Emotions Voting Chile
Emotions Voting Chile
Emotions Voting Chile
http://doi.org/10.1590/1807-01912020263452
e-ISSN 1807-0191
Carolina Segovia1
Ricardo Gamboa2
How do citizens decide who to vote for in an election? Traditional answers focus on
the role of political knowledge, party identification, and evaluations of the past
performance of governments as explanatory variables. In this study we evaluate an
alternative argument: the role of emotions. Using data from a survey carried out
following the Chilean general elections of December 2017, this article investigates
the association of emotions with the vote for Sebastián Piñera, and how emotions
interact with other relevant factors that correlate with the vote. We conclude that in
Chile, together with party identification and the evaluation of past governments, the
emotions aroused by candidates are strongly associated with the voting decision.
Keywords: emotions; vote; party identification; political knowledge; retrospective
evaluations; Chile
Introduction3
How do citizens decide who to vote for in an election? Faced with the ballot paper,
people must mark their preference for any of the candidates appearing on it in a process
that lasts just a few seconds. Some voters will go through this process on election day,
while others will have made their decision in advance. The literature on the vote in
particular, on decision-making processes, or on the expression of opinions in politics in
general, is extensive. As we will discuss later, research in this area has shown that people
decide their vote according to basic predispositions like party identification (Campbell et
al., 1960; Zaller, 1992), or voters’ knowledge and evaluation of the candidates and their
programs or proposals (Downs, 1957; Fiorina, 1981; Popkin and Dimock, 1999; Zaller,
1992). However, in contexts in which citizens have a low level of relevant political
knowledge (Converse, 1964, 2000; Delli Carpini and Keeter, 1991; Gilens, 2012), and in
1
Political Science Department - Universidad Diego Portales. Santiago (Metropolitan), Chile.
E-mail: <[email protected]>.
2 Institute of International Studies - Universidad de Chile. Santiago (Metropolitan), Chile.
E-mail: <[email protected]>.
3 This work was financed by Conicyt/Fondecyt grant #1170335.
which levels of political identification have fallen significantly, as they have in consolidated
democracies (Andeweg and Farrell, 2017; Bargsted and Somma, 2016), neither of these
two theoretical currents seems sufficient to explain how people vote.
From the field of political psychology, however, a third explanation has been
proposed. Without denying the existence of voters who base their decision on basic
predispositions or on cognitive processes involving evaluation of the proposals and
candidates (Brader and Marcus, 2013; Groenendyk, 2011; MacKuen et al., 2007), it
contributes important elements to explain the processes of political decision-making. These
theories are based on the importance assigned to emotions and the role they play in the
expression of opinion and decision-making in politics (MacKuen et al., 2007; Neuman et
al., 2007; Valentino et al., 2011). Indeed, research has shown that when people make a
political decision or give a political opinion they make use, not only of what they know and
think, but also of what they feel about the different issues and political processes.
In this paper we study if and how, along with other traditional variables such as
party identification, evaluation of the outgoing government and political knowledge,
citizens rely on their emotions towards candidates in making the decision to vote. On the
one hand, we expect that emotions aroused by candidates are associated with the vote
(Lerner and Keltner, 2000): people are said to vote for those candidates who produce
positive emotions in them, and to avoid voting for those who provoke negative emotions
(Marcus, Neuman, and MacKuen, 2000). On the other hand, emotions are expected to have
an indirect role on the vote, by interacting with other determinants of the vote and
specifying the type of strategy that voters will use in their decision-making process
(MacKuen et al., 2007). Emotions, then, come into play as moderators of other cognitive
and affective considerations (such as party identification) that explain the voting decision
(Druckman and McDermott, 2008; Lerner et al., 2015, Marcus, Neuman and MacKuen,
2000). In this paper we investigate, therefore, how emotions correlate with the vote and
with other factors in citizens’ voting choices.
To test our hypotheses, we used the data obtained in a survey conducted after the
second round of the 2017 presidential election in Chile. We analyzed the relationship of
emotions with the decision to vote for the president-elect, Sebastián Piñera. Chile is an
excellent case to address the question posed. On the one hand, it is a consolidated
democracy in which electoral processes work smoothly. In fact, the country receives high
marks in comparative studies on democratic development (The Economist, 2018; Freedom
House, 2018). Second, it is a presidential democracy, which allows the analysis to focus
on a first-order election. Finally, as we explain later, appeals to emotions during the
electoral contest were present in the various campaigns, and particularly in that of Piñera.
Chile, therefore, is an appropriate case for the analysis presented here.
The results show that emotions toward a candidate are associated with the
probability of voting for that candidate. Our evidence indicates, as expected, that the
probability of a vote for Piñera increases in positive emotion scenarios and decreases in
contexts of negative emotions. We also observe that the emotions that show the greatest
association with the vote are anger and optimism, particularly the latter. Finally, the results
show that emotions also mediate the role that other factors play with regard to the vote
decision, the most important being the interaction between emotions and the evaluation
of the previous government. Overall, the results indicate that emotions are associated with
the voting decision and that they also mediate the correlation of other –and more
traditional– factors that explain the vote.
The article is organized as follows. In the first section, “Determinants of the vote:
three approaches”, we briefly discuss the literature on the factors that determine the vote,
and we develop an argument that incorporates emotions as relevant explanatory variables.
In the second section, we describe “Data and methods” used. Then we present and discuss
the “Results” obtained. We end with some “Conclusions”.
How do citizens make political decisions? How do they decide which candidate to
vote for? Given the centrality of the vote in democratic theory, the way in which citizens
decide how and for whom to vote has become a central research question. Democratic
theory sets high standards for citizens. According to Berelson, Lazarsfeld, and McPhee
(1954, p. 308), for example, the democratic citizen is expected to be well informed about
political issues in order to have the requisite knowledge to make good decisions. At the
same time, however, research results have shown that this ideal is far from met in election
periods (Gilens, 2012). In this context of low levels of information, two central theories
have been developed in political science to explain the vote (Redlawsk and Pierce, 2017).
Both models spring from the observation that citizens know very little about politics, and
that in general they do not measure up to the bar in political sophistication that seems
necessary for voting (Campbell et al., 1960).
