Um, S. and Crompton, J. L. 1990. Attitude Determinants in Tourism Destination Choice. Annals of Tourism Research, 17
Um, S. and Crompton, J. L. 1990. Attitude Determinants in Tourism Destination Choice. Annals of Tourism Research, 17
Um, S. and Crompton, J. L. 1990. Attitude Determinants in Tourism Destination Choice. Annals of Tourism Research, 17
OO
Printed in the USA All rights reserved. Copyright 0 1990 Pergamon Press plc andJ. Jafan
ATTITUDE DETERMINANTS IN
TOURISM DESTINATION CHOICE
Seoho Urn
Kyonggi University, Korea
John L. Crompton
Texas A&M University, USA
INTRODUCTION
The image of a place as a pleasure travel destination is a gestalt. It is
an holistic construct which, to a greater or lesser extent, is derived from
5. D&nation Selection
where:
PF, represents the magnitude of perceived facilitators in selecting
alternative k
4, is the subjective probability that alternative k will be related to
perception i of destination attributes and assessed as a facili-
tator,
FE,, is the relative strength of the perception i as a facilitator, and
n is the number of perceptions of alternative k’s attributes, as-
sessed as facilitators.
PIk = 2 Bk,IEki
/=t
where:
PI, represents the magnitude of perceived inhibitors in selecting
alternative k
Bki is the subjective probability that alternative k will be related to
perception j of destination attributes and assessed as an
inhibitor,
IE, is the relative strength of the perceptionj as an inhibitor, and
m is the number of perceptions of alternative k’s attributes, as-
sessed as inhibitors.
TravelDestination Choice
METHODS
Data Collection
Figure 2(A) describes the first set of responses which had the highest
frequency (n, = 50). In the first survey, potential travelers’ awareness
sets (destinations A, B, C, D, E, and F) were identified and their
attitudes toward vacation places in the awareness set were measured. In
the second survey, these potential travelers responded that they had
already finalized their summer vacation destination(s) (B). They were
also asked to list all other places (C and D) which were close to being
selected before they made their final selection(s). Thus, n,, potential
travelers’ evoked sets of summer vacation destinations, consisted of
destinations B, C, and D and attitudes towards each of these were
measured.
In interpreting this first set of responses, it was recognized that
dissonance theory suggests that attitudes might change to confirm pre-
vious behavior to reduce post-decision conflict (Ginter 1974; Knox and
Inkster 1968). The attitudes towards vacation places in the evoked set,
which in this case were measured after a destination had been selected,
might be different from the ones on which the destination selection was
based. This is because potential travelers might tend to reinforce their
decisions by enhancing either their positive evaluation of the chosen
alternative or their relatively negative evaluations of the alternative
destinations which were discarded in the evoked set.
By definition, attitudes toward vacation places are composed of both
a subjective probability that a destination is perceived to possess speci-
lied attributes, and an evaluation of the importance of those perceived
attributes. In order to test whether or not potential travelers’ attitudes
changed after their destination selection decision, each respondent’s
mean scores on the 1 l-point evaluation scales before a destination was
selected (first survey) were compared with his or her evaluation scores
after a destination was selected (second survey).
It was found that the evaluations made after the destination selection
decision were significantly more positive than those made before the
destination selection decision (Table 2). For the second set of n2 re-
sponses (discussed later) of potential travelers who had not yet selected
their destination(s), the mean scores on the ll-point evaluation scales
in the first survey were also compared with the mean scores of the
evaluation scales in the second survey. In the n2 sample, there was no
significant difference in the two sets of evaluation scores (Table 2).
These results suggested two alternative interpretations for the n,
sample. First, the evaluation scores measured by the second survey
perhaps should be replaced by the scores measured by the first survey
in identifying the awareness set. This would remove the rationalization
effect which the data suggest may have taken place to alleviate post-
decision dissonance. An alternative interpretation is that changes in
subjective perceptions of alternative destination attributes occurring
between the first and second surveys might have been changed by active
information search between the first survey and the second survey.
Given this interpretation, destination attribute perceptions measured
by the second survey should not be replaced by those measured by the
first survey.
Like the first set of responses, respondents in the second set (n2 =
26) identified an awareness set, and attitudes toward each potential
Ihe fmt setof re.sponses tksecwdsa%ofreqmses tbethirdse10fresponses
II, = 50 IQ=26 $= 24
C-9 (3) Cc)
RESULTS
d = x, - x2.
Third, the standard error of the differences between the two groups’
mean scores was estimated from:
S, = (qd-d)‘ln(n - 1))1’2
where: n is the number of pairs. Fourth, the t value was calculated by:
t = 7/s- d’
Finally, the calculated t was compared to the tabled value at the .05
level of significance and with degrees of freedom equal to n - 1. A two-
tailed test was used. If the calculated statistics equalled or exceeded the
tabled value, the null hypothesis was rejected, and the alternative hy-
pothesis was accepted.
Table 4 shows the results of the related sample t test performed on the
alternatives selected from the awareness set for the evoked set. The
results indicated that the hypothesis could be accepted at the .05 level.
That is, the mean score PF, - PIk of the alternative destinations, which
were selected from the awareness set for inclusion in the evoked set, was
greater than those destinations that were not selected.
the mean score of the alternatives which are not selected as a travel
destination(s) from the evoked set.
Table 4 also shows the results of the related sample t test performed
on the alternatives selected as final destinations from the evoked set.
The results indicated that the hypothesis could be accepted at the .05
level. That is, the mean score PF’, - PI’, of the alternative(s) which
was selected as a travel destination(s) from the evoked set was greater
than the mean score of the alternatives which were not selected as a
travel destination(s).
The results of testing hypotheses 1 and 2 suggest that attitude is a
significant indicator for predicting whether or not a vacation place is
selected as a final destination from the alternatives in the awareness
set.
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES