Lecture 8 Memory Notes
Lecture 8 Memory Notes
After reading this chapter, then, you will be able to answer the following
questions:
■ What is memory?
■ Are there different kinds of memory?
■ What causes difficulties and failures in remembering?
■ What are the biological bases of memory?
■ What are the major memory impairments?
What is memory, and why do we remember certain events and activities and forget
others? To illustrate how psychologists answer these questions, consider what might
occur if you were asked to name the sea on which Bombay is located during a game
of Trivial Pursuit.
If you have trouble answering the question, your difficulty may be related to the
initial encoding stage of memory. Encoding refers to the process by which
information is initially recorded in a form usable to memory. You may never, for
instance, have been exposed to information regarding Bombay's location, or it simply
may not have registered in a meaningful way if it had been pointed out to you.
On the other hand, even if you had been exposed to the information and
originally knew the name of the sea, you may still be unable to recall it because of a
failure in the retention process. Memory specialists speak of placing information in
storage, the location in the memory system in which material is saved. If the
material is not stored adequately in the first place, it cannot be recalled later.
1
THE THREE STAGES OF MEMORY:
MEMORY STOREHOUSES
SENSORY MEMORY.
A momentary flash of lightning, the sound of a twig snapping, and the sting of a
pinprick all represent stimulation of exceedingly brief duration, but they may
nonetheless provide important information that can require some response. Such
stimuli are initially—and briefly—stored in sensory memory, the first repository of the
information that the world presents to us. Actually, the term "sensory memory"
encompasses several types of sensory memories, each related to a different source
of sensory information. There is iconic memory, which reflects information from our
visual system; echoic memory, which stores information coming from the ears; and
corresponding memories for each of the other senses.
2
form of memory, that information is lost for good. For instance, iconic memory
seems to last less than a second, although, if the initial stimulus is very bright, the
image may last a little longer (Long & Beaton, 1982). Echoic memory fades within
three or four seconds (Darwin, Turvey, & Crowder, 1972). However, despite the brief
duration of sensory memory, its precision is high: It is able to store an almost exact
replica of each stimulus to which it is exposed.
F T V C
K D N L
Y W B M
When exposed to this array for just one-twentieth of a second, most people
could accurately recall only four or five of the letters. Although they knew that they
had seen more, the memory had faded by the time they reported the first few letters.
It was possible, then, that the information had initially been accurately stored in
sensory memory, but during the time it took to verbalize the first four or five letters
the memory of the other letters faded.
The specific process by which sensory memories are transformed into short-
term memories is not yet clear. What is clear, however, is that unlike sensory
memory, which holds a relatively full and detailed—if short-lived—representation of
the world, short-term memory has incomplete representational capabilities.
CNQMWNT
3
Each letter here qualifies as a separate chunk, and—as there are seven of
them—they are easily held in short-term memory.
But a chunk might also consist of larger categories, such as words or other
meaningful units. For example, consider the following list of twenty-one letters:
TWACIAABCCBSMTVUSAAAA
Clearly, because the list exceeds seven chunks, it is difficult to recall the letters af ter
one exposure. But suppose they were presented to you as follows:
The reason the task suddenly became so simple was that the shapes could
be grouped together into one chunk—a word that we all recognize. Rather than
being nineteen separate symbols with no meaning, they are. recoded as just one
chunk.
Chunks can vary in size from single letters or numbers to categories that are
far more complicated, and the specific nature of what constitutes a chunk varies \
according to one's past experience. You can see this for yourself by trying an
experiment that was first carried out comparing expert and inexperienced chess
players (deGroot, 1966).
REHEARSAL
The transfer of material from short- to long-term memory proceeds largely on
the basis of rehearsal, the repetition of information that has entered short-term
memory. Rehearsal accomplishes two things. First, as long as the information is
repeated, it is kept alive in short-term memory. More important, however, rehearsal
allows the material to be transferred into long-term memory.
Whether the transfer is made from short- to long-term memory seems to depend
largely on the kind of rehearsal that is carried out. If the material is simply repeated
over and over again—as we might do with a telephone number while we rush from
the phone book to the telephone—it is kept current in short-term memory but it will
not necessarily be placed in long-term memory. Instead, as soon as we stop dialing,
the number is likely to be replaced by other information and will be completely
forgotten.
