Repetition Effects Study
Repetition Effects Study
Learning, Memory, and Cognition Copyright 1993 by the American Psychological Association, lnc
1993, Vol. 19. No. 1. 148-158 O278-7393/93/J3.0O
We examined whether repetition priming effects remain above baseline when explicit recognition
is reduced to chance or near chance levels by forgetting. Subjects studied a set of words, and
memory was tested explicitly by yes/no (Experiments 1 & 3) or forced-choice recognition
(Experiment 3) after a 20-min delay filled with an interfering task. Memory was then tested
implicitly by perceptual identification (Experiment 3) or lexical decision (Experiments 1 & 2) for
words seen only at study, at recognition, or both. In all experiments, recognition d' was about 0.75,
and repetition effects remained above baseline and constant across conditions. At delays of 24 hr
(Experiment 4) yes/no recognition fell to near chance (d' < 0.6) levels in a third of the subjects.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Repetition effects as measured by lexical decision were not significantly above baseline for words
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
seen only once in those subjects and 4 other subjects from Experiment 1 who had comparable
recognition rates. Reducing recognition by forgetting to near chance levels in normal people is
very difficult but when it does occur, repetition effects in normal people, in contrast with those in
amnesics, are also significantly reduced or eliminated.
Explicit tests of memory, such as recognition and recall, explicit tests is steep in comparison with the decline on
require conscious recollection of the past, whereas implicit some implicit tests such as repetition effects in lexical de-
or indirect tests of memory do not (Graf & Schacter, 1985; cision tasks (Scarborough, Cortese, & Scarborough, 1977),
Moscovitch, 1984). Instead, on implicit tests, memory for a perceptual identification (Jacoby & Dallas, 1981), speeded
particular item or event is inferred from changes in perfor- reading of geometrically transformed script (Cohen &
mance on tests involving that item, with the subject often Squire, 1980; Kolers, 1976; Moscovitch, Winocur, &
being unaware that memory is being probed. On some im- McLachlan, 1986), word-fragment completion (Sloman,
plicit tests, memory is reflected in the increased speed or Hayman, Ohta, Law, & Tulving, 1988), word-stem comple-
accuracy with which a repeated item is processed compared tion with unique solutions (Graf, Squire, & Mandler, 1984),
with when it was first presented or compared with a newly and picture naming (Mitchell & Brown, 1988).
presented item. We label this effect stimulus-specific repe- The similarity in the pattern of performance on explicit
tition priming effect, or simply repetition effect. and implicit tests between normal people tested at long
Evidence that performance on explicit and implicit tests delays and amnesic patients has led to the tacit assumption
of memory can be dissociated one from the other has accu- that evidence from both can be used interchangeably to
mulated during the last decade. The most dramatic and, support or refute theoretical positions regarding the organi-
perhaps, the most compelling evidence comes from amnesic zation of memory. It is possible, however, that organic am-
patients who often score at chance or baseline levels on nesia and extensive, but normal, forgetting have different
explicit tests but perform normally on implicit tests of mem- effects on performance. For example, although amnesic pa-
ory (for reviews, see Moscovitch, 1982, 1984; Schacter, tients with virtually no explicit memory can still perform
1987; Shimamura, 1986). A comparable, but less dramatic, relatively well on some implicit tests, it is not well estab-
pattern has been observed in healthy subjects who are tested lished whether healthy subjects matched with severe amne-
at long delays from study. The drop in their performance on sics for performance on explicit tests of memory, such as
recognition, will perform up to the level of amnesics on
implicit tests.
By extrapolating from reports of a more rapid decline in
Morris Moscovitch, Department of Psychology, Erindale Col-
lege, University of Toronto, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada;
performance on explicit than on implicit tests of memory, it
Shlomo Bentin, Department of Psychology and School of Educa- is possible to conclude that normal people, like amnesics,
tion, Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel. will continue to perform well on implicit tests even if their
The contributions of all authors to this study were equal. The performance on explicit tests has dropped to chance or
order of authors was determined randomly. This study was sup- baseline levels. This conclusion, however, is not warranted
ported by a grant from the Israeli Academy of Science, the Fund for a number of reasons. First, with the exception of studies
for Basic Research to Shlomo Bentin, and by a Natural Science examining memory for unattended or poorly perceived ma-
and Engineering Research Council of Canada grant to Morris terial, which we discuss shortly, there are, to our knowledge,
Moscovitch.
