Frisancho AR. 2010. The Study of Human Adaptation
Frisancho AR. 2010. The Study of Human Adaptation
Frisancho AR. 2010. The Study of Human Adaptation
A. Roberto Frisancho
Human Evolutionary Biology, ed. Michael P. Muehlenbein. Published by Cambridge University Press. # Cambridge University Press 2010.
17
18 A. Roberto Frisancho
Lifetime:
habituation
Primary acclimation
stress acclimatization
Biological
responses Restores
Disturbs homeostasis
homeostasis Growth: between
between developmental organism
organism acclimatization and
and environment
environment Cultural– Decrease
technological environmental
responses stress
2.1. Schematization of adaptation process and mechanisms that enable individual or population
to maintain homeostasis in the face of primary environmental disturbing stress. From Frisancho (1993).
heat-loss mechanisms. In either case, the organism processes that enable them to function and to be adapted
may maintain homeostasis, but the physiological to their environment.
processes will occur at a different set point. The attain- Not all responses made by the organism can be
ment of full homeostasis or full functional adaptation, considered adaptive. Although a given response might
depending on the nature of the stress, may require not be adaptive per se, through its effect on another
short-term responses, such as those acquired during structure or function it may prove beneficial to the
acclimation or acclimatization, or may require expos- organism’s function. Conversely, a given adaptive
ure during the period of growth and development as in response may aid the organism in one function, but
developmental acclimatization. actually have negative effects on other functions or
In theory, the respective contributions of genetic and structures. Thus, within all areas of human endeavor
environmental factors vary with the developmental stage a given trait is considered adaptive when its beneficial
of the organism–the earlier the stage, the greater the effects outweigh the negative ones. In theory, this is a
influence of the environment and the greater the valid assumption, but in practice, due to the relative
plasticity of the organism (Proser, 1964; Timiras, 1972; nature of adaptation, it is quite difficult to determine
Frisancho, 1975, 1993). However, as will be shown, the the true adaptive value of a given response. Every
principle does not apply to all biological parameters; response must be considered in the context of the
it depends on the nature of the stress, the developmental environmental conditions in which the response was
stage of the organism, the type of organism, and the measured and within the perspective of the length of
particular functional process that is affected. For time of the study and the subject population.
example, an adult individual exposed to high-altitude
hypoxia through prolonged residence may attain a level
of adaptation that permits normal functioning in all ADAPTIVE PROCESSES
daily activities and as such we may consider him
adapted. However, when exposed to stress that requires The term adaptation is used in the broad generic sense
increased energy, such as strenuous exercise, this of functional adaptation, and it is applied to all levels of
individual may not prove to be fully adapted. On the other biological organization from individuals to populations.
hand, through cultural and technological adaptation, A basic premise of this approach is that adaptation is a
humans may actually modify and thus decrease the process whereby the organism has attained a beneficial
nature of the environmental stresses so that a new adjustment to the environment (Mayr, 1963; Lewontin,
microenvironment is created to which the organism 1957; Proser, 1964; Dubos, 1965; Baker, 1966; Lasker,
does not need to make any physiological responses. 1969; Mazess, 1973; Frisancho, 1975, 1993). This adjust-
For example, cultural and technological responses permit ment can be either temporary or permanent, acquired
humans to live under extreme conditions of cold stress either through short-term or lifetime processes, and may
with the result that some of the physiological processes involve physiological, structural, behavioral, or cultural
are not altered. However, on rare occasions, humans changes aimed at improving the organism’s functional
have been able to completely avoid an environmental performance in the face of environmental stresses.
stress. Witness the fact that the Eskimos, despite their If environmental stresses are conducive to differential
advanced technological adaptation to cold in their mortality and fertility, then adaptive changes may
everyday hunting activities, are exposed to periods of cold become established in the population through changes
stress and in response have developed biological in genetic composition and thus attain a level of
The Study of Human Adaptation 19
MULTILEVEL SELECTION AND EVOLUTION single defector (Traulsen and Nowawk, 2006). Hence,
OF CO-OPERATION this simple condition ensures that selection favors
co-operators and opposes defectors.
