Peigneux Urbain Schmitz 2012 SleepandtheBrain

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 84

Sleep and the Brain – page 1/1

SLEEP AND THE BRAIN

Philippe PEIGNEUX 1,2,*and Charline URBAIN 1,*, Remy SCHMITZ1

1
Neuropsychology and Functional Neuroimaging Research Unit, Université Libre de

Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium.

2
Cyclotron Research Centre, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium.

*
These authors share equal first authorship. To whom correspondence should be addressed.

Mailing address:

Neuropsychology and Functional Neuroimaging Research Unit [UR2NF]

Faculty of Psychological and Educational Sciences,

Université Libre de Bruxelles (ULB)

Campus du Solbosch, CP191

Avenue F.D. Roosevelt 50

B-1050 Bruxelles

BELGIUM

Web: http://dev.ulb.ac.be/ur2nf

Email: [email protected] or [email protected]


Sleep and the Brain – page 2/2

ABSTRACT

The phenomenological experience of sleep as a cessation of waking activity is misleading.

Indeed, it suggests that sleep constitutes, like a switch, a simple mechanism by which are shut

off all neurophysiological processes associated with an active and costly wake state of

vigilance. In this chapter, we present a summary description of sleep and its defining features,

viewed from behavioural, neurobiological, neurophysiological and functional

neuroanatomical perspectives. Given the universality of sleep and/or sleep-like phenomena

across animal species, we review also the phylogenesis of sleep. As the reader will realize, the

simplistic view that sleep is a mere state of inactivity must be replaced by the conception of a

complex, multidimensional and active state of the brain.

Keywords: NREM, REM, SWS, USWS, Neurobiology, Neurophysiology, Functional

Neuroanatomy, Phylogenesis, EEG, PET, fMRI, MEG

Acknowledgments: CU is supported by a grant from the Fondation Vigneron; RS is

supported by the Fonds National de la Recherche Scientifique (FNRS); Belgium.


Sleep and the Brain – page 3/3

At the behavioural level, sleep presents a number of definite features that humans share with

most animal species (Borbely, Hayaishi, Sejnowski, & Altman, 2000). It is a state of physical

quiescence, mostly characterized by no or minimal movements, closure of the eyes and a

stereotypic body posture (e.g. in man, lying down or curling; in bats, hanging upside down).

As compared to quiet wakefulness, there is also reduced responsiveness to external

stimulation during sleep. Additionally, rapid reversibility between states (e.g. the fact that the

sleeper will wake up if the stimulation is reasonably distinctive and/or the need for sleep is

exhausted) distinguishes sleep from other specific states of altered vigilance like coma, brain

death, hypothermia or hibernation.

However, behavioural observations alone are usually not sufficient to ensure that the

individual under scrutiny is genuinely sleeping, rather than being in a relaxed state of

wakefulness or in an intermediate state of drowsiness. Furthermore, mere observation cannot

capture the dramatic changes in cerebral activity from wakefulness to sleep, nor the fact that

sleep is composed of two distinct physiological stages: Non-Rapid Eye Movement (NREM)

and Paradoxical or Rapid Eye Movement (REM) sleep. In humans, NREM sleep is further

subdivided into 3 stages according to sleep depth (Silber et al., 2007), from the intermediate

stage 1 (drowsiness and sleep onset) to light stage 2 sleep then the deepest stage 3 sleep

(previously subdivided into NREM sleep stages 3 and 4; see below), also known as Slow

Wave Sleep (SWS). Under normal nocturnal conditions, NREM always precedes REM sleep

within an ultradian cycle lasting on average 90 minutes and repeating itself (Figure 1), with

the consequence that the sleeper may complete up to five cycles in a typical night (Dement,

1978).

--- INSERT FIGURE 1 HERE ---


Sleep and the Brain – page 4/4

Additionally, sleep is a regulated phenomenon, whose timing and duration are contingent

upon two main processes: homeostatic and circadian (Borbely, 1982; Daan, Beersma, &

Borbely, 1984; see Figure 2). During the course of a normal day-night cycle, the pressure for

sleep continuously accumulates with time spent awake, then dissipates during the night of

sleep. Extending the waking period above normal levels (e.g. in a sleep deprivation protocol)

results in a sleep debt with a rebound on the subsequent night where both duration and

intensity are increased, demonstrating the homeostatic regulation of sleep. This gradual

change in sleep pressure is counterbalanced by the circadian process, e.g. a nearly 24-hr

endogenous oscillatory variation in alertness and sleep propensity. The circadian signal for

sleep/alertness is high/low in the early hours and low/high in the evening, with the

consequence that homeostatic and circadian processes work in opposition during the day but

in synchrony during the night to ideally ensure consolidated periods of wakefulness and sleep.

The interaction between these two processes also exerts an influence on variations in

cognitive performance throughout the day, especially in the attentional domain (Schmidt,

Collette, Cajochen, & Peigneux, 2007; Schmidt et al., 2009). Due to the close relationship

between Slow Wave Activity (SWA) in SWS and sleep pressure dissipation (e.g. the

homeostatic process) along the night of sleep, the first half of the night is particularly rich in

slow wave activity (about 80% of time) whereas in each subsequent cycle, the length of REM

periods increases while the length of deep SWS is decreasing (Figure 1), the amount of stage

2 sleep remaining fairly constant.

--- INSERT FIGURE 2 HERE ---

--- INSERT INFORMATION BOX 1 (Polysomnographic Techniques) ABOUT HERE ---

It is Hans Berger who performed in 1929 the first recording of the electrical activity of the

brain, that he named electroencephalography (EEG). He described different oscillation


Sleep and the Brain – page 5/5

patterns in the EEG, the best known being the Berger’s wave or alpha (8-12 Hz) occipital

rhythm, observable during quiet wakefulness but suppressed by the opening of the eyes. He

also showed first that the frequency of EEG oscillations is slowing down with sleep, followed

by Loomis (1937) who divided sleep into five levels (A to E) representing the spectrum of

waking to NREM sleep stages 1-4. REM sleep was only later on discovered as a distinct stage

of sleep, with low-voltage rapid oscillations in brain activity, muscular atonia and rapid ocular

movements (Aserinsky & Kleitman, 1953; Dement & Kleitman, 1957). These findings and

the availability of electrophysiological and physiological techniques (see Information Box 1)

eventually led to the first consensus on how human sleep should be recorded and scored: the

“Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques and Scoring System for Sleep Stages of

Human Subjects” published by Rechtschaffen and Kales (1968). Nowadays, sleep research

has evolved from a fascinating field of human physiology towards a dedicated scientific

discipline and a recognized medical speciality, with accredited clinical units for patients

suffering from a wide spectrum of sleep disorders. Although the classification and

codification of sleep patterns still remains performed more than 40 years later upon visual and

computer-based inspection of 30-second polysomnographic epochs of

electroencephalographic (EEG), electrooculographic (EOG) and electromyographic (EMG)

signals, technological advances have allowed the development of novel methods for the

analysis of sleep (e.g. spectral analysis thought to better reflect specific discharge patterns

observed at the cellular level in NREM sleep stages; Corsi-Cabrera, Guevara, Del Rio-

Portilla, Arce, & Villanueva-Hernandez, 2000; Corsi-Cabrera, Munoz-Torres, Del Rio-

Portilla, & Guevara, 2005). Notwithstanding, limitations of the Rechtschaffen and Kales’s

classification system have been thoroughly discussed (e.g. see Himanen & Hasan, 2000),

leading to revised international consensus guidelines implemented in the “New Sleep Scoring

Manual” (Iber, Ancoli-Israel, Chesson, & Quan, 2007), which we will use below to delineate
Sleep and the Brain – page 6/6

the different stages of sleep and wakefulness (see also Summary Table 1 at the end of this

section). One salient modification amongst others in the “New Sleep Scoring Manual” is the

coalescence of NREM sleep stages 3 and 4 from Rechtschaffen and Kales (1968) into a

single, unified NREM sleep stage 3. This change is actually fitting with an established clinical

and research practice where stages 3 and 4 are commonly considered together in the analyses

and interpretations.

Wakefulness

Wakefulness can be defined as a state of full consciousness, allowing interaction with the

environment. In humans the waking EEG is mostly characterized by a so-called

desynchronized pattern of neuronal firing, reflected in the EEG by rapid, high frequency

waves in the beta (β) range (14–40 Hz) with low amplitude (10 to 30 µV) and high muscular

tonus at the EMG. When subjects are awake in a quiet relaxed state or resting, especially with

eyes closed, EEG recorded from the posterior part of the brain (mostly at occipital locations)

displays patterns of alpha (α) or Berger waves of lower frequency (8–12 Hz) and increased

amplitude (20 to 40 µV; Figure 3), reflecting the increased synchrony of underlying neural

activity in non-stimulated brain regions (Borbely et al., 2000).

--- INSERT FIGURE 3 HERE ---

NREM sleep

NREM sleep stages are characterized in humans by regular respiration, decreased in muscular

tonus and gradual slowing down of the EEG frequency patterns with increasing amplitude

(Figure 3), from stages N1 to N2 or light sleep then N3 or deep slow wave sleep (SWS),

reflecting the progressive synchronisation of activity within and between neuronal

populations. Spectral analyses additionally reveal a background pattern of slow wave activity
Sleep and the Brain – page 7/7

(SWA) below 1 Hz (Achermann & Borbely, 1997), not visually apparent in the EEG

recording but having important physiological implications (Steriade, Contreras, Curro Dossi,

& Nunez, 1993). Superimposed on slow rhythmic activities are transient events, mainly K

complexes and sleep spindles in N2 whose recurrence (every 3-10 seconds) is regulated by

the underlying SWA. (Steriade & Amzica, 1998)

NREM sleep stage N1 and sleep onset

NREM sleep stage N1 is a drowsiness transition period between wakefulness and sleep. Stage

N1 is characterized by the occurrence of theta (θ) waves in the EEG, slower in frequency (4-8

Hz) and greater in amplitude (50 to 100 µV) than alpha (α) waves, a decrease in muscular

activity and slow eye movements. A valid electrophysiological marker of sleep onset is the

attenuation of the alpha rhythm, which signs the transition from relaxed wakefulness to stage

N1. It is therefore important to record EEG at occipital derivations where the alpha rhythm is

prominent to accurately determine sleep onset (Silber et al., 2007). At the behavioural level,

subjects start failing to respond at the presentation of auditory tones, although awakening

thresholds remain low, to the point that subjects awoken during this stage may often report

not having been asleep at all.

NREM sleep Stage N2

During stage N2, also known as light NREM sleep, eye movements cease, heart rate slows

down and body temperature decreases, preparing the body to enter in deep sleep. Background

EEG oscillations decrease below 5 Hz in the delta (δ) range (1-4 Hz). Superimposed on slow

oscillations are periodical, transient EEG events named sleep spindles and K complexes

(figure 3). Spindles are short-lasting (0.5-3 sec), waxing and waning oscillations in the sigma

(σ) range (11-16 Hz), whose maximal amplitude is mostly found over central regions

(Borbely et al., 2000; De Gennaro, Ferrara, & Bertini, 2000; McCormick, Nielsen, Nicolas,
Sleep and the Brain – page 8/8

Ptito, & Montplaisir, 1997). Behavioural, electrophysiological (De Gennaro and Ferrara,

2003) and neuroimaging (Schabus et al., 2007) data further suggest a functional

differentiation and distinct neuroanatomical bases for slow (11-13 Hz) and fast (13-15 Hz)

spindles. The K complexes are graphoelements characterized by a negative sharp wave of

high amplitude immediately followed by a positive component standing out from the

background EEG, with a total duration at least equal to 0.5 second. Because of their

morphology and duration, K complexes are sufficiently distinctive and no amplitude criterion

is needed (Borbely et al., 2000). K complexes are optimally recorded at frontal electrodes

(e.g. F4 or Fz). Stage N2 should be persistently scored in the absence of K complexes or

spindles until a transition to stage N3 or REM sleep, or the occurrence of a major body

movement followed by slow eye movements or an arousal.

NREM sleep Stage N3 or SWS

N3 is the deepest stage of NREM sleep, also called “Slow Wave Sleep” (SWS), which can be

scored when slower waves in the 0.5-2 Hz frequency range represent at least 20% of the 30-

second EEG epoch. Because the cut-off was previously set at 4 Hz for the superior limit of

slow waves, which may raise confusions with delta activity, revised guidelines strongly

discourage the use of the term “delta waves” (e.g. in the 1-4 Hz range) to mention SWS

(Silber et al., 2007). Usually, slow waves are maximally expressed at frontal sites (Happe et

al., 2002), as well as K complexes. N3 being the deepest stage of sleep, arousal thresholds are

very high, with a longer period of sleep inertia after awakening and possible post-awakening

confusion or disorientation (also known as “sleep drunkenness”).

REM sleep

Contrary to NREM stages, REM sleep is characterized by the presence of desynchronized,

rapid low-amplitude EEG activity visually close to that recorded during wakefulness, hence
Sleep and the Brain – page 9/9

its other name of “paradoxical” sleep (Jouvet, Michel, & Courjon, 1959). Five main features

determine REM sleep: low amplitude-mixed frequency (15-30Hz, <50 µV) EEG background,

rapid eye movements, muscular atonia and breath/cardiovascular irregularity. Rapid eye

movements (REMs) are conjugate, irregular and sharply peaked eye movements with an

initial deflection usually lasting less than 500 milliseconds. Presence of saw tooth waves or

transient muscle activity (phasic twitches) is strongly supportive evidence, especially when

one or more of the basic features are equivocal. During the first REM period of the night, one

may observe interspersed K complexes or sleep spindles but in the presence of unequivocal

REMs, theses epochs should be scored as REM sleep until there is a transition to stage W, N2

or N3, manifestations of arousal or major body movements followed by slow eye movements.

--- INSERT SUMMARY TABLE 1 HERE ---

THE PHYLOGENESIS OF SLEEP

Alternation between activity and sleep-like resting states has been found across all studied

animal species up to now, although physiological and neurophysiological characteristics may

largely differ. The universality of sleep-like resting states suggests an important functionality

(or most probably a series of important functionalities) that has/have built up with evolution,

hence the paramount importance to explore the phylogenesis of sleep. Furthermore, even

though sleep appears to be an universal phenomenon, studies conducted in insects,

amphibians, reptiles, avians, mammals, etc, suggest that the differentiation between NREM

and REM sleep states, typical of mammalians and birds, is not present -- or at least cannot be

convincingly disclosed -- in all branches of the evolutionary tree, raising supplementary

questions about the complementary role of our two main states of sleep in evolution. Even if a

logical scheme has not yet been clearly disclosed and adaptations have occurred in some
Sleep and the Brain – page 10/10

species, studying sleep in the animal kingdom should eventually allow us to understand the

phylogenetic characteristics of sleep and target its essential components.

As a word of caution, a main caveat when comparing species is that similar levels of

investigations are not always available. On the one hand, sophisticated intra-cerebral

recordings obtained in cats and rodents are only available in exceptional circumstances in

humans (e.g. during pre-surgical recordings) due to their invasive character. On the other

hand, polysomnographic recordings (EEG, EMG, and EOG) in animals have usually to be

carried in laboratory settings, hence preventing their observation in the natural environment,

potentially biasing the results. Although modern telemetric recording techniques make it now

possible to equip the animal of a signal wireless transmitter, allowing recording in more

natural settings, these are not yet generalized and well adapted to all species. Therefore, many

species have been mostly studied at the behavioural level, which limits the scope of possible

comparisons.