We thank Maureen Patchett for help in preparing the manuscript.
no studies comparing performance on explicit and implicit
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to tests in normal people when performance on explicit tests
Morris Moscovitch, Department of Psychology, Erindale College, has been reduced to chance by forgetting. It is conceivable,
University of Toronto, Mississauga Road, Mississauga, Ontario, despite the rapid initial decay, that performance on recog-
Canada L5L 1C6. Electronic mail may be sent to momos@utorvm. nition remains above chance even after extremely long de-
bitnet. lays. Second, if performance on explicit tests can be reduced
148
REPETITION EFFECTS WITH LOW STIMULUS RECOGNITION 149
to chance, then it is still possible that no savings would be show robust savings on implicit tests of memory. If they
apparent on implicit tests. would, it would strengthen the claim that memory as in-
An example from a widely cited study by Kolers (1976) ferred from performance on some implicit tests is more
on the longevity of repetition effects speaks to the first resilient to forgetting than memory when it is tested explic-
point. In that study, Kolers found that subjects reread pages itly. On the other hand, this claim would be placed in doubt
of geometrically transformed script faster the second time by a failure to find preserved memory on implicit tests in
than the first, even though 13 to 15 months elapsed between normal people when recognition induced by forgetting is
presentations. To quote Kolers (1976), "Although small near chance. Such an outcome would favor the view that
[about 5%], the [repetition] effect is reliable and its signif- performance on both implicit and explicit tests depends, in
icance lies with its occurrence, not its size" (p. 559). What part, on gaining access to a common trace or engram. To
has been often overlooked is that performance on an explicit anticipate the results, we note at the outset that we had great
recognition test for the same material also remained signif- difficulty in reducing recognition to near chance by forget-
icantly above chance: 65% of passages were classified cor- ting. This itself is an interesting finding when compared
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
rectly as old or new. Kolers's statement, therefore, can be with the ease with which encoding manipulations can
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
applied with equal force to performance on the explicit achieve these results. In those few subjects in whom we
recognition tests in normal people. In marked contrast, the succeeded in reducing recognition substantially, perfor-
performance of amnesic patients on a comparable explicit mance on implicit tests also dropped significantly. The im-
test drops to chance by 2 hr, whereas that on the comparable plications of these results for theories of normal and patho-
implicit test is normal (Moscovitch et al., 1986). logical memory and forgetting are discussed when we
Two other findings are worth noting in Kolers's study. present our findings.
When compared with the scores achieved on immediate The design and procedure was similar for all of the ex-
testing (Kolers, 1973), performance on both implicit and periments we conducted. Each experiment consisted of four
explicit tests declined noticeably, although that on the ex- successive phases: a study phase, an interpolated interfering
plicit test was steeper. Second, overall performances on the task, an explicit recognition test, and an implicit memory
two tests correlated poorly with each other. It is important to test in which a repetition effect was sought.
emphasize, however, that not all repetition effects are as
In the study phase, subjects made lexical decisions to 240
long-lasting as those reported by Kolers. Some, such as stem
strings of letters that were evenly divided into words and
completion with nonunique solutions, disappear within min-
nonwords. Depending on the experiment, the recognition
utes or hours (Diamond & Rozin, 1984; Squire, Shimamura,
& Graf, 1987), which is much quicker than performance on procedure was either yes/no recognition or modified forced-
explicit tests of memory. However, if one considers only choice. Memory was next tested implicitly in a lexical de-
long-lasting repetition effects, the data reported by Kolers cision or perceptual identification task. The words in the
are similar to those reported by others in the literature (eg. implicit test were encountered in the study phase, only in the
Komatusu & Ohta, 1984; Mitchell & Brown, 1988; Sloman recognition phase, in both the study and recognition phase,
et al., 1988). In all instances, repetition effects decline or were new words that had never been presented in the
slowly in comparison with recognition, but performance on experiment up to that point. Repetition effects, a measure of
tests of recognition remain substantially above chance when savings on the implicit test, were calculated by computing
tested at the same intervals. the difference in latency or identification accuracy between
old, repeated items and new items encountered for the first
In contrast, recognition can be reduced to chance by per- time.