Ever since Darwin (1871) indicated that the competi- The concept of group selection has been a major
tion between groups can lead to selection of co-opera- tenet of behavioral ecology. The major premise of
tive behavior, the concept of multilevel selection has behavioral evolutionary ecology is that genetic and
been developed. He stated that, “there can be no doubt behavioral traits are two distinct expressions of a
that a tribe including many members who were single evolutionary process. (Trivers, 1971; Cronk,
always ready to give aid to each other and to sacrifice 1991; Strier, 2000; Silk, 2001). In behavioral ecology,
themselves for the common good, would be victorious behaviors are treated like any other biological trait
over other tribes; and this would be natural selection” and are potentially subject to natural selection. The
(Darwin, 1871, p. 166). Over many years, Wilson and processes involved in behavioral evolution are
colleagues have been the main proponent of the idea of equivalent to those in genetic evolution: natural selec-
group selection (Wilson, 1975; Sober and Wilson, 1998; tion influences the frequency of a trait transmitted
Traulsen and Nowak, 2006). It is assumed that group from parent to offspring through differential fertility
selection is an important organizing principle that and mortality. In the evolutionary perspective, bio-
permeates evolutionary processes from the emergence logical structures have been custom tailored to motiv-
of the first cells to development of nations. According ate behaviors that are likely to enhance individual
to multilevel selection, groups consist of genetically fitness. Therefore, behavioral variants with a high
unrelated individuals, and successful groups attract fitness have been favored and these perpetuate the
new individuals, which learn the strategies of others evolutionary origin of fitness-enhancing biological
in the same group. A population can be subdivided traits. It follows then, that the behavioral traits that
into groups, and the individuals interact with other enhance fitness also accentuate biological fitness.
members of the group, and depending on their repro- In other words, a change occurring in one system
ductive fitness, individuals can lead to larger groups entails a change in the fitness governing evolution in
that split more often. In other words, higher-level or the other system. Therefore, both genetic and behav-
group selection emerges as a by-product of individual ioral selection tend to favor those existing variants
reproduction. whose net effect is to increase the average fitness
A fundamental condition for the success of the of the individual and population to the prevailing
group, therefore, must be co-operation among indi- conditions. Studies of primates indicate that they
viduals, and thus group selection favors co-operative use a diversity of behaviors that increase the likeli-
altruistic behavior and opposes defectors. The fitness hood of gaining access to mates and guarantee the
of an individual, and the group at large, also depends survival of their offspring; which, in turn, insures the
on the altruistic behavior of nonrelatives. When an passing of their traits to the next generation. In this
altruist gives an alarm call, it benefits not only his or context, behavioral actions that lead to a higher
her relatives, but also other unrelated members of reproductive success will become adaptive and the
the group because a primate troop does not only genes associated with such behavior will be trans-
include relatives. Thus, altruism can be selected ferred to the next generation faster than those that
if these nonrelated individuals can be counted on to are less adaptive. Therefore, the differences in fitness
reciprocate the favor when the need arises. between individuals and populations will determine
A recipient of an altruistic behavior who fails to recip- the behavioral pattern of a given primate group.
rocate is a cheater. Studies of nonhuman primates In other words, a specific behavioral strategy that
indicate that a cheater may gain in the short run by contributes to the survival and reproductive fitness
receiving aid without any costs to their own fitness of the individual – and eventually the population –
(Strier, 2000). However, reciprocity is necessary becomes part of the genetic milieu of the species.
for future support in the long run because the In summary, co-operation and altruism evolve by
cheater’s fitness is lower when compared to the group selection or multilevel selection. Human behav-
individual who reciprocated. In view of the fact that ioral ecology rests upon a foundation of evolutionary
primates constantly need to protect themselves from theory, which include sexual selection, whereby indi-
neighboring communities and predators, one can viduals within one sex secure mates and produce
assume that reciprocal altruism must have been offspring at the expense of other individuals within
selected for because it enhanced their fitness, not the same sex, which can cause changes in gene
because the animals are conscious of their motives frequency across generations that are driven at least
or the reproductive consequences of their behavior. in part by interactions between related individuals
Mathematical models indicate that a single co- referred to as kin selection, and be expressed as the
operator has a greater fixation probability than a sum of an individual’s own reproductive success.
