each protein of interest. not affect mEPSC per se.
Statistical analyses Further experiments in which LTP was studied through
For each experiment, sample size relied on power analyses (␣ ⫽ 0.05, patch-clamp technique showed that 200 pM oA42 increased
power 1- ⫽ 0.80) calculated by G-Power 3.1 software suggesting for EPSC amplitude recorded for 30 min after a high-frequency stim-
each condition a minimum of 6 slices (electrophysiology) and 8 mice ulation (2 trains at 100 Hz for 1 s separated by 20 s) only when
(behavioral studies) to obtain an effect size ⫽ 0.62. Experimenters were extracellularly applied, not when injected into CA1 pyramidal
blinded to treatment. All data are expressed as mean ⫾ SEM. neurons through the patch pipette (Fig. 1I ). These findings dem-
After data collection, statistical analysis was performed by SigmaPlot onstrate that picomolar concentrations of oA42 extracellularly
12.0 (RRID:SCR_003210), Systat 9 (RRID:SCR_010455) and GraphPad
modulate glutamatergic transmission and plasticity and it they
Prism 7 (RRID:SCR_002798) software. A preliminary analysis of normal
distribution was performed by Shapiro–Wilk normality test. We also
do act through a direct intracellular mechanism.
used the following tests: (1) ANOVA for repeated measures to analyze
PPF and LTP (120 min after tetanus) curves; (2) one-way ANOVA with Picomolar concentrations of oA42 affect PPF and convert
Bonferroni’s or Tukey’s post hoc correction for LTP graphs displaying
E-LTP into L-LTP and short-term memory (STM) into LTM
residual potentiation (average of the last 5 min of LTP recording) for
analyses of discrimination index and total exploration time among con- The increase of mEPSC frequency prompted us to investigate
ditions in NOR and for WB; (3) two-samples t test to compare conditions whether oA42 affects PPF, a presynaptic form of short-term
in dual-patch recording; and (4) one-sample t test to compare discrimi- plasticity linked with changes in release probability (Zucker and
nation index with zero. Given the non-normal distribution of electron Regehr, 2002). We found that 20 min perfusion with 200 pM
microscopical data, as assessed by D’Agostino and Pearson normality oA42 decreased PPF (Fig. 2A), thus suggesting an increase of the
test, comparison of the number of pool vesicles, docked vesicles, area of release probability (Dobrunz and Stevens, 1997). A DR curve
spines, and PSD length between all groups was made by nonparametric confirmed that 200 pM was the dose capable of affecting PPF (Fig.
Kruskal–Wallis test with Dunn’s multiple-comparisons test. The per- 2B). To further investigate the role of A in PPF, we suppressed
centages of nonperforated and perforated synapses were compared using
endogenous A through the anti-A antibody M3.2, specifically
nonparametric contingency analysis with Fisher’s test. The level of sig-
nificance was set at p ⬍ 0.05.
targeting murine A. This induced the opposite effect, an en-
hancement of PPF, which was rescued by concomitant perfusion
Results with human 200 pM oA42 (Fig. 2C), not recognized by M3.2.
Picomolar concentrations of extracellular oA42 affect These findings are consistent with a positive modulatory role of
spontaneous neurotransmitter release and synaptic plasticity A onto neurotransmitter release probability.
Intraneuronal uptake and accumulation of high doses of oA42 Next, we investigated whether 200 pM oA42 influenced the
are key events leading to impairment of synaptic transmission, E-LTP, a form of protein-synthesis-independent plasticity that
LTP, and memory (Ripoli et al., 2014; Puzzo et al., 2017). How- involves a change in presynaptic neurotransmitter release and
ever, it is not known whether the capability of oA42 to enhance short-term kinase activity (Huang, 1998). We found that pre-
synaptic function when at low concentrations is triggered by an treatment with 200 pM oA42 converted E-LTP obtained through
extracellular mechanism or if it requires protein internalization. a weak tetanic stimulation (1 TBS) into L-LTP (Fig. 2D). The
To address this issue, we first characterized our oA42 prepa- potentiation induced by pairing 200 pM oA42 with 1 TBS was
rations and confirmed that they contained both monomers and comparable to that induced by a strong tetanic stimulation con-
oligomers (Fig. 1A), as recently shown (Gulisano et al., 2018b). sisting of 3 TBSs elicited with a 15 s intertrain interval. To confirm
DR curves obtained by treating hippocampal slices with different that 200 pM oA42 converted E-LTP into the traditional protein-
doses of oA42 (from 2 pM to 200 nM) for 20 min before tetanus synthesis-dependent L-LTP (Johnstone and Raymond, 2011),
confirmed that 200 pM oA42 was the concentration inducing the slices were continuously perfused with the translation inhibitor
maximum enhancement of LTP (Fig. 1B), consistent with our ANI (20 M; 30 min before and 25 after tetanus), which pre-
previous findings (Puzzo et al., 2008, 2012). vented the oA42-induced L-LTP (Fig. 2E). Additionally, we con-
Next, we studied the effect of extracellular and intracellular firmed that ANI perfusion inhibits 3-TBS-induced L-LTP
200 pM oA42 on glutamatergic basal synaptic transmission without affecting 1-TBS-induced E-LTP (Fig. 2E), as the latter is
through dual patch-clamp whole-cell recordings of adjacent CA1 known to not involve new protein synthesis (Johnstone and Ray-
pyramidal neurons of organotypic slice cultures in the following mond, 2011). These observations support a role for oA42 in
experimental conditions: (1) extracellular 200 pM oA42, with triggering new protein synthesis and gene transcription.
one patch pipette filled with vehicle and the other with an anti- Because LTP represents the cellular surrogate of memory, we
body raised against human A42 (6E10) and (2) extracellular assessed whether bilateral intrahippocampal injections with 200
vehicle with one patch pipette filled with vehicle and the other pM oA42 20 min before training were able to convert STM into
with 200 pM oA42 (Fig. 1C). We found no effect of oA42 onto LTM. To this end, we used an NOR protocol in which the short
AMPA and NMDA glutamatergic receptor evoked currents when time exposure (3 min) during the training phase (T1) does not
the peptide was administered either extracellularly or intracellu- allow animals to discriminate between the old and the new object
larly (Fig. 1 D, E). Conversely, extracellular administration of after a 24 h long-term interval due to natural forgetting (Bollen et al.,
oA42 increased the spontaneous release of neurotransmitter 2014; Palmeri et al., 2017). The analyses of the discrimination
from the presynaptic terminal measured through the mEPSC index indicated that mice receiving a weak stimulus (i.e., short
frequency without altering their amplitude 20 min after its appli- exposure in T1) paired with 200 pM oA42 were able to discrim-
cation (Fig. 1 F, G). Remarkably, intracellular application of 6E10 inate between the old and the familiar object after 24 h compared
did not block the effect of extracellular oA42, confirming that with vehicle-treated animals showing natural forgetting. This
the oA42-induced modification of glutamatergic transmission oA42-induced LTM was comparable to that induced by a stron-
does not require peptide internalization. Control experiments ger training stimulus (10 min of exposure in T1) (Fig. 2F ). Total
Gulisano et al. • Physiological Role of A Oligomers at the Synapse J. Neurosci., July 24, 2019 • 39(30):5986 – 6000 • 5991
Figure 1. Extracellular, but not intracellular, picomolar concentrations of oA42 affect spontaneous release of neurotransmitter and synaptic plasticity. A, Characterization of synthetic human
oA42 solutions by WB analysis showing the presence of monomers, dimers, trimers, and tetramers for 200 nM oA42 solution and the presence of monomers and tetramers for 200 pM oA42. B,