In the first model it is postulated that the vote (and other political decisions)
depend on basic predispositions, particularly party identification, that have been learned
from an early age and that allow us to make decisions without much thought (Krampen,
2000; Sears and Brown, 2013; Zaller, 1992). Faced by ignorance of the candidates and
their public policy proposals, it is enough for the voter to know who represents the political
party they identify with and to choose in line with that predisposition (Druckman and Lupia,
2000; Gilens, 2012; Lau and Redlawsk, 2006; Lupia, 1994).
Research has shown consistently that party identification is one of the most
important determinants of voting, both in contexts of consolidated democracies (Green
and Baltes, 2017) and in countries with more recent democratic histories (Lupu, 2015).
Identification with a party learned in the first years of political socialization tends to have
lasting effects over time, allowing the vote to be predictable and stable from one election
to another, absent other variables (Dinas, 2017; Hetherington, 2016).
One problem with this model, however, is the assumption that voters –or at least
a majority of them– have in fact come to identify with some group, party or political
position. Where levels of political identification are low, as in the case of Chile (Bargsted
and Somma, 2016), this theory can explain only a small fraction of the preferences
expressed on election day. In fact, it has been observed that the vote is more unstable
and more difficult to predict in contexts where the party system is weak and generates few
ties with citizens that allow for stable preferences over time (Mainwaring and Torcal, 2006).
In these contexts, study of voting decisions requires the consideration of other factors that
shed light on the process.
The second model postulates that citizens consider rationally the electoral
alternatives presented to them, choosing among those representing or responding best to
their interests (Chong, 2013; Elster, 2007; Fiorina, 1981). In particular, the idea of an
economic vote is raised, in which people vote for those considered to be responsible for
past economic performance (the retrospective vote). In these rational choice theories,
there is also a need for less knowledge than that required by democratic theory, with
special emphasis on the definition of one's own interests as a threshold for the decision
(Kinder, 1998).
The evaluation of past economic performance or the previous government can also
be understood as a shortcut that reduces the need for information. In effect, it has been
shown that people use these evaluations to decide their vote: if the previous government
is well evaluated, the likelihood of voting for the same party increases. Likewise, if the
evaluation of previous government’s performance is poor, we would expect an increase in
the likelihood of voting for the opposing party (Fiorina, 1981). Evidence has been found
that provides important support for this theory (Lewis-Beck and Costa Lobo, 2017),
although its role seems to be mediated by other factors, including party identification and
the possibility of attributing responsibility to the government (Silva and Whitten, 2017).
However, its effectiveness also depends on the importance that citizens attribute to
economic management when deciding their vote (Gélineau and Singer, 2015). It has been
observed, for example, that other issues may be more relevant and may reduce the role
of retrospective voting (Pérez, 2015).
What the theory of party identification and economic voting share is the emphasis
they put on cognitive or rational aspects in the decision-making process. Although party
identification also includes affective elements (Campbell, 1960), emphasis has been placed
on its cognitive dimension. This emphasis on cognitive aspects has been debated in the
last 20 or 30 years and researchers have begun to include other variables in the analysis
of decision-making processes that take into consideration other characteristics of people.
Thus, it has been noted that emotions – and also other aspects such as personality – have
begun to be used in the field of political science to explain phenomena such as voting,
political participation, and opinions about groups, as well as other issues (Groenendyk,
2011).
Using voting theories that incorporate the role of emotions is important in this
context, as they allow inclusion in the decision of elements that go beyond individual
cognitive processes. In particular, the theory of affective intelligence (developed by
Marcus, Neuman and MacKuen, 2000), which is based on the appraisal theory of emotions
(Moors et al., 2013), and the theory developed by Lodge and Taber (2013) which is based
on research into online processing, motivated reasoning, and hot cognition (Lau and
Redlawsk, 2006; Lerner et al., 2015; Lodge and Taber, 2013), allow an explanation of the
vote that, without neglecting the factors mentioned above, incorporate them into a general
model in which emotions and affects play a central role (Brader, 2012; Brader and Marcus,
2013; Groenendyk, 2011). Although these theories differ in important respects, they agree
on the role that emotions have on the vote. Thus, following these studies, we argue in this
paper that emotions will have both a direct and an indirect association with the vote
(Redlawsk and Pierce, 2017; Brader, 2012).
Although or perhaps because emotions are part of our daily experiences, research
has been little consistent in defining them (Dixon, 2012; Mulligan and Scherer, 2012;
Scarantino, 2012; Demertzis, 2013). In general, however, we can find three central
elements of any definition. First, emotions are a response to an object or event that is
relevant to people: in other words, emotions are about something (Marcus, Neuman and
MacKuen, 2000). It is possible, therefore, that people have emotional responses to
candidates in an election, especially in one of major importance such as a presidential
contest. This has been confirmed by empirical research showing that people have
emotional reactions towards candidates (Abelson et al., 1982; Ottati, Steenbergen and
Riggle, 1992; Capelos, 2013), and that these emotions have consequences for their
attitudes and behaviors (Capelos, 2013). In other words, emotions towards candidates will
be independent of the final voting decision.
Second, emotions are aroused before we arrive at a conscious evaluation of those
same objects or events (Marcus, 2013), but for the former to be relevant regarding
attitudes and behaviors they must reach a state of consciousness (Marcus, Neuman and
MacKuen, 2000; Spezio and Adolphs, 2007). In this way, emotions can be related to the
way we evaluate different objects or events (Marcus, Neuman and MacKuen, 2000;
Capelos, 2013).
Finally, emotions are accompanied by the tendency to do something (whether the
action materializes or not). In other words, they are motivational impulses (Brader, 2012,
Marcus, Neuman and MacKuen, 2000, Neuman et al., 2007). This makes it reasonable to
suppose that emotions towards the candidates will be associated with the voting decision.
Specifically, it has been observed that certain emotions motivate political participation,
whether through voting (Valentino et al., 2011) or other political activities (Sabucedo and
Vilas, 2014; Weber, 2012; Valentino et al., 2011; Jasper, 2011, 2014), and the decision
about who to vote for (Marcus and MacKuen, 1993; Inbar et al., 2012).
However, not all emotions seem to be relevant for political action (Brader, 2012).