Sleep or “close to sleep” states in insects

Insects have a daily rhythm of rest and activity. It is not merely due to the influence of the

dark-light cycle since it continues being observed even when time synchronizers (also named

“zeitgebers”) are removed from the environment. Evidence of a close-to-sleep state in insects

has long been mostly behavioural. For instance, it has been found that the scorpion adopts a

specific position where the body and the head rest flat on the ground, whereas in the bee the

antennae are folded up against the head (Tobler & Neuner-Jehle, 1992). Still, because mere

observation cannot easily delineate boundaries between sleep and quiet wakefulness in

insects, presence of a sleep-analogue condition has been further corroborated using the

homeostatic sleep pressure criteria. In the fruit fly (Drosophila melanogaster), the most

studied kind given its relative simplicity, a homeostatically regulated sleep state has been
Sleep and the Brain – page 11/11

observed, that in many respects is similar to sleep in mammals (Hendricks et al., 2000; Huber

et al., 2004; Shaw, Cirelli, Greenspan, & Tononi, 2000). Indeed, insects exhibit an increase in

the duration of the resting state when prior wakefulness is artificially maintained above usual

duration levels of activity, e.g. by moving the insect’s container or introducing a congener in

its environment (Bell-Pedersen et al., 2005; Huber et al., 2004). Furthermore, genetic

mutations affect sleep in Drosophila; conversely, sleep affects gene expression (Cirelli &

Bushey, 2008). Notwithstanding, although changes in brain electrical activity (Nitz, van

Swinderen, Tononi, & Greenspan, 2002) and changes in synaptic markers (Gilestro, Tononi,

& Cirelli, 2009) are reliably correlated with activity state, sleep in invertebrates seems to lack

the large-scale, slow, synchronous neuronal activity similar to that occurring during

mammalian and avian SWS (Cirelli & Bushey, 2008; Hendricks & Sehgal, 2004; van

Swinderen & Andretic, 2003).

Activity/inactivity states in vertebrates without thermal regulation

Fish

Although several studies have reported behavioural sleep in Fishes (specific resting posture,

periods of reduced activity …), only few studies have investigated EEG (for a review see

Hartse, 1994). In tench (Tinca Tinca), simultaneous recording was made of the electric

activity of the brain, muscles and gills activity, respiratory and heartbeat rates, and reactivity

to sensorial stimulations. There was no clear difference between the rapid, low voltage

electrical brain activity during the activity (night) as compared to the rest (day), nor was it

possible to evidence periods of eye movements correlated with vegetative variations

(Peyrethon & Dusan-Peyrethon, 1967; Zhdanova, 2006 for similar results in the zebra fish).

At variance in the catfish (Ictalurus nebulosus), the transition from activity to rest was

characterized by an increase in low frequency activity and spikes (Karmanova, 1978).

Additionally, there is no indication that a periodic state similar to paradoxical sleep exists in
Sleep and the Brain – page 12/12

fishes, besides evidence for a global sleep-like state. Also, it remains a matter of debate

whether unihemispherical sleep (see below) exists in constantly swimming fishes, doubts

being raised in part by the absence of unilateral eye closure (Kavanau, 1998).

Amphibians

Available data on sleep in Amphibians are scarce (for a review see Hartse, 1994). Some

species maintain constant vigilance levels during behavioural rest (e.g. the bull-frog), whereas

reactivity to sensorial stimulations decreases in others, e.g. the tree-dwelling frog. Similarly,

EEG changes correlated with behavioural sleep have been observed in some species, but not

in others. Also, there is no evidence for periods of ocular movements during the quiescent

stage in Amphibians. Unilateral eye closure has only been reported in the bull frog (Rana

catesbiana) (Hobson, 1967), however only occurring briefly during respiratory acts and

apparently unrelated with behavioural sleep.

Reptiles

Mammals and reptiles are neighbours in evolution, having led many researchers to investigate

vigilance states in reptiles. Sleep has been studied using continuous polysomnographic

recordings (EEG, EMG, EOG, ECG, respiration) in chelonians (tortoises), crocodilians

(alligators and caimans) and saurians (lizards and snakes). Unequivocal signs of sleep are

present in reptiles at the behavioural level, associated in most studies with intermittent high-

amplitude, spikes and sharp waves in the EEG. At variance, few other studies have reported

an association with high-amplitude, low-frequency activity (Warner, Huggins, 1978) or failed

to detect any sleep-related changes in the EEG (Walker & Berger, 1973).

Although still controversial, there are convincing arguments to support the hypothesis

that high-amplitude, spikes and sharp waves define a reptilian sleep state functionally

homologous to the mammalian and avian SWS (Hartse, 1994). Indeed, EEG spikes are
Sleep and the Brain – page 13/13

associated with the presence of a behavioural sleep state in reptilians, and their density

increases after sleep deprivation in chelonian (Flanigan, 1974), iguanid (Flanigan, 1973) and

crocodilian reptiles (Flanigan, Wilcox, & Rechtschaffen, 1973), suggesting homeostatic

regulation. In vitro (Lorenzo, Macadar, & Velluti, 1999; Lorenzo, Velluti, 2004) and in vivo

(Gaztelu, Garcı´a-Austt & Bullock, 1991; Lorenzo et al., 1999) studies in turtles further

suggest that sleep-related spikes in reptilians originate in the medial cortex (the anatomical

and functional homologous of the mammalian hippocampus; Lopez, Vargas, Gomez, & Salas,

2003), then propagate in the adjacent dorsal cortex.

The lack of high amplitude, slow waves in reptiles has long been attributed to the

absence in these species of the thick six-layered neocortex responsible for generating EEG

slow waves in mammals (Hartse, 1994). Alternate hypotheses propose nowadays (a) that the

reptilian dorsal cortex lacks the interconnectivity necessary to generate sleep-related slow

waves in the electroencephalogram (Rattenborg, 2006; 2007), (b) that the

thalamocorticothalamic circuitry is less elaborate in reptiles despite similarities across

mammals, birds and reptiles in the basic neurochemistry, neuroanatomy and neurophysiology

of the thalamic reticular nucleus (Kenigfest et al., 2005; Llinas & Steriade, 2006), involved in

the genesis of sleep-related neuronal oscillations in mammals (Steriade, 2006), and/or (c) that

differences in the type and/or density of glia (Ari & Kalman, 2008) may explain why reptiles

lack SWS (Rattenborg, Martinez-Gonzalez, & Lesku, 2009), given that glia may amplify the

effect of corticocortical connectivity on neuronal synchrony during sleep (Amzica & Steriade,

2000).

Regarding to REM sleep analogues in reptiles, recorded sleep episodes associated with

rapid eye movements and heartbeat variations are very brief (a few seconds) and extremely

rare (a few minutes in several weeks of recording ) in crocodilians and lizards. The analogy of

these episodes with paradoxical sleep is still discussed. Also, the prevalence of unilateral eye
Sleep and the Brain – page 14/14

closure during behavioural sleep suggests that reptilian sleep may occur unihemispherical

(Flanigan et al., 1973; Tauber, Roffwarg, & Weitzman, 1966).

REM and NREM sleep in birds and mammals

Since the discovery of REM sleep as a distinct phase in mammalian sleep, researchers have

searched for the origin and causes of the duality between REM and NREM sleep. Because

birds and mammals have evolved independently without common reptilian ancestors, the

presence of REM sleep in both suggests that this state has appeared independently in the two

phylogenetic branches to fulfil a function essential to more complex brains (Jouvet, 1994),

leaving unsolved for now the question of its origins.

Sleep in birds

Like mammals, birds have NREM-REM sleep cycles (Campbell & Tobler, 1984; Lesku,

Roth, Rattenborg, Amlaner, & Lima, 2009; Rattenborg, Amlaner, 2002). They are also the

only other taxonomic group than mammals exhibiting high-amplitude slow waves in the EEG

during NREM sleep (Rattenborg et al., 2009), departing from the high-amplitude spikes and

sharp waves of reptilian sleep. Although some controversies persist, slow wave activity

during SWS appears to be homeostatically regulated in birds. For instance, SWA increases in

pigeons following short-term sleep deprivation (Martinez-Gonzalez, Lesku, & Rattenborg,

2008). It is hypothesised that the confluent evolution of homeostatically regulated SWS in

birds and mammals is directly linked to their common evolution towards large, heavily

interconnected brains capable of performing complex cognitive processes (Rattenborg et al.,

2009).

Adaptative sleep in birds

Like most mammals, birds exhibit bihemispheric NREM and REM sleep in many conditions.

However, they have also developed the ability to sleep with one eye open, the contralateral
Sleep and the Brain – page 15/15

hemisphere being awake to overcome the problem of sleeping in specific situations. Spooner

(1964) was first to observe an association between unilateral eye closure and

unihemispherical slow wave sleep (USWS) in young chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus). In

those, the hemisphere contralateral to the open eye showed EEG activity typical of

wakefulness, whereas in the other hemisphere EEG activity was typical of SWS. The fact that

birds are able to control which hemisphere can sleep in response to changes in anticipated

risks suggests that USWS and unilateral eye closure serve a predator detection function.

Indeed, mallard ducks positioned at the edge of the group during rest (a position perceived by

the animals as dangerous; Bednekoff & Ritter, 1994; Elgar, 1989) exhibit a 150% increase in

USWS and a strong preference for directing the open eye toward the potential source of

danger (Rattenborg, Lima, & Amlaner, 1999), as compared to ducks safely sleeping in the

centre of the group, who display bihemispheric sleep. Thus, birds not only have the capacity

to control unihemispherical sleep, but also use this ability in a manner that reduces the

conflict between sleep need and predator detection. Although birds may also use USWS to

continuously monitor their environment for other purposes (e.g. food availability, weather

variations, etc.), predator detection is likely to be the most important given its surviving value.

Additional evidence suggests that some birds use unihemispherical sleep while migrating,

allowing sleep while engaged in long overseas travels.


Sleep and the Brain – page 16/16

Sleep in mammals

Even though mammals represent a small fraction of living species, they have been logically

the most thoroughly investigated, humans belonging to this class. Alternating patterns

between NREM and REM sleep were found in almost one hundred species studied using

polysomnographic recordings, suggesting the generality of NREM-REM sleep patterns in

mammalians, but few exceptions discussed later (Fuchs, Maury, Moore, & Bingman, 2009).

REM sleep in mammals

Following the discovery of rapid eye movements (REMs) during sleep by Aserinsky and

Kleitman in 1953, tentatively linked to dreaming activity, William Dement found in 1957 an

association between REMs and distinctive patterns of cortical EEG activity in the sleeping

cat, hence the name of REM sleep for this state. At the same time, the French neurobiologist

Michel Jouvet, who was investigating subcortical activities in sleeping cats, observed periods

of muscle atonia arousing simultaneously with high-voltage spiky waves in the EEG, similar

enough to the waking stage (Jouvet & Michel 1959). The finding was curious and certainly a

paradoxical phenomenon, because at that time fast cortical activity was still regarded as the

unmistakable electrophysiological sign of wakefulness and muscle atonia as an invariable sign

of sleep. This apparent discrepancy led Jouvet (1959) to name this strange state of wake-like

brain activity ‘‘paradoxical sleep’’. It was soon recognized that REM and Paradoxical sleep

were two names for the same phenomenon, which was further studied by Jouvet and others in

more than one hundred species of vertebrates. Their main conclusion was that this essential

phase of sleep exists only among warm-blooded animals.

Outstanding exceptions to standard NREM-REM sleep patterns are archaic

monotremes echidna and platypus who are primitive mammals that lay eggs, as birds or

reptiles do, and cetaceans (whales and dolphins).


Sleep and the Brain – page 17/17

Archaic sleep in monotremes

Most studies investigating mammalian sleep have been performed on eutherians or

marsupials, the two main subclasses of mammals. The third subclass is monotremes, found in

Australia and New Guinea, whose two only alive species are echidna and platypus. Fossil and

genetic evidence indicate that monotremes diverged from other mammalian lines about 150

million years ago, and that echidna and platypus are derived from a platypus-like ancestor

(Clemens, 1989; Flannery, 1989; Westerman, 1992). They have more genetic and

physiological similarities with reptiles and birds than other mammals, and are thought to

possess characteristics of the common mammalian ancestor (Grutzner et al., 2004), hence

there are considered “living fossil mammals” which may inform us about the evolution of

sleep.

Initial studies have suggested that echidna lacks REM sleep (Allison & Van Twyver,

1972), because no typical REM sleep low-voltage EEG pattern is observed at the cortical

level like in eutherian and marsupial mammals. However, further investigations have shown

that brainstem activity is different from that seen in other mammals during NREM sleep and

resemble that seen in REM sleep. Indeed, activity of brainstem neurons decreases like during

NREM sleep, but variability increases as during REM sleep (Siegel, Manger, Nienhuis,

Fahringer, & Pettigrew, 1996). Since it is known that brainstem neuronal activity generates

REM sleep, these results suggest that echidna has a REM sleep-like state, but not

accompanied by low-voltage cortical EEG like in other mammals. In the platypus who was

also thought devoid of REM sleep, behavioural observation and telemetric measures have

demonstrated atonia with rapid eye movements, twitching and the electrocardiogram pattern

of rapid eye movement, altogether with moderate of high voltage cortical EEG typical of

NREM sleep and REM-like brainstem neuronal activation during 6-8 hours a day, actually

proposing the platypus as a champion of REM sleep (Siegel et al., 1999). These results in
Sleep and the Brain – page 18/18

monotremes suggest that low-voltage electroencephalogram and cortical desynchronization

are more recently evolved features of mammalian REM sleep, which may have had its

precursor state in reptilian species (Siegel et al., 1999).

The evolving sleep of marine mammals

In his book "The Promise of Sleep", William C. Dement (1999) notices that the dolphin,

originally a land mammal that has returned to the sea, maintains a number of terrestrial traits

including bearing live offspring and, unlike fish, breathing air. At variance with terrestrial

mammals however, breathing is under voluntary control in cetaceans -- a process that should

preclude sleep. In order to solve what seems an impossible equation, cetaceans have therefore

opted for unihemispherical sleep (Figure 4), allowing one half of the brain to go to sleep while

the other half remains awake (Lyamin, Manger, Ridgway, Mukhametov, & Siegel, 2008;

Siegel, 2005). Thus, high-voltage slow waves almost never occur in both hemispheres at the

same time, with the eye contra-lateral to the hemisphere with slow waves being closed while

the other eye remains open (Lyamin, Mukhametov, & Siegel, 2004; Mukhametov, Lyamin,

Chetyrbok, Vassilyev, & Diaz, 1992; Mukhametov, Supin, & Polyakova, 1977).

--- INSERT FIGURE 4 HERE ---

Unihemispherical SWS (USWS) may also serve additional functions like efficient predator

monitoring in the environment and thermogenesis, which has been probably important in the

evolution of USWS in cetaceans but also for their apparent lack of REM sleep (Lyamin et al.,

2008). Indeed, no REM sleep episodes have been detected in any of the four species examined

by Mukhametov and colleagues (Lyamin et al., 2002; Mukhametov, 1984; Mukhametov &

1995; Mukhametov, Oleksenko, & Poliakova, 1988; Mukhametov et al., 1977), despite long

periods of electrophysiological recording, making cetaceans the only studied mammals in

which REM sleep seems to be totally inexistent, at least when accounting for the unusual
Sleep and the Brain – page 19/19

form of REM sleep in monotremes. Only one published study has reported EEG features of

REM sleep with marked loss of tonus of trunk muscle in a pilot whale, however for no more

than 6 minutes and one time only out of three non-consecutive nights (Shurley, Serafetinides,

Brooks, Elsner, & Kenney, 1969). USWS has only been observed in three aquatic orders (e.g.