The Study of Human Adaptation 23
CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN ADAPTATION mellitus was a quick insulin trigger. Insulin’s main func-
RESEARCH tion is to assist in the homeostasis of glucose in the
blood. Specifically, when blood glucose levels are too
In the 1970s I postulated the hypothesis of develop- high, the pancreas releases insulin to increase tissue
mental adaptation to explain the enlarged lung volume uptake of glucose, thus reducing blood glucose levels.
and enhanced aerobic capacity that characterize the Conversely, when blood glucose levels are low, the
Andean high altitude natives. According to the organism secretes glucagon and growth hormone, which
developmental adaptation hypothesis, “adult biological in turn, induce the release of stored glucose and fatty
traits are the result of the effects of the environment acids into the blood stream raising serum glucose levels.
and the physiological responses that the organism The insulin response is to activate an uptake of glucose
makes during the developmental state” (Frisancho, into the muscle cells for storage, and in liver cells it
1970, 1975, 1977). This concept is based upon the fact influences the conversion of glucose to fatty acids for
that the organism’s plasticity and susceptibility to envir- storage in fat (adipose) tissue. This response was an
onmental influence is inversely related to developmen- asset during times of abundance because it would allow
tal states of the organism, so that the younger the an individual to build-up energy reserves more quickly
individual the greater is the influence of the environ- and thus better survive times of food scarcity. Under
ment and the greater the organism’s plasticity these conditions, the thrifty gene was selected to regulate
(Frisancho, 1975, 1977, 1993). Hence, variability in efficient intake and utilization of fuel stores. In other
physiological traits can be traced to the developmental words, during periods of food shortage and famine,
history of the individual (Figure 2.2). Currently this those with the thrifty genotype would have a selective
concept has been applied to explain the variability in advantage because they relied on larger, previously
adult behavioral traits such as in learning and crime and stored energy to maintain homeostasis; whereas those
delinquency (Yueh-Au Chien, 1994; Sroufe et al., 2005; without “thrifty” genotypes would be at a disadvantage
Kruger et al., 2008), in sensory inputs and auditory and less likely to survive and reproduce. However, under
spatial processing (Martin and Martin, 2002), in toler- modern conditions of abundant food and sedentary
ance to surgical intervention (Faury et al., 2003), in lifestyle, this genotype becomes perversely disadvanta-
variability in oxygen consumption and mitochondrial geous. With a constant abundance of food, insulin levels
membrane potential in energy metabolism of rat remain high, resulting in tissues becoming less sensitive
cortical neurons (Schuchmann et al., 2005), and in to the effects of insulin. This reduced sensitivity to the
variability in increased risk of adult obesity and cardio- effects of insulin results in chronically elevated blood
vascular problems associated with the metabolic glucose levels type II diabetes and related chronic health
syndrome (Barker, 1994). A common denominator of problems (e.g., obesity).
all these studies is that humans and many other A test of the genetic predisposition to type II
organisms are conditioned by experiences during devel- diabetes involved a comparative study of the Pima
opment and as developmental experiences is an import- Indians of southern Arizona and the Pima Indians of
ant contributor to variability in adult phenotypic the Sierra Madre mountains of northern Mexico
behavioral and biological traits. (Knowler et al., 1990; Price et al., 1992). These two
In this section I will summarize the evidence groups, which were separated 700 1000 years ago,
supporting the applicability of the concept of develop- differ in their life style. The Arizona Pima live under
mental adaptation to account for the origins of the high conditions of access to a high fat, highly refined diet
risk of the adult metabolic syndrome incorporating and low energy expenditure. In contrast, the Mexican
information derived from thrifty gene, thrifty pheno- Pima still pursue a much more traditional lifestyle
type, and epigenetics. The evolution of the metabolic and have a diet based on the occasional intake of
syndrome is also discussed at length in Chapter 30 of lamb and poultry, but mainly on beans, corn, and
this volume. potatoes, grown by traditional, and physically very
energy-demanding, techniques. These two groups
differ significantly in the frequency of obesity and
DEVELOPMENTAL ADAPTATION AND THE diabetes. The Arizona Pima adults have a body mass
THRIFTY GENOTYPE index (BMI) of 33.4 kg/m2; compared to a BMI of 24.9
kg/m2 in the Mexican Pima (Ravussin et al., 1994).