4
long-term memory (Craik & Lockhart, 1972). Elaborative rehearsal occurs when the
material is considered and organized in some fashion. The organization might
include expanding the information to make it fit into a logical framework, linking it to
another memory, turning it into an image, or transforming it in some other way. For
example, a list of vegetables to be purchased at a store could be woven together in
memory as items being used to prepare an elaborate salad; they could be linked to
the items bought on an earlier shopping trip; or they could be thought of in terms of
the image of a farm with rows of each item.
LONG-TERM MEMORY:
The Final Storehouse. Material that makes its way from short-term memory to long-
term memory enters a storehouse of almost unlimited capacity. Like a new book
delivered to a library, the information in long-term memory is filed and cataloged so
that it can be retrieved when we need it.
There are actually two kinds of memories held in long-term memory: episodic and
semantic (Tulving, 1983). Episodic memories relate to our individual lives, recalling
what we have done and the kinds of experiences we have had. When you recall your
first date, the time you fell off your bicycle, or what you felt like when you graduated
from high school, you are recalling episodic memories. The information in episodic
memory is connected with specific times and places. In contrast, semantic memories
consist of organized knowledge and facts about the world; because of semantic
memory, we know that 2x2 = 4, the earth is round, and "memorce" is misspelled.
5
read the following exchange between a researcher and a subject who was asked, in
a memory experiment, what he was doing "on Monday afternoon in the third week of
September two years ago."
Episodic memory, then, can provide information from events that happened
long in the past (Reynolds & Takooshian, 1988). But semantic memory is no less
impressive: By calling upon it, all of us are able to dredge up thousands of facts
ranging from the date of our birthday to the knowledge that $1 is less than $5. Both
individual pieces of information and the rules of logic for deducing other facts are
stored in semantic memory.
If subjects are able to identify the stimulus more readily than they identify stimuli
that have not been printed earlier, priming is said to have occurred. What is
interesting about the phenomenon of priming is that it occurs even when subjects
report having no conscious awareness of having seen the stimulus earlier.
The discovery that people have memories about which they are not aware has
been an important one. It has led to speculation that a form of memory, dubbed
"implicit memory," may exist side by side with explicit memory. We discuss this
possibility in the accompanying Cutting Edge box.
6
STORAGE IN LONG-TERM MEMORY.
Levels of Processing
So far, we have used the notion that the processing of information in memory
proceeds along three sequential stages, starting with sensory memory, proceeding
to short-term memory, and finally ending with long-term memory. However, not all
psychologists specializing in memory agree with such a view. Some suggest that a
single process accounts for how well information is remembered: the way in which
material is first perceived, considered, and understood.
7
exposure to material—meaning the degree to which it is analyzed and considered—
is critical; the greater the intensity of its initial processing, the more likely we are to
remember it.
The theory goes on to suggest that there are considerable differences in the
way information is processed at various levels of memory. At shallow levels,
information is processed merely in terms of its physical and sensory aspects; for
example, we may pay attention only to the shapes that make up the letters in the
word "dog." At an intermediate level of processing, the shapes are translated into
meaningful units—in this case, letters of the alphabet. These letters are considered
in the context of words, and a specific sound of the word may be attached to the
letters.
8
FLASHBULB MEMORIES
Where were you when you learned that the space shuttle Challenger had
exploded?
You probably have little trouble recalling your exact location and a variety of
other trivial details about occurrences that happened when you heard the news,
even though the accident happened years ago. The reason is a phenomenon known
as flashbulb memories. Flashbulb memories are memories centered around a
specific, important, or surprising event that are so vivid it is as if they represented a
snapshot of the event.
Several types of flashbulb memories are common among college students.
For example, involvement in a car accident, meeting one's roommate for the first
time, and the night of high school graduation are all typical flashbulb memories
(Rubin, 1985).