Cetacea, Pinnipedia and Sirenia) among mammals. In cetaceans, virtually all of their sleep is

of USWS type, whereas eared seals and manatees may also display bihemispheric SWS

(BSWS) and REM sleep (Rattenborg, Amlaner, & Lima, 2000)

Unlike the terrestrial environment, there are few warm, safe places to sleep in the

ocean. This may explain why, contrary to all terrestrial mammals, killer whales (Orcinus

orca) and dolphins have minimal amounts of sleep behaviour (e.g., immobility or eye closure)

at birth, slowly increasing to adult levels over months. It may allow the neonate to

thermoregulate in cold ocean water and to be protected during development while swimming

with its mother. As the animal gains mass and blubber and approaches adult size, adult-like

sleep behaviour emerges, including periods of immobility. The mother also presents a near

absence of typical sleep behaviour during the postpartum period (Siegel, 2005), for periods

that greatly exceed those of sleep deprivation reported to kill rats (Rechtschaffen, 1998).

Interestingly, neither mother nor calf seem to exhibit a rebound in the amount of sleep

behaviour after this period, questioning the presence of homeostatic regulation in cetacean

sleep. Still, some evidence has been observed in one study for a rebound after experimental

deprivation of slow waves, but responses were highly variable among animals (Oleksenko,

Mukhametov, Polyakova, Supin, & Kovalzon, 1992). Besides differences in recording

methods, the peculiar properties of cetaceans’ sleep (e.g. USWS combined with lack of

typical motor and sensorial inactivity and apparent lack of homeostatic regulation) are

questioning the very nature of the essential constituents of sleep. Further investigations are

necessary to determine which, if any, neurochemical and neurophysiological aspects of sleep,


Sleep and the Brain – page 20/20

besides unilateral neocortical slow waves and eye closure, are preserved in cetaceans and

might constitute the core substance of sleep (Manger, Ridgway, & Siegel, 2003).

NEUROBIOLOGY OF SLEEP

Initiation and control of sleep-wake cycles, and alternation between sleep states are regulated

by activity in specific neuronal populations located in two broad regions, namely the basal

forebrain and hypothalamus. Before describing their mode of action, it is first necessary to

review the neurochemical systems that promote cerebral arousal and prevent the organism

falling asleep (Figure 5), allowing further understanding of the basic mechanisms of sleep

initiation and maintenance (Datta & Maclean, 2007). Over the past decades, a large set of

neurochemical-specific wake-promoting cells have been identified within the Ascending

Activating System (AAS) in the reticular formation, namely acetylcholine, histamine,

norepinephrine, serotonin, hypocretin/orexine, dopamine and glutamate. Dorsal projections

from these pontine and midbrain wake-promoting cells activate thalamocortical systems,

whereas ventral projections activate hypothalamo-cortical and baso-cortical systems (Garcia-

Rill, 2002)

--- INSERT FIGURE 5 HERE ---

Acetylcholine

(Ach)-synthesizing cholinergic cells are mostly located in the pedunculopontine tegmentum

(PPT) of the brainstem and in the anterior hypothalamus (AH) of the basal forebrain (BF).

Activity in these neuronal groups is high during wakefulness (maximally active in the basal

forebrain) and REM sleep, and strongly diminished during NREM sleep (minimally active for

PPT cholinergic cells). From a functional point of view, PPT and BF cholinergic cells are part

of the main arousal systems in the brainstem and forebrain, respectively. These neuronal

populations are causally involved in the generation of the desynchronized EEG pattern both in
Sleep and the Brain – page 21/21

wake and REM sleep stages (Datta & Maclean, 2007; Steriade, Pare, Datta, Oakson, & Curro

Dossi, 1990) by releasing acetylcholine in the diencephalon, hence blocking oscillatory

discharges of thalamic mechanisms responsible for EEG spindles and slow waves

(McCormick, 1989; Steriade, McCormick, & Sejnowski, 1993). Although sharing many

properties, these two clusters of cholinergic cells also present specific features:

a) Cholinergic cells in the pedunculopontine tegmentum

The pedunculopontine tegmentum (PPT) nucleus, being one of the major aggregations

of cholinergic neurons in the mammalian brainstem, promotes wakefulness by

activating thalamo-cortical, hypothalamo-cortical, baso-cortical, suprachiasmatic, and

amygdaloid wake-promoting systems of the forebrain (Datta, 1995; Datta & Siwek,

1997). Bursting discharges of a subgroup of these PPT neurons are also involved in

generation of ponto-geniculo-occipital waves (PGO; see below). Additionally, some

PPT neurons selectively active during REM sleep participate to the suppression of

muscle tone preventing motor activity during this stage of sleep (Datta & Maclean,

2007; Lyamin et al., 2008).

b) Cholinergic cells of the basal forebrain

Cholinergic cells in the basal forebrain (BF) play an important role in hippocampal

and neocortical activation (Detari, Juhasz, & Kukorelli, 1984; Detari & Vanderwolf,

1987; Nunez, 1996; Stewart, MacFabe, & Vanderwolf, 1984). They receive input from

other brainstem and hypothalamic wake-promoting systems and, in turn, have

widespread projections to the cerebral cortex (Detari & Vanderwolf, 1987; Fisher,

Buchwald, Hull, & Levine, 1988; Gritti, Mainville, Mancia, & Jones, 1997;

Zaborszky, Carlsen, Brashear, & Heimer, 1986a; Zaborszky, Cullinan, & Braun, 1991;

Zaborszky, Heimer, Eckenstein, & Leranth, 1986b). BF cholinergic cells are widely
Sleep and the Brain – page 22/22

involved in wake-promoting behaviours, including attention, sensory processing, and

learning (Datta & Maclean, 2007).

Histamine

(HA)-synthesizing histaminergic cells are located in the posterior hypothalamus (PH),

especially the tuberomammillary nuclei (TMN). PH-TMN neurons are involved in promoting

and maintaining wakefulness (John, Wu, Boehmer, & Siegel, 2004), sending projections

through wake-promoting structures in brainstem and forebrain (Brown, Stevens, & Haas,

2001; Hass & Panula, 2003; Huang et al., 2001; Inagaki et al., 1988; Lin, Luppi, Salvert,

Sakai, & Jouvet, 1986; Panula, Pirvola, Auvinen, & Airaksinen, 1989; K. Sakai, El-Mansari,

& Jouvet, 1990; Sherin, 1998). Single cell recordings in freely moving cats and rats show that

the majority of TMN histaminergic neurons are active during wakefulness and silent during

sleep (Ko, Estabrooke, McCarthy, & Scammell, 2003; Sakai, Yoshimoto et al., 1990;

Takahashi, 2006; Vanni-Mercier, 1984). Conversely, PH lesions produce a comatose-like

continuous sleepiness (Saper, Chou, & Scammell, 2001). Cessation of histaminergic activity

may be related to the loss of consciousness during sleep (John et al., 2004).

Norepinephrine

Most (NE)-synthesizing noradrenergic cells are located in the locus coeruleus (LC) of the

pons. They project directly to the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, amygdala and other

subcortical areas such as thalamus, hypothalamus and BF (Berridge & Waterhouse, 2003;

Dahlstrom, 1964; Datta & Maclean, 2007; Lewis, 1987). During wakefulness, noradrenergic

neurons have a regular and tonic firing, slowing down during the initial phase of SWS (Aston-

Jones, 1981b; Foote, 1983). Like histaminergic cells, they are inactive during REM sleep

(Datta & Maclean, 2007). Cessation of activity in noradrenergic cells during sleep seems to be

related to loss of muscle tone (John et al., 2004; Lai, 2001). Activation/inactivation of locus

coeruleus neurons results in an increase/decrease of wakefulness, respectively (Berridge &


Sleep and the Brain – page 23/23

Foote, 1991; Berridge, Page, Valentino, & Foote, 1993), suggesting that activation of

noradrenergic cells participates in the process of cortical activation and behavioural arousal.

Serotonin

Serotonergic (5-HT)-synthesizing cells are located in cluster nuclei, called raphe nuclei, in the

brainstem. The two main clusters are the rostral cluster (a midbrain/pontine group sending

projections to the telencephalic hemispheres) and the caudal cluster (a medullar group

sending projections to the spinal cord) (Lyamin et al., 2008). Serotonergic cells have a similar

discharge pattern (maximal firing during wakefulness, decreased firing during SWS, no firing

during REM sleep; (Lydic, 1983; McGinty, 1976) and almost project to the same brain

regions than noradrenergic cell projections (Morgane, 2005; Tork, 1990; Vertes, 1988).

Unlike noradrenergic cells however, their role in promoting sleep and/or wakefulness and/or

sleep remains unclear (Datta & Maclean, 2007). It has been proposed that serotonin may play

a role in maintaining arousal, and regulating muscle tone and some phasic events of REM

sleep (John et al., 2004; Wu, Gulyani, Yau, et al., 1999; Wu, John, Boehmer et al., 2004). On

the other hand, a lesion in the raphe nuclei increases wakefulness and suppresses SWS (Sakai

& Crochet, 2001), suggesting that serotoninergic neurons are involved in SWS promotion.

Pharmacological studies also suggest that serotonin may suppress activity of

hypothalamocortical and baso-cortical wake-promoting systems (Cape, 1998; Khateb, 1993).

Another, non exclusive functional hypothesis is that tonic activity of serotonin during waking

tend to suppress phasic events; and during REM sleep allows high voltage bursts of electrical

activity and the generation of the PGO (Siegel, 2000).

Hypocretine/orexine

Hypocretinergic or orexinergic neurons, recently causally linked to sleep, are located in the

lateral hypothalamus, between the sleep-activator neurons of the anterior hypothalamus

(GABAergic cells, see below) and the wake-activator histamine neurons of the posterior
Sleep and the Brain – page 24/24

hypothalamus (Gerashchenko, 2004). Hypocretinergic cells promote wakefulness by their

strong projections towards many primary wake-promoting neuronal systems of the brain

(Gerashchenko, 2004; Siegel, 2004). Loss of these hypocretine/orexine cells is linked to

human narcolepsy (Overeem, 2002; Thannickal, 2003).

Dopamine

At variance with other wake-promoting neurotransmitters described above, (DA)-synthesizing

dopaminergic cells located in the substantia nigra compacta (SNc) and the ventral tegmental

area (VTA) present a similar pattern of activation across sleep–wake states (Steinfels, 1983;

Trulson & Preussler, 1984; Trulson, Preussler, & Howell, 1981). Notwithstanding,

extracellular concentrations of dopamine are significantly increased during wakefulness

periods (Feenstra, 2000; Trulson, 1985).

Glutamatergic cells

Glutamatergic cells are located in the mesencephalic reticular formation (MRF). They send

strong projections to thalamic nuclei and the hypothalamus, thereby activating the thalamo-

cortical and hypothalamo-cortical networks, respectively (Datta & Maclean, 2007). Non-

invasive neuroimaging techniques in human subjects have shown that MRF activity is higher

during wakefulness than during SWS (Braun et al., 1997; Kajimura et al., 1999; Maquet et al.,

1990), in line with the proposal that glutamatergic neurons are involved in the maintenance of

behavioural states of wakefulness (Datta & Maclean, 2007). Likewise histaminergic neurons

of the posterior hypothalamus, electrolytic lesions in the mesencephalic reticular formation

region may cause a comatose-like continuous sleepiness (Saper et al., 2001).

Generation and maintenance of NREM sleep


Sleep and the Brain – page 25/25

For one part, sleep initiation results from the decreasing power of wake-promoting brain

neurons. NREM sleep generation and maintenance also depend on the implication of four

types of molecules: adenosine, prostaglandin, cytokine, and most importantly inhibitory

amino acid (GABA). Already in 1930, Von Economo demonstrated the existence of a sleep-

promoting area in the brain, by showing a correlation between severe insomnia and damage to

the anterior hypothalamus. A few decades later, GABAergic neurons (also named “NREM-on

cells”, see below) were identified in the ventrolateral preoptic (VLPO) area of the

hypothalamus. GABA cells are unique because, by contrast to most neurons, they are

maximally active during NREM sleep, less active during REM sleep and minimally active

during wakefulness (Siegel, 2004; Szymusiak, 1995; Szymusiak, Steininger, & McGinty,

2001). In this respect, GABAergic neurons may be viewed as the most potent sleep-promoting

component of the brain. Additionally, bursting activity of GABAergic cells in the VLPO

abolishes transmission of incoming signal to the cortex, by inhibiting serotoninergic and

noradrenergic activity of the brainstem, but also in histaminergic and cholinergic systems, two

major arousal systems in the brain. By this way, continuous GABA release suppresses activity

in wake-promoting areas and activates NREM sleep (Datta & Maclean, 2007; Nitz & Siegel,

1997a; Nitz & Siegel, 1997b; Siegel, 2005). Slow Wave Sleep (SWS), which characterizes

deep NREM sleep, emerges by the reduction of neuronal activity in wake-promoting brain

regions due to the activation of GABAergic cells in the preoptic area, which in turn induces

SWS. GABAergic inhibitory function is facilitated by increased synthesis of the growth

hormone-releasing hormone (GHRH), also involved in increasing the depth and duration of

SWS (Krueger, 2003; Obal, 2004).

Adenosine may play a role in the homeostatic regulation of sleep. Indeed, sleep

duration and depth are modulated by adenosine concentration in the basal forebrain

cholinergic region. Adenosine level progressively increases during extended waking periods
Sleep and the Brain – page 26/26

and slowly declines during recovery sleep. It is hypothesised that increased adenosine

concentration in the cholinergic basal forebrain would decrease EEG arousal, increase

drowsiness and promote EEG delta wave activity during subsequent NREM sleep (Porkka-

Heiskanen et al., 1997). Additionally, adenosine may play a role in inter-individual variability

in sleep structure, NREM sleep intensity and detrimental effects of sleep loss on

neurobehavioral performance in humans (Landolt, 2008). The exact biochemical mechanisms

underlying the effects of adenosine on NREM sleep homeostasis remain a hotly debated topic

(for further discussion, see Siegel, 2006).

Generation and maintenance of REM sleep

As compared to NREM sleep, mechanisms involved in REM sleep generation and

maintenance are much more complex. In their pioneering study, Jouvet and colleagues

(Jouvet, 1962; Jouvet & Michel 1959, 1960) showed periodic occurrence of REM sleep signs

(REMs, muscle atonia, etc …) in a pontine preparation (i.e. transection just above the

midbrain-pontine junction), demonstrating that REM sleep generation originates from the

brainstem. Further studies have tried to provide a more detailed picture of the REM sleep

controlling network. They have shown that REM signs are controlled by specific neurons,

called “effector neurons” and concentrated for the most part in the reticular formation (RF) of

the pons (McCarley, 2007; Steriade & McCarley, 2005). Generation and maintenance of REM

sleep involve a complex system of neuronal connections between effector neurons, a system

regulated by ratios of aminergic and cholinergic neurotransmitters.