Neel and colleagues (Neel, 1962; Neel et al., 1998) Likewise, in the Arizona Pima 37% of men and 54%
attempted to explain the epidemic proportions of of women were diabetic, while in the Mexican Pima
diabetes in Native American populations, such as the only 2 of 19 women and 1 of 16 men were diabetic
Pima Native Americans, by postulating the existence of (Knowler et al., 1990; Price et al., 1992). In other
a “thrifty gene” that increased the risk of type II diabetes. words, although the Mexican Pima share the “thrifty
According to this hypothesis, the basic defect in diabetes gene” with Arizona Pima, their increased frequency of
24 A. Roberto Frisancho
obesity and diabetes is more evidence that an abun- Poor maternal nutrition
dance of fatty foods and modern sedentary lifestyles associated with fetal
undernutrition
are the real culprits. Thus, it is not the presence of a
“thrifty gene” alone that results in increased rates of
diabetes, but rather the interaction with modern diet- Fetal programing: Cellular,
ary and lifestyle conditions the results in increased physiological, and metabolic
rates of the chronic health problems. compensatory responses
resulting in energy conservation
In summary, the thrifty genotype hypothesis has
been used to explain the epidemic levels of obesity
and diabetes among non-Western populations, such Adult poor Adult good
postnatal postnatal
as South Pacific Islanders, sub-Saharan Africans,
nutrition and high nutrition and low
Native Americans in the southwestern United States, energy energy
Inuit, Australian aborigines, etc. (Eaton et al., 1988; expenditure expenditure
O’Dea, 1991); all of whom were newly introduced to
industrialized diets and environments. The fact that
the frequency of type II diabetes has recently increased Nondiabetic Type II diabetic
and good health and polor health
among Europeans that were not subjected to periodic
famines cannot be attributed to the action of a 2.3. The thrifty phenotype. The risk of type II diabetes and
so-called “thrifty” gene. metabolic syndrome in adulthood is associated with prenatal
undernutrition resulting in efficient physiological adaptation that
becomes detrimental when food is abundant and energy
expenditure is low.
DEVELOPMENTAL ADAPTATION AND
THE THRIFTY PHENOTYPE
the Dutch famine of World War II were found to have
Recently, Barker and colleagues (Barker, 2007; Hales impaired glucose tolerance and increased adiposity
and Barker, 1992) have reported an inverse relationship in adulthood (Stein et al., 1975, 2007).
between birthweight and the risk of hypertension,
cardiovascular disease, and type II diabetes in adulthood
when the individual is well nourished postnatally. DEVELOPMENTAL ADAPTATION AND
To account for these observations, Barker and colleagues EPIGENETICS
proposed that adverse effects in utero induce cellular,
physiological, and metabolic compensatory responses, Epigenetics refers to the transmission of phenotypic
such as insulin resistance, high blood plasma levels of traits from one generation to the next that do not
fatty acids, which result in energy conservation and depend on differences in DNA sequence (Waddington,
reduced somatic growth that enable the fetus to survive 1952; Jablonka, 2004; Holliday, 2006). During the last
undernutrition. This response is referred to as the thrifty two decades, there has been an accumulation of obser-
phenotype hypothesis (Armitage et al., 2005). These vations indicating that the expression of DNA traits can
responses that were adaptive under poor prenatal condi- be affected by environmental factors acting during
tions become a problem if food becomes abundant. development. Specifically, experimental studies
In this view, thrifty physiological mechanisms are showed that identical twin mice differ in the color of
adaptive in nutritionally poor environments, but in rich fur; one has brown fur and will grow up to be lean and
environments are maladaptive. That is, what was positive healthy, while the other has yellow fur and becomes
under reduced availability of nutrients, particularly obese and prone to cardiovascular disease. The differ-
during periods of rapid development, becomes negative ent phenotypes are due to the addition of a methyl
in rich environments because it facilitates nutrient group (-CH3); which is referred to as methylation.
absorption and hence increases the risk of adult obesity
and the suite of risk factors for cardiovascular disease
Methylation
known as the metabolic syndrome (Figure 2.3).