DR curve for the effect of oA42 (from 2 pM to 200 nM, n ⫽ 5 for each concentration) on CA1-LTP indicates that the peptide has a maximum stimulatory effect at 200 pM (321.04 ⫾ 17.92% of baseline
vs 228.63 ⫾ 20.29% of baseline; F(6,28) ⫽ 9.882, p ⬍ 0.0001; Bonferroni’s p ⫽ 0.031) and an inhibitory effect at 200 nM (133.25 ⫾ 11.59% of baseline; Bonferroni’s p ⫽ 0.023). The dotted
horizontal line corresponds to treatment with vehicle. The residual potentiation was calculated by averaging the last 5 min of LTP. C, Schematic representation of dual whole-cell recordings from
adjacent (#1 and #2) CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons (see Materials and Methods for details). D, Representative AMPAR- and NMDAR-EPSCs at time 0 (T0) and 20 min after extracellular
application of vehicle or 200 pM oA42 (T20). The amplitude of NMDA currents recorded at ⫹40 mV (holding potential) was measured at 50 ms poststimulus (red crosses). E, Neither intraneuronal
(in) nor extracellular (ex) 200 pM oA42 affected AMPAR/NMDAR ratios. Slices perfused with exVehicle: inVehicle T0 ⫽ 0.74 ⫾ 0.16, T20 ⫽ 0.97 ⫾ 0.28, n ⫽ 10; in 200 pM oA42 T0 ⫽ 0.92 ⫾
0.14, T20 ⫽ 0.90 ⫾ 0.16, n ⫽ 10. Slices perfused with ex 200 pM oA42: inVehicle T0 ⫽ 0.80 ⫾ 0.19, T20 ⫽ 0.86 ⫾ 0.16, n ⫽ 15; in6E10 T0 ⫽ 0.92 ⫾ 0.20, T20 ⫽ 1.04 ⫾ 0.21, n ⫽ 15. F,
Representative mEPSC traces recorded in neurons treated with vehicle and 200 pM inA42 at T0 and T20. Intraneuronal 200 pM oA42 had no significant effect on mEPSC frequency (from 0.82 ⫾ 0.18
Hz at T0 to 0.63 ⫾ 0.10 Hz at T20, n ⫽ 14; p ⫽ 0.10) or amplitude (from 11.33 ⫾ 2.58 pA at T0 to 8.64 ⫾ 2.06 pA at T20, n ⫽ 14; p ⫽ 0.07) compared with slices in which cells were injected with
vehicle and extracellularly perfused with vehicle (mEPSC frequencies from 0.87 ⫾ 0.13 Hz at T0 to 0.78 ⫾ 0.10 Hz at T20, n ⫽ 14; p ⫽ 0.07; mEPSC amplitudes from 13.58 ⫾ 2.77 pA at T0 to
10.73 ⫾ 2.97 pA at T20, n ⫽ 14; p ⫽ 0.19). G, Extracellular application of 200 pM oA42 significantly increased mEPSC frequency both in vehicle-injected neurons (from 0.79 ⫾ 0.08 Hz at T0 to
1.10 ⫾ 0.10 Hz at T20, n ⫽ 14; p ⫽ 0.002) and adjacent 6E10-injected neurons (from 0.85 ⫾ 0.11 Hz at T0 to 1.18 ⫾ 0.15 Hz at T20, n ⫽ 14; p ⫽ 0.0008) without altering the mEPSC amplitude
(mean amplitude) of: (1) vehicle: T0 ⫽ 10.6 ⫾ 1.9 pA, T20 ⫽ 12.2 ⫾ 1.6 pA, n ⫽ 14; p ⫽ 0.20 or (2i) 6E10: T0 ⫽ 9.7 ⫾ 1.4 pA, T20 ⫽ 11.2 ⫾ 2.7 pA, n ⫽ 14; p ⫽ 0.51. H, Extracellular application
of 200 pM oA42 paired with 6E10 or 6E10 alone did not modify mEPSC frequency in vehicle-injected neurons (exA⫹6E10: from 0.96 ⫾ 0.08 Hz at T0 to 1.00 ⫾ 0.15 Hz at T20, n ⫽ 18; p ⫽ 0.746;
6E10: from 0.83 ⫾ 0.07 Hz at T0 to 0.77 ⫾ 0.10 Hz at T20, n ⫽ 10; p ⫽ 0.543). In interleaved experiments, extracellular 200 pM oA42 was still capable of incresing the mEPSC frequency (from
1.00 ⫾ 0.10 Hz at T0 to 1.31 ⫾ 0.15 Hz at T20, n ⫽ 8; p ⫽ 0.029). I, Extracellular oA42 (n ⫽ 11) enhanced LTP elicited through a high-frequency stimulation (two trains at 100 Hz for 1 s separated
by 20 s) compared with vehicle (n ⫽ 10) (Bonferroni’s p ⫽ 0.003). By contrast, intracellular oA42 did not modify potentiation (Bonferroni’s p ⬎ 0.05). ANOVA among all: F(2,28) ⫽ 10.846; p ⫽
0.001. **p ⬍ 0.005; ***p ⬍ 0.001. Data are expressed as mean ⫾ SEM.
exploration time was similar in all groups (Fig. 2F ). Thus, 200 pM cific ultrastructural changes (i.e., vesicle pool, number of docked
oA42 converts STM into LTM. vesicles, area of spines, PSD length, and percentage of perforated
synapses; Table 1) at axospinous synapses of the CA1 stratum
Picomolar concentrations of oA42 induce ultrastructural radiatum in slices used for electrophysiological experiments col-
changes of hippocampal CA1 synapses at both presynaptic lected and stored at 120 min after A treatment. Quantitative
and postsynaptic levels electron microscopy showed an increase of docked vesicles in
Synaptic plasticity is accompanied by structural changes occur- axon terminals from slices treated with 200 pM oA42 and of PSD
ring at both the presynaptic and postsynaptic levels (Bourne et al., length in spines from slices treated with oA42 paired with a weak
2013). We therefore investigated whether oA42 determines spe- stimulation (Fig. 3). Together, these ultrastructural changes
5992 • J. Neurosci., July 24, 2019 • 39(30):5986 – 6000 Gulisano et al. • Physiological Role of A Oligomers at the Synapse
Figure 2. Picomolar concentrations of oA42 decrease PPF and convert E-LTP into L-LTP, as well as STM into LTM. A, PPF was decreased in slices perfused with 200 pM oA42 for 20 min
compared with control slices (facilitation at 100 ms interval here and in following panels: vehicle ⫽ 179.58 ⫾ 7.47%, n ⫽ 9; 200 pM oA42 ⫽ 145.67 ⫾ 8.74%, n ⫽ 10; ANOVA for
repeated measures for the entire curve F(1,17) ⫽ 6.262, p ⫽ 0.023; Bonferroni’s p ⫽ 0.041 at 100 ms. B, DR curve for the effect of oA42 (from 2 pM to 200 nM, n ⫽ 7 for each
concentration) on PPF indicates that the peptide has a maximum stimulatory effect at 200 pM. Curve showing the percentage of facilitation at 100 ms stimulus interval (Bonferroni’s p ⫽
0.022 between vehicle and 200 pM oA42). The dotted horizontal line corresponds to treatment with vehicle. C, Increase of PPF caused by the anti-murine A antibody M3.2 mAb
(193.75 ⫾ 8.43%, n ⫽ 11; F(1,18) ⫽ 4.749, p ⫽ 0.043 vs vehicle; Bonferroni’s p ⫽ 0.007, 0.011 and 0.006 at 20, 30 and 40 ms) was rescued by 200 pM human oA42 (175.39 ⫾ 17.22%,
n ⫽ 10; F(1,19) ⫽ 5.111, p ⫽ 0.036 vs M3.2 mAb; F(1,17) ⫽ 0.006, p ⫽ 0.939 vs vehicle). D, Twenty-minute perfusion with 200 pM oA42 converted E-LTP elicited through a weak tetanic
stimulation (1 TBS) to L-LTP (134.24 ⫾ 5.44% of baseline vs 220.10 ⫾ 17.74% of baseline, n ⫽ 7/7; F(1,12) ⫽ 9.883, p ⫽ 0.008), inducing a potentiation similar to that obtained with
a strong tetanic stimulation (3 TBS) (219.91 ⫾ 7.75% of baseline, n ⫽ 7; F(1,12) ⫽ 0.321, p ⫽ 0.582 comparing oA42 ⫹ 1 TBS vs vehicle ⫹ 3 TBS). E, Perfusion (30 min before and 25
min after tetanus) with the translation inhibitor ANI blocked the oA42-induced L-LTP (127.48 ⫾ 8.71% of baseline, n ⫽ 7; F(1,12) ⫽ 11.946, p ⫽ 0.005 vs oA42 ⫹ 1 TBS). ANI did not
modify E-LTP induced by 1 TBS (137.92 ⫾ 8.05% of baseline, n ⫽ 7; F(1,11) ⫽ 3.091; p ⫽ 0.106 vs vehicle ⫹ 1 TBS) but blocked L-LTP induced by 3 TBS (146.41 ⫾ 14.77% of baseline,
n ⫽ 6; F(1,12) ⫽ 25.976, p ⬍ 0.0001 vs vehicle ⫹ 3 TBS). Shaded area with dashed line corresponds to mean ⫹ SEM of the 5 last recorded point in slices treated with vehicle ⫹ 3 TBS
as in D. F, Evaluation of recognition memory indicated a difference in discrimination index (D; the exploration of novel object minus exploration of familiar object/total exploration time)
between vehicle-treated mice that underwent a 3 min exposition in T1 (short T1) or a 10 min exposition in T1 (long T1) (0.04 ⫾ 0.02 vs 0.31 ⫾ 0.03 comparing short T1 vs long T1, n ⫽
10/10 sex-balanced mice; Bonferroni’s p ⬍ 0.0001). Comparison of D with zero confirmed that a long T1 was able to induce LTM in a 24-h-delay novel object recognition task (t(9) ⫽
9.604, p ⬍ 0.0001), whereas a short T1 did not (t(9) ⫽ 1.62, p ⫽ 0.140). Intrahippocampal injections with 200 pM oA42 20 min before a short T1 converted STM into LTM (D ⫽ 0.38 ⫾
0.01, n ⫽ 10 sex-balanced mice; t(9) ⫽ 19.79, p ⬍ 0.0001 comparing D with zero in oA42 ⫹ Short T1). One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc corrections confirmed the significant
difference between D in vehicle- and oA42-treated mice previously exposed to a short T1 ( p ⬍ 0.0001). The increase of D induced by treatment with oA42 was similar to that obtained
in vehicle-treated mice that spent a longer period in T1 ( p ⫽ 0.314 comparing D in oA42 ⫹ short T1 vs vehicle ⫹ long T1). Right, Total exploration time was comparable in the 3 groups
of mice (F(2,27) ⫽ 0.086, p ⫽ 0.918). *p ⬍ 0.05, **p ⬍ 0.01, ****p ⬍ 0.0001; # difference from 0. Data are expressed as mean ⫾ SEM.