In 1975, McCarley and Hobson published the reciprocal interaction model to explain

the mechanisms of REM sleep. They proposed that REM sleep generation and maintenance

depends on reciprocal interactions between so-called “REM-on” and “REM-off” groups of

cells (Figure 6) (McCarley & Hobson, 1975). REM-on neurons are reticular cholinergic cells

located in the pedunculopontine tegmentum (PPT) and lateral dorsal tegmentum (LDT) of the
Sleep and the Brain – page 27/27

brainstem. REM-off cells are monoaminergic (serotoninergic and noradrenergic) neurons

located in the locus coeruleus (LC) (El Mansari, 1989; Thakkar, 1998). During wakefulness

and NREM sleep, high levels of activity in REM-off aminergic neurons actively inhibit the

action of cholinergic REM-on neurons. Throughout NREM sleep however, activity in REM-

off aminergic cells gradually decreases, eventually releasing inhibition on REM-on cells,

whose increased activity will in turn induce REM sleep (Brown & McCarley, 2008)

Additional evidences suggest that REM-on cells have an excitatory effect on REM-off cells

during REM sleep, which will terminate the REM sleep phase when a sufficient level of

activity has been attained as to inhibit again activity in REM-on cells (McCarley & Hobson,

1975). Brainstem PPT/LDT cholinergic neurons are promoters of REM sleep by sending

excitatory cholinergic projections to the effector neurons. For instance, they cause loss of

muscle tone during REM sleep by triggering simultaneous inhibition of and withdrawal of

excitation to motoneurons. When these neurons are artificially activated (e.g. by

microinjection of acetylcholine agonists into specific regions of the pons), prolonged REM

sleep periods are elicited, further showing their crucial role in the regulation of REM sleep

itself (Coleman, 2004)

--- INSERT FIGURE 6 HERE ---

SLEEP NEUROPHYSIOLOGY

Three regions are know to play a main role in the generation and maintenance of sleep stages

and their distinguishing features (e.g. spindles or ponto-geniculo-occipital waves): (i) the

thalamus, including nuclei such as the thalamic reticular nucleus sending and receiving

projections to and from the cortex; (ii) the hypothalamus located behind the thalamus and (iii)

the reticular formation, located throughout the pons and mesencephalon, sending projections

to the neocortex through thalamic nuclei (Marieb, 1999).


Sleep and the Brain – page 28/28

Whereas neurobiological investigations have been mostly conducted at the cellular

level in animals, the neurophysiological study of sleep and the macroscopic delineation of

sleep-related cerebral structures have also been achieved in human using non invasive

neuroimaging and electrophysiological approaches. In this respect, Positron Emission

Tomography (PET) and functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) techniques have

contributed to identify the cortical and subcortical functional neuroanatomical networks

involved in sleep and its components. The spatial accuracy of these techniques (up to a few

millimeters in resolution) is complementary to the high temporal resolution, at the millisecond

scale of neuronal transmission, of electrophysiological techniques that are playing a

fundamental role in the investigation of temporal patterns of neuronal synchronization

oscillatory rhythms using e.g. EEG (Happe et al., 2002), high-density EEG (Massimini,

Huber, Ferrarelli, Hill, & Tononi, 2004; Riedner, 2007) or magnetoencephalography (MEG;

Ioannides et al., 2004) both during REM and NREM sleep. Optimal combinations between

these techniques (e.g. simultaneous EEG-fMRI, see below for instance Dang-Vu et al., 2008;

Schabus et al., 2007) and their intrinsic advantages minimize their respective drawbacks,

providing important information on the physiology of sleep and its regulation.

In this section, we will offer a description of the cerebral networks implicated in a

permissive and/or executive manner in NREM and REM sleep stages. This

neurophysiological macroscopic level of description will be linked with a summary

description of the cortical and subcortical cellular and synaptic interaction processes thought

responsible for these cerebral patterns and their respective implication in the generation of

sleep stages and their specific features. All studies presented in this section refer to “canonical

sleep”, e.g. normal sleep in healthy young humans, without any associated pathology or

interferences due to prior sleep deprivation or pharmacological intervention, that are beyond

the scope of this chapter.


Sleep and the Brain – page 29/29

NREM sleep

As discussed above, there is a progressive change from wakefulness to NREM sleep in the

way neuronal populations are firing, from a desynchronized tonic mode towards bursting

synchronous activity. Self-sustained oscillatory activity in thalamic nuclei initiate a cascade of

neurophysiological events, eventually leading to the large-scale synchronization of cortical

neuronal populations responsible for the low frequency, high amplitude EEG typical of

NREM sleep. As illustrated Figure 7, three interconnected neuronal populations are involved

in the generation of NREM sleep oscillations: (i) corticothalamic neurons (neurons A), that

have excitatory synapses on thalamocortical neurons and thalamic reticular neurons; (ii)

thalamocortical relay neurons (neurons B) with their cell bodies in the thalamus and axons

making excitatory synapses on pyramidal neurons in the cerebral cortex and on neurons in the

thalamic reticular nucleus and (iii) thalamic reticular neurons (neurons C) located in the

reticular thalamic nuclei and forming inhibitory synapses on thalamocortical neurons.

Neurons A and B are excitatory glutamaergic neurons, whereas neurons C are GABAergic

inhibitory neurons. The resulting loops into the cortico-thalamo-cortical network are involved

by different ways in the generation of spindles and delta waves, which in turn are organized

by cortically generated slow oscillations (<1 Hz; Steriade, 2001). Altogether, these rhythms

are produced by a “unified oscillatory machine” encompassing thalamic and cortical

structures, under the control of brainstem and forebrain modulatory systems, and are

coalescent within specific time windows corresponding to the depolarizing phase (up state) of

the slow oscillations (Steriade, 2006).

--- INSERT FIGURE 7 HERE ---

Neural correlates of slow oscillations in NREM sleep


Sleep and the Brain – page 30/30

A consequence of large-scale neuronal synchronization is that coincident bursts of activity are

followed by equally synchronous depolarization periods (down state), during which neurons

are virtually silent. When cerebral activity is recorded and averaged over long periods, for

tens of seconds up to minutes as it is the case with PET scanning, it entails that areas in which

activity is most synchronous will be those in which regional cerebral blood flow (CBF) will

be most decreased due to the fact that averaging has included the long depolarization periods

during which energy demands are minimal (Maquet, 2000). Accordingly, PET studies have

consistently reported decreased regional CBF and oxygen consumption during NREM sleep,

as compared to wakefulness or REM sleep, in a set of subcortical and cortical regions

encompassing the dorsal pons and the mesencephalon, the cerebellum, the thalamus, the basal

ganglia, the basal forebrain and the anterior hypothalamus, and neocortical prefrontal and

anterior cingulate cortices, precuneus and mesial part of the temporal lobe (Figure 8)

(Andersson et al., 1998; Braun et al., 1997; Kajimura et al., 1999; Maquet, 2000; Maquet et

al., 1997). The relationship between the depth of NREM sleep and decreased rCBF thought to

reflect increased synchronization is further confirmed by correlations between increased EEG

spectral power for slow oscillations in the delta (1-4 Hz) range and decreased rCBF in most of

the cortical regions reported above, particularly in medial prefrontal regions where response

to homeostatic sleep pressure is prominent (Dang-Vu et al., 2005).

--- INSERT FIGURE 8 HERE ---

Above rCBF patterns reflecting synchronous activity within specific brain areas, more recent

studies have taken advantage of the better temporal resolution of fMRI (about one second)

combined with simultaneous EEG recordings to highlight transient changes in regional

cerebral activity specifically associated with slow (< 1Hz; >140 µV) and delta waves (0.5-

4Hz; 75–140 µV) during NREM sleep (Dang-Vu et al., 2008). Using an endogenous event-

related approach, these authors searched for brain regions in which changes in activity (i.e.
Sleep and the Brain – page 31/31

Blood Oxygen Level-Dependent [BOLD] responses in fMRI) were coincident with the active

depolarized (“up”) state of these oscillations, as indexed by peak negativity in the EEG.

Results show that increased cerebral activity is associated both with slow and delta waves in

several cortical areas including the inferior frontal and medial prefrontal cortices, and the

precuneus and posterior cingulate areas. Besides these commonalities however, slow

oscillations (< 1Hz) are associated with increased activity in the parahippocampical gyrus, the

cerebellum and the brainstem, whereas delta waves are specifically associated with increased

activity in frontal areas (Figure 9).

--- INSERT FIGURE 9 HERE ---

Additionally, Dang-Vu et al. (2008) found increased activity associated with slow oscillations

in the midbrain and the pontine tegmentum, a region that includes critical structures involved

in the regulation of sleep and wakefulness (especially cholinergic and aminergic nuclei, see

above), the activation encompassing the noradrenergic locus coeruleus (LC). This unexpected

result suggests that these structures are active in phase with slow oscillations, maybe allowing

the brain to periodically restore microwake-like activity patterns in order to facilitate neuronal

interactions. Taken together, these results further confirm that SWS is not, as may have been

thought, a state of brain quiescence, but genuinely is an active state during which phasic

increases in brain activity are synchronized to slow oscillations (Dang-Vu et al., 2008).

Functional neuroanatomy of sleep spindles

As mentioned above, spindles are short-lasting (0.5-3 sec), waxing and waning oscillations in

the sigma range (11-16 Hz) during NREM sleep, especially during Stage N2. Spindles are

affected by a series of factors including circadian modulation (Dijk, 1995a; Knoblauch,

Krauchi, Renz, Wirz-Justice, & Cajochen, 2002; Knoblauch, Martens, Wirz-Justice, Krauchi,

& Cajochen, 2003), age (Landolt, Dijk, Achermann, & Borbély, 1996), menstrual cycle
Sleep and the Brain – page 32/32

(Driver, 1996) and drugs (Aeschbach, 1994; Dijk et al., 1995). Behavioural and

electrophysiological data further suggest a functional differentiation between slow (11-13 Hz)

and fast (14-16 Hz) spindles (e.g. for a review De Gennaro & Ferrara, 2003), but their

functional neuroanatomy remained uncertain. PET studies have yielded conflicting results,

with a reported relationship between spindle activity and rCBF in the thalamus (Hofle et al.,

1997) contradicted later on (Dang-Vu et al., 2005). Using a combined EEG-fMRI approach,

Schabus and collaborators (2007) recently found that spindles occurrence is associated with

transient increases in regional brain activity in thalamic and paralimbic areas (anterior

cingulate and insular cortices), and superior temporal gyrus (Figure 10). Besides a common

neuroanatomical pattern, they also found that activity in distinctive thalamo-cortical networks

characterize fast and slow spindles. Whereas slow spindles only elicit additional increased

activity in the frontal gyrus, fast spindles are associated with specific activations in a large set

of cortical regions involved in sensorimotor processing, as well as in orbital and mesial

prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and anterior insula, further supporting the hypothesis that

generation of fast and slow spindles rely upon two functionally separated systems.

--- INSERT FIGURE 10 HERE ---

REM sleep

At variance with NREM, REM sleep and wakefulness are characterized by sustained,

desynchronized neuronal activity (Steriade & McCarley, 1990) associated with high energy

demands. Therefore, activity as measured using PET or fMRI will be increased in brain

regions actively involved in REM sleep processes as compared to NREM sleep, even above

activity levels observed during wakefulness. Using PET, REM sleep was associated with a

specific neuroanatomical pattern of increased activity in the mesopontine tegmentum (PPT),

thalamic nuclei, basal forebrain, cerebellum, caudate nucleus, limbic and paralimbic

structures (e.g. amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate cortex) and associative posterior
Sleep and the Brain – page 33/33

temporo-occipital regions (Braun et al., 1997, 1998; Maquet et al., 1996; Nofzinger, Mintun,

Wiseman, Kupfer, & Moore, 1997; Peigneux et al., 2001). In parallel, decreased regional CBF

was found in frontal and parietal regions (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate

gyrus, precuneus and inferior parietal cortex), as well as in primary sensory areas (Braun et

al., 1997; Maquet et al., , 1996, 2000, 2005), indicating that activity in these regions is

strongly diminished during REM sleep (Figure 11).

--- INSERT FIGURE 11 HERE ---

Increased regional CBF activity in the PTT, thalamus and basal forebrain is in line with the

neurobiological literature, indicating that REM-on neurons and other cellular populations

responsible for REM sleep induction are located in these regions and mediate widespread

cortical activation through innervations in the thalamus and basal forebrain (Capece, 1997;

Datta, 1995, 1997; Steriade, 1990). Also, increased activity in amygdaloid complexes

involved in the generation of REM sleep (Morrison, 1999) reflects their potential role in the

modulation of regional activation/deactivation patterns during REM sleep (Amaral & Price,

1984). Indeed, most activated areas during REM sleep are regions receiving numerous

amygdalar projections, whereas deactivated frontal and parietal cortices are practically devoid

of amygdalar afferents (Aggleton, 1993; Amaral, Price, Pitkänen, & Carmichael, 1992;

Amaral & Price, 1984). Also, animal data have shown that electrical stimulation of the central

nucleus of amygdaloid complexes increases phasic ponto-geniculo-occipital (PGO, see

below) activity (Calvo, Badillo, Morales-Ramirez, & Palacios-Salas, 1987), one of the most

distinguishing features of REM sleep.

Phasic and tonic activities in REM sleep

Widespread thalamocortical synchronized activity characterizes both REM sleep and

wakefulness, in line with PET results (Braun et al., 1997; Braun et al., 1998; Buchsbaum,
Sleep and the Brain – page 34/34

Hazlett, Wu, & Bunney, 2001; Maquet et al., 1996; Nofzinger et al., 1997; Peigneux et al.,

2001). Beyond regional patterns of sustained cerebral activity however, fMRI investigations

suggest that transient changes in thalamocortical activity reflect the transition between tonic

and phasic components during REM sleep (Wehrle et al., 2007). Indeed, within human REM

sleep, widespread activity in a thalamocortical network including limbic and parahippocampal

areas seems to occur selectively during phasic REM sleep (e.g. rapid eye movements periods)

as compared with the predominant tonic REM sleep background, suggesting that REM sleep

subdivides into tonic REM sleep with residual alertness and phasic REM sleep with the brain

acting as a functionally isolated and closed intrinsic loop. From an evolutionary point of view

this transition may be beneficial since phasic REM sleep constitutes an extremely vulnerable

state during which sensory inputs are not perceived on top of decreased reactivity capacities

due to general muscle atonia.

