© 2023 The Guilford Press: The Why, What, and How of Neuropsychological Interviewing
© 2023 The Guilford Press: The Why, What, and How of Neuropsychological Interviewing
1
The Why, What, and How
of Neuropsychological Interviewing
s
es
Pr
Yana Suchy
rd
Justin B. Miller
lfo
ui
G
e
C
Th
linical interviewing, along with behavioral observations and record review, represents
a core aspect of diagnostic decision making. These information-gathering methods
are shared by several disciplines, including clinical psychology, medicine, psychiatry, and
23
other allied health care professions. Over the years, these methods have taken on a variety
of forms, including formalized procedures, such as the mental status exam often preferred
20
findings that are placed in the context of a patient’s history and current complaints, as
well as appearance and overt behavior.
ht
Although widely adopted by our sister disciplines, clinical interviewing was not
ig
gathered via other standardized means, such as checklists) yielded more objective and
op
hence more valid conclusions (Chelune & Moehle, 1986; Russell, Neuringer, & Goldstein,
1970). This thinking was particularly prevalent among clinicians subscribing to the fixed
C
battery approach, which allowed collection of large amounts of actuarial data that were
used as the basis for clinical decision making. Despite these early notions, clinical neuro-
psychology has since fully come into the fold of clinical psychology, currently adhering
not only to the tried-and-true clinical practices of information gathering but also to the
American Psychological Association (APA; 2017) code of ethics, which virtually mandates
that no data be interpreted in the absence of a clinical interview and associated behavioral
observations.
In the current era, students who aspire to become clinical neuropsychologists obtain
training in foundational clinical skills, including the art of clinical interviewing and
1
2 Neuropsychological Interviewing of Adults
behavioral observation. Such training entails learning how to build rapport, how to main-
tain control over the interview, how and when to ask follow-up questions that maximize
the clinical utility of the patient’s report, and what domains of information are needed for
making diagnostic determinations. Additionally, this training involves identifying spe-
cific features of the patient’s appearance and behavior that are clinically and diagnosti-
cally relevant, as well as some basic understanding of how to review patients’ records. Yet,
as students progress from their general clinical training to more specialized settings in
clinical neuropsychology, they often find that their information-gathering knowledge and
skills barely scratch the surface of the mountain of information needed in a neuropsycho-
s
logical evaluation. The sheer amount of information that contributes to neuropsychologi-
es
cal decision making is further compounded by the fact that the specific content of a neu-
ropsychological interview can vary dramatically based on the setting, referral question,
Pr
population at hand, and specific diagnostic considerations. Despite these complexities,
no formalized materials are currently available to facilitate mastery of the neuropsycho-
rd
logical information-gathering process. This gap in neuropsychology reference materials
served as the impetus for this book.
lfo
ui
ABOUT THIS BOOKG
This book is intended to serve as a resource for trainees and clinical supervisors, as well
e
as for full-f ledged clinicians at all levels of professional development. Designed to serve
Th
both as a quick reference and as an in-depth resource, much information throughout the
book is presented in table format, and then further elaborated in text for those desir-
ing more extensive coverage. Tables cover a variety of topics, including the distinguishing
23
clinical features that contribute to a given differential diagnosis, sample interview ques-
tions, overviews of relevant behavioral observations, and even definitions of jargon terms
20
to read this introductory chapter, followed by the chapters that focus on a given setting
or a given patient population. Relatedly, clinical supervisors in specialized clinics may
yr
choose to routinely assign relevant chapters to all of their trainees at the beginning of
op
particular patient. In addition, the book is designed to have utility for experienced clini-
cians, especially those in generalist settings where a considerable breadth of knowledge is
required. For example, a clinician who typically sees referrals for suspected neurodegen-
erative disorders, acquired brain injury, and vague neurological issues with psychiatric
overlay may on occasion also encounter a less typical referral, such as a patient presenting
with cognitive sequelae of cancer treatment (see Chapter 11) or sequelae of hepatitis C (see
Chapter 14). In such cases, even an experienced clinician may benefit from a quick skim-
ming of tables within relevant chapters prior to seeing the patient, to ensure that all rel-
evant information is gathered. Last, experienced clinicians sometimes transition to a new
The Why, What, and How of Interviewing 3
setting altogether, and in such cases may benefit from reading the chapters that pertain
to the new setting in their entirety.