Table 1. Detailed results of TEM performed on hippocampal slices stored at 120 min after electrophysiological recordings in the following experimental conditions: vehicle
(V), vehicle ⴙ 1 TBS tetanus (V ⴙ T), oA42 200 pM (A), oA42 200 pM ⴙ 1 TBS tetanus (A ⴙ T)
Experimental
condition V V⫹T A A⫹T Statistical analysesa ( p value)
Asymmetric
synapses (n) n ⫽ 172 n ⫽ 151 n ⫽ 124 n ⫽ 113 V vs V⫹T V vs A V vs A⫹T V⫹T vs A A vs A⫹T V⫹T vs A⫹T
Vesicle pool (n) 74.8 ⫾ 2.4 70.2 ⫾ 2.9 72.9 ⫾ 3.1 77.3 ⫾ 3.0 0.48 ⬎0.99 ⬎0.99 ⬎0.99 ⬎0.99 0.73
Docked vesicles (n) 1.7 ⫾ 0.08 3.7 ⫾ 0.12 2.6 ⫾ 0.10 4.2 ⫾ 0.14 ⬍0.0001 ⬍0.0001 ⬍0.0001 ⬍0.0001 ⬍0.0001 0.13
Area of spines (m 2) 0.09 ⫾ 0.004 0.11 ⫾ 0.006 0.10 ⫾ 0.005 0.10 ⫾ 0.006 0.08 0.055 ⬎0.99 ⬎0.99 0.32 0.51
PSD length (m) 0.212 ⫾ 0.005 0.214 ⫾ 0.005 0.207 ⫾ 0.006 0.249 ⫾ 0.008 ⬎0.99 ⬎0.99 0.0037 ⬎0.99 0.0041 0.0398
Perforated PSD 4.07% 11.26% 5.65% 14.20% 0.018b 0.58b 0.0013b 0.132b 0.021b 0.508b
a
Statistical analyses performed by nonparametric Kruskal–Wallis test with Dunn’s test for multiple comparison. Number of microscopical fields: V (n ⫽ 87); V⫹T (n ⫽ 116); A (n ⫽ 92); A ⫹ T (n ⫽ 113). For vesicle pool, docked vesicles,
area of spines, and PSD length values are shown as mean ⫾ SEM. For perforated PSD, % indicates the proportion of asymmetric synapses with perforated PSD.
b
Statistical analysis performed by contingency analysis with Fisher’s test.
Gulisano et al. • Physiological Role of A Oligomers at the Synapse J. Neurosci., July 24, 2019 • 39(30):5986 – 6000 • 5993
Figure 3. Picomolar concentrations of oA42 increase the number of docked vesicles and PSD length. A, Representative asymmetric axospinous synapses of CA1 stratum radiatum from
hippocampal slices previously treated for electrophysiological recordings. Framed regions enlarged (bottom) show the synaptic contact for each synapse from different experimental conditions.
Arrows indicate docked vesicles (dv) at the active zone of axon terminals; arrowheads are the edges of PSD. AxT, Axon terminal; sp, spine. Scale bars: 500 nm for top, 100 nm for bottom. B, oA42
alone increased the number (n) of docked vesicles compared with vehicle ( p ⬍ 0.0001). The number of docked vesicles increased in tetanized slices treated with oA42 compared with nontetanized
slices treated with the peptide, as well as in tetanized slices treated with vehicle versus vehicle-treated nontetanized ones ( p ⬍ 0.0001). C, oA42 paired with a weak tetanic stimulation (1 TBS)
increased PSD length compared with other conditions. See Table 1 for detailed results and statistical analyses. V, Vehicle, A ⫽ 200 pM oA42, 1 TBS ⫽ weak tetanic stimulation. *p ⬍ 0.05; ***p ⬍
0.005; ****p ⬍ 0.0001. Data are expressed as mean ⫾ SEM.
robustly support the electrophysiological data. Most impor- molar concentrations of A, pM concentrations of the peptide
tantly, this body of evidence indicates that synapses undergo a did not require APP to produce their synaptic effects.
series of coordinated changes occurring both at the presynap- We then turned on ␣7nAChRs because of their interplay with
tic and postsynaptic site following application of oA42 at pM A in physiological conditions (Puzzo et al., 2008, 2011; Zappet-
concentrations. tini et al., 2012; Lawrence et al., 2014). When slices from
␣7nAChRs KO (␣7-KO) mice were treated with 200 pM oA42,
Effect of picomolar concentrations of oA42 on short-term the decrease of PPF and the conversion of E-LTP to L-LTP were
and long-term synaptic plasticity and memory requires ␣7 not elicited (Fig. 4C,D). To confirm that the effects of 200 pM
nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (␣7nAChRs) oA42 were mediated by ␣7nAChRs, we acutely blocked these
Because recent studies indicated that high nanomolar concentra- receptors by the selective antagonist MLA. This prevented 200 pM
tions of extracellular oA42 require APP to impair LTP and mem- oA42 from affecting PPF and LTP (Fig. 4 E, F ), suggesting that
ory (Puzzo et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2017), we tested whether the oA42 needs ␣7nAChRs to exert its enhancing effects.
plasticity-enhancing effect of extracellular picomolar concentra- To corroborate our findings indicating that ␣7nAChRs, but
tions of oA42 was APP dependent. We used APP-KO mice to not APP, were needed for 200 pM oA42 to enhance plasticity, we
determine whether the absence of endogenous APP prevented repeated experiments after the acute block of ␣7nAChR function
200 pM oA42 to exert its effects on PPF and LTP. We found that with MLA, which prevented the 200 pM oA42-mediated decrease
200 pM oA42 was still able to decrease PPF (Fig. 4A) and convert of PPF and conversion of E-LTP into L-LTP in slices from
E-LTP into L-LTP (Fig. 4B) in APP-KO mice. Thus, unlike nano- APP-KO mice (Fig. 4G,H ).
5994 • J. Neurosci., July 24, 2019 • 39(30):5986 – 6000 Gulisano et al. • Physiological Role of A Oligomers at the Synapse
4
mean ⫹ SEM of the last recorded point of 1 TBS-induced LTP in
WT slices treated with vehicle or 200 pM oA42 as shown in
Figure 2; the arrow indicates 1 TBS in this and the following
panels. C, 200 pM oA42 did not affect PPF in slices from ␣7-KO
mice (177.42 ⫾ 13.67% vs 178.03 ⫾ 8.52%, n ⫽ 8/8; F(1,16)
⫽ 0.388, p ⫽ 0.542). D, 200 pM oA42 did not convert E-LTP
into L-LTP in slices from ␣7-KO mice (131.12 ⫾ 3.46% of
baseline vs 124.43 ⫾ 11.09% of baseline, n ⫽ 7/7; F(1,12) ⫽
0.002, p ⫽ 0.964). E, 200 pM oA42 did not affect PPF in slices
from WT mice treated with the ␣7nAChR antagonist MLA (10
nM, 10 min before tetanus) (192.99 ⫾ 13.84% vs 188.02 ⫾
10.54%, n ⫽ 11/11; F(1,20) ⫽ 0.154, p ⫽ 0.699). F, 200 pM
oA42 did not convert E-LTP into L-LTP in slices from WT mice
treated with MLA (137.45 ⫾ 12.25% of baseline vs 128.57 ⫾
3.81% of baseline, n ⫽ 6/7; F(1,11) ⫽ 0.009, p ⫽ 0.927). G,
200 pM oA42 did not modify PPF in APP-KO mice treated with
MLA (189.90 ⫾ 6.86% vs 197.38 ⫾ 12.64%, n ⫽ 6/10; F(1,14)
⫽ 0.001, p ⫽ 0.973). H, 200 pM oA42 did not convert E-LTP
into L-LTP in slices from APP-KO mice treated with MLA
(130.70 ⫾ 9.68% of baseline vs 121.18 ⫾ 7.35% of baseline,
n ⫽ 6/7; F(1,11) ⫽ 0.589, p ⫽ 0.459). I, Intrahippocampal
injections with 200 pM oA42 20 min before a short T1 con-
verted STM into LTM in APP-KO mice (D ⫽ 0.06 ⫾ 0.04 vs
0.30 ⫾ 0.03; n ⫽ 10/10 sex-balanced mice; Bonferroni’s p ⫽
0.022 comparing APP-KO mice treated with vehicle or 200 pM
oA42; t(9) ⫽ 7.645, p ⬍ 0.0001 comparing D vs zero in APP
KO ⫹ 200 pM oA42). Conversely, 200 pM oA42 administra-
Figure 4. The ␣7-nAChR, but not APP, is needed for picomolar concentrations of oA42 to decrease PPF and convert both E-LTP tion did not induce LTM in ␣7-KO mice (D ⫽ ⫺0.07 ⫾ 0.07 vs
into L-LTP and STM into LTM. A, Twenty-minute perfusion with 200 pM oA42 was capable of decreasing PPF in slices from APP-KO 0.02 ⫾ 0.02; n ⫽ 10/10 sex-balanced mice; Bonferroni’s p ⫽
mice (% facilitation at 100 ms interval here and in following panels: APP KO ⫹ vehicle ⫽ 189.34 ⫾ 7.51%, n ⫽ 8; 200 pM oA42 1 comparing ␣7-KO mice treated with vehicle or 200 pM
⫽ 159.57 ⫾ 8.32%, n ⫽ 8; ANOVA for repeated measures F(1,14) ⫽ 5.234, p ⫽ 0.038; Bonferroni’s p ⫽ 0.013 at 50 ms and 0.044 oA42; t(9) ⫽ 0.999, p ⫽ 0.344 comparing D vs zero in ␣7 KO
at 100 ms). Gray shaded curves represent mean ⫹ SEM of PPF curves in WT slices treated with vehicle or 200 pM oA42 as shown ⫹ 200 pM oA42). J, Total exploration time was comparable in
in Figure 2. B, 200 pM oA42 converted E-LTP into L-LTP in slices from APP-KO mice (124.69 ⫾ 13.13% of baseline vs 210.28 ⫾ the 4 groups of mice (F(3,36) ⫽ 0.515, p ⫽ 0.674). *p ⬍ 0.05;
12.60% of baseline, n ⫽ 7/7; ANOVA for repeated measures F(1,12) ⫽ 14.77, p ⫽ 0.002). Gray shaded areas with lines represent # difference from 0. Data are expressed as mean ⫾ SEM.
Gulisano et al. • Physiological Role of A Oligomers at the Synapse J. Neurosci., July 24, 2019 • 39(30):5986 – 6000 • 5995
Figure 5. Conversion of E-LTP into L-LTP and STM into LTM by pM oA42 is mediated through the NO/cGMP/PKG cascade. A, 200 pM oA42 did not induce L-LTP in slices treated with the nNOS
inhibitor L-NAME (100 M for 40 min) (118.96 ⫾ 12.57% of baseline, n ⫽ 7; F(1,12) ⫽ 18.645, p ⫽ 0.001 vs 200 pM oA42 ⫹ 1 TBS), the sGC inhibitor ODQ (100 M for 40 min) (125.84 ⫾ 13.39%
of baseline, n ⫽ 6; F(1,11) ⫽ 8.903, p ⫽ 0.012), and the PKG inhibitor Rp-8-Br-cGMPS (10 M for 10 min) (140.48 ⫾ 19.61% of baseline, n ⫽ 7; F(1,12) ⫽ 9.161, p ⫽ 0.011). Shaded area with line
corresponds to mean ⫾ SEM of the last recorded point in WT slices treated with 200 pM oA42 ⫹ 1 TBS as in Figure 2. B, Treatment with L-NAME, ODQ, or Rp-8Br-cGMPS alone did not modify residual
potentiation induced by a weak tetanic stimulation compared with vehicle (n ⫽ 4 for each condition, F(1,12) ⫽ 0.315, p ⫽ 0.814). C, Blocking the NO/cGMP/PKG pathway inhibits L-LTP induced by
a strong tetanic stimulation compared with vehicle (n ⫽ 4 for each condition, F(3,14) ⫽ 13.391, p ⬍ 0.0001 among all; vehicle: 228.15 ⫾ 18.80% of baseline; L-NAME: 148.44 ⫾ 7.45% of baseline,
Bonferroni’s p ⫽ 0.006; ODQ: 131.24 ⫾ 9.22% of baseline, Bonferroni’s p ⫽ 0.001; Rp-8Br-cGMPS: 126.15 ⫾ 2.54% of baseline, Bonferroni’s p ⫽ 0.001). D, Top, Representative images of WB assay
(cropped images based on MW) performed on hippocampal slices (n ⫽ 4 for each lane) treated for electrophysiological experiments, collected, and stored at 120 min after treatment (V, vehicle; T,
1 TBS; A ⫽ oA42 200 pM). Bottom, Bar graph obtained by the average of two or three different membranes here in E and in G. -Tubulin expression is shown as one example of loading control.
An increase of nNOS expression (F(3,8) ⫽ 4.570; p ⫽ 0.038; Tukey’s p ⫽ 0.042) was detected in slices treated with A and a 1 TBS stimulation. E, nNOS expression increased in slices treated with
vehicle and 3TBS (F(1,2) ⫽ 52.412; p ⫽ 0.019). F, WB assay of PRPs. G, p-CREB expression was increased after treatment with A alone and further enhanced in A ⫹T (F(3,12) ⫽ 61.713; p ⬍
0.0001; Tukey’s p ⬍ 0.0001). H, No changes were detected in total CREB expression among different conditions (F(3,4) ⫽ 0.930; p ⫽ 0.504). I, J, A paired with 1 TBS increased p-CaMKII levels (F(3,8)
⫽ 11.746; p ⫽ 0.003) compared with V ( p ⫽ 0.017) or V⫹T ( p ⫽ 0.004) (I), whereas CaMKII expression was not modified (J; F(3,4) ⫽ 2.758; p ⫽ 0.176). K, A paired with 1 TBS increased BDNF
levels (F(3,8) ⫽ 5.686; p ⫽ 0.022) compared with V ( p ⫽ 0.034) and V⫹T ( p ⫽ 0.038). L, Evaluation of recognition memory indicated that 200 pM oA42 did not induce LTM in mice concurrently
treated with the PKG inhibitor Rp-8-Br-cGMPS [discrimination index (D) ⫽ 0.03 ⫾ 0.044, n ⫽ 10 sex-balanced mice for each condition; t(9) ⫽ 0.735, p ⫽ 0.481 (Figure legend continues.)
5996 • J. Neurosci., July 24, 2019 • 39(30):5986 – 6000 Gulisano et al. • Physiological Role of A Oligomers at the Synapse
memory depending upon its concentration (Puzzo et al., 2012; prevented both endogenous or exogenous A to exert its effects
Gulisano et al., 2018b). Here, we first performed a DR curve to at the synapse.
confirm that 200 pM oA42 was the dose capable of enhancing Because the probability of transmitter release contributes to
LTP, whereas 200 nM impaired it, consistent with previous works LTP induction and maintenance (Kleschevnikov et al., 1997), we
(Puzzo et al., 2008, 2011, 2012; Garcia-Osta and Alberini, 2009; investigated whether oA42 converted E-LTP into L-LTP, two
Morley et al., 2010; Lazarevic et al., 2017). different forms of plasticity characterized by different temporal
Next, because high concentrations of extracellular oA42 en- and mechanistic features (Huang, 1998). E-LTP can be induced
ter neurons (Ripoli et al., 2014; Puzzo et al., 2017), leading to by a weak tetanic stimulation, decays in 1 or 2 h, does not depend
synaptic dysfunction through direct intracellular targets (Ripoli on new protein synthesis, and is mostly due to activation of ki-
et al., 2014), we investigated whether pM oA42 acts at extracel- nases. Conversely, L-LTP needs a stronger stimulation to be in-
lular and/or intracellular levels when enhancing synaptic trans- duced, lasts several hours, and requires new protein synthesis and
mission and plasticity. We demonstrated that the effect of pM gene transcription (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993; Nguyen et al.,
oA42 is exerted only when the peptide is applied extracellularly, 1994; Huang, 1998). Several studies have demonstrated that ad-
because its administration inside neurons did not affect LTP. equate physiological or pharmacological stimuli might convert
Dual-patch experiments, in which we applied oA42 from the E-LTP into L-LTP, a mechanism that parallels transformation of
extracellular space while one of the two adjacent neurons was STM into LTM (for review, see Puzzo et al., 2016). Here, we
injected with the 6E10 antibody to neutralize human oA42 pos- found that treatment with picomolar concentrations of oA42
sibly accumulated inside neurons, confirmed that only extracel- before a weak tetanic stimulation converted E-LTP into L-LTP
lular oA42 enhances mEPSC frequency. and STM into LTM, suggesting that, at this concentration, the
The increase in mEPSC frequency and the decrease in PPF peptide acts as a cognitive enhancer and further begging the ques-
suggest a presynaptic mechanism of neurotransmitter release, tion of how its removal from the brain might be beneficial.
consistent with previous electrophysiological findings showing Ultrastructural analyses of hippocampal slices treated as for
that pM oA42 affect basal synaptic transmission, inducing an electrophysiology revealed that oA42 produces a series of con-
increase of fiber volley amplitude, an index of presynaptic re- comitant changes occurring both presynaptically and postsynap-
cruitment (Gulisano et al., 2018b), and posttetanic potentiation, tically, as one would expect for long-term normal synaptic
a form of short-term plasticity due to presynaptic calcium entry plasticity to occur (Antonova et al., 2001). Indeed, oA42 alone
(Puzzo et al., 2008). Furthermore, it has been recently demon- elevated the fraction of vesicles available for release (i.e., docked
vesicles) during the induction of plasticity (Bourne et al., 2013),
strated that A exerts an opposite effect on synaptic vesicle recy-
whereas a longer PSD, which is suggestive of plastic changes oc-
cling depending upon the dose (Lazarevic et al., 2017), consistent
curring at the postsynaptic site (Babits et al., 2016), was observed
with other studies showing a sustained increase of mEPSC fre-
in slices treated with oA42 paired with a weak tetanus.
quency after prolonged exposure to 200 pM oA42 (Koppen-
In the present study, we also sought to explore the possible
steiner et al., 2016) or treatment with inhibitors of A
extracellular targets used by pM oA42 to affect PPF and convert
degradation (Abramov et al., 2009). In these circumstances, high
E-LTP into L-LTP. We first focused on APP because it binds
levels of A may maintain neurotransmitter release for a longer
different A species (Lorenzo et al., 2000; Shaked et al., 2006;
period, leading to vesicle depletion (Parodi et al., 2010), or enter
Fogel et al., 2014) and is involved in the enhancement of neu-
neurons directly, affecting presynaptic proteins such as synapto- rotransmitter release, leading to hippocampal hyperactivity in
physin, VAMP2, or synapsin I (Russell et al., 2012; Koppensteiner AD (Bakker et al., 2012; Busche et al., 2012).
et al., 2016). Furthermore, recent studies have shown that, at high concen-
Here, we show that both exogenously applied human A at trations, extracellular A needs APP to impair synaptic plasticity
low concentrations and endogenous A are involved in neu- and memory (Puzzo et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2017). Here, we used
rotransmitter release. This is consistent with previous studies APP-KO mice at young age, when the impairment of LTP and
demonstrating that endogenous A is needed for synaptic plas- memory is not yet manifested (Dawson et al., 1999; Tyan et al.,
ticity and memory to occur (Garcia-Osta and Alberini, 2009; 2012), to investigate whether A needs APP to enhance synaptic
Morley et al., 2010; Puzzo et al., 2011) and that A production plasticity and memory when at low concentration. Because pM
physiologically increases during neuronal activity and memory oA42 still enhanced neurotransmitter release, synaptic plastic-
induction (Kamenetz et al., 2003; Cirrito et al., 2005; Brody et al., ity, and memory in APP-KO mice, we concluded that APP inter-
2008; Puzzo et al., 2011; Palmeri et al., 2017). Interestingly, we venes in A detrimental effects when the peptide is present at
have previously demonstrated that the impairment of LTP and high concentrations (Puzzo et al., 2017), but is not needed for
memory due to the inhibition of endogenous A could be res- oA42 at low concentrations to enhance synaptic plasticity.
cued by concomitant application of exogenous 200 pM A, with Because A induces a series of events mediated by an increase
300 pM A producing a more pronounced enhancement similar of Ca 2⫹ entry in the presynaptic terminal (Puzzo et al., 2008;
to that obtained with the administration of 200 pM exogenous A Lawrence et al., 2014), we focused on ␣7nAChRs, ionotropic
alone (Puzzo et al., 2011). In the same work, we demonstrated channels permeable to calcium that are highly expressed in the
that the threshold of A needed for normal synaptic plasticity hippocampus and implicated in a variety of cognitive functions
and memory is ⬃330 –380 pM. Thus, considering that A42 levels such as learning and memory, attention and reward (for review,
in basal conditions are equal to 180 –200 pM (Puzzo et al., 2008), see Picciotto et al., 2000). The link between A and ␣7nAChRs
adding 200 pM exogenous A is likely to induce a further en- has been widely studied and it is known that A binds ␣7nAChRs
hancement of synaptic plasticity and memory. Even if we cannot with high affinity, exerting an agonistic or antagonistic effect in a
exclude that exogenously applied and endogenous A might use dose-dependent manner (Wang et al., 2000; Mura et al., 2012).
different mechanisms to act, previous works have suggested that Using genetic and pharmacological approaches, we showed that
a common target might be represented by ␣7-nAchRs (Puzzo et ␣7nAChRs are needed for pM oA42 to increase neurotransmitter
al., 2008, 2011), the genetic or pharmacological deletion of which release and to consolidate LTP. Experiments performed in hip-
5998 • J. Neurosci., July 24, 2019 • 39(30):5986 – 6000 Gulisano et al. • Physiological Role of A Oligomers at the Synapse
pocampal slices from APP-KO mice treated with the ␣7nAChR whereas nM oA42 requires APP and intraneuronal internaliza-
antagonist MLA further confirmed that, at low concentrations, tion (Ripoli et al., 2014; Puzzo et al., 2017), even if it is not pos-
oA42 exerts its effect through ␣7nAChRs and not APP, in agree- sible to exclude an extracellular effect mediated by different
ment with previous studies (Puzzo et al., 2008, 2011; Mura et al., receptors. Conversely, low and high oA42 levels affect the same
2012; Lawrence et al., 2014). key intracellular pathways involved in synaptic plasticity and
The in vitro observation showing that ␣7nAChRs but not APP memory (i.e., the NO/cGMP/PKG/p-CREB cascade, CaMKII,
are necessary for pM oA42 to produce L-LTP was further con- BDNF), leading to their stimulation at picomolar concentrations
firmed by in vivo studies demonstrating that the oA42-induced or inhibition at nanomolar concentrations (Puzzo et al., 2005,
conversion of STM into LTM relies upon ␣7nAChRs and does 2009; Ghosh and Giese, 2015; Song et al., 2015; Opazo et al.,
not involve APP. 2018). The capability of oA42 to induce opposite effects depend-
Here, we also demonstrated that oA42 effects are mediated by ing upon its dose is consistent with previous studies (Calabrese,
the NO/cGMP/PKG cascade, already known to be involved in 2001; Puzzo et al., 2012), and this biphasic behavior characterized
LTP and memory induction and maintenance (Johnstone and by low-dose stimulation and high-dose inhibition might be
Raymond, 2011; Bollen et al., 2014). In fact, inhibition of this framed into the “hormesis” phenomenon (Calabrese, 2008;
pathway prevented oA42-induced L-LTP and LTM. Interest- Mattson, 2008). Indeed, even if the most used approach to model
ingly, we have recently demonstrated that in physiological con- a DR curve is the linear Hill model, several studies have pointed
ditions, cGMP stimulates A production to induce L-LTP and out that it might not be sufficient to explain the multiphasic
memory (Palmeri et al., 2017). This, together with the present response exerted by a variety of endogenous or exogenous com-
results, indicates that the NO/cGMP pathway acts both upstream pounds (for review, see Prickaerts et al., 2017). Although differ-
and downstream of A, suggesting the existence of a cGMP-A- ent mechanisms may underlie the hormetic effect (Mattson,
cGMP loop as a preferential intracellular mechanism involved in 2008), it is widely accepted that a compound might interact with
A-mediated plastic effects. a certain target depending upon its concentration. Consistently,
Additionally, pM oA42 increased the expression of nNOS, A binds different targets in a concentration-dependent manner
which is decreased by high concentrations of the peptide (Ven- (Mura et al., 2012; Zappettini et al., 2012). and, recently, it has
turini et al., 2002). This effect might be due to direct or indirect been shown that A directly affects ␣7-nAchR conformation and
mechanisms. Indeed, previous studies have demonstrated that function by acting as an agonist when at picomolar concentra-
A fragments can bind nNOS (Padayachee and Whiteley, 2011), tions or as an antagonist when at nanomolar concentrations (La-
suggesting the possibility of a direct interaction between the two sala et al., 2019). It should be also considered that at high A
proteins. Conversely, the oA42-induced increase of neurotrans- concentrations, the amount of oligomerization proportionally
mitter release might enhance Ca 2⫹ influx at the postsynaptic increases overrunning the physiological level (Gulisano et al.,
level, triggering increased nNOS expression (Sasaki et al., 2000). 2018a,b). This, in turn, might determine the interaction of A
The oA42-induced conversion of E-LTP into L-LTP suggests oligomers with different targets, including APP (Puzzo et al.,
a possible role for A in the synaptic tagging mechanism. Syn- 2017).
