Models 2
Models 2
What Is Intelligence?
The term "intelligence quotient," or IQ, was first coined in the early 20th
century by a German psychologist named William Stern.
Psychologist Alfred Binet developed the very first intelligence tests to help
the French government identify schoolchildren who needed extra academic
assistance. Binet was the first to introduce the concept of mental age or a
set of abilities that children of a certain age possess.2
Since that time, intelligence testing has emerged as a widely used tool that
has led to developing many other tests of skill and aptitude. However,
it continues to spur debate and controversy over the use of such testing,
cultural biases that may be involved, influences on intelligence, and even
the very way we define intelligence.
Theories of Intelligence
General Intelligence
People who performed well on one cognitive test tended to perform well on
other tests, while those who scored badly on one test tended to score badly
on others. He concluded that intelligence is a general cognitive ability that
can be measured and numerically expressed.
In the study of the human mind, intellect refers to and identifies the ability
of the mind to reach correct conclusions about what is true and what
is false, and about how to solve problems. The term intellect derives from
the Ancient Greek philosophy term nous, which translates to the
Latin intellectus (from intelligere, “to understand”) and into the French and
English languages as intelligence. Discussion of the intellect is in two areas
of knowledge, wherein the terms intellect and intelligence are related terms.
Development of intellect
Personal experience is crucial to the development of intellect, because in
resolving the problems of life a person can attain enlightenment, the full,
intellectual comprehension of a situation, and so improve his and
her behavior in order to act reasonably and appropriately in society. The
development of the intellect occurs consequent to the person's emotional
dissatisfaction that arises from the negative outcome of a given situation in
life; mental development occurs from the search for solutions to the
problems of life. Only experience can provide genuine and
thoughtful understanding of reality, which consequently contributes to the
person's intellectual development.
The Structure of Intellect (SI) model organizes intellectual functions in three
dimensions: (i) Operations, (ii) Contents, and (iii) Products. (Joy Paul
Guilford, 1955)
Intellectual understanding is built upon creating a conceptual model of
reality through perception and the reflective processes of cognition. The
process of finding solutions to the problems of life enriches the human
memory with the attributes of reality. The full potential of the intellect is
achieved when a person acquires an accurate understanding of the real
world, which is mirrored in the mind. The mature intellect is identified by the
capability of emotional “self-management”, the psychological state wherein
a person can encounter, face, and resolve problems as they arise in the
course of life, thereby being capable of personal change, and also capable
of altering his or her reality into a desirable life situation, thus success
and failure depend upon the person possessing a mature intellect.
Structure of intellect
In 1955, the psychologist Joy Paul Guilford (1897–1987) proposed a
Structural Intellect (SI) model in three dimensions: (i) Operations, (ii)
Contents, and (iii) Products. Each parameter contains specific, discrete
elements that are individually measured as autonomous units of the human
mind. Intellectual operations are represented by cognition and memory,
production (by divergent thinking and convergent thinking), and evaluation.
Contents are figurative and symbolic, semantic and behavioral. Products
are in units, classes, and relations, systems, transformations, and
implications.
Joy Paul Guilford (March 7, 1897 – November 26, 1987) was an
American psychologist best remembered for his psychometric study of
human intelligence, including the distinction
between convergent and divergent production.
Developing the views of L. L. Thurstone, Guilford rejected Charles
Spearman's view that intelligence could be characterized in a single
numerical parameter. He proposed that three dimensions were necessary
for accurate description: operations, content, and products. A Review of
General Psychology survey, published in 2002, ranked Guilford as the 27th
most cited psychologist of the 20th century.
Criticism
Various researchers have criticized the statistical techniques used by
Guilford. According to Jensen (1998), Guilford's contention that a g-factor
was untenable was influenced by his observation that cognitive tests of
U.S. Air Force personnel did not show correlations significantly different
from zero. According to one reanalysis, this resulted from artifacts and
methodological errors. Applying more robust methodologies, the
correlations in Guilford's data sets are positive. In another reanalysis,
randomly generated models were found to be as well supported as
Guilford's own theory.
Guilford's Structure of Intellect model of human abilities has few supporters
today. Carroll (1993) summarized the view of later researchers:
"Guilford's SOI model must, therefore, be marked down as a
somewhat eccentric aberration in the history of intelligence models.
The fact that so much attention has been paid to it is disturbing to the
extent that textbooks and other treatments of it have given the
impression that the model is valid and widely accepted, when clearly
it is not."
Louis Leon Thurstone (29 May 1887 – 30 September 1955) was
a U.S. pioneer in the fields of psychometrics and psychophysics. He
conceived the approach to measurement known as the law of comparative
judgment, and is well known for his contributions to factor analysis.
