Use of Scripts and Script-Fading Proc
Use of Scripts and Script-Fading Proc
organizations ― 1: Institute for Child Development, Gdansk, Poland · 2: Princeton Child Development Institute,
Princeton, USA
authors’ contribution ― A: Study design · B: Data collection · C: Statistical analysis · D: Data interpretation ·
E: Manuscript preparation · F: Literature search · G: Funds collection
corresponding author ― Anna Budzińska, Ph.D., Institute for Child Development, 139 Malczewskiego Str., 80-114 Gdansk,
Poland, tel. +48 58 341 44 41 or +48 665 07 80 00, e-mail: [email protected]
to cite this article ― Budzińska, A., Lubomirska, A., Wójcik, M., Krantz, P.J. & McClannahan, L. (2014). Use of scripts
and script-fading procedures and activity schedules to develop spontaneous social interaction in a three-year-old
girl with autism. Health Psychology Report, 2(1), 67–71. DOI: 10.5114/hpr.2014.42791
Background Behavior Scale, Second Edition, Sparrow, Cicchetti,
& Balla, 2005). Jane’s Adaptive Behavior Compos-
Autism is characterized by qualitative impairments ite score showed that general adaptive functioning
in social interaction and communication and restrict- was low; she scored higher than less than 1% of
ed, repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behavior similarly aged individuals in the Vineland-II norm
(DSM-IV, American Psychiatric Association, 2000). sample. Her standard score for the Communication
Impairments in social interaction include marked Domain was significantly lower than the median
deficits in the use of nonverbal behavior such as eye- score for all of the Vineland-II domains.
to-eye gaze. There may also be a lack of social reci- The setting was a preschool and research center
procity of facial expressions, body posture, and ges- for children with autism. Sessions were conducted in
Anna Budzińska, tures. In addition, some children with autism do not a typical classroom furnished with desks, chairs, and
Anna Lubomirska, spontaneously seek to share enjoyment, interests, or bookcases, and in a corridor that contained bookcas-
Marta Wójcik, achievements with others (that is, they display defi- es and toys, a gym with sports equipment and toys,
Patricia J. Krantz, cits in joint attention, such as showing, bringing, or a toilet and a dining room; generalization sessions
Lynn McClannahan pointing out objects of interest to others). were held in all of these settings, but with different
Deficits in joint attention are among the earliest teachers.
signs of autism in young children (Charman, 2003).
Such deficits are typically evident before one year of
age (Jones & Carr, 2004) and have been associated Experimental conditions
with difficulties in subsequent language development and measurement procedures
(Mundy, Sigman & Kasari, 1990).
Many studies indicate that joint attention skills Research design. The study used an A-B design (Bai-
may be necessary for the emergence of communica- ley & Burch, 2002). Phase A was baseline and Phase B
tion and it is important to ensure that newly acquired was intervention.
joint attention repertoires are functional – they are Measurement. In the course of therapy, Jane’s
used in different settings and with different partners, progress was recorded. The data were collected once
and are emitted in a variety of ways (Stokes & Baer, a week and graphed as percentage correct responses.
1977). In order to develop these skills, teaching must Each teaching level was considered completed if Jane
be conducted across a variety of stimuli until partic- achieved at least 80% correct, based on teaching and
ipants have acquired generalized repertoires (Reeve, generalization measures. Data collection procedures
Reeve, Townsend & Poulson, 2007). In addition, to were identical in each teaching step. At least once
increase the likelihood of response generalization, every three months, interobserver agreement (IOA)
children with autism must be taught to initiate bids data were collected to determine whether the data
for joint attention in a variety of ways. recorded by two independent observers were con-
gruent. Percentage interobserver agreement was cal-
culated by dividing the number of agreements by the
Participants and procedure number of agreements plus disagreements and multi-
plying by 100. Mean interobserver agreement on the
Participants and setting occurrence of correct responses was 96%.