Indeed, research in this field has focused on the role of fear and anxiety4, as well as
enthusiasm or optimism, anger, and pride (Brader, 2012). Some authors analyze these
emotions grouped according to their valence (if they are positive or negative), assuming
that their characteristics and results will be similar (Lodge and Taber, 2013). We follow a
different strategy, by analyzing the characteristics and the role that each emotion has in
particular (Capelos, 2013). The analysis of discrete emotions has the advantage of
distinguishing the type of appraisal that is carried out and how these emotions are
transformed into political attitudes and behaviors (Huddy et al., 2005; Yates, 2016). In
addition, it has been found that emotions with the same valence, like anger and fear, have
different political consequences, indicating the need for a more systematic exploration of
these differences (Huddy, Feldman and Cassese, 2007).
With respect to fear, it is argued that this occurs in the presence of a threat to
people’s well-being. Fear is experienced on those occasions where there is uncertainty
about results and it can lead to citizens being more alert and more open to persuasion. It
can also motivate escape from danger or a lesser inclination to accept risks (Brader, 2012;
Lerner et al., 2015).
Anger, on the other hand, occurs when obstacles are seen to prevent the
attainment of certain ends, or when others are perceived to have been harmed
undeservedly. Anger, then, motivates people to remove obstacles, to punish aggressors,
to be more prone to take risks, and to make compromise less possible (Brader, 2012;
Lerner et al., 2015).
So, even though anger and fear are emotions that share the same valence and
even appear to be related in observational studies, their origins and consequence are
different (Huddy, Feldman and Cassese, 2007; Steenbergen and Ellis, 2006, p. 109-110;
Petersen, 2010). Steenbergen and Ellis (2006), using survey data in the United States, for
example, show that anger is a reaction to negative results such as an economic crisis or a
political scandal that is blamed on the president or a political leader who is being evaluated.
Fear, on the other hand, is generated in response to unusual or unexpected events (Rico,
Guinjoan and Anduiza, 2017).
Enthusiasm or optimism, and pride, finally, are related emotions that have been
less studied (Brader and Marcus 2013). The first occurs when progress is observed towards
desired objectives. Pride emerges, on the other hand, when the objectives have been
achieved, generating satisfaction (Yates, 2016). In the field of political action, enthusiasm
and pride can increase motivation to pursue desired ends, lessen willingness to evaluate
4 Fear and anxiety are used as equivalent concepts in the literature as we also do here.
alternatives carefully, and inspire greater confidence in what is observed (Brader, 2012;
Lerner et al., 2015).
Based on these definitions and on results observed in previous research, our first
set of hypotheses refers to the direct role that these emotions will have on the vote. In
general, we argue that emotions have a direct association with the decision of who to vote
for. The emotions experienced towards the candidates, therefore, will be correlated with
the election of the preferred candidate and the vote.
Fear occurs when a threat to wellbeing is perceived, generating a search for an
escape from the threat. In an election, the way to escape from a threat is by voting for the
opposing candidate. If a candidate is perceived as a threat, therefore, we expect the
probability of voting for that candidate to diminish (Ottati, Steenbergen and Riggle, 1992;
Miller, 2011; Druckman and McDermott, 2008).
H1a. The probability of voting for a candidate decreases when the candidate
arouses fear.
Anger produced by the perception of obstacles or undeserved harm, on the other
hand, leads to a search for the obstacle to be eliminated. If a candidate generates anger
among the voters, therefore, we expect the probability of voting for that candidate to also
diminish (Ottati, Steenbergen and Riggle, 1992; Miller, 2011; Druckman and McDermott,
2008).
H1b. The probability of voting for a candidate decreases when the candidate
arouses anger.
If a candidate generates optimism or pride in the voters, people are expected, for
their part, not to carefully evaluate the proposals of other candidates, to have greater
confidence in the candidate in question, and therefore seek to maintain and continue with
the desired objectives. Specifically, then, we expect that the probability of voting for a
candidate is greater among those who feel pride or optimism towards that candidate
(Miller, 2011; Yates, 2016; Just, Crigler and Belt, 2007; Druckman and McDermott, 2008).
H1c. The probability of voting for a candidate increases when the candidate
arouses pride.
H1d. The probability of voting for a candidate increases when the candidate
arouses optimism.
Research has also shown that emotions are indirectly associated with the vote.
Indirect associations are those that occur when, aroused by different emotions, the role of
other variables on the vote changes (MacKuen et al., 2007). In other words, emotions will
also serve to alter the role of other cognitive considerations (knowledge, party identification
and retrospective evaluation, in this case) that explain the choice of vote (Druckman and
McDermott, 2008).
On the other hand, Marcus, Neuman and MacKuen (2000), for example, have
shown that fear about presidential candidates is associated with greater concern over the
outcome of the election, more interest in the campaign, and more attention to news in the
media (Ladd and Lenz, 2008; Marcus and MacKuen, 1993). The decision on the vote, then,
would be based on a search of information and on cognitive processes that allow a more
informed and reasoned decision to be made. In this way, it is expected that those emotions
that activate the "surveillance system" will lead to a greater importance of political
knowledge (Parker and Isbell, 2010; Ladd and Lenz, 2008; Weeks, 2015).
Based on these results, then, we expect that party identification and retrospective
evaluations will play a more important role among those who express enthusiasm and pride
toward the candidate. For those who feel anger or fear toward the candidate, we expect to
find that knowledge has greater role on the vote.
H2a. In the presence of optimism or pride toward the candidate, the importance
of partisan identification and retrospective evaluations on the vote will be greater than in
the absence of such emotions.
H2b. In the presence of fear or anger toward the candidate, the importance of
knowledge on the vote will be greater than in the absence of such emotions.
For the purposes of this study, some features of the 2017 presidential election need
to be mentioned. First, the current president Sebastián Piñera was elected after two rounds
(in Chile a ballotage system is used), since none of the eight participants in the first round
obtained an absolute majority. Second, the range of contenders was particularly broad,
with the participation of candidates of different ideological tendencies, from the far right
to candidates with a very clear leftist position (Toro and Valenzuela, 2018). Third, even
though all the candidates presented a clear electoral program, in fact the debate on specific
public policies played a secondary role. This sparse debate revolved mainly around the
success or failure of certain reforms implemented by the Bachelet government (education
and taxation) and whether or not they needed to be modified.