A Review of General Psychology survey, published in 2002, ranked
Thurstone as the 88th most cited psychologist of the 20th century, tied
with John Garcia, James J. Gibson, David Rumelhart, Margaret Floy
Washburn, and Robert S. Woodworth.
Intelligence Testing
Binet-Simon Scale
During the early 1900s, the French government enlisted the help of
psychologist Alfred Binet to understand which children were going to be
slower learners and thus require more assistance in the classroom (Binet et
al., 1912).
As a result, he and his colleague, Theodore Simon, began to develop a
specific set of questions that focused on areas such as memory and
problem-solving skills.
They tested these questions on groups of students aged three to twelve to
help standardize the measure (Binet et al., 1912). Binet realized that some
children were able to answer advanced questions that their older peers
were able to answer.
As a result, he created the concept of a mental age, or how well an
individual performs intellectually relative to the average performance at that
age (Cherry, 2020).
Ultimately, Binet finalized the scale, known as the Binet-Simon scale, that
became the basis for the intelligence tests still used today.
The Binet-Simon scale of 1905 comprised 30 items designed to measure
judgment, comprehension, and reasoning which Binet deemed the key
characteristics of intelligence.
Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale
When the Binet-Simon scale made its way over to the United States,
Stanford psychologist Lewis Terman adapted the test for American
students, and published the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale in 1916
(Cherry, 2020).
The Stanford-Binet Scale is a contemporary assessment which measures
intelligence according to five features of cognitive ability, including fluid
reasoning, knowledge, quantitative reasoning, visual-spatial processing
and working memory. Both verbal and nonverbal responses are measured.
This test used a single number, referred to as the intelligence quotient (IQ)
to indicate an individual’s score.
The average score for the test is 100, and any score from 90 to 109 is
considered to be in the average intelligence range. Score from 110 to 119
are considered to be High Average. Superior scores range from 120 to 129
and anything over 130 is considered Very Superior.
To calculate IQ, the student’s mental age is divided by his or her actual (or
chronological) age, and this result is multiplied by 100. If your mental age is
equal to your chronological age, you will have an IQ of 100, or average. If,
however, your mental age is, say, 12, but your chronological age is only 10,
you will have an above-average IQ of 120.
WISC and WAIS
Just as theories of intelligence build off one another, intelligence tests do
too. After Terman created Stanford-Binet test, American psychologist David
Wechsler developed a new tool due to his dissatisfaction with the
limitations of the Stanford-Binet test (Cherry, 2020).
Just like Thurstone, Gardner, and Sternberg, Wechsler believed that
intelligence involved many different mental abilities and felt that the
Stanford-Binet scale too closely reflected the idea of one general
intelligence.
Because of this, Wechsler created the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Children (WISC) and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) in 1955,
with the most up-to-date version being the WAIS-IV (Cherry, 2020).
The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC), developed by
David Wechsler, is an IQ test designed to measure intelligence and
cognitive ability in children between the ages of 6 and 16. It is currently in
its fourth edition (WISC-V) released in 2014 by Pearson.
Extremes of Intelligence
IQ scores are generally normally distributed (Moore et al., 2013). That is,
roughly 95% of the population has IQ scores between 70 and 130. But
what about the other 5%?
Individuals who fall outside this range represent the extremes of
intelligence.
Those who have an IQ above 130 are considered to be gifted (Lally &
French, 2018), such as Christopher Langan, an American horse rancher,
who has an IQ score around 200 (Gladwell, 2008).
Those individuals who have scores below 70 do so because of an
intellectual disability, marked by substantial developmental delays,
including motor, cognitive, and speech delays (De Light, 2012).
Some of the time, these disabilities are the product of genetic mutations.
Down syndrome, for example, resulting from extra genetic material from or
a complete extra copy of the 21st chromosome, is a common genetic
cause of an intellectual disability (Breslin, 2014). As such, many individuals
with down syndrome have below average IQ scores (Breslin, 2014).
Savant syndrome is another example of an extreme of intelligence. Despite
having significant mental disabilities, these individuals demonstrate certain
abilities in some fields that are far above average, such as incredible
memorization, rapid mathematical or calendar calculation ability, or
advanced musical talent (Treffert, 2009).
The fact that these individuals who may be lacking in certain areas such as
social interaction and communication make up for it in other remarkable
areas, further illustrates the complexity of intelligence and what this
concept means today, as well as how we must consider all individuals
when determining how to perceive, measure, and recognize intelligence in
our society.