apses that have received a weak stimulation enter a receptive state These observations might be useful to understand the mech-
(tagging) that, if associated with the synthesis of PRPs, will lead to anisms switching the positive into the negative effects exerted by
LTP maintenance. The initial changes in the synaptic weight are oA, and might be a basis for further studies to ensure novel, safe,
given by the efficacy of the presynaptic and postsynaptic elements and rational personalized therapeutic approaches to cure patients
and require a third factor to persist (Redondo and Morris, 2011). affected by AD.
Consistent with this possibility, pretreatment of hippocampal
slices with oA42 induced the synaptic changes underlying References
Abramov E, Dolev I, Fogel H, Ciccotosto GD, Ruff E, Slutsky I (2009)
L-LTP, such as the increased phosphorylation of CREB known to Amyloid-beta as a positive endogenous regulator of release probability at
lead to PRP expression needed for L-LTP appearance (Bourtchu- hippocampal synapses. Nat Neurosci 12:1567–1576.
ladze et al., 1994). Moreover, p-CREB further increased in slices Aizenstein HJ, Nebes RD, Saxton JA, Price JC, Mathis CA, Tsopelas ND,
in which oA42 was paired with a weak tetanus, confirming that Ziolko SK, James JA, Snitz BE, Houck PR, Bi W, Cohen AD, Lopresti BJ,
CREB phosphorylation continues to rise during LTP mainte- DeKosky ST, Halligan EM, Klunk WE (2008) Frequent amyloid deposi-
nance (Leutgeb et al., 2005) and that the NO/cGMP/PKG cascade tion without significant cognitive impairment among the elderly. Arch
Neurol 65:1509 –1517.
can trigger gene expression through CREB phosphorylation dur-
Antonova I, Arancio O, Trillat AC, Wang HG, Zablow L, Udo H, Kandel ER,
ing L-LTP (Lu et al., 1999). Hawkins RD (2001) Rapid increase in clusters of presynaptic proteins at
Interestingly, pairing oA42 with a weak tetanus induced an onset of long-lasting potentiation. Science 294:1547–1550.
increase of CaMKII phosphorylation and BDNF expression, Babits R, Szőke B, Sótonyi P, Rácz B (2016) Food restriction modifies ultra-
molecules known to stimulate CREB activation, thus inducing structure of hippocampal synapses. Hippocampus 26:437– 444.
L-LTP and LTM. In particular, p-CaMKII, like nNOS, responds Bakker A, Krauss GL, Albert MS, Speck CL, Jones LR, Stark CE, Yassa MA,
Bassett SS, Shelton AL, Gallagher M (2012) Reduction of hippocampal
to NMDA-receptor-mediated Ca 2⫹ entry during LTP and is in-
hyperactivity improves cognition in amnestic mild cognitive impairment.
volved in tag signaling (Lisman et al., 2012) and metaplasticity Neuron 74:467– 474.
(Deisseroth et al., 1995); BDNF is responsible for upregulating Barone E, Tramutola A, Triani F, Calcagnini S, Di Domenico F, Ripoli C,
gene transcription and promoting structural changes during Gaetani S, Grassi C, Butterfield DA, Cassano T, Perluigi M (2019) Biliv-
L-LTP (Bekinschtein et al., 2007). This further suggests that erdin reductase-A mediates the beneficial effects of intranasal insulin in
oA42 may prime the synapse to be more responsive, as also Alzheimer disease. Mol Neurobiol 56:2922–2943.
indicated by ultrastructural results. Bekinschtein P, Cammarota M, Igaz LM, Bevilaqua LR, Izquierdo I, Medina
JH (2007) Persistence of long-term memory storage requires a late pro-
In conclusion, our findings evidenced interesting differences tein synthesis- and BDNF-dependent phase in the hippocampus. Neuron
and similarities between low versus high oA42 concentrations. 53:261–277.
Indeed, the effects of picomolar concentrations of A42 are ex- Bliss TV, Collingridge GL (1993) A synaptic model of memory: long-term
erted at the extracellular level and depend upon ␣7nAChRs, potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361:31–39.
Gulisano et al. • Physiological Role of A Oligomers at the Synapse J. Neurosci., July 24, 2019 • 39(30):5986 – 6000 • 5999
Bollen E, Puzzo D, Rutten K, Privitera L, De Vry J, Vanmierlo T, Kenis G, Kamenetz F, Tomita T, Hsieh H, Seabrook G, Borchelt D, Iwatsubo T, Sisodia
Palmeri A, D’Hooge R, Balschun D, Steinbusch HM, Blokland A, Prick- S, Malinow R (2003) APP processing and synaptic function. Neuron
aerts J (2014) Improved long-term memory via enhancing cGMP-PKG 37:925–937.
signaling requires cAMP-PKA signaling. Neuropsychopharmacology Kleschevnikov AM, Sokolov MV, Kuhnt U, Dawe GS, Stephenson JD, Vo-
39:2497–2505. ronin LL (1997) Changes in paired-pulse facilitation correlate with in-
Bon CL, Garthwaite J (2003) On the role of nitric oxide in hippocampal duction of long-term potentiation in area CA1 of rat hippocampal slices.
long-term potentiation. J Neurosci 23:1941–1948. Neuroscience 76:829 – 843.
Bourne JN, Chirillo MA, Harris KM (2013) Presynaptic ultrastructural Koo EH, Park L, Selkoe DJ (1993) Amyloid -protein as a substrate interacts
plasticity along CA3¡CA1 axons during long-term potentiation in ma- with extracellular matrix to promote neurite outgrowth. Proc Natl Acad
ture hippocampus. J Comp Neurol 521:3898 –3912. Sci U S A 90:4748 – 4752.
Bourtchuladze R, Frenguelli B, Blendy J, Cioffi D, Schutz G, Silva AJ (1994) Koppensteiner P, Trinchese F, Fà M, Puzzo D, Gulisano W, Yan S, Poussin A,
Deficient long-term memory in mice with a targeted mutation of the Liu S, Orozco I, Dale E, Teich AF, Palmeri A, Ninan I, Boehm S, Arancio
cAMP-responsive element-binding protein. Cell 79:59 – 68. O (2016) Time-dependent reversal of synaptic plasticity induced by
Brody DL, Magnoni S, Schwetye KE, Spinner ML, Esparza TJ, Stocchetti N, physiological concentrations of oligomeric A42: an early index of Alz-
Zipfel GJ, Holtzman DM (2008) Amyloid-beta dynamics correlate with heimer’s disease. Sci Rep 6:32553.
neurological status in the injured human brain. Science 321:1221–1224. Lasala M, Fabiani C, Corradi J, Antollini S, Bouzat C (2019) Molecular
Busche MA, Chen X, Henning HA, Reichwald J, Staufenbiel M, Sakmann B, modulation of human ␣7 nicotinic receptor by amyloid- peptides. Front
Konnerth A (2012) Critical role of soluble amyloid- for early hip- Cell Neurosci 13:37.
pocampal hyperactivity in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Proc Lawrence JL, Tong M, Alfulaij N, Sherrin T, Contarino M, White MM, Bell-
Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:8740 – 8745. inger FP, Todorovic C, Nichols RA (2014) Regulation of presynaptic
Calabrese EJ (2001) Amyloid beta-peptide: biphasic dose responses. Crit Ca2⫹, synaptic plasticity and contextual fear conditioning by a
Rev Toxicol 31:605– 606. N-terminal -amyloid fragment. J Neurosci 34:14210 –14218.