Independent variables. Use of scripts and script-
One participant, Jane, began therapy at the Institute fading procedures (McClannahan & Krantz, 2005) is
for Child Development in Gdansk in September 2012, a strategy that has been applied to increase sponta-
at the age of 2 years and 10 months. She was the only neous language among individuals with autism. These
child in a two-parent family. She attended the Insti- procedures teach children to use written scripts or
tute’s intervention program for 4.5 hours per day, audiotaped recordings that provide models of appro-
5 days per week. Jane met the DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) priate language. As the learners begin to use scripted
criteria for autism and autism had been diagnosed language in their interactions, scripted phrases or sen-
by outside agencies. Based on DSM-IV-TR criteria, tences are systematically faded from end to beginning.
she had deficits in communication and social skills Scripts are embedded in the children’s daily activity
and displayed stereotypic behavior. At a biologi- schedules.
cal age of 2 years 10 months, she was tested using An activity schedule is a set of pictures or words
Schopler’s (1990) Psycho-Educational Profile-Revised that cues someone to engage in a sequence of activi-
(PEP-R). Her developmental age score was 1 year, 3 ties, perform tasks, or enjoy rewards (McClannahan
months. The most significant deficits were in the & Krantz, 1999). Jane’s activity schedule book con-
areas of imitation (3 months), receptive language (11 tained several pages, each of which displayed one
months), expressive language (5 months) and fine picture that showed an activity. Manual prompts
motor skills (11 months). Prior to intervention, adap- were used to teach her to follow the activity schedule
tive behaviors were evaluated (Vineland Adaptive and point to textual cues or listen to recorded scripts
40
developmental age – months
35 chronological age
30
25
20
15
10
5
0
imitation perception fine motor gross motor eye-hand cognitive cognitive developmental
integration performance verbal age
passing scores
emerging scores
80
70
developmental age – months
60
50 chronological age
40
30
20
10
0
imitation perception fine motor gross motor eye-hand cognitive cognitive developmental
integration performance verbal age
passing scores
emerging scores
Discussion Cooper, J.O., Heron, T.E. & Heward, W.L. (1987). Ap-
plied Behavior Analysis. Upper Sadle River: Pren-
Use of ABA
At the beginning of intervention, Jane presented no tice-Hall.
teaching
appropriate verbal or social behavior, did not imitate Jones, E.A. & Carr, E.G. (2004). Joint Attention in techniques to
sounds or name objects, did not follow others’ gaze, Children With Autism: Theory and Intervention. develop social
and did not notice others’ responses, or changes in Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabil- interaction in
her environment. Further, she did not initiate inter- ities, 19, 13-26. a small girl
action with others, nor did she bring objects to oth- McClannahan, L.E. & Krantz, P.J. (1999). Activity with autism
ers or point to objects of interest. In short, she had schedules for children with autism: Teaching in-
no joint attention skills, and she frequently engaged dependent behavior. Bethesda, MD: Woodbine
in stereotypy. The results of the PEP-R Test showed House.
that her developmental age score was lower than her McClannahan, L.E. & Krantz, P.J. (2005). Teaching
chronological age score. conversation to children with autism: Scripts and
Jane’s intensive, daily therapy was based on ap- script fading. Bethesda, MD: Woodbine House.
plied behavior analysis (ABA), which focused on Mundy, P., Sigman, M. & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitu-
shaping verbal and social behavior. Skill deficits were dinal study of joint attention and language devel-
primarily addressed using scripts and script-fading opment in autistic children. Journal of Autism and
procedures and activity schedules. Teaching episodes Developmental Disorders, 20, 115-128.
were brief and concise. After twelve months of in- Reeve, S.A., Reeve, K.F., Townsend, D.B. & Poulson,
tervention, her developmental age scores increased C.L. (2007). Establishing a generalized repertoire
and were comparable to her chronological age. She of helping behavior in children with autism. Jour-
initiated social interaction with others about new or nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 123-136.
unfamiliar objects and events, about new people, and Schopler, E., Reichler, R.J., Bashford, A., Lansing, M.
about topics of interest to her. Anecdotal observation & Marcus, L. (1990). Individualized assessment for
indicated that her verbal repertoire includes previ- autistic and developmentally disabled children: psy-
ously taught conversation skills. Social validity mea- choeducational profile revised (PEP-R). Austin, TX:
sures showed that her spontaneous social interac- PRO-ED.
tions were comparable to those of two typical peers. Sparrow, S.S., Cicchetti, D.V. & Balla, D.A. (2005).
Presently, the differences between Jane and typically Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale (2nd ed.). Livonia,
developing children are that her interactions are lim- MN: Pearson Assessments.
ited to one or two sentences, although children her Stokes, T.F. & Baer, D.M. (1977). An implicit technol-
age typically continue interaction using three or four ogy of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior
sentences. Our future research will focus on using Analysis, 10, 349-367.
scripts and script-fading procedures to build more
and longer sentences used in conversation.
References