In summary, it appears that nutrition and other Methylation refers to the altering of the genetic envir-
environmental factors during prenatal and early post- onment through the addition of a methyl group (-CH3)
natal development influence cellular plasticity; thereby to the fifth position of cytosine, which is largely
altering susceptibility to adult cardiovascular disease, confined to CpG dinucleotides. This addition, by modi-
type II diabetes, obesity, and other chronic diseases fying the CpG islands, prevents signaling molecules
referred as the adult metabolic syndrome. This hypoth- from reaching the promoter site to turn the gene on
esis is supported by the finding that the offspring of and prevent the expression of the dark coat color.
women who were starved and became pregnant during In other words, the additional methyl group attaches
The Study of Human Adaptation 25
to and shuts off the gene that controls dark fur color the organism; so that the younger the individual, the
and allows the yellow color to be expressed. Thus, the greater the epigenetic marks, including CpG methyla-
process of methylation works as a kind epigenome that tion. Despite the great interest in molecular genetics,
dictates which genes in the genome are turned on and there is scant incontrovertible evidence indicating
which are not. This process can differ even between epigenetic effects in humans. Considering society’s
identical twins. increased concern about environmental pollutants, this
Recently, experimental studies indicate that area of research should a good direction for human
bisphenol A (BPA) can alter gene expression and biologists.
affect adult phenotype by modifying CpG methylation
at critical epigenetically labile genomic regions (Water-
land and Jirtle, 2004). Bisphenol A is used in the produc- OVERVIEW
tion of polycarbonate and plastic containers and in
the organism acts like the body’s own hormones. Thus, The term adaptation encompasses the physiological,
there is concern that long-term exposure to BPA may cultural, and genetic adaptations that permit individ-
induce chronic toxicity in humans (vom Saal and uals and populations to adjust to the environment in
Hughes, 2005). Fortunately, the effects of methylation which they live. These adjustments are complex, and
are not permanent but reversible, as shown by the fact the concept of adaptation cannot be reduced to a simple
that the yellow agouti (Avy), whose diet was supple- rigid definition without oversimplification. The func-
mented with folic acid, vitamin B12, choline, betaine, tional approach in using the adaptation concept permits
and zinc, counteracted the DNA methylation and its application to all levels of biological organization
changed coat color from yellow to dark brown coat from unicellular to multicellular organisms, from early
(Dolinoy and Jirtle, 2008), which is associated with a embryonic to adult stages, and from individuals to popu-
low risk of cardiovascular disease. lations. In this context, human biological responses to
environmental stress can be considered as part of a
continuous process whereby past adaptations are modi-
Transgenerational epigenetic effects
fied and developed to permit the organism to function
It has been suggested that the epigenetic modifications and maintain equilibrium within the environment to
brought about by parental conditions may be which it is daily exposed.
expressed even in grandchildren. Extensive records of The mechanisms for attaining full functional adap-
a population in Overkalix cohorts, northern Sweden, tation include acclimation, acclimatization, habitu-
found that an association between grandparental ation, and accommodation. The role played by each
prepubertal slow growth periods (SGP) or rapid of these processes depends on the nature of the stress
growth periods (RGP), and parental periods of low or or stresses, the organ system involved, and the devel-
high food availability, with grandchildren’s mortality opmental stage of the organism. It is emphasized that
and disease risk (Kaati et al., 2007). If the SGP of the the goal of the organism’s responses to a given stress is
grandparent was a period of high food availability, to maintain homeostasis within an acceptable normal
then the male grandchild had reduced longevity but range with itself and with respect to other organisms
an increased mortality. The extent to which these and the environment (as schematized in Figure 2.1).