In this context, Piñera focused much of his campaign on highlighting the negative
effects of Bachelet's administration, especially with respect to economic growth, which was
weak between 2014 and 2018. Hence, the main slogan of his campaign was "Lift up your
hearts ... better times are coming," which is also a clear appeal to voters’ optimism and
enthusiasm. At the same time, it is worth noting that Piñera made an extensive use of
images and symbols that seek to arouse pride among voters. For example, the use of the
Chilean flag and other patriotic symbols in all of his public appearances. He also constantly
appealed to pride with respect to his first government’s achievements
Participants
For this study we use the results obtained in the Emociones y Política (Emotions
and Politics)(Segovia and Gamboa, 2018) survey, which was conducted immediately after
the second round of the presidential election. This survey represents Chile’s population
over 18 years of age living in the country and includes both urban and rural areas. Two
thousand people aged 18 and over were interviewed in their homes between December
18, 2017 and January 31, 2018. Sampling error was estimated at +/– 2.2% on the
assumption of maximum variance and for a 95% confidence level.
The sample selection method was probabilistic and proportional to population for
each of the stages in which it was carried out: at a first stage, 110 communes were
selected. In each region of the country the communes were ranked by population from the
largest to the smallest; all the communes with more than 50,000 inhabitants (88 in
number) were automatically included and a random sample of approximately 22
communes with less than 50,000 inhabitants was selected. The second stage involved the
selection of residential blocks, which were chosen by stratified random sampling, with a
probability of selection proportional to the size of the unit, in terms of the number of private
homes it contained. In the third stage, private dwellings within each block were selected
by systematic random sampling. Finally, the person to interview in each dwelling was
5 According to data from “Centro de Estudios Públicos” (CEP) surveys, for example, while in 1991 68% of
respondents identified themselves with a political party, by the end of 2017 that percentage had fallen to
24%.
selected by using a Kish Table, which ensures that those eligible are chosen by a random
procedure that ensures equal chances of selection.
The data collection was carried out through the application of face-to-face personal
interviews in the participants’ homes. These interviews were voluntary, the participants
were informed that the results would be published, and they were assured of the
confidentiality of personal information.
Measurement
Bachelet did over the last four years?". It is important to note that Sebastián Piñera is from
the coalition opposed to the Bachelet government, so we expect there to be a negative
relationship between a favorable evaluation of that government and the probability of a
vote for Piñera. We computed a dummy variable indicating those who considered Bachelet's
government to be good or very good (M = 0.58, SD = 0.494).
Control variables. We included variables measuring the sex (M = 0.48, SD = 0.50),
age (M = 44.49, SD = 17.269), years of schooling (M = 12.02, SD = 4.063), and
socioeconomic status (M = 4.77, SD = 1.54) of respondents.
Data Analysis
To evaluate the direct associations of emotions with the vote, we conducted two
logistic regression models on the vote for Sebastián Piñera: one for the vote in the first
round of the election, and another for the second round. To evaluate the indirect role of
emotions, we estimated the marginal effects of the interaction between emotions and other
relevant variables, and voting.
Results
What factors help to explain the vote for Piñera in the first and second rounds of
the presidential election? As we noted earlier, the main theories of voting hold that citizens
decide (a) according to their party identification and (b) according to their retrospective
evaluation of the government or the economy. Both explanations require citizens to have
some political knowledge. However, as in other consolidated democracies, Chilean voters
declare a very low level of identification with parties and have little political knowledge,
which leads to the conclusion that these two explanations on their own would be
insufficient. Indeed, the results obtained show that 68% of respondents do not identify
with any political coalition and that 50% do not answer correctly any of the questions
included to measure political knowledge.
Chilean voters do mention, on the other hand, having felt emotions toward the
candidates during the campaign. Regarding Sebastián Piñera, 29% felt fear, 41% felt
anger, 40% felt optimism and 32% felt pride. In general, 73% of those interviewed said
they felt at least one of these emotions towards the candidate.
To measure the association of emotions and other variables with the vote decision,
we perform a logistic regression of them against the vote for Sebastián Piñera in the first
and second electoral rounds. Table 1 presents the results obtained (logistic regression
coefficients):
Table 1
Logistic regression model on the vote for Piñera
First Round Second Round
Coef. Robust Std. Coef. Robust Std.
Error Error
Evaluation of previous -0.990*** 0.209 -1.239*** 0.206
government
Party ID (1 = “Chile Vamos”) 1.450*** 0.282 2.264*** 0.356
Political knowledge -0.340* 0.140 -0.194 0.133
Fear -1.167** 0.344 -0.780** 0.265
Anger -1.488*** 0.287 -1.587*** 0.238
Optimism 1.673*** 0.287 2.199*** 0.272
Pride 1.001*** 0.281 1.056*** 0.281
Gender (1 = male) -0.051 0.216 -0.215 0.209
Age 0.005 0.006 -0.007 0.007
Years of schooling -0.057 0.031 0.003 0.032
Socioeconomic status 0.232** 0.068 0.174* 0.076
Constant -1.615* 0.640 -0.733 0.674
N 1121 1131
Wald chi2 304.80*** 302.68***
Pseudo R2 0.472 0.537
Source: Emotions and Politics Survey, Chile, Dec. 2017 – Jan. 2018 (Segovia and Gamboa, 2018).
* p < 0,05; ** p < 0,01; *** p < 0,001.
Based on the results shown in Table 1, we also computed the change in the
probability of voting for Piñera when each variable changes from its minimum to its
maximum value6. These results show that emotions towards the candidate are strongly
correlated with the probability of a vote for Piñera, both in the first and in the second
round. The results obtained for the first round indicate that the predicted probability of a
vote for Piñera is 0.21 greater for individuals who felt optimism, and 0.12 greater for those
who felt pride for the candidate. At the same time, the predicted probability of a vote for
Piñera is 0.16 smaller for those in whom the candidate inspired anger and 0.12 smaller for
those in whom he inspired fear. In other words, having felt emotions such as optimism and
pride in Piñera increase the probability of a vote for him, ceteris paribus, while fear and
anger decrease that probability in a statistically significant way, and in considerable
magnitudes.
In the case of the second round, the predicted probability of a vote for Piñera is
0.30 greater for those who felt optimism and 0.12 greater for those who felt pride in him.