Probably the most distinguishing phasic feature of REM sleep are ponto-geniculo-

occipital (PGO) waves which are prominent phasic bioelectrical potentials occurring in

isolation or in bursts during the transition from NREM to REM sleep or during REM sleep

itself (for reviews, see Callaway, Lydic, Baghdoyan, & Hobson, 1987; Datta, 1999). In

animals, REMs are closely related to the occurrence of PGO which propagates throughout the

brain but prominently in the pons, the lateral geniculate bodies and the occipital cortex. PGO

are though a fundamental process of REM sleep, playing a significant role in central nervous

system maturation (Datta & Patterson, 2003). In human, intracerebral recordings in epileptic

patients (Salzarulo, Lairy, Bancaud, & Munari, 1975) as well as non-invasive PET (Peigneux

et al., 2001), fMRI (Wehrle et al., 2005) and magnetoencephalography (MEG; Ioannides et

al., 2004) studies in healthy volunteers indicate that rapid eye movements observed during

REM sleep are generated by mechanisms similar or identical to PGO waves in animals.
Sleep and the Brain – page 35/35

CONCLUSIONS

In this chapter, we have tried to guide the reader beyond the appearances of a cessation of

waking activity during sleep. We have seen that despite evolutionary adaptations and

divergences amongst species, sleep is a universal phenomenon whose generation and

maintenance are under control of complex regulatory mechanisms. Most importantly, we

have learnt that above all, sleep is a state of the brain, and even more a series of active

states of the brain. Sleep-related changes in brain activity can be observed at

neurobiological, neurophysiological and neuroanatomical levels, including amongst others

cellular and molecular interactions, re-equilibration of the neurotransmitters balance and

changes in functional neuroanatomical patterns, eventually leading to REM and NREM

sleep stages and their characteristic tonic and phasic patterns in man. Our exponentially

growing knowledge about the way sleep is regulated and organized at the brain level

should ultimately allow answering the fundamental questions of the vital functions played

by sleep in the life span. Indeed, one fascinating but still pending question is the

delineation of the exact function(s) played by sleep. Although we know with certainty that

sleep is an essential process for living species as different as humans and flies, we are still

unable to fully understand why sleep is so tremendously important. Amongst others,

functional hypotheses have proposed that sleep may promote restorative mechanisms for

the brain and body metabolism (e.g. cortical maintenance, energy conservation, body

mass, metabolism and sleep control), whereas others have considered that sleep plays a

prominent role in brain plasticity and memory consolidation processes (see related chapter

“The functions of sleep”). These proposals should not be viewed as mutually exclusive,

but rather reflecting multiple and complementary functions of sleep. An important task to

achieve nowadays is the identification of which of these hypothesized functions may only

be achieved during sleep, and which others may be executed during both waking and
Sleep and the Brain – page 36/36

sleep, with sleep merely being a more efficient time for their accomplishment. It also

remains to be determined which, if any, of the proposed functions are universal across

mammalian species and across the lifespan, and which may be limited to particular

species or phases of development. Despite advances that have refined our understanding

of the relationships between sleep and cognitive processes, these underlying mechanisms

still remain to be fully elucidated.

RELATED CHAPTERS

The regulation of human sleep and wakefulness

Sleep homeostasis; circadian physiology; the two-process model; circadian, diurnal and

ultradian factors in sleep & wakefulness; sleep chronotypes; respiratory control;

thermoregulation

The functions of sleep

Evolutionary perspectives; body restitution; cerebral restitution; core and optional sleep;

perspectives on REM sleep; sleep deprivation and its effects

FURTHER READING

Borbely, A. A., Hayaishi, O., Sejnowski, T. J., & Altman, J. S. (Eds.). (2000). The regulation

of sleep. Strasbourg: HFSP.

Datta, S., & Maclean, R. R. (2007). Neurobiological mechanisms for the regulation of

mammalian sleep-wake behavior: reinterpretation of historical evidence and inclusion


Sleep and the Brain – page 37/37

of contemporary cellular and molecular evidence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 31(5), 775-

824.

Hartse, K. M. (1994). Sleep in insects and nonmammalian vertebrates. In M. H. Kryger, Roth,

T., Dement, W.C. (Eds.) (Ed.), Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine.1st ed. (pp.

95–104). Philadelphia: Elsevier Saunders.

Iber, C., Ancoli-Israel, S., Chesson, A., & Quan, S. F. (Eds.). (2007). The AASM Manual for

the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical

Specifications. Westchester: American Academy of Sleep Medicine.

Maquet, P., Smith, C., & Stickgold, R. (Eds.). (2003). Sleep and Brain Plasticity. Oxford:

Oxford University Press.

Rattenborg, N. C., Martinez-Gonzalez, D., & Lesku, J. A. (2009). Avian sleep homeostasis:

convergent evolution of complex brains, cognition and sleep functions in mammals

and birds. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 33(3), 253-270.

Siegel, J. M. (2005). Clues to the functions of mammalian sleep. Nature, 437(7063), 1264-

1271.
Sleep and the Brain – page 38/38

BIBLIOGRAPHICAL REFERENCES

Achermann, P. & Borbely, A. A. (1997). Low-frequency (<1 Hz) oscillations in the human

sleep electroencephalogram. Neuroscience, 81(1), 213-222.

Aeschbach, D., Dijk, D. J., Trachsel, L., Brunner, D. P. and Borbély, A. A. (1994). Dynamics

of slow-wave activity and spindle frequency activity in the human sleep EEG: effect

of midazolam and zopiclone. Neuropsychopharmacology 11, 237-244.

Aggleton, J. P. (1993). The contribution of the amygdala to normal and abnormal emotional

states. Trends in Neuroscience, 16, 328-333.

Allison, T. & Van Twyver, H. (1972). Electrophysiological studies of the echidna,

Tachyglossus aculeatus. II. Dormancy and hibernation. Archives italiennes de

biologie, 110(2), 185-194.

Amaral, D. G., Price, J. L., Pitkänen, A., & Carmichael, S. T. (1992). Anatomical

organization of the primate amygdaloid complex. In: J. P. Aggelton (Ed.), The

amygdala: neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and mental dysfuntion. (pp.

1-66). New York: Wiley-Liss.

Amaral, D. G. & Price, J.L. (1984). Amygdalo-cortical projections in the monkey (Macaca

fascicularis). Journal of comparative neurology, 230(4), 465–496.

Amzica, F. & Steriade, M. (2000). Neuronal and glial membrane potentials during sleep and

paroxysmal oscillations in the neocortex. J Neurosci, 20(17), 6648-6665.

Andersson, J. L. R., Onoe, H., Hetta, J., Lidstrom, K., Valind, S., Lilja, A., et al. (1998). Brain

networks affected by synchronized sleep visualized by positron emission tomography.

Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 18(7), 701-715.


Sleep and the Brain – page 39/39

Ari, C. & Kalman, M. (2008). Evolutionary changes of astroglia in Elasmobranchii comparing

to amniotes: a study based on three immunohistochemical markers (GFAP, S-100, and

glutamine synthetase). Brain Behav Evol, 71(4), 305-324.

Aserinsky, E. & Kleitman, N. (1953). Regularly occurring periods of eye motility during

sleep. Science, 118, 273-274.

Aston-Jones, G. & Bloom, F.E. (1981b). Nonrepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons

in behaving rats exhibit pronounced responses to non-noxious environmental stimuli.

Journal of Neuroscience, 1, 887–900.

Bednekoff, P. A. & Ritter, R. (1994). Vigilance in nxai pan springbok, Antidorcasmarsupialis.

Behavioural Brain Research, 129, 1–11.

Bell-Pedersen, D., Cassone, V. M., Earnest, D. J., Golden, S. S., Hardin, P. E., Thomas, T. L.,

et al. (2005). Circadian rhythms from multiple oscillators: lessons from diverse

organisms. Nat Rev Genet, 6(7), 544-556.

Berger, H. (1929). Über das elektroenkelphalogramm des Mensen. Arch. Pyschiatr.

Nervenkr, 87, 527-570.

Berridge, C. W. & Foote, S.L. (1991). Effects of locus coeruleus activation on

electroencephalographic activity in neocortex and hippocampus. Journal of

Neuroscience, 11, 3135–3145.

Berridge, C. W., Page, M.E., Valentino, R.J., Foote, S.L. (1993). Effects of locus coeruleus

inactivation on electroencephalographic activity in neocortex and hippocampus.

Neuroscience, 55, 381–393.


Sleep and the Brain – page 40/40

Berridge, C. W. & Waterhouse, B.D. (2003). The locus coeruleus-noradrenergic system:

modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes. Brain

Research. Brain Research Reviews, 42, 33–84.

Borbely, A. A. (1982). A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol, 1(3), 195-

204.

Borbely, A. A., Hayaishi, O., Sejnowski, T. J., & Altman, J. S. (Eds.). (2000). The regulation

of sleep. Strasbourg: HFSP.

Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesensten, N. J., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., Baldwin, P., et al.

(1997). Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle - An (H2O)-O-

15 PET study. Brain, 120(7), 1173-1197.

Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesensten, N. J., Gwadry, F., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., et al.

(1998). Dissociated pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections during

human rapid eye movement sleep. Science, 279(5347), 91-95.

Brown, R. E., Stevens, D. R., & Haas, H. L. (2001). The physiology of brain histamine. Prog

Neurobiol, 63(6), 637-672.

Brown R. E. and McCarley R. W. (2008). Neuroanatomy of REM sleep systems. In J. M. M.

a. al. (Ed.), Neurochemistry of Sleep and Wakefulness. Cambridge Cambridge

University Press.

Buchsbaum, M. S., Hazlett, E. A., Wu, J., & Bunney, J., William E. (2001). Positron

Emission Tomography with Deoxyglucose-F18 Imaging of Sleep.

Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(1), S50-S56.


Sleep and the Brain – page 41/41

Callaway, C. W., Lydic, R., Baghdoyan, H. A., & Hobson, J. A. (1987).

Pontogeniculooccipital waves: spontaneous visual system activity during rapid eye

movement sleep. Cell Mol.Neurobiol., 7(2), 105-149.

Calvo, J. M., Badillo, S., Morales-Ramirez, M., & Palacios-Salas, P. (1987). The role of the

temporal lobe amygdala in ponto-geniculo-occipital activity and sleep organization in

cats. Brain Research, 403, 22-30.

Campbell, S. S., & Tobler, I. (1984). Animal sleep: a review of sleep duration across

phylogeny. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 8(3), 269-300.

Cape, E. G., Jones, B.E. . (1998). Differential modulation of high-frequency gamma-

electroencephalogram activity and sleep–wake state by noradrenaline and serotonin

microinjections into the region of cholinergic basalis neurons. Journal of

Neuroscience, 18, 2653–2666.

Capece, M. L., Efange, S.M., Lydic, R. . (1997). Vesicular acetylcholine transport inhibitor

suppresses REM sleep. . Neuroreport, 8(2), 481–484.

Cirelli, C., & Bushey, D. (2008). Sleep and wakefulness in Drosophila melanogaster. Ann N Y

Acad Sci, 1129, 323-329.

Clemens, W. A. (1989). Diagnosis of the class mammalia. . In W. D. W. a. R. B. J (Ed.),

Fauna of Australia. 401–406. Canberra. : Australian Government Publishing.

Coleman, C. G., Lydic, R., Baghdoyan, H. A. (2004). M2 muscarinic receptors in pontine

reticular formation of C57BL/6J mouse contribute to rapid eye movement sleep

generation. Neuroscience, 126, 821–830.


Sleep and the Brain – page 42/42

Corsi-Cabrera, M., Guevara, M. A., Del Rio-Portilla, Y., Arce, C., & Villanueva-Hernandez,

Y. (2000). EEG bands during wakefulness, slow-wave and paradoxical sleep as a

result of principal component analysis in man. Sleep, 23(6), 738-744.

Corsi-Cabrera, M., Munoz-Torres, Z., Del Rio-Portilla, Y., & Guevara, M. A. (2005). Power

and coherent oscillations distinguish REM sleep, stage 1 and wakefulness.

International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60(1), 59-66.

Daan, S., Beersma, D. G., & Borbely, A. A. (1984). Timing of human sleep: recovery process

gated by a circadian pacemaker. Am J Physiol, 246(2 Pt 2), R161-183.

Dahlstrom, A., Fuxe, K. (1964). Localization of monoamines in the lower brain stem. .

Experientia, 20, 398–399.

Dang-Vu, T. T., Desseilles, M., Laureys, S., Degueldre, C., Perrin, F., Phillips, C., et al.

(2005). Cerebral correlates of delta waves during non-REM sleep revisited.

NeuroImage, 28(1), 14-21.

Dang-Vu, T. T., Schabus, M., Desseilles, M., Albouy, G., Boly, M., Darsaud, A., et al.

(2008). Spontaneous neural activity during human slow wave sleep. Proc Natl Acad

Sci U S A, 105(39), 15160-15165.

Datta, S. (1995). Neuronal activity in the peribrachial area: relationship to the behavioral state

control. Neurosci.Biobehav.Rev., 19, 67-84.

Datta, S. (1997). Cellular basis of pontine ponto-geniculo-occipital wave generation and

modulation. Cell Mol Neurobiol, 17(3), 341-365.


Sleep and the Brain – page 43/43

Datta, S. (1999). PGO wave generation: mechanism and functional significance. In B. N.

Mallick & S. Inoue (Eds.), Rapid Eye Movement Sleep (pp. 91-106). New Dehli:

Narosa Publishing House.

Datta, S., & Maclean, R. R. (2007). Neurobiological mechanisms for the regulation of

mammalian sleep-wake behavior: reinterpretation of historical evidence and inclusion

of contemporary cellular and molecular evidence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 31(5), 775-

824.

Datta, S., & Patterson, E. H. (2003). Activation of phasic pontine wave (P-wave): A

mechansims of learning and memory processing. In P. Maquet, C. Smith & R.

Stickgold (Eds.), Sleep and Brain Plasticity (pp. 135-156). Oxford: Oxford University

Press.

Datta, S., & Siwek, D. F. (1997). Excitation of the brain stem pedunculopontine tegmentum

cholinergic cells induces wakefulness and REM sleep. . Journal of Neurophysiology

77, 2975–2988.

De Gennaro, L., & Ferrara, M. (2003). Sleep spindles: an overview. Sleep Medicine Reviews,

7(5), 423-440.

De Gennaro, L., Ferrara, M., Bertini, M. (2000). Topographical distribution of spindles:

variations between and within NREM sleep cycles. Sleep Res Online, 3, 155-160

Dement W, K. N. (1957). Cyclic variations in EEG during sleep and their relation to eye

movements, body motility, and dreaming. Clin Neurophysiol 9, 673-690.

Dement, W. C. (Ed.). (1978). Some must watch while some must sleep. New York : W.W.

Norton
Sleep and the Brain – page 44/44

Dement, W. C. a. V., C. (1999). The promise of sleep. A Pioneer in Sleep Medicine Explores

the Vital Connection between Health, Happiness, and a Good Night's Sleep. New

York Delacorte Press.

Detari, L., Juhasz, G., & Kukorelli, T. (1984). Firing properties of cat basal forebrain

neurones during sleep–wakefulness cycle. . Electroencephalography and Clinical

Neurophysiology 58, 362–368.

Detari, L., & Vanderwolf, C. H. (1987). Activity of identified cortically projecting and other

basal forebrain neurones during large slow waves and cortical activation in

anaesthetized rats. Brain Research, 437, 1-8.

Dijk, D.-J., Roth, C., Landolt, H.-P., Werth, E., Aeppli, M., Achermann, P., et al. (1995).

Melatonin effect on daytime sleep in men: suppression of EEG low frequency activity

and enhancement of spindle frequency activity*1. Neuroscience Letters, 201(1), 13-

16.

Dijk, D. J. a. C., C. A. (1995a ). Circadian control of the EEG in non-REM sleep. . Sleep Res,

24, 518.

Driver, H. S., Dijk, D. J., Werth, E., Biedermann, K.and Borbély, A. A. . (1996). Sleep and

the sleep electroencephalogram across the menstrual cycle in young healthy women. .

J Clin Endocrinol Metab, 81, 728-735.

El Mansari, M., Sakai, K., Jouvet, M. . (1989). Unitary characteristics of presumptive

cholinergic tegmental neurons during the sleep-waking cycle in freely moving cats. .

Exp. Brain. Res., 76, 519–529.

