0% found this document useful (0 votes)
8 views8 pages

The Biological Roots of Intelligence

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1/ 8

The Biological Roots of Intelligence

Imaging, behavioral, and genetic data yield clues to what’s


behind effective thinking.

In 1987, political scientist James Flynn of the University of Otago in New Zealand documented a
curious phenomenon: broad intelligence gains in multiple human populations over time. Across
14 countries where decades’ worth of average IQ scores of large swaths of the population were
available, all had upward swings—some of them dramatic. Children in Japan, for example, gained
an average of 20 points on a test known as the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children between
1951 and 1975. In France, the average 18-year-old man performed 25 points better on a
reasoning test in 1974 than did his 1949 counterpart.1

Flynn initially suspected the trend reflected faulty tests. Yet in the ensuing years, more data and
analyses supported the idea that human intelligence was increasing over time. Proposed
explanations for the phenomenon, now known as the Flynn effect, include increasing education,
better nutrition, greater use of technology, and reduced lead exposure, to name but four.
Beginning with people born in the 1970s, the trend has reversed in some Western European
countries, deepening the mystery of what’s behind the generational fluctuations. But no
consensus has emerged on the underlying cause of these trends.

A fundamental challenge in understanding the Flynn effect is defining intelligence. At the dawn of
the 20th century, English psychologist Charles Spearman first observed that people’s average
performance on a variety of seemingly unrelated mental tasks—judging whether one weight is
heavier than another, for example, or pushing a button quickly after a light comes on—predicts
our average performance on a completely different set of tasks. Spearman proposed that a single
measure of general intelligence, g, was responsible for that commonality.

Scientists have proposed biological mechanisms for variations among individuals’ g levels
ranging from brain size and density to the synchrony of neural activity to overall connectivity
within the cortex. But the precise physiological origin of g is far from settled, and a simple
explanation for differences in intelligence between individuals continues to elude researchers.
A recent study of 1,475 adolescents across Europe reported that intelligence, as measured by a
cognitive test, was associated with a panoply of biological features, including known genetic
markers, epigenetic modifications of a gene involved in dopamine signaling, gray matter density
in the striatum (a major player in motor control and reward response), and the striatum’s
activation in response to a surprising reward cue.2

Understanding human smarts has been made even more challenging by the efforts of some
inside and outside the field to introduce pseudoscientific concepts into the mix. The study of
intelligence has at times been tainted by eugenics, “scientific” racism, and sexism, for example.
As recently as 2014, former New York Times science writer Nicholas Wade drew fire for what
critics characterized as misinterpreting genetics studies to suggest race could correlate with
average differences in intelligence and other traits. The legitimacy of such analyses aside, for
today’s intelligence researchers, categorization isn’t the end goal.

“The reason I’m interested in fluid intelligence tests”—which home in on problem-solving ability
rather than learned knowledge—“is not really because I want to know what makes one person do
better than another,” says University of Cambridge neuroscientist John Duncan. “It’s important for
everybody because these functions are there in everybody’s mind, and it would be very nice to
know how they work.”

In search of g
G, and the IQ (or intelligence quotient) tests that aim to measure it, have proven remarkably
durable since Spearman’s time. Multiple studies have backed his finding of a measurable
correlation among an individual’s performances on disparate cognitive tests. And g interests
researchers because its effects extend far beyond academic and work performance. In study
after study, higher IQ is tied to outcomes such as greater income and educational attainment, as
well as to lower risks of chronic disease, disability, and early death.

Early studies of people with brain injuries posited the frontal lobes as vital to problem solving. In
the late 1980s, Richard Haier of the University of California, Irvine, and colleagues imaged the
brains of people as they solved abstract reasoning puzzles, which revved up specific areas in the
frontal, parietal, and occipital lobes of the brain, as well as communication between them. The
frontal lobes are associated with planning and attention; the parietal lobes interpret sensory
information; and the occipital lobe processes visual information—all abilities useful in puzzle
solving. But more activity didn’t mean greater cognitive prowess, notes Haier. “The people with
the highest test scores actually showed the lowest brain activity, suggesting that it wasn’t how
hard your brain was working that made you smart, but how efficiently your brain was working.”

In 2007, based on this and other neuroimaging studies, Haier and the University of New Mexico’s
Rex Jung proposed the parieto-frontal integration theory, arguing that the brain areas identified in
Haier’s and others’ studies are central to intelligence.3 (See infographic.) But Haier and other
researchers have since found that patterns of activation vary, even between people of similar
intelligence, when performing the same mental tasks. This suggests, he says, that there are
different pathways that the brain can use to reach the same end point.