ceptually degrading the stimulus at study (Bonnano & Still-
ing, 1986; Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980; Mandler, Naka- Although items were not counterbalanced within experi-
mura, & Van Zandt, 1987; Seaman, Brody, & Kauff, 1983; ments, different words from the same pool were chosen at
Seaman, Marsh, & Brody, 1984) or by diverting attention random across Experiments 1,3, and 4 to form the different
from it (Bentin, Kutus, & Hillyard, 1990; Eich, 1984; conditions. For example, the set comprising the repeated
Merikle & Reingold, 1991). Despite chance recognition, words in one experiment was different from the set in the
performance on implicit tests remained significantly above other experiments. The same is true of the other conditions.
chance and, in many cases (Merikle & Reingold, 1991; The only exception is that, in Experiments 1 and 2, the same
Seaman et al., 1983), remained stable in the face of wide items were used in corresponding conditions in both exper-
variations in recognition. Comparison of the effects of en- iments. The words in the different conditions were equated
coding manipulations and forgetting on implicit and explicit for frequency, imageability, and length.
tests of memory only serves to bring our question into relief: The first three experiments were devoted to assessing the
Would performance on implicit tests in normal people re- relative sensitivity of two different explicit tests of memory,
main stable and above baseline as recognition is reduced to yes/no recognition and modified forced choice, and two
near chance levels by forgetting? If not, what accounts for different implicit tests, improvement in lexical decisions
the differences between the two types of studies? and in identification accuracy with repetition. On the basis
The purpose of the experiments reported in this article, of this information, a fourth experiment was conducted in an
therefore, was to try to reduce, by forgetting, performance attempt to lower performance on the explicit test even fur-
on explicit tests of memory to chance or near chance levels ther and determine what effect that manipulation had on
in normal people to determine whether they would still performance on the implicit test.
150 MORRIS MOSCOVITCH AND SHLOMO BENTIN
Table 2 Experiment 2
Mean Reaction Times (RTs) in the Implicit Test
(Experiment 1) In Experiment 1, memory was tested explicitly by a
yes/no recognition procedure in which the ratio of new to
Seen once old items was 2:1. The purpose of Experiment 2 was to
before
determine whether a modified forced-choice recognition
In Seen twice procedure would prove to be a more sensitive, explicit test
recognition before of memory, perhaps a more worthy rival to the implicit test.
Study Unrepeated In
Measure list words study CR FA Hit Miss The modification consisted of presenting the subjects with a
printed list of 120 items of which they had to choose 40 that
RT 592 615 568 577 540 573 556 they believed they had seen before.
SEM 24 29 22 27 23 33 31
Note. Mean RTs are expressed in milliseconds. CR = correct
rejection; FA = false alarm. Method
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
The results of Experiment 2 indicate that, insofar as the Procedure. The stimuli were the same as in Experiments 1
materials and procedures that we used are concerned, mod- and 2, as was their random assignment to each condition. As be-
ified forced choice is no more sensitive than yes/no recog- fore, Experiment 3 consisted of a study phase, an interpolated
nition as an explicit test of memory. Because we did not use task, an explicit recognition test, and an implicit test. The interpo-
forced choice in an easier recognition condition, as we did lated task consisted of trials during which subjects tried to iden-
for old/new recognition (Bentin & Moscovitch, 1990), we tify perceptually degraded words to establish an exposure dura-
tion threshold of 50% correct identification. In each trial, a word
cannot tell whether performance on the two types of recog-
was exposed, immediately followed by a mask consisting of
nition tests decayed at the same rate. We can conclude, @@@@@@@ that lasted for 500 ms. Following each trial, the
however, that under the comparable conditions of Experi- subject identified the word by naming. To establish the threshold,
ments 1 and 2, recognition as measured by the two types of lists of 20 trials each were prepared. The exposure time of the
tests is roughly equivalent. words in the first list was 45 ms. In the second list, the exposure
It is also worth noting that a similar repetition effect of time was 15 ms. On the basis of the subject's performance on
40-50 ms was found in both Experiments 1 and 2 and was these two lists, the exposure time for words in the next four lists
was increased and decreased until identification was about 50%
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
the same whether the words were seen only at study, only at
correct. The last list was used to test the reliability of the thresh-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
tified more poorly than any of the repeated old items (p < mal and severely impaired memory can serve both as a
.01), with no significant differences among the old items. yardstick against which the performance of our subjects can
The results of Experiment 3 show that the pattern of be compared and as a guide for interpreting the results of
repetition effects on perceptual identification resembles that this experiment.