Throughout this book, we assume that clinical interviewing is inherently coupled
with behavioral observations, with the two processes informing and complementing each
other. Additionally, we assume that the direction of a clinical interview is initially deter-
mined by the type of information made available beforehand, whether it be the wording
of the referral question or a thorough review of the patient’s available records. In line
with these assumptions, the entire process of gathering information beyond test data (i.e.,
record review, clinical interview, behavioral observations) is covered in this book, with
s
dedicated chapters for discrete patient populations and common clinical settings encoun-
es
tered by neuropsychologists. Each chapter begins with an overview of the setting, relevant
referral sources, and disorders encountered by neuropsychologists in that setting. Next,
Pr
each chapter reviews the goals of a neuropsychological evaluation in that setting (along
with the types of information needed for accomplishing those goals), as well as how best
rd
to obtain such information via review of records, interview with a patient and/or a collat-
eral source, and behavioral observations. Last, potential red flags or pitfalls encountered
lfo
in a given setting are highlighted.
By design, there are several topics that are omitted from this book. First, this book
ui
focuses on adults. Given the growing specialization within clinical neuropsychology, the
G
inclusion of chapters on both adult and pediatric settings in a single volume was thought
to be unwieldy. Rather, a separate volume focusing purely on pediatric settings is pre-
e
ferred. Second, this book focuses on clinical issues and settings, purposely avoiding dis-
cussion of forensic issues. Much has been written about forensic neuropsychology, and a
Th
single chapter on forensic interviewing would not do justice to all of the complexities and
nuances of forensic evaluations. Third, since this book focuses on clinical populations
23
and clinical settings, it does not contain chapters on different formats or different social
contexts of information gathering. Thus, for example, the book does not contain a chapter
20
ters to focus purely on those aspects of information gathering that are unique to each
ig
In this section, we provide a brief overview of why it is crucial for clinicians to meet with
and interview their patient prior to initiating test administration.
Rapport Building
A clinical interview is an opportunity to build rapport with the patient and to reduce
some of the ambiguity surrounding the evaluation process. Often, simply just offering an
explanation of the clinical utility of the evaluation and how the test data will be used goes
4 Neuropsychological Interviewing of Adults
Gathering Information
Beyond Test Data
s
Record
es
Rapport
Review
Current Fx and Sx Prior Fx/Change
Pr
Battery
Selection Interview
rd
Reliability and Patient -Specific
lfo
Validity of Test Context
Observations
Data
ui
Referral
G
Question
e
Th
FIGURE 1.1. An overview of the main components of the why, the what, and the how of clinical infor-
mation gathering. Fx, functioning; Sx, symptoms.
23
a long way toward putting an anxious patient at ease or ameliorating a skeptical patient’s
20
tural or demographic context. Some patients may feel apprehensive about the evaluation
ig
because of a preconceived notion about how they might be perceived by the examiner due to
their age, sex/gender, country of origin, or racial or ethnic background. Such preconceived
yr
and concerns and engaging in an open, honest, and supportive conversation about the
issues at hand. Importantly, a patient who has good rapport with the clinician is not only
more likely to perform at their best during testing but is also more likely to take the time
to return for in-person feedback and, in turn, to follow through with recommendations.
Battery Selection
As the fixed battery approach began to give way to a flexible battery (Sweet, Klipfel, Nelson,
& Moberg, 2021), test selection has become an important step in the assessment process.
The Why, What, and How of Interviewing 5
Aside from the primary referral question, the clinical interview, records review, and behav-
ioral observations all play a critical role in determining the most appropriate assessment
instruments. Of note, review of records alone is not sufficient and should never be used as
a substitute for a one-on-one conversation with a patient. In fact, it is common for clini-
cians to make substantial adjustments to their battery once they meet their patient face-
to-face. Issues such as motor, sensory, or speech limitations, English as a second language,
fatigue, or behavioral problems may be unduly exaggerated or downplayed in the records,
or may not be described with the type of detail and nuance that is needed for determin-
ing the patient’s capacity to participate in all aspects of testing. For example, a note in
s
the patient records of “hemiparesis in the right arm” cannot be interpreted until hand
es
dominance is established. Similarly, a note that the patient is “legally blind” may fail to
acknowledge that the blindness pertains to peripheral vision and the patient can normally
Pr
perceive written material. In other words, the degree to which motor, sensory, cognitive,
and behavioral limitations interfere with test performance can often only be gleaned from
rd
a face-to-face interaction.