Thus, optimism increases in importance as a determinant of the vote in the second round,
while the importance of pride remains constant between one round and another. Regarding
the role of anger and fear, the predicted probability of voting for Piñera is 0.17 smaller for
those who felt anger and 0.07 for those who felt fear. As far as these emotions are
concerned, then, in the case of anger the association remains constant between the two
6We estimated average marginal effects (AME) that is the mean of the marginal effects. According to Garson
(2016), this measure is preferred when using factor variables, as we do here.
electoral rounds, while fear has a weaker association to the vote in the second round.
Overall, emotions have associations with the vote that are statistically significant and
substantial in terms of their magnitude.
The associations of traditional variables, moreover, also show the expected results:
controlling for other factors, the probability of a vote for Piñera increases among those who
identify with the right-wing alliance “Chile Vamos” (Average Marginal Effects (AME) = 0.17
in the first round, AME = 0.26 in the second round), and decreases significantly among
those who express a positive evaluation of the Bachelet government (AME = – 0.11 and
– 0.13 in the first and second rounds, respectively). The level of knowledge turns out to
be of little importance. The coefficient is negative for both the first and second rounds,
with those who voted for Piñera having, on average, less political knowledge. The
coefficient, however, is statistically significant only for the first electoral round (AME = -
0.03).
The control variables show no statistically significant associations with the vote,
with the exception of the socioeconomic level of the interviewees. The probability of a vote
for Piñera increases among those located higher on the social scale. These data are
consistent with results obtained in other investigations that show that the vote for right-
wing parties in Chile is higher in the most affluent sectors7.
7
It might be argued that there is a problem of endogeneity between our dependent and independent
variables. Research published elsewhere clearly discard this problem (see, for example, Marcus and Mackuen,
1993; Ottati, Steenbergen and Riggle, 1992; Inbar et al., 2012; Capelos, 2013; Huddy et al., 2005;
Steenbergen and Ellis, 2006; Petersen, 2010). Nevertheless, we conducted 2SLS analysis of the data, testing
different possible instrumental variables. We validated these instruments using Stata command estat endog.
The results indicate that no endogeneity is observed in the data, so the hypothesis is rejected. Results are
available upon request.
Table 2
Logistic regression model on the vote for Piñera,
with interactions (standard errors omitted)
First Round Second Round
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Eval. of previous -0.992*** -0.954*** -1.185** -1.253*** -1.233*** -1.217***
government
Party ID (1 = 1.481** 1.434*** 1.478*** 2.040*** 2.261*** 2.318***
“Chile Vamos”)
Political -0.325* -0.659* -0.362* -0.183 -0.256 -0.225
knowledge
Fear -1.240** -1.040* -0.784 -0.843** -0.742* -1.081*
Anger -1.680*** -1.374*** -1.312** -1.664*** -1.683*** -0.990**
Optimism 1.715*** 1.570*** 0.856* 2.222*** 2.255*** 1.362***
Pride 1.027** 0.716* 1.668*** 1.083*** 0.962* 1.937***
Party ID * Fear 0.257 0.665
Party ID * Anger 0.878 0.696
Party ID * -0.379 -0.067
Optimism
Party ID * Pride -0.037 -0.427
Knowledge * -0.201 -0.044
Fear
Knowledge * -0.138 0.142
Anger
Knowledge * 0.191 -0.083
Optimism
Knowledge * 0.451 0.155
Pride
Previous Govt. * -0.849 0.580
Fear
Previous Govt. * -0.322 -1.100*
Anger
Previous Govt. * 1.731** 1.720**
Optimism
Previous Govt. * -1.420* -1.819**
Pride
Gender (1 = -0.052 -0.042 -0.076 -0.217 -0.220 -0.223
male)
Age 0.005 0.005 0.005 -0.007 -0.007 -0.008
Years of -0.054 -0.055 -0.055 0.003 0.003 0.002
schooling
Socioeconomic 0.224** 0.234** 0.255** 0.173* 0.175* 0.171*
status
Constant -1.595* -1.514* -1.486* -0.696 -0.711 -0.654
types of associations. As expected, in the case of the interaction between the evaluation
of Bachelet’s government and pride, the results indicate a statistically significant
coefficient. The likelihood of voting for Piñera is higher when voters feel proud of the
candidate and evaluate the previous government negatively. For those who have a good
evaluation of Bachelet’s government, on the other hand, there is no change in the
probability of voting for Piñera.
On the other hand, the interaction of the evaluation of previous government with
optimism toward the candidate is statistically significant in both elections, but it indicates
an association in the opposite direction to what was expected. The results indicate that the
likelihood of voting for Piñera is higher when the candidate arouses optimism. In the
absence of optimism towards Piñera, a positive evaluation of previous government
decreases the likelihood of voting for Piñera. The interaction with fear is not statistically
significant, in both rounds of the elections. In the case of the interaction with anger, the
interaction is statistically significant only for the second round, indicating that the likelihood
of voting for Piñera increases when voter do not express anger and they have a negative
evaluation of Bachelet’s government.
The interactions between emotions and party identification, on the other hand, are
not statistically significant. The general results hold: a vote for Piñera is more likely among
those who felt optimism and pride for the candidate, and less likely among those who felt
fear and anger. The results also show that the association of emotions with the vote,
especially of optimism and anger, occur both among those who identify with “Chile Vamos”
and among those who do not identify with this political coalition. In other words, the
interaction between emotions and political identification reinforces the importance of both
variables, producing direct and indirect associations with the vote. In the case of pride and
fear, the associations are statistically significant only for those who do not identify with the
political coalition, respectively reinforcing and diminishing the probability of voting for
Piñera. Finally, it is also important to stress that these results occur both in the first and in
the second round of the elections. In other words, the relationship between emotions and
political identification appears to remain quite stable regardless of the electoral context in
which the decision is made.
Finally, regarding the association between emotions and political knowledge, and
the vote choice, the results show that these interactions are not statistically significant.
Therefore, H2b is not supported by the data. The lack of significant interactions between
political knowledge and fear might be due to the extremely low levels of knowledge as
measured by the survey.
Conclusions
In this work we investigate the direct and indirect relationships of emotions and
the vote choice. We argue that emotions are associated with the vote decision, and that
they provide new considerations for the study of voting in contexts of low levels of political
identification and knowledge. We also argue that emotions have indirect associations with
the vote, which are expressed in the interaction between emotions and other variables that
are traditionally used in research in this area. The research is based on results obtained in
a public opinion survey, conducted with a representative sample of voters after the second
round of the 2017 presidential election in Chile. The results obtained provide evidence that
supports the hypotheses proposed above.