on lexical decision, despite the fact that the tests themselves
are not similar and that performance is measured by accu- Method
racy in one test and by latency in the other. In both tests, the
repetition effect for old words was not affected by recency Subjects. Participants in Experiment 4 were 24 undergradu-
or by the number of repetitions. The resemblance between ates at the Hebrew University who met the same inclusion crite-
the two tests was remarkable in that both showed the small- ria as subjects in Experiments 1, 2, and 3. None of these subjects
est repetition effect for words that were correctly rejected at participated in the previous experiments.
recognition. Although subsequent studies might find differ- Stimuli and procedure. The stimuli were identical to those
used in Experiment 1, and assignment to each condition was ran-
ences in sensitivity between the two tests, at the moment dom. The procedure was the same as that of Experiment 1, ex-
there is no reason to regard one as being a more sensitive
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
cept that the delay between the interpolated task and the recogni-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Table 6 RT data from the two groups yielded a significant main ef-
Mean Reaction Times (RTs) in the Implicit Test fect of repetition, F(3, 66) = 12.34, MSe = 845, p < .0001.
(Experiment 4) The average repetition effect in the high and low d groups
Repeated words was 56 ms and 26 ms, respectively. A t test for indepen-
dent groups revealed that this difference was statistically
Measure Unrepeated words In study ]In recognition Both
reliable, f(22) = 2.46, p < .025. Subsequent comparisons
RT 556 527 518 512 between the two groups, in each of the conditions sepa-
SEM 13 12 13 10 rately, yielded significant group differences for items that
Note. Mean RTs are expressed in milliseconds. were previously presented only at study, f(22) = 4.08, p <
.05 and only at recognition, f(22) = 7.03, p < .01, but not
for the doubly repeated items (t < 1).
At first glance, it seems that the 24-hr lag between study Ideally, we would have preferred that recognition in the
and test did not significantly lower recognition and had lit- entire group be at least as low as in the sample of 12 sub-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
tle influence on the pattern of repetition effects on the im- jects. However, lowering recognition to chance is very dif-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
plicit test. Closer inspection of the data, however, revealed ficult. Before interpreting these results, it is important,
that about one third of the subjects had a a" below 0.60 therefore, to establish that the performance of the 12 sub-
(hits = 0.56, FA = 0.44). It takes over 100 trials, or obser- jects with poor recognition was not artifactual. First, these
vations, to distinguish a a" of 0 from one of 0.05. Because subjects were selected at random from a pool of under-
our value of a" was based on only 40 trials, we did not graduate volunteers. Their performance was in other ways
know for certain whether these 8 subjects were performing comparable with that of the other subjects. Their mean RT
above chance. It is a reasonable assumption, however, that for lexical decisions at study (551 ms) was not reliably dif-
their performance was perilously close to it. Repetition ef- ferent from that of the other group (609 ms; p > .1). There
fects for these 8 subjects are presented in Table 7. is no reason to believe that something other than poor
An ANOVA showed a significant main effect of repeti- memory accounts for their performance, although the
tion, F(3, 21) = 3.27, MSe = 628, p < .05. Post hoc shorter RTs among the poor recognizers may suggest a re-
Tukey-A tests revealed, however, that only doubly re- lationship between rapid encoding and forgetting. Second,
peated items yielded significant repetition effects. RTs to it is noteworthy that the largest proportion of subjects with
words presented only at study or only at recognition were poor memory, at least twice that in any other condition,
not significantly shorter than RTs to new items (minimum was found in Experiment 4, in which delay and interfer-
significant RT difference = 41.46 ms). ence were highest. This suggests that factors affecting for-
To increase the power of the statistical tests, four addi- getting rather than only encoding are responsible for sub-
tional subjects with a" less than 0.61 were added from Ex- jects' poor memory.