Elgar, M. A. (1989). Predator vigilance and group size in mammals and birds: a critical

review of the empirical evidence. Biol Rev, 64, 13–33.


Sleep and the Brain – page 45/45

Feenstra, M. G., Botterblom, M.H., Mastenbroek, S. . (2000). Dopamine and noradrenaline

efflux in the prefrontal cortex in the light and dark period: effects of novelty and

handling and comparison to the nucleus accumbens. Neuroscience Letters, 100, 741–

748.

Finelli, L. A., Borbely, A. A., & Achermann, P. (2001). Functional topography of the human

nonREM sleep electroencephalogram. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(12),

2282-2290.

Fisher, R. S., Buchwald, N. A., Hull, C. D., & Levine, M. S. (1988). GABAergic basal

forebrain neurons project to the neocortex: the localization of glutamic acid

decarboxylase and choline acetyltransferase in feline corticopetal neurons. . Journal of

Comparative Neurology, 272, 489–502.

Flanigan, W. F., Jr. (1973). Sleep and wakefulness in iguanid lizards, Ctenosaura pectinata

and Iguana iguana. Brain Behav Evol, 8(6), 401-436.

Flanigan, W. F., Jr. (1974). Sleep and wakefulness in chelonian reptiles. II. The red-footed

tortoise, Geochelone carbonaria. Arch Ital Biol, 112(3), 253-277.

Flanigan, W. F., Wilcox, R. H., & Rechtschaffen, A. (1973). The EEG and behavior

continuum of the crocodilian, Caiman sclerops. Electroenceph Clin Neurophys 34,

521–538.

Flannery, T. F. (1989). Origins of the Australo-Pacific mammal fauna. Aust. zool. Rev., 1, 15–

24.

Foote, S. L., Bloom, F.E., Aston-Jones, G. (1983). Nucleus locus ceruleus: new evidence of

anatomical and physiological specificity. . Physiological Reviews, 63, 844–914.


Sleep and the Brain – page 46/46

Fuchs, T., Maury, D., Moore, F. R., & Bingman, V. P. (2009). Daytime micro-naps in a

nocturnal migrant: an EEG analysis. biology letters 5, 77–80.

Garcia-Rill, E. (2002). Mechaninsm of sleep and wakefulness. In J. Lee- Chiong, T.L.,

Sateia, M.J., Carskadon, M.A. (Ed.), Sleep Medicine. (pp. 31–39.). Phildelphia:

Hanley and Belfus Inc.

Gaztelu, J. M., Garcı´a-Austt, E., & Bullock, T. H. (1991). Electrocorticograms of

hippocampal and dorsal cortex of two reptiles: comparison with possible mammalian

homologs. Brain Behavior and Evolution, 37, 144–160. .

Gerashchenko, D. S., P.J. (2004). Different neuronal phenotypes in the lateral hypothalamus

and their role in sleep and wakefulness. Mol. Neurobiol., 29, 41–59.

Gilestro, G. F., Tononi, G., & Cirelli, C. (2009). Widespread changes in synaptic markers as a

function of sleep and wakefulness in Drosophila. Science, 324(5923), 109-112.

Gritti, I., Mainville, L., Mancia, M., & Jones, B. E. (1997). GABAergic and other

noncholinergic basal forebrain neurons, together with cholinergic neurons, project to

the mesocortex and isocortex in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 383, 163–

177.

Grutzner, F., Rens, W., Tsend-Ayush, E., El-Mogharbel, N., O'Brien, P. C., Jones, R. C., et al.

(2004). In the platypus a meiotic chain of ten sex chromosomes shares genes with the

bird Z and mammal X chromosomes. Nature, 432(7019), 913-917.

Happe, S., Anderer, P., Gruber, G., Klosch, G., Saletu, B., & Zeitlhofer, J. (2002). Scalp

topography of the spontaneous K-complex and of delta-waves in human sleep. Brain

Topogr, 15, 43-49.


Sleep and the Brain – page 47/47

Hartse, K. M. (1994). Sleep in insects and nonmammalian vertebrates. In M. H. Kryger, Roth,

T., Dement, W.C. (Eds.) (Ed.), Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine.1st ed. (pp.

95–104). Philadelphia: Elsevier Saunders.

Hass, H., & Panula, P. (2003). The role of histamine and the tuberomamillary nucleus in the

nervous system. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 121–130.

Hendricks, J. C., Finn, S. M., Panckeri, K. A., Chavkin, J., Williams, J. A., Sehgal, A., et al.

(2000). Rest in Drosophila is a sleep-like state. Neuron, 25(1), 129-138.

Hendricks, J. C., & Sehgal, A. (2004). Why a fly? Using Drosophila to understand the

genetics of circadian rhythms and sleep. Sleep, 27(2), 334-342.

Himanen S, Hasan J. (2000) Limitations of Rechtschaffen and Kales. Sleep Med Rev, 4(2),

149-167.

Hobson, J. A. (1967). Electrographic correlates of behavior in the frog with special reference

to sleep. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 22(2), 113-121.

Hofle, N., Paus, T., Reutens, D., Fiset, P., Gotman, J., Evans, A. C., et al. (1997). Regional

cerebral blood flow changes as a function of delta and spindle activity during slow

wave sleep in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(12), 4800-4808.

Huang, Z. L., Qu, W. M., Li, W. D., Mochizuki, T., Eguchi, N., Watanabe, T., et al. (2001).

Arousal effect of orexin A depends on activation of the histaminergic system.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98, 9965–9970.

Huber, R., Hill, S. L., Holladay, C., Biesiadecki, M., Tononi, G., & Cirelli, C. (2004). Sleep

homeostasis in Drosophila melanogaster. Sleep, 27(4), 628-639.


Sleep and the Brain – page 48/48

Iber, C., Ancoli-Israel, S., Chesson, A., & Quan, S. F. (Eds.). (2007). The AASM Manual for

the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical

Specifications. Westchester: American Academy of Sleep Medicine.

Inagaki, N., Yamatodani, A., Ando-Yamamoto, M., Tohyama, M., Watanabe, T., & Wada, H.

(1988). Organization of histaminergic fibers in the rat brain. Journal of Comparative

Neurology, 273, 283–300.

Ioannides, A. A., Corsi-Cabrera, M., Fenwick, P. B., del Rio Portilla, Y., Laskaris, N. A.,

Khurshudyan, A., et al. (2004). MEG tomography of human cortex and brainstem

activity in waking and REM sleep saccades. Cereb Cortex, 14(1), 56-72.

John, J., Wu, M. F., Boehmer, L. N., & Siegel, J. M. (2004). Cataplexy-active neurons in the

hypothalamus: implications for the role of histamine in sleep and waking behavior.

Neuron, 42, 619–634.

Jouvet, M. (1962). Research on the neural structures and responsible mechanisms in different

phases of physiological sleep. . Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 100, 125–206.

Jouvet, M. (1994). [Phylogeny of sleep stages]. Acta Psychiatr Belg, 94(4-6), 256-267.

Jouvet, M., & Michel , F. (1959). Corrélations électromyographiques du sommeil chez le Chat

décortiqué et mésencéphalique chronique. C.R.Soc.Biol.(Paris), 153, 422-425.

Jouvet, M., Michel, F., & Courjon, J. (1959). [On a stage of rapid cerebral electrical activity

in the course of physiological sleep.]. C R Seances Soc Biol Fil, 153, 1024-1028.

Jouvet, M., Michel, F. (1960). On the neural tract responsible for the rapid cortical activity

during the course of physiological sleep in the cat (paradoxal phase). Comptes Rendus

des Séances de la Société de Biologie et de ses Filiales, 154, 995–998.


Sleep and the Brain – page 49/49

Kajimura, N., Uchiyama, M., Takayama, Y., Uchida, S., Uema, T., Kato, M., et al. (1999).

Activity of midbrain reticular formation and neocortex during the progression of

human non-rapid eye movement sleep. J Neurosci, 19(22), 10065-10073.

Karmanova, I. G., Lazarev, S.G. (1978). Neurophysiological characteristics of primary sleep

in fish and amphibians. Paper presented at the Sleep: fourth European congress on

sleep research.

Kavanau, J. L. (1998). Vertebrates that never sleep: Implications for sleep's basic function.

Brain Res Bull, 46(4), 269-279.

Kenigfest, N., Belekhova, M., Reperant, J., Rio, J. P., Ward, R., & Vesselkin, N. (2005). The

turtle thalamic anterior entopeduncular nucleus shares connectional and

neurochemical characteristics with the mammalian thalamic reticular nucleus. J Chem

Neuroanat, 30(2-3), 129-143.

Khateb, A., Fort, P., Alonso, A., Jones, B.E., Muhlethaler, M. (1993). Pharmacological and

immunohistochemical evidence for serotonergic modulation of cholinergic nucleus

basalis neurons. . European Journal of Neuroscience, 5, 541–547.

Knoblauch, V., Krauchi, K., Renz, C., Wirz-Justice, A., & Cajochen, C. (2002). Homeostatic

Control of Slow-wave and Spindle Frequency Activity during Human Sleep: Effect of

Differential Sleep Pressure and Brain Topography. Cereb. Cortex, 12(10), 1092-1100.

Knoblauch, V., Martens, W., Wirz-Justice, A., Krauchi, K., & Cajochen, C. (2003). Regional

differences in the circadian modulation of human sleep spindle characteristics. Eur J

Neurosci, 18(1), 155-163.

Ko, E. M., Estabrooke, I. V., McCarthy, M., & Scammell, T. E. (2003). Wakerelated activity

of tuberomammillary neurons in rats. Brain Research, 992, 220–226.


Sleep and the Brain – page 50/50

Krueger, J. M., Obal Jr., F. (2003). Sleep function. Front in Biosciences, 8, d511-d519

Lai, Y. Y., Kodama, T., Siegel, J.M. (2001). Changes in monoamine release in the ventral

horn and hypoglossal nucleus linked to pontine inhibition of muscle tone: an in vivo

microdialysis study. J. Neurosci., 21, 7384–7391.

Landolt, H. P. (2008). Sleep homeostasis: a role for adenosine in humans? Biochem

Pharmacol, 75(11), 2070-2079.

Landolt, H. P., Dijk, D. J., Achermann, P. and Borbély, A. A. (1996). Effect of age on the

sleep EEG: slow-wave activity and spindle frequency activity in young and middle-

aged men. . Brain Res, 738, 205-212.

Lesku, J. A., Roth, T. C., 2nd, Rattenborg, N. C., Amlaner, C. J., & Lima, S. L. (2009).

History and future of comparative analyses in sleep research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev,

33(7), 1024-1036.

Lewis, S. W. (1987). Brain imaging in a case of Capgras' syndrome. British Journal of

Psychiatry, 150, 117-121.

Lin, J. S., Luppi, P. H., Salvert, D., Sakai, K., & Jouvet, M. (1986).

Histamineimmunoreactive neurons in the hypothalamus of cats. Comptes Rendus de l

Academie des Sciences SERIE III, 303, 371–376.

Llina´ s, R. R., & Steriade, M. (2006). Bursting of thalamic neurons and states of vigilance.

Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 3297–3308.

Lo´ pez, J. C., Vargas, J. P., Go´ mez, Y., & Salas, C. (2003). Spatial and non-spatial learning

in turtles: the role of medial cortex. . Behavioural Brain Research, 143, 109–120.
Sleep and the Brain – page 51/51

Loomis, A. L., Harvey, E.N., Hobart, G.A. . (1937). Cerebral states during sleep, as studied

by human brain potentials. . J. Exp. Psychol., 21, 127–144.

Lorenzo, D., Macadar, O., & Velluti, J. C. (1999). Origin and propagation of spontaneous

electrographic sharp waves in the in vitro turtle brain: a model of neuronal

synchronization. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110, 1535–1544.

Lorenzo, D., Velluti, J. C., & (2004). Noradrenaline decreases spike voltage threshold and

induces electrographic sharp waves in turtle medial cortex in vitro. Brain Behavior

and Evolution 64, 104–114.

Lyamin, O. I., Manger, P. R., Ridgway, S. H., Mukhametov, L. M., & Siegel, J. M. (2008).

Cetacean sleep: an unusual form of mammalian sleep. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 32(8),

1451-1484.

Lyamin, O. I., Mukhametov, L. M., & Siegel, J. M. (2004). Relationship between sleep and

eye state in Cetaceans and Pinnipeds. Arch Ital Biol, 142(4), 557-568.

Lyamin, O. I., Mukhametov, L. M., Siegel, J. M., Nazarenko, E. A., Polyakova, I. G., &

Shpak, O. V. (2002). Unihemispheric slow wave sleep and the state of the eyes in a

white whale. Behav Brain Res, 129(1-2), 125-129.

Lydic, R., McCarley, R.W., Hobson, J.A. (1983). The time-course of dorsal raphe discharge,

PGO waves, and muscle tone averaged across multiple sleep cycles. Brain Research,

274, 365–370.

Manger, P. R., Ridgway, S. H., & Siegel, J. M. (2003). The locus coeruleus complex of the

bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) as revealed by tyrosine hydroxylase

immunohistochemistry. J Sleep Res, 12(2), 149-155.


Sleep and the Brain – page 52/52

Maquet, P. (2000). Functional neuroimaging of normal human sleep by positron emission

tomography. Journal of Sleep Research, 9(3), 207-231.

Maquet, P., Degueldre, C., Delfiore, G., Aerts, J., Péters, J.-M., Luxen, A., et al. (1997).

Functional neuroanatomy of human slow wave sleep. The Journal of Neuroscience,

17(8), 2807-2812.

Maquet, P., Dive, D., Salmon, E., Sadzot, B., Franco, G., Poirrier, R., et al. (1990). Cerebral

glucose utilization during sleep-wake cycle in man determined by positron emission

tomography and [18F] 2-fluoro-2-deoxy-D-glucose method. Brain Research, 513,

136-143.

Maquet, P., Laureys, S., Peigneux, P., Fuchs, S., Petiau, C., Phillips, C., et al. (2000).

Experience-dependent changes in cerebral activation during human REM sleep.

Nature Neuroscience, 3(8), 831-836.

Maquet, P., Péters, J.-M., Aerts, J., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., et al. (1996).

Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and dreaming. Nature,

383, 163-166.

Maquet, P., Ruby, P., Maudoux, A., Albouy, G., Sterpenich, V., Dang-Vu, T., et al. (2005).

Human cognition during REM sleep and the activity profile within frontal and parietal

cortices: a reappraisal of functional neuroimaging data. Prog Brain Res, 150, 219-227.

Maquet, P., Smith, C., & Stickgold, R. (Eds.). (2003). Sleep and Brain Plasticity. Oxford:

Oxford University Press.

Marieb, E. N. (Ed.). (1999). Anatomie et physiologie humaine 4e édition Paris: De Boeck

Université.
Sleep and the Brain – page 53/53

Martinez-Gonzalez, D., Lesku, J. A., & Rattenborg, N. C. (2008). Increased EEG spectral

power density during sleep following short-term sleep deprivation in pigeons

(Columba livia): evidence for avian sleep homeostasis. Journal of Sleep Research 17,

140–153.

Massimini, M., Huber, R., Ferrarelli, F., Hill, S., & Tononi, G. (2004). The Sleep Slow

Oscillation as a Traveling Wave. J. Neurosci., 24(31), 6862-6870.

McCarley, R. W. (2007). Neurobiology of REM and NREM sleep. Sleep Medicine, 8, 302–

330.

