One-Shot Case Study Design (One-Group Posttest Only)
One-Shot Case Study Design (One-Group Posttest Only)
In this design, a single group is exposed to a treatment or intervention, and then a post-
treatment observation or measurement is made. There is no pretest or control group for
comparison, making it difficult to attribute any observed effects solely to the treatment.
In this design, a single group is measured before and after a treatment or intervention.
This allows for a comparison of the pre- and post-treatment results, providing some
indication of change over time. However, without a control group, other factors like
maturation, testing effects, or external influences could affect the outcome.
This design involves two groups, where one group receives the treatment and the other
does not (control group). However, the groups are not randomly assigned, which means
they might not be equivalent at the start. This design allows for some level of
comparison, but any observed differences could be due to pre-existing disparities rather
than the treatment itself.
Lack of Control: These designs typically have fewer controls in place to isolate
the treatment effect, leading to a greater risk of confounding variables.
Limited Internal Validity: The inability to randomize groups or control for other
factors makes it difficult to establish causality.
Subject to Bias: Pre-experimental designs can be influenced by researcher bias
or participant expectations, affecting the outcomes.
True experimental design is the gold standard in experimental research due to its ability
to establish cause-and-effect relationships with high internal validity. A key feature of
true experimental design is random assignment, where participants are randomly
allocated to either the experimental group(s) or the control group(s). This randomization
helps ensure that the groups are comparable at the start of the experiment, minimizing
the risk of confounding variables. Let's discuss the main types of true experimental
design, along with examples for each.
In this design, participants are randomly assigned to either the experimental group
(which receives the treatment) or the control group (which does not). Both groups are
measured before and after the treatment, allowing for comparison between them and
within each group.
Here are the main types of quasi-experimental designs, along with examples for each:
In this design, there are experimental and control groups, but participants are not
randomly assigned to these groups. Instead, they are selected based on existing
conditions or non-random criteria. This design allows for comparison between groups,
but the lack of random assignment can introduce bias.
Example: A study investigating the impact of a new traffic law on road safety
might examine accident rates over several years before and after the law's
implementation. If there's a noticeable change in the trend after the law is
enacted, it could suggest a causal effect.
In this design, participants are assigned to groups based on a cutoff score on a pretest
or some other continuous variable. Those above the cutoff are placed in the
experimental group, while those below are in the control group. This design allows
researchers to examine the effect of an intervention on participants close to the cutoff.
Similar to the nonequivalent control group design, this approach includes a pretest
measurement, allowing researchers to examine changes over time. While this provides a
baseline for comparison, the lack of random assignment means that other factors could
influence the results.