periment 1, thereby forming a group of 12 subjects with Moreover, there is already ample evidence in the litera-
mean a" of 0.37 (SD = .15, range = -.03 to .61; see low d ture that poor encoding, to the point that the subjects may
group, Figure 1). The hit and FA rates were .53 and .40, re- not be aware that a stimulus was presented, results in nor-
spectively. The RTs in this group were analyzed by mal, often long-lasting, repetition effects on implicit tests
ANOVA, which confirmed the reliability of the trend ob- of memory. Our stimuli, on the other hand, were clearly
served in the smaller group: a main effect of repetition, visible and certainly of sufficient duration for the subjects
F(3, 33) = 3.35, p < .01, with post hoc Tukey-A tests to respond with nearly perfect accuracy in lexical decision
(honestly significant difference = 41.5 ms) indicating that on the initial presentation. Poor encoding is not likely to
only doubly repeated words yielded shorter RTs than did
be the cause of the poor repetition effects seen in our sub-
new words. Repetition effects for words seen only once
jects. The most parsimonious explanation for their poor
were not reliable (p > .05).
performance is that they were more prone than others to
As a final test, the group of low d subjects was com- forget the target items.
pared with a group of 12 subjects (6 from Experiment 1
The results of Experiment 4 suggest, therefore, that
and 6 from Experiment 4) whose d was above 1 (mean
when recognition approaches chance levels, long-lasting
d= 1.26, SD = .17, range = 1.1 to 1.6). Hit and FA rates
repetition effects are substantially reduced or even elimi-
were .70 and .24, respectively. An ANOVA performed on
nated. For the group of subjects with a low recognition
performance (d < 0.61), repetition effects were absent for
Table 7 words seen only once, either at study or at recognition. In
Mean Reaction Times (RTs) in the Implicit Test for contrast, a single previous presentation was sufficient to
Subjects With Low d (Experiment 4) produce repetition effects that were not significantly re-
Seen once before duced over a period of 24 hr in subjects with a higher rec-
Unrepeated Seen twice ognition level (d > 1). In a similar manner, presenting
Measure words In study In recognition before
items twice, at study and at recognition, led to substantial
RT 540 521 515 497 repetition priming effects, even in subjects with poor mem-
SEM 22 24 30 16 ory, presumably because those items were remembered
Note. N = 8. Mean RTs are expressed in milliseconds. better.
REPETITION EFFECTS WITH LOW STIMULUS RECOGNITION 155
60-
Seen once in study
CO
50- Seen once in recognition
Seen twice
o
o
40- I
vt
20
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
£| 10-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
a
0
Low d High d
Figure 1. Mean differences in reaction time on the implicit test between new items and old items
seen at study, at recognition, or on both occasions for subjects with high d' and low d! scores on
explicit recognition tests in Experiments 1 and 4.
On the basis of evidence from our own study as well as The reduction in performance on implicit tests with poor
other studies, we can chart the time course of repetition ef- explicit recognition due to forgetting also contrasted with
fects for words relative to that for recognition. First, there findings from studies that manipulated encoding to lower
is a steep initial decline in repetition effects that occurs performance on explicit tests. As we noted, when perfor-
within seconds to minutes after stimulus presentation mance on explicit tests was reduced to chance by degrading
(Bentin & Moscovitch, 1988; Ratcliff et al., 1985; Sloman the stimulus at study or by diverting attention from it, rep-
et al., 1988; but see Dannenbring & Briand, 1982); this is etition effects were not significantly reduced but, as with
followed by a relatively long-lasting, stable period for rep- amnesics, remained reliably above baseline (Bentin in press;
etition effects in comparison with explicit recognition per- Bonnano & Stilling, 1986; Eich, 1984; Mandleret al., 1987;
formance, which gets steadily lower; finally, when recog- Merikle & Reingold, 1991; Seaman et al., 1983; Seaman et
nition approaches chance levels, repetition effects are also al., 1984).