Of course, flashbulb memories do not contain every detail of an original
scene. For instance, I remember vividly that some three decades ago I was sitting in
Mr. Sharp's tenth-grade geometry class when I heard that President Kennedy had
been shot. Although I recall where I was sitting and how my classmates reacted to
the news, I do not recollect what I was wearing or what I had for lunch that day.
Flashbulb memories, then, are not complete and unfailingly accurate, and just how
much their essential nature differs from everyday memories remains an open
question (McCloskey et al., 1988; Cohen, McCloskey, 8c Wible, 1990; Pillemer,
1990; Christianson & Loftus, 1990).
The notion that memory is based on constructive processes was first put
forward by Sir Frederic Bartlett, a British psychologist, who suggested that people
tend to remember information in terms of schemas, general themes that contain
relatively little specific detail (Bartlett, 1932). He argued that such schemas were
based on the information provided not only by a stimulus but also by our
understanding of the situation, our expectations about the situation, and our
awareness of the motivation underlying people's behavior.
9
This example, which is drawn from a classic experiment, illustrates the role of
expectations in memory. The migration of the razor from the white person's hand to
the black person's hand in memory clearly indicates that expectations about the
world—reflecting, in this case, the unwarranted prejudice that blacks may be more
violent than whites and thus more apt to be holding a razor—have an impact upon
how events are recalled.
Your memory of your own past might well be a fiction—or at least distortion of
what actually occurred. The same constructive processes also make us inaccurately
recall the behaviour of others also reduce the accuracy of autobiographical
memories. Autobiographical memories refer to our recollections of the facts about
our own lives (Bradburn, Rips, & Shevell, 1987).
For example, people tend to forget information about their past that is
congruent with the way in which they currently see themselves. One study found that
adults who were well adjusted but who had been treated for emotional problems
during the early years of their lives tended to forget important but troubling childhood
events. For instance, they forget such details as their family s receipt of welfare
when they were children, being in foster care, and living in a home for delinquents
(Robbins, 1988). Similarly, people who are depressed remember sad events from
their past more easily than happy people do, and people who report being happy in
adulthood remember more happy events than depressing ones (Bower & Cohen,
1982).
He could remember, quite literally, nothing—nothing, that is, that had happened
since the loss of his brain's temporal lobes and hippocampus during experimental
surgery to reduce epileptic seizures. Until that time, his memory had been quite
normal. Rut after the operation he was unable to recall anything for more than a few
minutes, and then the memory was seemingly lost forever. He did not remember his
address, or the name of the person to whom he was talking. He would read the
same magazine over and over again. According to his own description, his life was
like waking from a dream and being unable to know where he was or how he got
there (Milner, 1966).
10
and particularly in the first hour. After nine hours, the rate of forgetting slows and
declines little, even after the passage of many days.
Efforts at understanding the problem of why we forget have yielded two major
solutions. One theory explains forgetting in terms of a process called decay, or the
loss of information through its non use. This explanation assumes that when new
material is learned, a memory trace or engram—an actual physical change in the
brain—occurs. In decay, the trace simply fades away with nothing left behind,
because of the mere passage of time..
Because decay is not able to fully account for forgetting, memory specialists
have proposed an additional mechanism: interference. In interference, information
in memory displaces or blocks out other information, preventing its recall.
Most research suggests that interference is the key process in forgetting (Potter,
1990). We mainly forget things not because the memory trace has decayed; but
because new memories interfere with the retrieval ones.
11
MEMORY DYSFUNCTIONS: AFFLICTIONS OF FORGETTING
Harold's problem: he suffers from Alzheimer's disease, the illness that includes
severe memory problems among its symptoms. Alzheimer's, discussed first
in Chapter 3, is the fourth leading cause of death among U.S. adults. It strikes
around 10 percent of people over the age of 65, and almost half of all people who
live beyond 85 develop the disease .(Gelman, 1989b; Dickinson, 1991).
Although the causes of Alzheimer's disease are not fully understood, recent
evidence suggests that it may be linked to a specific inherited defect. The flaw leads
to difficulties in the production of the protein beta amyloid, necessary for the
maintenance of nerve cell connections. When the manufacture of beta amyloid goes
awry, it leads to the deterioration of nerve cells in the brain—producing the
symptoms of Alzheimer's (Hardy et al., 1991).