Calabrese EJ (2008) Hormesis and medicine. Br J Clin Pharmacol 66: Lazarevic V, Fieńko S, Andres-Alonso M, Anni D, Ivanova D, Montenegro-
594 – 617. Venegas C, Gundelfinger ED, Cousin MA, Fejtova A (2017) Physiolog-
Caraci F, Gulisano W, Guida CA, Impellizzeri AA, Drago F, Puzzo D, Palmeri ical concentrations of amyloid beta regulate recycling of synaptic vesicles
A (2015) A key role for TGF-1 in hippocampal synaptic plasticity and via alpha7 acetylcholine receptor and CDK5/calcineurin signaling. Front
memory. Sci Rep 5:11252. Mol Neurosci 10:221.
Cirrito JR, Yamada KA, Finn MB, Sloviter RS, Bales KR, May PC, Schoepp Lesné SE, Sherman MA, Grant M, Kuskowski M, Schneider JA, Bennett DA,
DD, Paul SM, Mennerick S, Holtzman DM (2005) Synaptic activity reg- Ashe KH (2013) Brain amyloid- oligomers in ageing and Alzheimer’s
ulates interstitial fluid amyloid- levels in vivo. Neuron 48:913–922. disease. Brain 136:1383–1398.
Cirrito JR, Kang JE, Lee J, Stewart FR, Verges DK, Silverio LM, Bu G, Menn- Leutgeb JK, Frey JU, Behnisch T (2005) Single cell analysis of activity-
erick S, Holtzman DM (2008) Endocytosis is required for synaptic dependent cyclic AMP-responsive element-binding protein phosphory-
activity-dependent release of amyloid-beta in vivo. Neuron 58:42–51. lation during long-lasting long-term potentiation in area CA1 of mature
Dawson GR, Seabrook GR, Zheng H, Smith DW, Graham S, O’Dowd G, rat hippocampal-organotypic cultures. Neuroscience 131:601– 610.
Bowery BJ, Boyce S, Trumbauer ME, Chen HY, Van Der Ploeg LH, Siri- Lisman J, Yasuda R, Raghavachari S (2012) Mechanisms of CaMKII action
nathsinghji DJ (1999) Age-related cognitive deficits, impaired long- in long-term potentiation. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:169 –182.
term potentiation and reduction in synaptic marker density in mice Lorenzo A, Yuan M, Zhang Z, Paganetti PA, Sturchler-Pierrat C, Staufenbiel
lacking the -amyloid precursor protein. Neuroscience 90:1–13. M, Mautino J, Vigo FS, Sommer B, Yankner BA (2000) Amyloid beta
DeFelipe J, Marco P, Busturia I, Merchán-Pérez A (1999) Estimation of the interacts with the amyloid precursor protein: a potential toxic mechanism
number of synapses in the cerebral cortex: methodological consider- in Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Neurosci 3:460 – 464.
ations. Cereb Cortex 9:722–732. Lu YF, Kandel ER, Hawkins RD (1999) Nitric oxide signaling contributes to
Deisseroth K, Bito H, Schulman H, Tsien RW (1995) Synaptic plasticity: a late-phase LTP and CREB phosphorylation in the hippocampus. J Neu-
molecular mechanism for metaplasticity. Curr Biol 5:1334 –1338. rosci 19:10250 –10261.
Dobrunz LE, Stevens CF (1997) Heterogeneity of release probability, facili- Maarouf CL, Daugs ID, Kokjohn TA, Walker DG, Hunter JM, Kruchowsky
tation, and depletion at central synapses. Neuron 18:995–1008. JC, Woltjer R, Kaye J, Castaño EM, Sabbagh MN, Beach TG, Roher AE
Fogel H, Frere S, Segev O, Bharill S, Shapira I, Gazit N, O’Malley T, Slomowitz (2011) Alzheimer’s disease and non-demented high pathology control
E, Berdichevsky Y, Walsh DM, Isacoff EY, Hirsch JA, Slutsky I (2014) nonagenarians: comparing and contrasting the biochemistry of cogni-
APP homodimers transduce an amyloid--mediated increase in release tively successful aging. PLoS One 6:e27291.
probability at excitatory synapses. Cell Rep 7:1560 –1576. Mattson MP (2008) Hormesis defined. Ageing Res Rev 7:1–7.
Garcia-Osta A, Alberini CM (2009) Amyloid beta mediates memory forma- Melone M, Bellesi M, Ducati A, Iacoangeli M, Conti F (2011) Cellular and
tion. Learn Mem 16:267–272. synaptic localization of EAAT2a in human cerebral cortex. Front Neuro-
Geinisman Y (2000) Structural synaptic modifications associated with hip- anat 4:151.
pocampal LTP and behavioral learning. Cereb Cortex 10:952–962. Melone M, Ciappelloni S, Conti F (2015) A quantitative analysis of cellular
Ghosh A, Giese KP (2015) Calcium/calmodulin-dependent kinase II and and synaptic localization of GAT-1 and GAT-3 in rat neocortex. Brain
Alzheimer’s disease. Mol Brain 8:78. Struct Funct 220:885– 897.
Gulisano W, Maugeri D, Baltrons MA, Fà M, Amato A, Palmeri A, D’Adamio Morley JE, Farr SA, Banks WA, Johnson SN, Yamada KA, Xu L (2010) A
L, Grassi C, Devanand DP, Honig LS, Puzzo D, Arancio O (2018a) Role physiological role for amyloid-beta protein:enhancement of learning and
of amyloid- and tau proteins in Alzheimer’s disease: confuting the am- memory. J Alzheimers Dis 19:441– 449.
yloid cascade. J Alzheimers Dis 68:415. Mura E, Zappettini S, Preda S, Biundo F, Lanni C, Grilli M, Cavallero A,
Gulisano W, Melone M, Li Puma DD, Tropea MR, Palmeri A, Arancio O, Olivero G, Salamone A, Govoni S, Marchi M (2012) Dual effect of beta-
Grassi C, Conti F, Puzzo D (2018b) The effect of amyloid- peptide on amyloid on ␣7 and ␣42 nicotinic receptors controlling the release of
synaptic plasticity and memory is influenced by different isoforms, con- glutamate, aspartate and GABA in rat hippocampus. PLoS One 7:e29661.
centrations, and aggregation status. Neurobiol Aging 71:51– 60. Nguyen PV, Abel T, Kandel ER (1994) Requirement of a critical period of
Hardy J, Selkoe DJ (2002) The amyloid hypothesis of alzheimer ’ s disease: transcription for induction of a late phase of LTP. Science 265:1104 –1107.
progress and problems on the road to therapeutics. Science 297:353–356. Opazo P, Viana da Silva S, Carta M, Breillat C, Coultrap SJ, Grillo-Bosch D,
Herrup K (2015) The case for rejecting the amyloid cascade hypothesis. Nat Sainlos M, Coussen F, Bayer KU, Mulle C, Choquet D (2018) CaMKII
Neurosci 18:794 –799. metaplasticity drives A oligomer-mediated synaptotoxicity. Cell Rep 23:
Huang EP (1998) Synaptic plasticity: Going through phases with LTP. Curr 3137–3145.
Biol 8:R350 –R352. Padayachee ER, Whiteley CG (2011) Spectrofluorimetric analysis of the in-
Johnstone VP, Raymond CR (2011) A protein synthesis and nitric oxide- teraction of amyloid peptides with neuronal nitric oxide synthase: impli-
dependent presynaptic enhancement in persistent forms of long-term cations in Alzheimer’s disease. Biochim Biophys Acta 1810:1136 –1140.
potentiation. Learn Mem 18:625– 633. Palmeri A, Ricciarelli R, Gulisano W, Rivera D, Rebosio C, Calcagno E, Tro-
6000 • J. Neurosci., July 24, 2019 • 39(30):5986 – 6000 Gulisano et al. • Physiological Role of A Oligomers at the Synapse
pea MR, Conti S, Das U, Roy S, Pronzato MA, Arancio O, Fedele E, Puzzo amyloid- (A) protein plays a major role in A-induced alterations of
D (2017) Amyloid- peptide is needed for cGMP-induced long-term glutamatergic synaptic transmission and plasticity. J Neurosci 34:
potentiation and memory. J Neurosci 37:6926 – 6937. 12893–12903.