noticeably reduced or fall to zero. Together, studies of normal and amnesic subjects suggest
that the following conditions must be met for explicit re-
membering: (a) The stimulus event must first be consciously
General Discussion and fully apprehended. The cited encoding studies suggest
that without full conscious apprehension of the stimulus at
The primary question addressed by our study was whether
encoding, explicit remembering is very ineffective. Con-
normal people would perform above baseline levels on im-
scious apprehension is not, however, a sufficient condition
plicit tests of memory when explicit recognition was re-
for explicit remembering, as the evidence from amnesic
duced to chance or near chance levels by forgetting. The
first point to note is that lowering recognition to that level in subjects indicates. Amnesic subjects can attend, perceive,
normal people proved to be difficult. This fact is not appre- and apprehend the stimuli, yet their conscious recollection is
ciated very much, as investigators are more impressed by severely impaired, (b) Clearly, an additional process is nec-
the initial rapid forgetting than by the longevity of memories essary to make this information available for long-term
as revealed by explicit recognition tests. This is a worthy explicit remembering. This process, often termed consoli-
topic for future research but not one that we can pursue in dation, is identified with the operation of the hippocampus
this article. and related limbic structures (Scoville & Milner, 1957;
Once recognition approaches chance levels, however, our Squire, 1987). In amnesic subjects, the deficit in conscious
evidence suggests that there are differences in implicit test recollection arises either because the apprehended informa-
performance between amnesic patients and normal people tion is not delivered to the hippocampal system or because
whose recognition was reduced by forgetting. Unlike am- that system is absent or malfunctioning. The information,
nesics who often have normal repetition effects in the ab- however, is not stored in the hippocampus, (c) Retrieval
sence of recognition (see, e.g., Moscovitch, 1985), normal cues are necessary to recover the consolidated information
subjects, who score at or near chance on recognition, have and deliver it to consciousness. Forgetting occurs either
repetition effects that are substantially reduced or even elim- because the consolidated information has decayed or be-
inated. cause retrieval cues are ineffective or both. These points and
156 MORRIS MOSCOVITCH AND SHLOMO BENTIN
the ones made in the discussion that follows are elaborated By this account, with increased forgetting, performance
in Moscovitch (1989, 1992) and Moscovitch and Umilta on explicit and implicit tests becomes driven primarily by
(1991) where a neuropsychological model of memory is the same effective cue. When recognition approaches
presented. chance or near chance levels, it is either because the stim-
None of these processes is necessary for storage and ulus item alone is no longer an effective cue for evoking the
retention of information on implicit tests. For normal per- memory trace or because the trace itself may have dissipated
formance on implicit tests, it is only necessary that the or been altered with time. Our finding of a drop in repetition
stimulus be picked up by neural structures necessary for effects as recognition approaches chance is more consistent
decoding it. The mere pickup of information by these struc- with the second alternative. Because there is no reason to
tures, even without full conscious apprehension (as the en- believe that the effective stimulus for reactivating the record
coding studies show), is sufficient to form long-term records (i.e., driving performance on implicit tests) is altered, the
or engrams in memory. These records are not consciously drop in repetition priming effects as recognition approaches
accessible.' To become accessible, the input must be both chance probably occurs because of a change in the engrams
consciously apprehended and delivered to the hippocampal
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
implies that recognition, even when it is based on familiar- of implicit memory performance. Bulletin of the Psychonomic
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
ity, is mediated by processes that are distinct from those Society, 28, 346-352.
mediating performance on implicit tests of memory. At the Bentin, S., Moscovitch, M., & Heth, I. (1992). Memory with and
moment, we must admit that there is little evidence in the without awareness: Performance and electrophysiological evi-
literature to distinguish among the alternative proposals. dence of savings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn-
Some findings, however, are more consistent with our pro- ing, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1270-1283.
posal and we review these briefly. Bonnano, G. A., & Stilling, N. A. (1986). Preference familiarity
It is significant to note that performance based only on and recognition after repeated brief exposures to random geo-
familiarity was well above chance (Gardiner, 1988; Gar- metric shapes. American Journal of Psychology, 99, 403^15.
diner & Java, 1990). This was true also, and especially, Cohen, J., & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention
of pattern analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing
when nonwords were used. By themselves, these results are how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207-210.