The people with the highest test scores actually showed the lowest brain activity, suggest-
ing that it wasn’t how hard your brain was working that made you smart, but how effi-
ciently your brain was working.

Richard Haier, University of California, Irvine


Another problem with locating the seat of g via brain imaging, some argue, is that our instruments
are still simply too crude to yield satisfying answers. Haier’s PET scans in the 1980s, for instance,
tracked radiolabeled glucose through the brain to get a picture of metabolic activity during a 30-
minute window in an organ whose cells communicate with one another on the order of
milliseconds. And modern fMRI scans, while more temporally precise, merely track blood flow
through the brain, not the actual activity of individual neurons. “It’s like if you’re trying to
understand the principles of human speech and all you could listen to is the volume of noise
coming out of a whole city,” Duncan says.

Models of intelligence
Beyond simply not having sharp-enough tools, some researchers are beginning to question the
premise that the key to intelligence can be seen in the anatomical features of the brain. “The
dominant view of the brain in the 20th century was anatomy is destiny,” says
neurophysiologist Earl Miller of MIT’s Picower Institute for Learning and Memory; but it’s become
clear over the past 10 to 15 years that this view is too simplistic.

Researchers have begun to propose alternative properties of the brain that might undergird
intelligence. Miller, for example, has been tracking the behavior of brain waves, which arise when
multiple neurons fire in synchrony, for clues about IQ. In one recent study, he and colleagues
hooked up EEG electrodes to the heads of monkeys that had been taught to release a bar if they
saw the same sequence of objects they’d seen a moment before. The task relied on working
memory, the ability to access and store bits of relevant information, and it caused bursts of high-
frequency γ and lower-frequency β waves. When the bursts weren’t synchronized at the usual
points during the task, the animals made errors.4
Miller suspects that these waves “direct traffic” in the brain, ensuring that neural signals reach the
appropriate neurons when they need to. “Gamma is bottom-up—it carries the contents of what
you’re thinking about. And beta is top-down—it carries the control signals that determine what
you think about,” he says. “If your beta isn’t strong enough to control the gamma, you get a brain
that can’t filter out distractions.”

The overall pattern of brain communications is another candidate to explain intelligence. Earlier
this year, Aron Barbey, a psychology researcher at the University of Illinois at Urbana-
Champaign, proposed this idea, which he calls the network neuroscience theory,5 citing studies
that used techniques such as diffusion tensor MRI to trace the connections among brain regions.
Barbey is far from the first to suggest that the ability of different parts of the brain to communicate
with one another is central to intelligence, but the whole-brain nature of network neuroscience
theory contrasts with more established models, such as parieto-frontal integration theory, that
focus on specific regions. “General intelligence originates from individual differences in the
system-wide topology and dynamics of the human brain,” Barbey tells The Scientist.

General intelligence originates from individual differences in the system-wide topology


and dynamics of the human brain.

Aron Barbey, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign

Emiliano Santarnecchi of Harvard University and Simone Rossi of the University of Siena in Italy
also argue that intelligence is a property of the whole brain, but they see overall plasticity as the
key to smarts. Plasticity, the brain’s ability to reorganize, can be measured via the nature of the
brain activity generated in response to transcranial magnetic or electrical stimulation,
Santarnecchi says. “There are individuals that generate a response that is only with the other
nodes of the same network that we target,” he says.And then there are people in whose brains
“the signal starts propagating everywhere.” His group has found that higher intelligence, as
measured by IQ tests, corresponds with a more network-specific response, which Santarnecchi
hypothesizes “reflects some sort of. . . higher efficiency in more-intelligent brains.”

Despite the hints uncovered about how intelligence comes about, Santarnecchi finds himself
frustrated that research has not yielded more-concrete answers about what he considers one of
neuroscience’s central problems. To address that shortcoming, he’s now spearheading a
consortium of cognitive neuroscientists, engineers, evolutionary biologists, and researchers from
other disciplines to discuss approaches for getting at the biological basis of intelligence.
Santarnecchi would like to see manipulations of the brain—through noninvasive stimulation, for
example—to get at causal relationships between brain activity and cognitive performance. “We
know a lot now about intelligence,” he says, “But I think it’s time to try to answer the question in a
different way.”