lfo
Reliability and Validity1 of Test Data
ui
Another reason for collecting information beyond test data is to determine whether test
G
data are meaningful. There is much to be learned in the context of a face-to-face interaction
that simply cannot be gleaned from any other source. For example, it is important to note
e
whether the patient is adequately aroused and alert, whether any sensory or motor issues
Th
might interfere with testing, or whether attitude toward the examiner might impact test
performance. In other words, information about these aspects of the evaluation is bound
to influence the clinical interpretation of test results. Furthermore, gross inconsistencies
23
between apparent functional capacity and test results can also sometimes be gleaned from
a face-to-face interaction. For example, a patient who is moderately impaired on memory
20
measures may demonstrate an exquisite capacity for using compensatory strategies while
answering interview questions.
©
Perhaps the most salient reason for collecting information beyond test data is that diag-
ig
nostic and functional decisions can never be based on test scores alone. Even for diagnoses
that are virtually defined by test scores, such as intellectual disability, it is necessary to
yr
ascertain the presence of a functional impairment in daily life before a diagnosis can be
op
made. Additionally, while test data may indicate levels of cognitive impairment, they alone
virtually never differentiate among specific diagnoses. Typically, to arrive at a differen-
C
tial diagnosis, additional information needs to be gathered from records, interview, and
behavioral observation that is specific to a given disorder. Fluctuations in a patient’s daily
functioning, changes in sleeping or eating habits, a recent loss of a loved one, inappropri-
ate behaviors, or an increase in falls are but a few examples of information beyond test data
that may have important ramifications for a differential diagnosis. Importantly, even if
1 The term validity in this context is intended to communicate general psychometric concepts of construct and
criterion validity and is not intended to be limited to a performance validity issue secondary to purposeful
feigning or exaggeration of deficits.
6 Neuropsychological Interviewing of Adults
such information is noted in the patient’s records, the clinical interview is an opportunity
to clarify and confirm such information and to further refine initial clinical impressions.
In this section, we briefly review what domains of information are routinely needed for neu-
ropsychological decision making. Once again, such information is best gathered through
a combination of a record review, face-to-face interview, and behavioral observations.
s
es
Current Functioning
Pr
Most diagnoses inherently require that a clinician integrate test data with information
about the patient’s functioning in daily life. For example, a formal diagnosis of dementia
rd
(or major neurocognitive disorder) stipulates that instrumental activities of daily living
(IADLs) be compromised secondary to the cognitive impairment evident on objective test-
lfo
ing. Additionally, despite the fact that the clinician collects test data to address current
cognitive ability, it is important to place such data in the context of daily cognitive func-
ui
tioning as perceived by the patient (and perhaps also as perceived by collateral sources or
G
as described in records). Such information provides a way of cross-validating test results
and also allows the clinician to place test performance into a meaningful context in the
e
clinical report or during a feedback session with the patient or family. As seen in Table
1.1, information about daily functioning should cover a variety of domains beyond cogni-
Th
tion and IADLs. Although many interviews cover most or all of the functional domains
listed in Table 1.1, the relative importance and degree of detail required in a given domain
23
may vary across different populations and settings, which is highlighted as appropriate in
subsequent chapters—for example, activities of daily living can be assumed to be intact in
20
with patients whose diagnoses have already been established. In such cases, evaluations
may be conducted purely for the purpose of characterizing the patient’s current cogni-
ht
tive abilities and for making functional determinations, such as determinations about a
ig
patient’s ability to continue to work or to live independently. The general type of infor-
mation gathered for this purpose is virtually identical to that gathered for diagnostic
yr
and informed determinations about the patient’s functional strengths and weaknesses,
insight, and the ability or willingness to use compensatory strategies. Chapter 2 provides
C
s
• Speed of processing
es
• Spatial and nonverbal reasoning
Pr
Behavioral • Impulse control
• Initiation/motivation
• Task completion/persistence
rd
• Social appropriateness
• Social engagement
lfo
• Personality
ui
Sensory/motor • Sensory (vision, hearing, smell, and taste)
• Motor (gross and fine)
• Falls
G
•
e
Instrumental activities of daily livinga Shopping
• Cooking/meal preparation
Th
(if relevant)b
• Mobility
ht
Psychological • Mood
• Anxiety
ig
• Apathy/anhedonia
• Delusions/hallucinations
op
• Suicidal/homicidal ideation
Vegetative • Sleep
C
Social • Friends
• Family
• Romantic partners
s
• Family distress (divorce, illness, family feud, etc.)
es
• Grief (loss of a loved one, a pet, driving privileges, etc.)