consistent with all of the proposals. When evidence from Dannenbring, G. L., & Briand, K. (1982). Semantic priming and
amnesic subjects is taken into account, however, it becomes the word repetition effect in a lexical decision task. Canadian
possible to distinguish between the predictions made by the Journal of Psychology, 36, 435-444.
various proposals. Performance on recognition tests can be Diamond, R., & Rozin, P. (1984). Activation of exiting memories
at chance in amnesics even when repetition effects are nor- in anterograde amnesia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93,
mal for the same material. In addition, repetition effects for 98-105.
nonwords are very difficult to obtain in amnesic subjects. If Eich, E. (1984). Memory for unattended events: Remembering
processes leading to recognition judgments based on famil- with and without awareness. Memory & Cognition, 12, 105-111.
iarity were the same as those mediating repetition effects on Gardiner, J. M. (1988). Functional aspects of recollective experi-
implicit tests of memory, we would expect the performance ence. Memory & Cognition, 16, 309-313.
of amnesics to resemble that of normal people more than it Gardiner, J. M., & Java, R. I. (1990). Recollective experience in
does. Such similarity as does exist between familiarity ef- word and nonword recognition. Memory & Cognition, 18, 2 3 -
30.
fects in recognition and repetition effects on implicit tests of
Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory
memory probably occurs because both are based on reacti- for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of
vation of the same engram or record. Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
Bentin and Moscovitch (1990; Bentin, Moscovitch, and 11, 501-518.
Heth, 1992) found electrophysiological evidence of repeti- Graf, P., Squire, R. L., & Mandler, G. (1984). The information that
tion effects in both implicit and explicit tests of memory. amnesic patients do not forget. Journal of Experimental Psy-
This result is consistent with the idea that a common record chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 501-518.
or engram is reactivated in both cases. The additional ob- Jacoby, L. L. (1983). Remembering the data: Analyzing interactive
processes in reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
servation that repetition effects on explicit and implicit tests Behavior, 22, 485-508.
are associated with different components of the evoked re- Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between
sponse suggests that different neural processes mediate their autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of
occurrence on the two types of tests. It would be important Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306-340.
to determine whether one is associated with recognition due Jacoby, L. L., & Witherspoon, D. (1982). Remembering without
to familiarity and the other with repetition effects on im- awareness. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 300-324.
plicit tests. Johnston, W. A., Dark, V. J., & Jacoby, L. L. (1985). Perceptual
To summarize, our finding, that performances on implicit fluency and recognition judgments. Journal of Experimental
and explicit tests are interdependent when explicit recogni- Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 3-11.
Kirsner, K., & Dunn, D. (1985). The perceptual record: A common
tion is very low, suggests that they are both mediated by
factor in repetition priming and attribute retention. In M. I.
common engrams. In implicit tests, only the record is ac- Posner & D. S. M. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance XI
cessed by using the repeated stimulus as a cue. In the (pp. 447-566). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
explicit test, reactivation of the entire memory trace is nec- Kolers, P. A. (1973). Remembering operations. Memory & Cog-
essary, a process which is performed by the hippocampal nition, 1, 347-355.
system and which requires conscious processing. Kolers, P. A. (1976). Reading a year later. Journal of Experimental
158 MORRIS MOSCOVITCH AND SHLOMO BENTIN
Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 554-565. recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114,
Komatusu, S.-L, & Ohta, N. (1984). Priming effects in word frag- 435-150.
ment completion for short and long retention intervals. Japanese Richardson-Klavehn, A., & Bjork, R. A. (1988). Measures of
Psychological Research, 26, 194-200. memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 475-543.
Kunst-Wilson, W. R., & Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discrim- Roediger, H. L. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without re-
ination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557— membering. American Psychologist, 45, 1043-1056.
558. Scarborough, D. L., Cortese, C , & Scarborough, H. S. (1977).
Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgement of previous oc- Frequency and repetition effects in lexical memory. Journal of
currence. Psychological Review, 87, 252-271. Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
Mandler, G., Nakamura, Y., & Van Zandt, B. J. S. (1987). Non- 3, 1-17.
specific effects of exposure on stimuli that cannot be recognized. Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current sta-
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and tus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
Cognition, 13, 646-648. and Cognition, 13, 501-518.
Masson, M. E. S., & Freedman, L. (1990). Fluent identification of Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after
repeated words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosur-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.