associations represent multigenerational epigenetic Such adaptations are usually reversible, but the revers-
effects is unwarranted, in part because ruling out ibility depends on the developmental stage of the
genetic and societal confounders, and in the absence organism at which the adaptive response occurs and
of molecular evidence, is extremely difficult. Hence, the nature of the environmental stress. This character-
future research must be focused on long-term transge- istic allows organisms to adapt to a wide range of
nerational studies whereby many birth cohorts are environmental conditions. Furthermore, an adaptation
studied using intensive prenatal and perinatal genotyp- is always a compromise between positive and negative
ing across generations. Only then can variability in the effects. Every adaptation involves a cost. The process
expression of phenotypic traits can be attributed to of adaptation is always positively beneficial; without
epigenetic changes. which the organism would be worse off, however the
In summary, epigenetic effects exist that are not organism has to pay a price for the benefit. The benefit
necessarily adaptive, and in many of these cases, the derived from a given response depends on the circum-
inherited phenotype is actually detrimental to the organ- stances and the conditions where it occurs. As recently
ism. Environmental exposure to nutritional, chemical, pointed out (Young and Marchini, 1990), every adap-
and physical factors can alter gene expression and affect tation involves a choice. For example, a man has
adult phenotype: a process known as epigenetics. In all 6 hours in which to walk 11 km. If he walks slowly,
of these studies, the extent of DNA methylation depends he saves energy expenditure, and therefore it may be
on and is inversely related to the developmental state of adaptive if the energy resources are limited; however
26 A. Roberto Frisancho
he has no time left to do anything else. On the other 7. Compare developmental and adult adaptation.
hand, if he walks fast, he saves time at the cost of using Which is more likely to be reversible and why?
more energy. Thus, the adaptive importance of given 8. Discuss the applicability of the concept of develop-
type of response depends on the conditions. mental adaptation to the hypothesis thrifty geno-
The concept of developmental adaptation has type and thrifty phenotype that account for the
become a major focus for studying the origins of human increased frequency of the adult metabolic syn-
diversity (Figure 2.2). The applicability of this research drome among native and nonnative populations.
strategy is based upon the premise that human biological 9. Discuss the relationship of the concept of develop-
responses to environmental stress represent a continu- mental adaptation to the field of epigenetics.
ous process whereby past adaptations are modified and
developed to permit the organism to function and main-
tain equilibrium within the environment to which it is REFERENCES
daily exposed. From the studies of the thrifty genotype Armitage, J. A., Taylor, P. D. and Poston, L. (2005). Experi-
and thrifty phenotype, and their relationship to the mental models of developmental programming: conse-
etiology of metabolic syndrome, it is evident that what quences of exposure to an energy rich diet during
was positive under reduced availability of nutrients, development. Journal of Physiology, 565, 3–8.
particularly during periods of rapid prenatal develop- Baker, P. T. (1966). Human biological variation as an
ment, becomes negative in rich environments. Research adaptive response to the environment. Eugenics Quarterly,
in epigenetics may provide the bridge between the thrifty 13, 81–91.
Barker, D. J. P. (1994). Mothers, Babies, and Disease in
genotype and thrifty phenotype to unravel the interrela-
Later Life. London: BMJ Publishing.
tions of how the impact of early diet helps how the
Barker, D. J. P, (2007). The Origins of the Developmental
organism adapts to a given environmental condition that
Origins Theory. Journal of Internal Medicine, 261,
differs in nutritional resources; resulting in the diverse 412–417.
phenotypic expression of physiology, body size, and Bligh, J. and Johnson, K. G. (1973). Glossary of terms for
health risk of contemporary and past populations. thermal physiology. Journal of Applied Physiology, 35,
The study of individuals exposed to stressful conditions 941 961.
in natural and laboratory environments is one of Cronk, L. (1991). Human behavioral ecology. Annual Review
the most important approaches for understanding the of Anthropology, 20, 25–53.
mechanisms whereby human populations adapt to a Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man and Selection in
given environment. Knowledge of human adaptation is Relation to Sex, 2nd edn. London: Murray.
basic in our endeavors to understand past and present Dolinoy, D. C. and Jirtle, R. L. (2008). Environmental epige-
human variation in morphology and physiological nomics in human health and disease. Environmental and
performance. The insights we have gained during the Molecular Mutagenesis, 49, 4–8.
Dubos, R. (1965). Man Adapting. New Haven, CT: Yale
last decade have stimulated new approaches to study
University Press.
human adaptation, not only for understanding behavior,
Eagan, C. J. (1963). Introduction and terminology. Federation
but of how ecology shapes function both in the present
Proceedings, 22, 930–932.
and in the past, and for understanding variability in the Eaton, S. B., Konner, M. and Shostak, M. (1988). Stone agers
immune system, thermo-regulation, coping with limited in the fast lane: chronic degenerative diseases in evolu-
and excessive amounts of foods, low oxygen pressure, tionary perspective. American Journal of Medicine, 84,
and high and low solar radiation. 739–749.