Parodi J, Sepúlveda FJ, Roa J, Opazo C, Inestrosa NC, Aguayo LG (2010) Russell CL, Semerdjieva S, Empson RM, Austen BM, Beesley PW, Alifragis P
Beta-amyloid causes depletion of synaptic vesicles leading to neurotrans- (2012) Amyloid- acts as a regulator of neurotransmitter release disrupt-
mission failure. J Biol Chem 285:2506 –2514. ing the interaction between synaptophysin and VAMP2. PLoS One
Pearson HA, Peers C (2006) Physiological roles for amyloid beta peptides. 7:e43201.
J Physiol 575:5–10. Sasaki M, Gonzalez-Zulueta M, Huang H, Herring WJ, Ahn S, Ginty DD,
Peters A, Palay SL, deF Webster H (1991) The fine structure of the nervous Dawson VL, Dawson TM (2000) Dynamic regulation of neuronal NO
system neurons and their supportive cells. New York: OUP. synthase transcription by calcium influx through a CREB family tran-
Picciotto MR, Caldarone BJ, King SL, Zachariou V (2000) Nicotinic recep- scription factor-dependent mechanism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
tors in the brain. Links between molecular biology and behavior. Neuro- 97:8617– 8622.
psychopharmacology 22:451– 465. Schmidt SD, Nixon RA, Mathews PM (2005) ELISA method for measure-
Pozzo-Miller LD, Gottschalk W, Zhang L, McDermott K, Du J, Gopalakrish- ment of amyloid- levels. In: Amyloid proteins (Sigurdsson EM, ed), pp
nan R, Oho C, Sheng ZH, Lu B (1999) Impairments in high-frequency 279 –298. Totowa, NJ: Humana.
transmission, synaptic vesicle docking, and synaptic protein distribution Schneider CA, Rasband WS, Eliceiri KW (2012) NIH Image to ImageJ: 25
in the hippocampus of BDNF knockout mice. J Neurosci 19:4972– 4983. years of image analysis. Nat Methods 9:671– 675.
Prickaerts J, Van Goethem NP, Gulisano W, Argyrousi EK, Palmeri A, Puzzo Shaked GM, Kummer MP, Lu DC, Galvan V, Bredesen DE, Koo EH (2006)
D (2017) Physiological and pathological processes of synaptic plasticity Abeta induces cell death by direct interaction with its cognate extracellu-
and memory in drug discovery: do not forget the dose-response curve. lar domain on APP (APP 597– 624). FASEB J 20:1254 –1256.
Eur J Pharmacol 817:59 –70. Shepherd GM, Harris KM (1998) Three-dimensional structure and compo-
Puzzo D, Vitolo O, Trinchese F, Jacob JP, Palmeri A, Arancio O (2005) sition of CA3-⬎CA1 axons in rat hippocampal slices: implications
Amyloid-beta peptide inhibits activation of the nitric oxide/cGMP/ for presynaptic connectivity and compartmentalization. J Neurosci 18:
cAMP-responsive element-binding protein pathway during hippocampal
8300 – 8310.
synaptic plasticity. J Neurosci 25:6887– 6897.
Song JH, Yu JT, Tan L (2015) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor in Alzhei-
Puzzo D, Privitera L, Leznik E, Fà M, Staniszewski A, Palmeri A, Arancio O
mer’s disease: risk, mechanisms, and therapy. Mol Neurobiol 52:
(2008) Picomolar amyloid-beta positively modulates synaptic plasticity
1477–1493.
and memory in hippocampus. J Neurosci 28:14537–14545.
Spinelli M, Fusco S, Mainardi M, Scala F, Natale F, Lapenta R, Mattera A,
Puzzo D, Staniszewski A, Deng SX, Privitera L, Leznik E, Liu S, Zhang H, Feng
Rinaudo M, Li Puma DD, Ripoli C, Grassi A, D’Ascenzo M, Grassi C
Y, Palmeri A, Landry DW, Arancio O (2009) Phosphodiesterase 5 inhi-
(2017) Brain insulin resistance impairs hippocampal synaptic plasticity
bition improves synaptic function, memory, and amyloid-beta load in an
and memory by increasing GluA1 palmitoylation through FoxO3a. Nat
Alzheimer’s disease mouse model. J Neurosci 29:8075– 8086.
Puzzo D, Privitera L, Fa’ M, Staniszewski A, Hashimoto G, Aziz F, Sakurai M, Commun 8:2009.
Ribe EM, Troy CM, Mercken M, Jung SS, Palmeri A, Arancio O (2011) Stine WB Jr, Dahlgren KN, Krafft GA, LaDu MJ (2003) In vitro character-
Endogenous amyloid- is necessary for hippocampal synaptic plasticity ization of conditions for amyloid-beta peptide oligomerization and fibril-
and memory. Ann Neurol 69:819 – 830. logenesis. J Biol Chem 278:11612–11622.
Puzzo D, Privitera L, Palmeri A (2012) Hormetic effect of amyloid- pep- Tyan SH, Shih AY, Walsh JJ, Maruyama H, Sarsoza F, Ku L, Eggert S, Hof PR,
tide in synaptic plasticity and memory. Neurobiol Aging 33:1484.e15–24. Koo EH, Dickstein DL (2012) Amyloid precursor protein (APP) regu-
Puzzo D, Gulisano W, Arancio O, Palmeri A (2015) The keystone of Alzhei- lates synaptic structure and function. Mol Cell Neurosci 51:43–52.
mer pathogenesis might be sought in A physiology. Neuroscience Venturini G, Colasanti M, Persichini T, Fioravanti E, Ascenzi P, Palomba L,
307:26 –36. Cantoni O, Musci G (2002) Beta-amyloid inhibits NOS activity by sub-
Puzzo D, Fiorito J, Purgatorio R, Gulisano W, Palmeri A, Arancio O, Nicholls tracting NADPH availability. FASEB J 16:1970 –1972.
R (2016) Molecular mechanisms of learning and memory. In: Genes, Wang HY, Lee DH, Davis CB, Shank RP (2000) Amyloid peptide Abeta(1–
Environment and Alzheimer’s Disease (Lazarov O, Tesco G, ed), pp 1–28. 42) binds selectively and with picomolar affinity to alpha7 nicotinic ace-
Oxford: Academic Press. tylcholine receptors. J Neurochem 75:1155–1161.
Puzzo D, Piacentini R, Fá M, Gulisano W, Li Puma DD, Staniszewski A, Wang Z, Jackson RJ, Hong W, Taylor WM, Corbett GT, Moreno A, Liu W, Li
Zhang H, Tropea MR, Cocco S, Palmeri A, Fraser P, D’Adamio L, Grassi S, Frosch MP, Slutsky I, Young-Pearse TL, Spires-Jones TL, Walsh DM
C, Arancio O (2017) LTP and memory impairment caused by extracel- (2017) Human brain-derived A oligomers bind to synapses and disrupt
lular A and Tau oligomers is APP-dependent. eLife 6:e26991. synaptic activity in a manner that requires APP. J Neurosci 37:
Redondo RL, Morris RG (2011) Making memories last: the synaptic tagging 11947–11966.
and capture hypothesis. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:17–30. Zappettini S, Grilli M, Olivero G, Mura E, Preda S, Govoni S, Salamone A,
Ripoli C, Piacentini R, Riccardi E, Leone L, Li Puma DD, Bitan G, Grassi C Marchi M (2012) Beta amyloid differently modulate nicotinic and mus-
(2013) Effects of different amyloid -protein analogues on synaptic carinic receptor subtypes which stimulate in vitro and in vivo the release
function. Neurobiol Aging 34:1032–1044. of glycine in the rat hippocampus. Front Pharmacol 3:146.
Ripoli C, Cocco S, Li Puma DD, Piacentini R, Mastrodonato A, Scala F, Puzzo Zucker RS, Regehr WG (2002) Short-term synaptic plasticity. Annu Rev
D, D’Ascenzo M, Grassi C (2014) Intracellular accumulation of Physiol 64:355– 405.