The first set of hypotheses referred to the direct associations of emotions and the
vote. They predicted that the probability of voting for a candidate would decrease if the
voter had felt fear or anger towards him or her during the campaign (H1a and H1b), while
the probability of the vote would increase if the voter had felt pride or optimism (H1c and
H1d). The evidence supports these hypotheses, showing that the vote is directly related to
the emotions that the candidates arouse in voters, and in the direction expected.
Additionally, the results show that the association of emotions with the vote can be
observed in similar magnitudes in different, but related, electoral contexts.
We also argue that not all emotions have the same explanatory capacity. Thus, the
results indicate that the emotions with the strongest association with the vote are anger
and optimism, and that fear and pride are less important. These results are interesting if
we consider, on the one hand, the directionality of emotions and, on the other, the
temporality on which they are based. The results indicate that both positive emotions (such
as optimism) and negative emotions (such as anger) towards candidates affect the
probability of voting for them. There is no exclusive role, then, of emotions with a single
directionality on the vote; rather, emotions with different valences will be relevant to
different voters.
Regarding the temporality of emotions, anger and pride are emotions that originate
in the evaluation of a past event, while optimism and fear are associated with what is
expected in the future. The results obtained show that the emotions most relevant for
voting are both oriented towards the past (anger) as well as oriented towards the future
(optimism).
The second set of hypotheses referred to the indirect role of emotions, measured
in terms of their interaction with other important variables to explain the vote. The
evidence provides partial support for these hypotheses, indicating that only the interaction
of optimism and pride with the evaluation of previous government presents statistically
significant coefficients. Substantively, the results indicate that, in the presence of pride,
the association of the previous government’s evaluation with the vote is greater, as
expected. On the other hand, however, the association of the previous government’s
evaluation with the vote is greater in the absence of optimism.
In considering these results, two other points should be highlighted. In the first
place, one must ask what these results suggest in the current context of the Chilean
political system and its future development. The evidence provided should be considered
carefully. Although emotions are not the only explanatory variable of the vote in Chile as
we have stressed, it is certainly clear that they matter. And their importance can only
increase with the events of the second half of 2019, in which trust and identification with
representative institutions, especially the parties, reached an all-time low. This, in turn,
has been reflected in a low capacity to lead the political process. In this sense, it cannot
be ruled out that, in the future, spaces will increase for the emergence of leaderships that
seek to develop structures or forms of representation different from those that until now
have predominated in the Chilean political system. And in which the recourse to emotions
occupies a central place as the basis of the relationship between representatives and
represented.
In the second place, these results open the door to some interesting questions for
future research. First, if political action can be related to both positive and negative
affective evaluation of candidates, it is important to address the question of what voter
characteristics lead to the greater or lesser relevance of positive or negative emotions in
their vote. It is also important, secondly, to consider how emotions affect other decisions
or political actions that citizens may take. To what extent are the vote and other decisions
such as participation in protests, party membership, etc., associated with positive or
negative emotions? Third, it is important to consider when the past or the future, and the
emotions they arouse, are more relevant to the vote, and to what extent those who vote
in response to different types of emotions differ. Finally, it is important to advance in
determining if these results are replicated or not in other electoral contexts or in other
types of political decision.
References
ABELSON, R. P., et al. “Affective and semantic components in political person perception”. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 42, nº 4, p. 619-663, 1982.
ANDEWEG, R. B.; FARRELL, D. M. Legitimacy decline and party decline. In: VAN HAM, C., et al. (eds.).
Myth and reality of the legitimacy crisis. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2017.
BARGSTED, M.; SOMMA, N. “Social cleavages and political dealignment in contemporary Chile, 1995-
2009”. Party Politics, vol. 22, nº 1, p. 105-124, 2016.
BRADER, T. The emotional foundations of democratic citizenship. In: BERINSKY, A. J. (ed.). New
directions in public opinion. New York: Routledge, 2012.
BRADER, T.; MARCUS, G. E. Emotion and political psychology. In: HUDDY, L.; SEARS, D. O.; LEVY, J. S.
(eds.). The Oxford handbook of political psychology. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2013.
CAMPBELL, A., et al. The American voter. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1960.
CAPELOS, T. Understanding anxiety and aversion: the origins and consequences of affectivity in
political campaigns. In: DEMERTZIS, N. (ed.). Emotions in politics – The affect dimension in political
tension. New York: Palgrave Macmillan, 2013.
CHONG, D. Degrees of rationality in politics. In: HUDDY, L.; SEARS, D. O.; LEVY, J. S. (eds.). The
Oxford handbook of political psychology. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2013.
CONVERSE, P. E. The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In: APTER, D. E. (ed.). Ideology and
discontent. New York: The Free Press of Glencoe, 1964.
________ . “Assessing the capacity of mass electorates”. Annual Review of Political Science, vol. 3,
p. 331-153, 2000.
DELLI CARPINI, M. X.; KEETER, S. “Stability and change in the U.S. public’s knowledge of politics”.
Public Opinion Quarterly, vol. 55, p. 583-612, 1991.
DEMERTZIS, N. Theorizing the emotions-politics nexus. In: DEMERTZIS, N. (ed.). Emotions in politics –
The affect dimension in political tension. New York: Palgrave Macmillan, 2013.
DINAS, E. The evolving role of partisanship. In: ARZHEIMER, K.; EVANS, J.; LEWIS-BECK, M. S. (eds.).
The Sage handbook of electoral behaviour. Los Angeles: Sage, 2017.
DIXON, T. “Emotion: the history of a keyword in crisis”. Emotion Review, vol. 4, nº 4, p. 338-344,
2012.
DRUCKMAN, J. N.; LUPIA, A. “Preference formation”. Annual Review of Political Science, vol. 3, p. 1-24,
2000.
DRUCKMAN J. N.; MCDERMOTT, R. “Emotion and the framing of risky choice”. Political Behavior, vol. 30,
nº 3, p. 297-321, 2008.
ELSTER, J. Explaining social behavior: more nuts and bolts for the social sciences. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press, 2007.