Eaton, S. B., Strassman, B. I., Nesse, R. M., et al. (2002).
Evolutionary health promotion. Preventative Medicine,
DISCUSSION POINTS 34, 109–118.
Faury, G., Pezet, M., Knutsen, R. H., et al. (2003). Develop-
1. What are the four processes of functional adaptation? mental adaptation of the mouse cardiovascular system to
2. Why does the organism have to respond to environ- elastin haplo insufficiency. Journal of Clinical Investigation,
mental stimuli? Give examples. 112, 1419–1428.
Folk, G. E. Jr (1974). Textbook of Environmental Physiology.
3. When does a given functional adaptation become a
Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger.
genetic adaptation?
Frenk, S. (1986). Metabolic adaptation in protein-energy
4. Discuss the role of multiple-level selection in the
malnutrition. Journal of the American College of Nutrition,
expression of biological and behavioral traits. 5, 371–381.
5. Compare accommodation and adaptation. Give Frisancho, A. R. (1970). Developmental responses to high
specific examples. altitude hypoxia. American Journal of Physical Anthropology,
6. Discuss how technological and cultural adapta- 32: 401–407.
tions have created new environmental stresses. Frisancho, A. R. (1975). Functional adaptation to high altitude
Give examples. hypoxia. Science, 187, 313–319.
The Study of Human Adaptation 27
Frisancho, A. R. (1977). Developmental adaptation to high Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B,
altitude hypoxia, International Journal of Biometeorology, 334, 233–240.
21, 135–146. Price, R. A., Lunetta, K., Ness, R., et al. (1992). Obesity in
Frisancho, A. R. (1993). Human Adaptation and Accommo- Pima Indians. Distribution characteristics and possible
dation. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. thresholds for genetic studies. International Journal of
Frisancho, A. R. and Schechter, D. E. (1997). Adaptation. Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders, 16, 851–857.
In History of Physical Anthropology: an Encyclopedia, Proser, C. L. (1964). Perspectives of adaptation: theoretical
vol. 1. Garland, TX: Garland Press, pp. 6–12. aspects. In Handbook of Physiology, vol. 4, D. B. Dill, E. F.
Hales, C. N. and Barker, D. J. (1992). Type 2 (non-insulin- Adolph and C. G. Wilber (eds). Washington, DC: American
dependent) diabetes mellitus: the thrifty phenotype Physiological Society, pp. 11–26.
hypothesis. Diabetologia, 35, 595–601. Rappaport, R. A. (1976). Maladaptation in social systems.
Holliday, R. (2006). Epigenetics: a historical overview. In Evolution in Social Systems, J. Friedman and
Epigenetics, 1, 76–80. M. Rowlands (eds). London: Gerald Duckworth, pp. 49–71.
Jablonka, E. (2004). Epigenetic epidemiology. International Ravussin, E., Valencia, M. E., Esparza, J., et al. (1994).
Journal of Epidemiology, 33, 929–935. Effects of a traditional lifestyle on obesity in Pima Indians.
Kaati, G., Bygren, L. O., Pembrey, M., et al. (2007). Transge- Diabetes Care, 17, 1067–1074.
nerational response to nutrition, early life circumstances Schuchmann, S., Buchheim, K., Heinemann, U., et al. (2005).
and longevity. European Journal of Human Genetics, Oxygen consumption and mitochondrial membrane poten-
15, 784–790. tial indicate developmental adaptation in energy meta-
Knowler, W. C., Pettitt, D. J., Saad, M. F., et al. (1990). Dia- bolism of rat cortical neurons. European Journal of
betes mellitus in the Pima Indians: Incident, risk factors Neuroscience, 21, 2721–2732.
and pathogenesis. Diabetes/Metabolism Research and Silk, J. (2001). Ties that bind: the role of kinship in primate
Reviews, 6, 1–27. societies. In New Directions in Anthropological Kinship,
Kruger, D. J., Reisch, T., and Zimmerman, M. A. (2008).Time L. Stone (ed.). Lanham, MD: Rowman and Littlefield
perspective as a mechanism for functional developmental Publishers, pp. 71–92.
adaptation. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Sober, E. and Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto Others: the Evolu-
Psychology, 2, 1–22. tion and Psychology of Unselfish Behavior. Cambridge,
Lasker, G. W. (1969). Human biological adaptability. Science, MA: Harvard University Press.