12
receives a blow to the head and is unable to remember anything from his or her past.
In reality, amnesia of this type, known as retrograde amnesia, is quite rare. In
retrograde amnesia, memory is lost for occurrences prior to a certain event. There is
usually a gradual reappearance of lost memory, although it may take as long as
several years for a full restoration to occur. In certain cases, some memories are lost
forever (Raddeley, 1982).
We may be grateful, then, that being somewhat forgetful plays a useful role in
our lives.
■ The keyword technique. Suppose you are taking a class in a foreign language
and need to learn long lists of vocabulary words. One way of easing this process is
to use the keyword technique, in which a foreign word is paired with a common
English word that has a similar sound. This English word is known as the kevword.
For example, to learn the Spanish word duck (pato, pronounced poko), the keyword
might be "pot"; for the Spanish word for horse {caballo, pronounced aah»eyi>yo), the
keyword might be "eye."
13
■ Method of loci. If you have ever had to give a talk in class, you know the difficulty
of keeping in mind all the points you want to make. One technique that works quite
effectively was developed by the ancient Greeks: When Greek orators sought to
memorize long speeches, they used the method of loci (loci is the Latin word for
places) to organize their recollections of what they wanted to say. With this
technique, each part of a speech is imagined as "residing" in a different location of a
building.
For instance, you might think of the preface of a talk as being in the house’s
entryway, the first major point being in the living room, the next major point residing
in the dining room, and so forth, until the end of the speech is reached at the back
bedroom of the house.
This technique can easily be adapted to learning lists of words; each word on
the list is imagined as being located in a series of sequential locations. I lie method
works best by using the most outlandish images possible: II you wanted to
remember a list of groceries consisting of bananas, ketchup, and milk, for instance,
you might think of a banana intertwined in the leaves of your living-room begonia, the
ketchup spilled over the end table, and the milk spraying from the top of a table
lamp. When you got to the supermarket, you could mentally "walk" through your
living room, recalling the items easily.
Organization of text material, Most of life's more important recall tasks involve
not lists of words but rather texts that have been read. How can you facilitate recall
of such material? One proven technique for improving recall of written material
consists of organizing the material in memory as it is being read for the first time. To
do this, you should first identify' any advance information about the structure and
content of the material—scanning by using the table of contents, chapter outline,
headings, and even the end-of-chapter summary—before reading a given chapter.
Understanding the structure of the material will enable you to recall it better.
Another technique is to ask yourself questions that integrate the material you have
read, and then answer them. Asking questions will enable you to make connections
and see relationships between the various specific facts, thereby promoting the
professing of the material at a deeper level. As the levels-of-processing approach to
memory that we discussed earlier suggests, doing so will aid later recall (Royer &:
Feldman, 1984). For example, you might at this moment ask yourself, "What are the
major techniques for remembering material in textbooks?" and then try to answer the
question.
14
■ Organization of lecture notes. "Less is more" is perhaps the best advice for
taking lecture notes that aid in recall. Rather than trying to jot down every detail of a
lecture, it is better to listen and think about the material, taking down the main points
after you have considered them in a broader context. In effective note taking,
thinking about the material initially is more important than writing it down. This is one
reason that borrowing someone else's notes is a bad pr opposition, since you will
have no framework in memory to use in understanding them (Peper & Mayer, 1978).
■ Practice and rehearsal. Although practice does not necessarily make perfect, it does
help. By studying and rehearsing material past the point of initial mastery—a process called
overlearning—people are able to show better long-term recall than if they stop
practicing after their initial learning of the material.
Eventually, of course, practice has little or no effect; you probably already
know your address so well that no amount of additional practice will make you recall
it any better than YOU already do. But it is safe to say that, given the volume of
material covered ill most courses, academic material is rarely so securely retained,
and you would generally be wise to review material a few times even after you feel
vou have learned it, in order to reach a true level of overlearning.
Finally, people who cram for tests should note that the best retention comes
from practice that is distributed over many sessions, rather than left for one long
session. Research clearly demonstrates that fatigue and other factors prevent long
practice sessions from being as effective as distributed practice.
15