McCarley, R. W., Hobson, J. A. (1975). Neuronal excitability modulation over the sleep

cycle: a structural and mathematical model. Science, 189, 58–60.

McCormick, D. A. (1989). Cholinergic and noradrenergic modulation of thalamocortical

processing. Trends Neurosci, 12(6), 215-221.

McCormick, L., Nielsen, T., Nicolas, A., Ptito, M., Montplaisir, J. (1997). Topographical

distribution of spindles and K-complexes in normal subjects. Sleep, 20, 939-941.

McGinty, D. J., Harper, R.M. . (1976). Dorsal raphe neurons: depression of firing during sleep

in cats. Brain Research, 101, 569–575.

Morgane, P. J., Galler, J.R., Mokler, D.J. . (2005). A review of systems and networks of the

limbic forebrain/limbic midbrain. Progress in Neurobiology, 75, 143–160.

Morrison, A. R., Sanford, L. D. and Ross, R. J. (1999). Initiation of rapid eye movement

sleep: beyond the brainstem. . In B. M. a. S. Inoue (Ed.), Rapid Eye Movement Sleep

(pp. 51-68) New Dehli: Narosa Publishing.

Mukhametov, L. M. (1984). Sleep in marine mammals. Exp. Brain Res., 8, 227–238.


Sleep and the Brain – page 54/54

Mukhametov, L. M. (1995). Paradoxical sleep peculiarities in aquatic mammals. Sleep Res.,

24A, 202.

Mukhametov, L. M., Lyamin, O. I., Chetyrbok, I. S., Vassilyev, A. A., & Diaz, R. P. (1992).

Sleep in an Amazonian manatee, Trichechus inunguis. Experientia, 48(4), 417-419.

Mukhametov, L. M., Oleksenko, A. I., & Poliakova, I. G. (1988). [Quantitative characteristics

of the electrocorticographic sleep stages in bottle-nosed dolphins]. Neirofiziologiia,

20(4), 532-538.

Mukhametov, L. M., Supin, A. Y., & Polyakova, I. G. (1977). Interhemispheric asymmetry of

the electroencephalographic sleep patterns in dolphins. Brain Res, 134(3), 581-584.

Nitz, D., Siegel, J.M. (1997a.) GABA release in the locus coeruleus as a function of

sleep/wake state. Neuroscience, 78, 795–801.

Nitz, D., Siegel, J.M., . (1997b. ). GABA release in the dorsal raphe nucleus: role in the

control of REM sleep. Am. J. Physiol., 273, R451–R455.

Nitz, D. A., van Swinderen, B., Tononi, G., & Greenspan, R. J. (2002). Electrophysiological

correlates of rest and activity in Drosophila melanogaster. Curr Biol, 12(22), 1934-

1940.

Nofzinger, E. A., Mintun, M. A., Wiseman, M., Kupfer, D. J., & Moore, R. Y. (1997).

Forebrain activation in REM sleep: an FDG PET study. Brain Research, 770(1-2),

192-201.

Nunez, A. (1996). Unit activity of rat basal forebrain neurons: relationship to cortical activity.

Neuroscience, 72, 757-766.

Obal Jr., F., Krueger, J.M. (2004). GHRH and sleep. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 8, 367–377.
Sleep and the Brain – page 55/55

Oleksenko, A. I., Mukhametov, L. M., Polyakova, I. G., Supin, A. Y., & Kovalzon, V. M.

(1992). Unihemispheric sleep deprivation in bottlenose dolphins. J Sleep Res, 1(1), 40-

44.

Overeem, S., van Hilten, J. J., Ripley, B., Mignot, E., Nishino, S., Lammers, G. J. . (2002).

Normal hypocretin-1 levels in Parkinson’s disease patients with excessive daytime

sleepiness. Neurology, 58(3), 465–468.

Panula, P., Pirvola, U., Auvinen, S., & Airaksinen, M. S. (1989). Histamineimmunoreactive

nerve fibers in the rat brain. Neuroscience 28, 585–610.

Peigneux, P., Laureys, S., Fuchs, S., Delbeuck, X., Degueldre, C., Aerts, J., et al. (2001).

Generation of rapid eye movements during paradoxical sleep in humans. Neuroimage,

14(3), 701-708.

Peyrethon, J., & Dusan-Peyrethon, D. (1967). [Polygraphic study of the wakefulness-sleep

cycle of a teleostean (Tinca tinca)]. C R Seances Soc Biol Fil, 161(12), 2533-2537.

Porkka-Heiskanen, T., Strecker, R. E., Thakkar, M., Bjokum, A. A., Greene, R. W., &

McCarley, R. W. (1997). Adenosine: a mediator of the sleep-inducing effects of

prolonged wakefulness. Science, 276, 1265-1268.

Rattenborg, N. C. (2006). Evolution of slow-wave sleep and palliopallial connectivity in

mammals and birds: A hypothesis. Brain Research Bulletin, 69(1), 20-29.

Rattenborg, N. C. (2007). Response to commentary on evolution of slow-wave sleep and

palliopallial connectivity in mammals and birds: a hypothesis. . Brain Research

Bulletin 72, 187–193.


Sleep and the Brain – page 56/56

Rattenborg, N. C., Amlaner, C. J., & Lima, S. L. (2000). Behavioral, neurophysiological and

evolutionary perspectives on unihemispheric sleep. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 24(8),

817-842.

Rattenborg, N. C., Amlaner, C.J. . (2002). Phylogeny of sleep. . In T. Lee-Chiong, Sateia, M.,

Carskadon, M. (Eds.), (Ed.), Sleep Medicine. (pp. 7–22). Philadelphia: Hanley and

Belfus.

Rattenborg, N. C., Lima, S. L., & Amlaner, C. J. (1999). Facultative control of avian

unihemispheric sleep under the risk of predation. Behav Brain Res, 105(2), 163-172.

Rattenborg, N. C., Martinez-Gonzalez, D., & Lesku, J. A. (2009). Avian sleep homeostasis:

convergent evolution of complex brains, cognition and sleep functions in mammals

and birds. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 33(3), 253-270.

Rechtschaffen, A. (1998). Current perspectives on the function of sleep. Perspect. Biol. Med.,

41, 359–390.

Rechtschaffen, A., & Kales, A. A. (1968). A manual of standardized terminology, techniques

and scoring system for sleep stages of human sujects. Bethesda: US Department of

Health, Education and Welfare.

Riedner, B. A., V.V. Vyazovskiy, R. Huber, M. Massimini, S. Esser, M. Murphy, and G.

Tononi. (2007). Sleep homeostasis and cortical synchronization: III. A high-density

EEG study of sleep slow waves in humans. Sleep 30(12), 1643-1657.

Sakai, K., Crochet, S. (2001). Role of dorsal raphe neurons in paradoxical sleep generation in

the cat: no evidence for a serotonergic mechanism. . European Journal of

Neuroscienc, 13, 103–112.


Sleep and the Brain – page 57/57

Sakai, K., El-Mansari, M., & Jouvet, M. (1990). Inhibition of carbachol microinjections of

presumptive cholinergic PGO-on neurons in freely moving cats. Brain Research, 527,

213-223.

Sakai, K., Yoshimoto, Y., Luppi, P. H., Fort, P., El Mansari, M., Salvert, D., et al. (1990).

Lower brainstem afferents to the cat posterior hypothalamus: a double-labeling study.

Brain Research Bulletin, 24, 437–455.

Salzarulo, P., Lairy, G. C., Bancaud, J., & Munari, C. (1975). direct depth recording of the

striate cortex during REM sleep in man: are there PGO potentials? EEG

Clin.Neurophysiol., 38, 199-202.

Saper, C. B., Chou, T. C., & Scammell, T. E. (2001). The sleep switch: hypothalamic control

of sleep and wakefulness. Trends in Neurosciences, 24(12), 726-731.

Schabus, M., Dang-Vu, T. T., Albouy, G., Balteau, E., Boly, M., Carrier, J., et al. (2007).

Hemodynamic cerebral correlates of sleep spindles during human non-rapid eye

movement sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 104(32), 13164-13169.

Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C., & Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: circadian

rhythms in human cognition. Cogn Neuropsychol, 24(7), 755-789.

Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Leclercq, Y., Sterpenich, V., Vandewalle, G., Berthomier, P., et al.

(2009). Homeostatic sleep pressure and responses to sustained attention in the

suprachiasmatic area. Science, 324(5926), 516-519.

Shaw, P. J., Cirelli, C., Greenspan, R. J., & Tononi, G. (2000). Correlates of sleep and waking

in Drosophila melanogaster. Science, 287(5459), 1834-1837.


Sleep and the Brain – page 58/58

Sherin, J. E., Elmquist, J.K., Torrealba, F., Saper, C.B. (1998). Innervation of histaminergic

tuberomammillary neurons by GABAergic and galaninergic neurons in the

ventrolateral preoptic nucleus of the rat. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 4705–4721.

Shurley, J. T., Serafetinides, E. A., Brooks, R. E., Elsner, R., & Kenney, D. W. (1969). Sleep

in Cetaceans: I. The pilot whale, Globicephala scammoni. Psychophysiology 6, 230.

Siegel J, R. M., Heller HC, Alam MN, Szymusiak R, McGinty D, et al. . (2006).

Commentaries on: Blanco-Centurion C, Xu M, Murillo-Rodriguez E, et al. Adenosine

and sleep homeostasis in the basal forebrain. J Neurosci August 2, 2006;26(31):8092-

100. Sleep 29, 1381–1389.1399.

Siegel, J. M. (2000). Brainstem mechanisms generating REM sleep. . In M. H. Kryger, Roth,

T., Dement, W.C. (Ed.), Principles and Practices of Sleep Mechanisms. (pp. 112–

133). Philadelphia: WB Saunders.

Siegel, J. M. (2004). The Neurotransmitters of sleep. . J. Clin. Psychiatry, 55, 4 -7.

Siegel, J. M. (2005). Clues to the functions of mammalian sleep. Nature, 437(7063), 1264-

1271.

Siegel, J. M., Manger, P., Nienhuis, R., Fahringer, H. M., & Pettigrew, J. (1996). The echidna

Tachyglossus aculeatus combines REM and nonREM aspects in a single sleep state:

implications for the evolution of sleep. . J. Neurosci., 16, 3500–3506.

Siegel, J. M., PR, M., R, N., HM, F., T, S., & JD, P. (1999). Sleep in the platypus. .

Neuroscience, 91, 391–400.


Sleep and the Brain – page 59/59

Silber, M. H., Ancoli-Israel, S., Bonnet, M. H., Chokroverty, S., Grigg-Damberger, M. M.,

Hirshkowitz, M., et al. (2007). The visual scoring of sleep in adults. J Clin Sleep Med,

3(2), 121-131.

Spooner, C. E. (1964). Observations on the use of the chick in the pharmacological

investigation of the central nervous system. Los Angeles. PhD. dissertation

Steinfels, G. F., Heym, J., Strecker, R.E., Jacobs, B.L. (1983). Behavioral correlates of

dopaminergic unit activity in freely moving cats. Brain Research, 258, 217–228.

Steriade, M. (2001). Active neocortical processes during quiescent sleep. Arch Ital Biol,

139(1-2), 37-51.

Steriade, M. (2006). Grouping of brain rhythms in corticothalamic systems. Neuroscience,

137(4), 1087-1106.

Steriade, M., & Amzica, F. (1998). Coalescence of sleep rhythms and their chronology in

corticothalamic networks. Sleep Research Online, 1(1), 1-10.

Steriade, M., Contreras, D., Curro Dossi, R., & Nunez, A. (1993). The slow (< 1Hz)

oscillation in reticular thalamic and thalamocortical neurons: scenario of sleep rhythm

generation in interacting thalamic and cortical networks. Journal of Neuroscience, 13,

3284-3299.

Steriade, M., & McCarley, R. W. (1990). Brainstem control of wakefulness and sleep. New

York: Plenum Press.

Steriade, M., McCarley, R. W. (Ed.). (2005). Brainstem Control of Wakefulness and Sleep.

New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publisher.


Sleep and the Brain – page 60/60

Steriade, M., McCormick, D. A., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1993). Thalamocortical oscillations in

the sleeping and aroused brain. Science, 262, 679-685.

Steriade, M., Pare, D., Datta, S., Oakson, G., & Curro Dossi, R. (1990). Different cellular

types in mesopontine cholinergic nuclei related to ponto-geniculo-occipital waves. J

Neurosci, 10(8), 2560-2579.

Steriade M., B. G. (1990). Parallel activation of thalamic and cortical neurons by brainstem

and basal forebrain cholinergic systems. In S. M. a. B. D (Ed.), Brain Cholinergic

Systems. (pp. 3-52). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Stewart, D. J., MacFabe, D. F., & Vanderwolf, C. H. (1984). Cholinergic activation of the

electrocorticogram: role of the substantia innominata and effects of atropine and

quinuclidinyl benzilate. Brain Research Bulletin, 322, 219–232.

Szymusiak, R. (1995). Magnocellular nuclei of the basal forebrain: substrates of sleep and

arousal regulation. Sleep, 18, 478-500.

Szymusiak, R., Steininger, T., Alam, N., McGinty, D. (2001). Preoptic area sleepregulating

mechanisms. Arch. Ital. Biol., 139, 77–92.

Takahashi, K., Lin, J.S., Sakai, K. (2006). Neuronal activity of histaminergic

tuberomammillary neurons during wake-sleep states in the mouse. Journal of

Neuroscience, 26, 10292–10298.

Tauber, E. S., Roffwarg, H. P., & Weitzman, E. D. (1966). Eye movements and

electroencephalogram activity during sleep in diurnal lizards. Nature, 212, 1612–1613.


Sleep and the Brain – page 61/61

Thakkar, M. M., Strecker, R. E., McCarley, R. W. (1998). Behavioral state control through

differential serotonergic inhibition in the mesopontine cholinergic nuclei: a

simultaneous unit recording and microdialysis study. J. Neurosci., 18, 5490–5497.

Thannickal, T. C., Siegel JM, Moore RY. (2003). Pettern of hypocretin (orexin) soma and

axon loss, and gliosis, in human narcolepsy. Brain Pathol, 13, 340-351.

Tobler, I. I., & Neuner-Jehle, M. (1992). 24-h variation of vigilance in the cockroach Blaberus

giganteus. J Sleep Res, 1(4), 231-239.

Tork, I. (1990). Anatomy of the serotonergic system. Annals of the New York Academy of

Sciences, 600, 9–34 discussion 34–35.

Trulson, M. E. (1985). Simultaneous recording of substantia nigra neurons and voltammetric

release of dopamine in the caudate of behaving cats. Brain Research Bulletin, 15, 221-

223.

Trulson, M. E., Preussler, D.W. (1984). Dopamine-containing ventral tegmental area neurons

in freely moving cats: activity during the sleep–waking cycle and effects of stress. .

Experimental Neurology, 83, 367–377.

Trulson, M. E., Preussler, D.W., Howell, G.A. (1981). Activity of substantia nigra units

across the sleep–waking cycle in freely moving cats. Neuroscience Letters, 26, 183–

188.

van Swinderen, B., & Andretic, R. (2003). Arousal in Drosophila. Behavioral Processes, 64,

133–144.
Sleep and the Brain – page 62/62

Vanni-Mercier, G., Sakai, K., Jouvet, M. . (1984). Neurones spécifiques de l’éveil dans

l’hypothalamus postérieur du Chat. . Comptes Rendus de l’Academie des Sciences ;

Serie III 298, 195–200.