166, 1480–1486. Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., et al. (2005). The
Lewontin, R. C. (1957). The adaptations of populations to Development of the Person: the Minnesota Study of Risk
varying environments. In Cold Spring Harbor Symposia and Adaptation from Birth to Adulthood. New York:
on Quantitative Biology, vol.22. Boston, MA: Cold Spring Guilford Press.
Harbor Laboratory of Quantitative Biology. Stein, Z., Susser, M., Saenger, G., et al. (1975). Famine and
Livingstone, F. B. (1958). Anthropological implications of Human Development. The Dutch Hunger Winter of 1944 45.
sickle-cell gene distribution in West Africa. American New York: Oxford University Press.
Anthropologist, 60, 533–562. Stein, A. D., Kahn, H. S., Rundle, A., et al. (2007). Anthropo-
Martin, P. and Martin, M. (2002). Proximal and distal metric measures in middle age after exposure to famine
influences on development: The model of developmental during gestation: Evidence from the Dutch famine. Ameri-
adaptation. Developmental Review, 22, 78–96. can Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 85, 869–876.
Mayr, E. (1963). Animal Species and Evolution. Cambridge, Strier, K. B. (2000). Primate Behavioral Ecology. Boston:
MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. Allyn and Bacon.
Mayr, E. (1997). The objects of selection. Proceedings of the Thomas, R. B. (1975). The ecology of work. In Physiological
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Anthropology, A. Damon (ed.). New York: Oxford University
94, 2091 2094. Press, pp. 59–79.
Mazess, R. B. (1973). Biological adaptation: aptitudes and Timiras, P. S. (1972). Developmental Physiology and Aging.
acclimatization. In Biosocial Interrelations in Population New York: Macmillan.
Adaptation, E. S. Watts, F. E. Johnson and G. W. Lasker Traulsen, A. and Nowak, M. A. (2006). Evolution of cooper-
(eds). The Hague: Mouton, pp. 9–18. ation by multilevel selection. Proceedings of the National
Moran, E. F. (1979). Human Adaptability: an Introduction to Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
Ecological Anthropology. North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press. 103, 10952–10955.
Neel, J. V. (1962). Diabetes mellitus: A “thrifty” genotype Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism.
rendered detrimental by “progress”? American Journal of Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.
Human Genetics, 14, 353–362. Vom Saal, F. S. and Hughes, C. (2005). An extensive new
Neel, J. V., Weder, A. B. and Julius, S. (1998). Type II dia- literature concerning low-dose effects of bisphenol
betes, essential hypertension, and obesity as “syndromes A shows the need for a new risk assessment. Environmen-
of impaired genetic homeostasis”: the “thrifty genotype” tal Health Perspectives, 113, 926–933.
hypothesis enters the twenty-first century. Perspectives in Waddington, C. H. (1952). Epigenetics of Birds. Cambridge:
Biology and Medicine, 42, 44–74. Cambridge University Press.
O’Dea, K. (1991). Traditional diet and food preferences of Waterland, R. and Jirtle, R. (2004). Early nutrition, epigen-
Australian aboriginal hunter-gatherers. Philosophical etic changes at transposons and imprinted genes, and
28 A. Roberto Frisancho
enhanced susceptibility to adult chronic diseases. Nutrition, Young, V. R. and Marchini, J. S. (1990). Mechanisms and
20, 63–68. nutritional significance of metabolic responses to altered
Waterlow, J. C. (1990). Nutritional adaptation in man: intakes of protein and amino acids, with reference to
General introduction and concepts. American Journal of nutritional adaptation in humans. American Journal of
Clinical Nutrition, 51, 259–263. Clinical Nutrition, 51, 270–289.
Wilson, D. S. (1975). A theory of group selection. Proceedings Yueh-Au Chien, C. (1994). Developmental Adaptation of
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of Social Learning Theory: the Etiology of Crime and Delin-
America, 72, 143–146. quency. Denver, CO: University of Colorado Press.