FIORINA, M. Retrospective voting in American national elections. New Haven: Yale University Press,
1981.
FREEDOM HOUSE. Freedom in the world 2018. Democracy in crisis. Freedom House, 2018. Available
at: <https://freedomhouse.org/report/freedom-world/freedom-world-2018.> Access: 12 jan. 2019.
GÉLINEAU, F.; SINGER, M. M. The economy and incumbent support in Latin America. In: CARLIN, R. E.;
SINGER, M. M.; ZECHMEISTER, E. J. (eds.). The Latin American voter: pursuing representation and
accountability in challenging contexts. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press, 2015.
GILENS, M. Two-thirds full? Citizen competence and democratic governance. In: BERINSKY, A. J. (ed.).
New directions in public opinion. New York: Routledge, 2012.
GREEN, D. P.; BALTES, S. Party identification: meaning and measurement. In: ARZHEIMER, K.; EVANS,
J.; LEWIS-BECK, M. S. (eds.). The Sage handbook of electoral behaviour. Los Angeles: Sage, 2017.
GROENENDYK, E. “Current emotion research in political science: how emotions help democracy
overcome its collective action problem”. Emotion Review, vol. 3, nº 4, p. 455-463, 2011.
HUDDY, L., et al. “Threat, anxiety, and support of antiterrorism policies”. American Journal of Political
Science, vol. 49, p. 593-608, 2005.
HUDDY, L.; FELDMAN, S.; CASSESE, E. On the distinct political effects of anxiety and anger. In: CRIGLER,
A., et al. (eds.). The political dynamics of feeling and thinking. Chicago: University of Chicago Press,
2007.
INBAR, Y., et al. “Disgust sensitivity, political conservatism, and voting”. Social Psychological and
Personality Science, vol. 3, nº 5, p. 537-544, 2012.
JASPER, J. M. “Emotions and social movements: twenty years of theory and research”. Annual Review
of Sociology, vol. 37, p. 1-9, 2011.
________. “Constructing indignation: anger dynamics in protest movements”. Emotion Review, vol.
6, nº 3, p. 208-213, 2014.
JUST, M. R.; CRIGLER, A. N.; BELT, T. L. Don’t give up hope: emotions, candidate appraisals, and votes.
In: NEUMAN, W. R., et al. The affect effect: dynamics of emotion in political thinking and behavior.
Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 2007.
KINDER, D. R. “Communication and opinion”. Annual Review of Political Science, vol. 1, p. 167-197,
1998.
LADD, J.; LENZ, G. S. “Reassessing the role of anxiety in vote choice”. Political Psychology, vol. 29, nº
2, p. 275-296, 2008.
LAU, R. R.; REDLAWSK, D. P. How voters decide: information processing during election campaigns.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2006.
LERNER, J. S., et al. “Emotion and decision making”. Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 66, p. 799-
823, 2015.
LEWIS-BECK, M. S.; COSTA LOBO, M. The economic vote: ordinary vs. extraordinary times. In:
ARZHEIMER, K.; EVANS, J.; LEWIS-BECK, M. S. (eds.). The Sage handbook of electoral behaviour. Los
Angeles: Sage, 2017.
LODGE, M.; TABER, C. S. The rationalizing voter. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2013.
LUPIA, A. “Shortcuts versus encyclopedias: information and voting behavior in California insurance
reform elections”. American Political Science Review, vol. 88, nº 1, 63-76, 1994.
LUPU, N. Partisanship in Latin America. In: CARLIN, R. E.; SINGER, M. M.; ZECHMEISTER, E. J. (eds.). The
Latin American voter: pursuing representation and accountability in challenging contexts. Ann Arbor:
The University of Michigan Press, 2015.
MACKUEN, M., et al. The third way: the theory of affective intelligence and American democracy. In:
NEUMAN, W. R., et al. (eds.). The affect effect: dynamics of emotion in political thinking and
behavior. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 2007.
MAINWARING, S.; TORCAL, M. Party system institutionalization and party system theory after the third
wave of democratization. In: KATZ, R. S.; CROTTY, W. (eds.). Handbook of party politics. London:
Sage Publications, 2006.
MARCUS, G. E. The theory of affective intelligence and liberal politics. In: DEMERTZIS, N. (ed.).
Emotions in politics – The affect dimension in political tension. New York: Palgrave Macmillan, 2013.
MARCUS, G. E.; MACKUEN, M. “Anxiety, enthusiasm and the vote: the emotional underpinnings of
learning and involvement during presidential campaigns”. American Political Science Review, vol. 87,
nº 3, p. 672-685, 1993.
MARCUS, G. E.; NEUMAN, W. R.; MACKUEN, M. Affective intelligence and political judgment. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press, 2000.
MOORS, A., et al. “Appraisal theories of emotion: state of the art and future development”. Emotion
Review, vol. 5, nº 2, p. 119-124, 2013.
MULLIGAN, K.; SCHERER, K. R. “Toward a working definition of emotion”. Emotion Review, vol. 4, nº 4,
p. 345-357, 2012.
NEUMAN, W. R., et al. Theorizing affect’s effects. In: NEUMAN, W. R., et al. (eds.). The affect effect:
dynamics of emotion in political thinking and behavior. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press,
2007.
OTTATI, V.; STEENBERGEN, M.; RIGGLE, E. “The cognitive and affective components of political attitudes:
measuring the determinants of candidate evaluations”. Political Behavior, vol. 14, nº 4, p. 423-442,
1992.
PARKER, M. T.; ISBELL, L. M. “How I vote depends on how I feel: the differential impact of anger and
fear on political information processing”. Psychological Science, vol. 21, nº 4, p. 548-550, 2010.
PÉREZ, O. J. The impact of crime on voter choice in Latin America. In: CARLIN, R. E.; SINGER, M. M.;
ZECHMEISTER, E. J. (eds.). The Latin American voter: pursuing representation and accountability in
challenging contexts. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press, 2015.
POPKIN, S. L.; DIMOCK, M. A. Political knowledge and citizen competence. In: ELKIN, S. K.; SOLTAN, K.
E. (eds.). Citizen competence and democratic institutions. University Park: Penn State University
Press, 1999.