Vertes, R. P., Martin, G.F. (1988). Autoradiographic analysis of ascending projections from

the pontine and mesencephalic reticular formation and the median raphe nucleus in the

rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 275, 511–541.

von Economo, C. (1930) Sleep as a problem of localization. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 71, 249–259.

Walker, J. M., & Berger, R. J. (1973) A polygraphic study of the tortoise (Testudo

denticulata): absence of electrophysiological signs of sleep. BrainBehav Evol, 8, 453–

467.

Warner, B. F., Huggins, S. E., & (1978 ). An electroencephalographic study of sleep in young

caimans in a colony. Comp Biochem Physiol A, 59, 139–144.

Wehrle, R., Czisch, M., Kaufmann, C., Wetter, T. C., Holsboer, F., Auer, D. P., et al. (2005).

Rapid eye movement-related brain activation in human sleep: a functional magnetic

resonance imaging study. Neuroreport, 16(8), 853-857.

Wehrle, R., Kaufmann, C., Wetter, T. C., Holsboer, F., Auer, D. P., Pollmacher, T., et al.

(2007). Functional microstates within human REM sleep: first evidence from fMRI of

a thalamocortical network specific for phasic REM periods. Eur J Neurosci, 25(3),

863-871.

Westerman, M. a. E. D. (1992). DNA hybridization and the phylogeny of monotremes. In A.

M. (Ed.), Platypus and Echidnas. (pp. 28–34). Mosman: Royal Zoological Society of

NSW.
Sleep and the Brain – page 63/63

Wu, M. F., Gulyani, S., Yau, E., and al. (1999). Locaul coeruleus neurons : cessation of

activity during cataplexy. Neuroscience, 91, 1389-1399.

Wu, M. F., John, J., Boehmer, L. N. and al. (2004) Activity of dorsal raphe cells across the

sleep-waking cycle and during cataplexy in narcoleptic dogs. J Physiol, 554 (pt 1),

202-215.

Zaborszky, L., Carlsen, J., Brashear, H. R., & Heimer, L. (1986a) Cholinergic and

GABAergic afferents to the olfactory bulb in the rat with special emphasis on the

projection neurons in the nucleus of the horizontal limb of the diagonal band. . Journal

of Comparative Neurology, 243, 488–509.

Zaborszky, L., Cullinan, W. E., & Braun, A. (1991) Afferents to basal forebrain cholinergic

projection neurons: an update. . Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology 295,

43-100.

Zaborszky, L., Heimer, L., Eckenstein, F., & Leranth, C. (1986b). GABAergic input to

cholinergic forebrain neurons: an ultrastructural study using retrograde tracing of HRP

and double immunolabeling. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 250, 282–295.

Zhdanova, I. V. (2006). Sleep in zebrafish. Zebrafish, 3(2), 215-226.


Sleep and the Brain – page 64/64

INFORMATION BOX 1. POLYSOMNOGRAPHIC TECHNIQUES

Polysomnography is the multi-modality recording of sleep using an array of

electrophysiological and physiological techniques allowing the analysis and scoring of sleep.

The main measures are electroencephalography, electrooculography and electromyography.

1. Electroencephalography (EEG) records electrical activity produced by synchronous


post-synaptic potentials of brain neurons. Recording is made possible using metal current

conductor electrodes placed in contact with the scalp. The resulting signal is amplified and

digitized, filtered and cleaned from artefacts, then examined for rhythmic and transient

activities. Rhythmic activities are continuous oscillations patterns in the EEG that vary in

frequency (cycles per second, expressed in Hertz [Hz]) and amplitude (expressed in

microvolts [µV]). Transient activities are isolated, short-duration events with

characteristic features (e.g. K-complexes, vertex waves and spindles in sleep, see main

text). When the EEG is recorded at multiple scalp locations according to international

conventions (e.g. the 10-20 systems; see Information Box Figure 1 below), it additionally

allows topographic, spatially-based analysis of specific sleep patterns (Finelli, Borbely &

Achermann, 2001).

--- INSERT INFORMATION BOX FIGURE 1 HERE ---

2. Electrooculography (EOG) records changes in voltage induced by the eye rotation, the
moving eyeball acting as a small battery, with the retina negative relative to the cornea.

Two electrodes located lateral to the eyes allow recording of horizontal eye movements,

two other electrodes above and below one eye allow recording vertical movements.
Sleep and the Brain – page 65/65

3. Electromyography (EMG) records electrical activity generated by muscles activity. The


EMG is typically recorded from under the chin, where muscular tonus changes with sleep

stages are most obvious.

Besides EEG, EOG and EMG, cardiac rhythms (electrocardiogram, ECG) and respiratory

activity can also be recorded simultaneously. Specific wires for EEG, EOG, EMG, ECG …

electrodes are connected through signal amplifiers to systems allowing recording and pre-

processing, storing and reviewing of the signal.

Thanks to the development of computer techniques, early paper-based analog

recordings have evolved toward digital polygraphs with high-capacity storage allowing

simultaneous recording and on-screen reviewing of multiple channels (up to 256 in high-

density EEG). According to the international consensus guidelines (Iber et al., 2007), EEG

with 16 or more channels would be an optimum to analyse sleep patterns, although a basic

scoring into different stages of sleep can be achieved with a minimum of 3 derivations located

frontally at F4-M1, centrally at C4-M1 and occipitally at O2-M1 (i.e. using right sided active

cortical electrodes F4, C4, O2 and a reference electrode M1 over the left mastoid in the

international 10-20 system; see Information Figure 1). Equally acceptable alternative

derivations are Fz-Cz, Cz-Oz, and C4-M1. In addition, many sleep units have a video camera

located in the bedroom, allowing the observation of the patient’s sleeping behaviour in

relation with variations in polysomnography recording, which is particularly useful in the

investigation of sleep-related disorders.


Sleep and the Brain – page 66/66

TABLE 1. Summary of the main features of each sleep stage.

Stages EEG Waveform Characteristics

(Frequency (Amplitude type

-Hz) -µV)

AWAKE 14-40 10 - 30 β State of full consciousness

characterized by a desynchronized

EEG pattern of high beta frequency

waves of low amplitude.

SLEEP 8-12 20 - 40 α Quiet or resting state characterized by

ONSET posterior alpha rhythm (especially over

OR PRE- the occipital cortex) with a lower

SLEEP frequency (8–12 Hz) and increased

synchrony and amplitude (20 to 40

µV).

N1 3-7 50-100 θ Transition period with little or no body

movement characterized by the

diminution of alpha waves and the

appearance of theta waves, slower in

frequency (3 to 7 Hz) and greater in


Sleep and the Brain – page 67/67

amplitude (50 to 100 µV) than alpha

waves.

N2 11-16 50-150 Occasional Basal oscillations below 5 Hz called

"sleep “slow waves” on which are


or
spindles" periodically superimposed sleep
Light sleep
spindles (short-lasting, waxing and
(σ)
waning oscillations in the 11- 16 Hz
Occasional
frequency range) and K complexes
"K"
(negative sharp waves immediately
complexes
followed by a positive component

standing out from the background EEG

with a total duration at least equal to

0.5 second).

N3 0.5-2 > 75 Slow The deepest possible state of physical

(previously waves rest. SWS is characterized by high-


Or
0.5-4) amplitude slow oscillations in the 0.5-2
(previously
Slow Wave
Hz frequency range, maximally
δ)
Sleep
expressed at frontal locations.
(SWS)

Slow oscillations (<1Hz) are cortical


<1
oscillations occurring during NREM
>140
sleep. These slow rhythms play a major
Sleep and the Brain – page 68/68

role in the synchronization of other

NREM sleep oscillations and events

(e.g. spindles).

REM 15-30 <50 Desynchronized EEG activity close to

that recorded during wakefulness,


Or
characterized by low amplitude-mixed
Paradoxical
frequency (15-30Hz, <50 µV) EEG
sleep
background, rapid eye movements and

muscular atonia.
Sleep and the Brain – page 69/69

FIGURE LEGENDS

INFORMATION BOX FIGURE 1. International 10-20 system for topographical disposition

of the electrodes on the scalp (simplified representation). Electrodes are located along

imaginary lines across the head between bony landmarks that intersect at 10% or 20% of their

total length distance intervals, hence the “10-20” name. Letters indicate locations according to

the brain structure below the electrode, Numbers indicate left-right lateral location, and

midline electrodes have the suffix “z” : F (Fp1-Fpz-Fp2-F7-F3-Fz-F4-F8), Frontal; C (C3-Cz-

C4), Central; T (T3-T4-T5-T6), Temporal; P (P3-Pz-P4), Parietal; O, (O1-Oz-O2), Occipital;

M (M1-M2), Mastoid. The use of the 10-20 system ensures symmetrical and reproducible

electrode placement for within- and between-subject comparisons. A basic sleep recording

may be obtained using 3 electrodes (F4-C4-O2) and one reference electrode (M1), represented

here in orange colour.

FIGURE 1. Hypnogram illustrating sleep stages and their cycle in the course of a canonical

night in a healthy young sleeper. Grey bars indicate changes in sleep stages (or wakefulness)

across time. Note the progressive decrease in NREM sleep prevalence from the first half to

the second half of the night, whereas the reverse pattern is observed for REM sleep.

FIGURE 2. Schematic representation of the 3 main processes involved in the regulation of

sleep. The homeostatic process maintains sleep propensity (duration and intensity of sleep)

which builds up during wakefulness and declines during sleep. The circadian process is a

nearly 24-hr endogenous oscillatory variation that determines the timing of the propensity to
Sleep and the Brain – page 70/70

sleep. Ultradian mechanisms underlie the NREM–REM sleep cycle. As the sleep episode

progresses, the intensity of NREM sleep declines and the duration of successive REM sleep

intervals increases. W: awake state; S: sleep. From Borbely et al. (2000).

FIGURE 3. EEG patterns of vigilance states.

FIGURE 4. EEG recording in a bottlenose dolphin during waking (A) and unihemispheric

slow wave sleep (USWS) in the right (B) and left (C) hemispheres (1-right cortex, 2-left

cortex, 3-right thalamus, 4-left thalamus). From Mukhametov et al., 1977.

FIGURE 5. Sagittal view of human brain depicting the main neurotransmitter structures and

pathways involved in the generation and the maintenance of sleep and wakefulness.

Acetylcholine (Ach)-synthesizing (cholinergic) cells in the pedunculopontine tegmentum

(PPT) and in the anterior hypothalamus (AH) of the basal forebrain (BF). (HA)-synthesizing

histaminergic cells in the posterior hypothalamic tuberomammillary nucleus (PH-TMN).

Norepinephrine (NE)-synthesizing (noradrenergic) cells in the locus coeruleus (LC).

Serotonin (5-HT)-synthesizing (serotonergic) cells in the raphe nuclei (RN). Hypocretine-

orexine cells in the lateral hypothalamus (LH). Dopamine (DA)-synthesizing dopaminergic

cells in the substantia nigra compacta (SNc) and ventral tegmental area (VTA). (Glut)-

synthesizing glutamatergic cells in the mesencephalic reticular formation (MRF). From Datta

& MacLean (2007).


Sleep and the Brain – page 71/71

FIGURE 6. The reciprocal inhibition model for REM sleep (McCarley et Hobson, 1975).

During wakefulness and NREM sleep, noradrenergic (NE) and serotoninergic (5-HT) activity

inhibits cholinergic (Ach) REM-on neurons in the pedunculopontine tegmentum (PPT) and

lateral dorsal tegmentum (LDT). Throughout the course of a NREM sleep episode, inhibitory

feedback gradually decreases NE and 5-HT activity in the locus coeruleus and dorsal raphe

nucleus (5-HT) respectively, releasing inhibition on acetylcholine REM-on cells, whose

increased activity induces REM sleep. Activity in REM-on cells in turn exerts an excitatory

effect on REM-off cells during REM sleep, eventually leading to the termination of REM

sleep when a sufficient level of inhibitory activity has been attained in NE and 5-HT REM-off

cells.

FIGURE 7. Neuronal loops in corticothalamic networks involved in generating the coherent

low-frequency oscillations characteristic of NREM sleep (spindles, delta and slow

oscillations). [A] corticothalamic neurons; [B] thalamocortical relay neurons; [C] thalamic

reticular neurons. From Borbely (2000).

FIGURE 8. Brain areas in which regional CBF (rCBF) decreases during NREM sleep as

compared to wakefulness and REM sleep, measured using PET. Mid- sagittal (left panel) and

transverse (right panel) sections show higher synchronization (as reflected by decreasedrCBF)

in central core structures (brainstem and thalamic nuclei), basal forebrain, basal ganglia,

anterior hypothalamus, orbito-frontal and anterior cingulate cortices, and precuneus. Numbers

in the left panel refer to the corresponding transverse sections on the right. From Maquet et al.

(1997).
Sleep and the Brain – page 72/72

FIGURE 9. Combined EEG-fMRI investigation of human NREM sleep. Panels show brain

regions in which increased event-related BOLD responses are commonly associated with the

depolarized (up) state of slow and delta waves. Associated curves show the time course of

fitted response amplitudes (arbitrary units) during slow or delta waves in the corresponding

circled brain area. (A) Pontine tegmentum. (B) Parahippocampal gyrus. (C) Cerebellum. (D)

Medial prefrontal cortex. (E) Inferior frontal gyrus (F) Posterior cingulate cortex. From Dang-

Vu et al. (2008).

FIGURE 10. Functional neuroanatomy of fast and slow spindles. Panels show larger brain

responses for fast (red) than slow (black) spindles in the hippocampus (a), mesial prefrontal

cortex (b), precentral gyrus (c) and postcentral gyrus (d). From Schabus et al. (2007).

FIGURE 11. Schematic representation of the functional neuroanatomy of normal human

REM sleep (Braun et al., 1997; Maquet et al., 2000; Maquet et al., 1996; Nofzinger et al.,

1997). Regions in red colour are those in which there is a relative increase in neural activity

associated with REM sleep; regions in blue correspond to relative decreases in neural activity

associated with REM sleep: (a) lateral view; (b) medial view; (c) ventral view. A: amygdala;

B: basal forebrain; Ca: anterior cingulate gyrus; Cp: posterior cingulate gyrus and precuneus;

F: prefrontal cortex (middle, inferior and orbito-frontal cortices); H: hypothalamus; M: motor

cortex; P: parietal cortex (inferior parietal lobule); PH: parahippocampical gyrus; O: occipital-

lateral cortex; Th: thalamus; T-O: temporo-occipital extrastriate cortex; TP: pontine

tegmentum. From Schwartz and Maquet (2002).


Sleep and the Brain – page 73/73

INFORMATION BOX FIGURE 1.


Sleep and the Brain – page 74/74

FIGURE 1.
Sleep and the Brain – page 75/75

FIGURE 2.
Sleep and the Brain – page 76/76

FIGURE 3.
Sleep and the Brain – page 77/77

FIGURE 4.
Sleep and the Brain – page 78/78

FIGURE 5
Sleep and the Brain – page 79/79

FIGURE 6
Sleep and the Brain – page 80/80

FIGURE 7
Sleep and the Brain – page 81/81

FIGURE 8
Sleep and the Brain – page 82/82

FIGURE 9
Sleep and the Brain – page 83/83

FIGURE 10
Sleep and the Brain – page 84/84

FIGURE 11

You might also like