Putting the g in genes


As neuroscientists interrogate the brain for how its structure and activity relate to intelligence,
geneticists have approached intelligence from a different angle. Based on what they’ve found so
far, psychology researcher Sophie von Stumm of the London School of Economics estimates that
about 25 percent of individual variation in intelligence will turn out to be explained by single
nucleotide polymorphisms in the genome.

To find genes at play in intelligence, researchers have scanned the genomes of thousands of
people. Earlier this year, for example, economist Daniel Benjamin of the University of Southern
California and colleagues crunched data on upwards of 1.1 million subjects of European descent
and identified more than 1,200 sites in the genome associated with educational attainment, a
common proxy for intelligence.7Because subjects in many types of medical studies in which DNA
is sequenced are asked about their educational status to help control for socioeconomic factors in
later analyses, such data are plentiful. And while the correlation between education and
intelligence is imperfect, “intelligence and school achievement are highly correlated, and
genetically very highly correlated,” says von Stumm, who recently coauthored a review on the
genetics of intelligence.8 Altogether, the genes identified so far accounted for about 11 percent of
individual variation in education level in Benjamin’s study; household income, by comparison,
explained 7 percent.
Such genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have been limited in what they reveal about the
biology at work in intelligence and educational attainment, as much remains to be learned about
the genes thus far identified. But there have been hints, says Benjamin. For example, the genes
with known functions that turned up in his recent study “seem to be involved in pretty much all
aspects of brain development and neuron-to-neuron communication, but not glial cells,” Benjamin
says. Because glial cells affect how quickly neurons transmit signals to one another, this
suggests that firing speed is not a factor in differences in educational attainment.

Other genes seem to link intelligence to various brain diseases. For example, in a
preprint GWASpublished last year, Danielle Posthuma of VU University Amsterdam and
colleagues identified associations between cognitive test scores and variants that are negatively
correlated with depression, ADHD, and schizophrenia, indicating a possible mechanism for
known correlations between intelligence and lower risk for mental disorders. The researchers also
found intelligence-associated variants that are positively correlated with autism.9

Von Stumm is skeptical that genetic data will yield useful information in the near term about how
intelligence results from the brain’s structure or function. But GWAS can yield insights into
intelligence in less direct ways. Based on their results, Benjamin and colleagues devised a
polygenic score that correlates with education level. Although it’s not strong enough to be used to
predict individuals’ abilities, Benjamin says the score should prove useful for researchers, as it
enables them to control for genetics in analyses that aim to identify environmental factors that
influence intelligence. “Our research will allow for better answers to questions about what kinds of
environmental interventions improve student outcomes,” he says.

Von Stumm plans to use Benjamin’s polygenic score to piece together how genes and
environment interact. “We can test directly for the first time,” says von Stumm, “if children who
grow up in impoverished families. . . with fewer resources, if their genetic differences are as
predictive of their school achievement as children who grow up in wealthier families, who have all
the possibilities in the world to grab onto learning opportunities that suit their genetic
predispositions.”

Upping IQ
The idea of manipulating intelligence is enticing, and there has been no shortage of efforts to do
just that. One tactic that once seemed to hold some promise for increasing intelligence is the use
of brain-training games. With practice, players improve their performance on these simple video
games, which rely on skills such as quick reaction time or short-term memorization. But reviews
of numerous studies found no good evidence that such games bolster overall cognitive abilities,
and brain training of this kind is now generally considered a disappointment.

Transcranial brain stimulation, which sends mild electrical or magnetic pulses through the skull,
has shown some potential in recent decades for enhancing intelligence. In 2015, for example,
neurologist Emiliano Santarnecchi of Harvard Medical School and colleagues found that subjects
solved puzzles faster with one type of transcranial alternating current stimulation, while a
2015 meta-analysis found “significant and reliable effects” of another type of electrical stimulation,
transcranial direct current stimulation (Curr Biol, 23:1449–53).

While magnetic stimulation has yielded similarly enticing results, studies of both electrical and
magnetic stimulation have also raised doubts about the effectiveness of these techniques, and
even researchers who believe they can improve cognitive performance admit that we’re a long
way from using them clinically.

See “Noninvasive Brain Stimulation Modulates Memory Networks”

One proven way researchers know to increase intelligence is good old-fashioned education. In
a meta-analysis published earlier this year, a team led by then University of Edinburgh
neuropsychologist Stuart Ritchie (now at King’s College London) sifted out confounding factors
from data reported in multiple studies and found that schooling—regardless of age or level of
education—raises IQ by an average of one to five points per year (Psychol Sci, 29:1358–69).
Researchers, including University of British Columbia developmental cognitive
neuroscientist Adele Diamond, are working to understand what elements of education are most
beneficial to brains.