REDLAWSK, D. P.; CIVETTINI, A. J. W.; LAU, R. R. Affective intelligence and voting: information
processing and learning in a campaign. In: NEUMAN, W. R., et al. (eds.). The affect effect: dynamics
of emotion in political thinking and behavior. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 2007.
REDLAWSK, D. P.; PIERCE, D. R. Emotions and voting. In: ARZHEIMER, K.; EVANS, J.; LEWIS-BECK, M. S.
(eds.). The Sage handbook of electoral behaviour. Los Angeles: Sage, 2017.
RICO, G.; GUINJOAN, M.; ANDUIZA, E. “The emotional underpinnings of populism: how anger and fear
affect populist attitudes”. Swiss Political Science Review, vol. 23, nº 4, p. 444-461, 2017.
SABUCEDO, J. M.; VILAS, X. “Anger and positive emotions in political protest”. Universitas Psychologica,
vol. 13, nº 3, p. 829-838, 2014.
SEARS, D. O.; BROWN, C. Childhood and adult political development. In: HUDDY, L.; SEARS, D. O.;
LEVY, J. S. (eds.). The Oxford handbook of political psychology. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University
Press, 2013.
SEGOVIA, C.; GAMBOA, R. “Primera Encuesta Nacional Emociones y Política en Chile”. [Archivo de
Datos]. Santiago: Proyecto Fondecyt 1170335, 2018.
SILVA, T.; WHITTEN, G. D. Clarity of responsibility and vote choice. In: ARZHEIMER, K.; EVANS, J.; LEWIS-
BECK, M. S. (eds.). The Sage handbook of electoral behaviour. Los Angeles: Sage, 2017.
SPEZIO, M. L.; ADOLPHS, R. Emotional processing and political judgment: toward integrating political
psychology and decision neuroscience. In: NEUMAN, W. R., et al. (eds.). The affect effect: dynamics
of emotion in political thinking and behavior. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 2007.
STEENBERGEN, M. R.; ELLIS, C. Fear and loathing in American elections: context, traits, and negative
candidate affect. In: REDLAWSK, D. P. (eds.). Feeling politics: emotion in political information
processing. New York: Palgrave Macmillan, 2006.
THE ECONOMIST. Democracy Index 2018: Me too? Political participation, protest and democracy,
2018. Available at:
<https://www.eiu.com/public/topical_report.aspx?campaignid=Democracy2018>. Access: 12 jan.
2019.
TORO, S. Y.; VALENZUELA, M. “Chile 2017: ambiciones, estrategias y expectativas en el estreno de las
nuevas reglas electorales”. Revista de Ciencia Política, vol. 38, nº 2, p. 207-232, 2018.
VALENTINO, N. A., et al. “Is a worried citizen a good citizen? Emotions, political information seeking,
and learning via the internet”. Political Psychology, vol. 29, nº 2, p. 247-273, 2008.
________. “Election night’s alright for fighting: the role of emotions in political participation”. The
Journal of Politics, vol. 73, nº 1, p. 156-170, 2011.
WEBER, C. “Emotions, campaigns, and political participation”. Political Research Quarterly, vol. 66, nº
2, p. 414-428, 2012.
WEEKS, B. E. “Emotions, partisanship, and misperceptions: how anger and anxiety moderate the
effect of partisan bias on susceptibility to political misinformation”. Journal of Communication, vol.
65, p. 699-719, 2015.
YATES, H. E. The politics of emotions, candidates and choices. New York: Palgrave Pivot, 2016.
ZALLER, J. R. The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1992.
Resumo
Entre o saber e o sentimento: as emoções e o voto na eleição presidencial chilena de 2017
Como os cidadãos decidem em quem votar nas eleições? As respostas tradicionais enfocam o papel
do conhecimento político, identificação de partidos e avaliações do desempenho passado dos governos
como variáveis explicativas. Neste artigo, avaliamos um argumento alternativo: o papel
desempenhado pelas emoções. Usando dados de uma pesquisa realizada após as eleições gerais
chilenas de dezembro de 2017, investigamos a associação das emoções com o voto em Sebastián
Piñera e como as emoções interagem com outros fatores relevantes que se correlacionam com o voto.
Concluímos que no Chile, juntamente com a identificação do partido e a avaliação de governos
anteriores, as emoções dos eleitores em relação aos candidatos estão associadas à decisão do voto.
Palavras-chave: emoções; voto; identificação partidária; conhecimento político; avaliação
retrospectiva; Chile
Resumen
Entre el saber y el sentir: emociones y voto en la elección presidencial chilena de 2017
¿Cómo deciden los ciudadanos a quién votar en una elección? Las respuestas tradicionales se centran
en el papel del conocimiento político, la identificación de los partidos y la evaluación del desempeño
pasado de los gobiernos como variables explicativas. En este estudio evaluamos un argumento
alternativo: el papel desempeñado por las emociones. Utilizando datos de una encuesta realizada
después de las elecciones generales chilenas de diciembre de 2017, este artículo investiga la relación
de las emociones con el voto por Sebastián Piñera y cómo las emociones interactúan con otros factores
relevantes que se correlacionan con el voto. Concluimos que en Chile, junto con la identificación de
los partidos y la evaluación retrospectiva, las emociones de los electores hacia los candidatos están
asociadas a su decisión de por quién votar.
Palabras clave: emociones; voto; identificación partidaria; conocimiento político; evaluación
retrospectiva; Chile
Résumé
Entre savoir et sentiment: émotions et vote à l'élection présidentielle chilienne 2017
Comment les citoyens décident-ils pour qui voter lors d'une élection? Les réponses traditionnelles
mettent l'accent sur le rôle des connaissances politiques, l'identification des partis et les évaluations
des performances passées des gouvernements en tant que variables explicatives. Dans cette étude,
nous évaluons un argument alternatif: le rôle joué par les émotions. À l'aide des données d'une
enquête réalisée à la suite des élections générales chiliennes de décembre 2017, cet article examine
l’association des émotions avec le vote pour Sebastián Piñera et la façon dont les émotions
interagissent avec d'autres facteurs pertinents en corrélation avec le vote. Nous concluons qu'au Chili,
avec l'identification des partis et l'évaluation des gouvernements antérieurs, les émotions des
électeurs envers les candidats sont associées à la décision de qui voter.
Mots-clés: émotions; vote; identification du parti; connaissances politiques; évaluation rétrospective;
Chili