Intelligence is predictive of a whole host of important things,” such as educational attainment,


career success, and physical and mental health, Ritchie writes in an email to The Scientist, “so
it would be extremely useful if we had reliable ways of raising it.”

Thinking about thinking


It’s not just the biology of intelligence that remains a black box; researchers are still trying to wrap
their minds around the concept itself. Indeed, the idea that g represents a singular property of the
brain has been challenged. While g’s usefulness and predictive power as an index is widely
accepted, proponents of alternative models see it as an average or summation of cognitive
abilities, not a cause.

Last year, University of Cambridge neuro-scientist Rogier Kievit and colleagues published a study
that suggests IQ is an index of the collective strength of more-specialized cognitive skills that
reinforce one another. The results were based on vocabulary and visual reasoning test scores for
hundreds of UK residents in their late teens and early 20s, and from the same subjects about a
year and a half later. With data on the same people at two time points, Kievit says, the
researchers could examine whether performance on one cognitive skill, such as vocabulary or
reasoning, could predict the rate of improvement in another domain. Using algorithms to predict
what changes should have occurred under various models of intelligence, the researchers
concluded that the best fit was mutualism, the idea that different cognitive abilities support one
another in positive feedback loops.10

In 2016, Andrew Conway of Claremont Graduate University in California and Kristóf Kovács, now
of Eötvös Loránd University in Hungary, made a different argument for the involvement of multiple
cognitive processes in intelligence.11 In their model, application-specific neural networks—those
needed for doing simple math or navigating an environment, for example—and high-level,
general-purpose executive processes, such as breaking down a problem into a series of small,
manageable blocks, each play a role in helping a person complete cognitive tasks. It’s the fact
that a variety of tasks tap into the same executive processes that explains why individuals’
performance on disparate tasks correlates, and it’s the average strength of these higher-order
processes, not a singular ability, that’s measured by g, the researchers argue. Neuroscientists
might make more progress in understanding intelligence by looking for the features of the brain
that carry out particular executive processes, rather than for the seat of a single g factor, Kovács
says.

As researchers grapple with the intractable phenomenon of intelligence, a philosophical question


arises: Is our species smart enough to understand the basis of our own intelligence? While those
in the field generally agree that science has a long way to go to make sense of how we think,
most express cautious optimism that the coming decades will yield major insights.

“We see now the development, not only of mapping brain connections in human beings . . . we’re
also beginning to see synapse mapping,” Haier says. “This will take our understanding of the
basic biological mechanisms of things like intelligence . . . to a whole new level.”

References
1. J. Flynn, “Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure,” Psychol Bull,
101:171-91, 1987.
2. J.A. Kaminski et al., “Epigenetic variance in dopamine D2 receptor: A marker of IQ
malleability?” Transl Psychiat, 8:169, 2018.
3. R.E. Jung, R.J. Haier, “The parieto-frontal integration theory (P-FIT) of intelligence:
Converging neuroimaging evidence,” Behav Brain Sci, 30:135–87, 2007.
4. M. Lundqvist et al., “Gamma and beta bursts during working memory readout suggest
roles in its volitional control,” Nat Comm, 9:394, 2018.
5. A.K. Barbey, “Network neuroscience theory of human intelligence,” Trends Cogn Sci,
22:8–20, 2018.
6. E. Santarnecchi, S. Rossi, “Advances in the neuroscience of intelligence: From brain
connectivity to brain perturbation,” Span J Psychol, 19:E94, 2016.
7. J.J. Lee et al., “Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association
study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals,” Nat Genet, 50:1112–21, 2018.
8. R. Plomin, S. von Stumm, “The new genetics of intelligence,” Nat Rev Genet, 19:148–59,
2018.
9. J.E. Savage et al., “Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals
identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence,” Nat Genet, 50:912–19, 2018.
10. R.A. Kievit et al., “Mutualistic coupling between vocabulary and reasoning supports
cognitive development during late adolescence and early adulthood,” Psychol Sci,
28:1419–31, 2017.
11. K. Kovács, A.R.A. Conway, “Process overlap theory: A unified account of the general
factor of intelligence,” Psychol Inq, 27:151-177, 2016.

You might also like