• Immigration status, discrimination
Pr
Current resources • Current social support (friends, family, acquaintances, etc.)
• Recreational outlets (hobbies, etc.)
rd
• Socioeconomic status/financial resources
• Health insurance
lfo
• Transportation
a See also Table 2.3. b See also Table 2.4.
ui
G
history of a reading disorder may struggle with a reading test, which may result in an
underestimate of the patient’s overall premorbid capacity. The types of information that
e
facilitate accurate estimates of premorbid functioning may include records of work and/or
Th
or attained degree) should be explored, including issues such as the ease or difficulty in
achieving those positions or degrees. Such information almost never comes from just one
20
source—rather, interviews with the patient and collateral sources, as well as record review,
may all contribute. See Table 1.2 for the most relevant aspects of premorbid functioning.
©
Many diagnoses require that some discrete pieces of information be gathered that are
ig
information about whether a realistic trauma to the head occurred, or about the imme-
op
diate sequelae of such a trauma (e.g., the extent of retrograde and anterograde amnesia).
Similarly, when evaluating a patient for a possible neurodegenerative disorder, behavioral
C
changes that typify a behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia are never captured
by testing and must instead be assessed via records review, interview, and observations.
Details about such setting- and diagnosis-specific information are presented in subse-
quent chapters.
premorbid and current functioning or about the specific diagnostic criteria. Contextual
information comes from a variety of sources, including psychosocial and developmental
history, educational and occupational history, medical and psychiatric history, legal his-
tory, and substance use history. Additionally, contextual factors surrounding the current
symptoms are important, such as stressors precipitating or exacerbating symptom onset.
Thus, for example, if a middle-age patient presents with unexplained cognitive decline,
substance use history could represent a potential culprit. Similarly, if a patient reports
psychiatric difficulties following a mild concussion, the clinician needs to inquire about
any pending civil litigations related to the patient’s injury, or a history of psychiatric dif-
s
ficulties prior to the accident. And if an older patient experiences a sudden decline in
es
functioning, it is important to understand whether such a change was precipitated by an
unusual stressor, such as the death of a spouse or a serious medical illness. An overview
Pr
of domains of contextual information that are typically assessed in a neuropsychological
interview can be found in Table 1.3.
rd
lfo
TABLE 1.2. Assessment of Historical/Premorbid Level of Functioning
Functional domains Specific topics
Educational Hx • Highest milestone completed
ui
G
| Contextualized via educational opportunities
• Objective academic performance across educational levels
e
| Grades
Th
| Gifted program
| Academic accommodations/remediations
| Repeated or skipped grades
| Scaffolding/tutors
23
| Behavioral problems
Everyday planning, • Prior experience with and ease versus difficulty with
yr
Instrumental activities
of daily living | Shopping
| Cooking/meal preparation
| Cleaning/household chores/home maintenance
| Finances
| Communication/Internet use
| Medication/medical regimen management
| Schedule and appointments
| Driving and transportation
s
es
Medical Hx • Neurological CNS disorders (seizures, multiple sclerosis, stroke,
neurodegenerative disorders, brain tumor, etc.)
Pr
• Non-neurological disorders with CNS ramifications (e.g., hypertension,
diabetes, heart disease, cancers and cancer treatments, hormonal
disorders, liver disease, kidney disease, COPD, infections affecting CNS)
rd
• Other major medical illnesses and hospitalizations
• Major surgeries and adverse surgical events
lfo
• Major injuries (including specific inquiries about ABIs)
ui
Psychiatric Hx • Disorders characterized by cognitive Sx (ADHD, ASD, learning
disorders, intellectual disability, etc.)
• Psychiatric diagnoses
G
• Psychiatric symptoms, without diagnosis
e
• Psychiatric hospitalizations
• Suicidal ideation and/or attempts
Th
spectrum disorder.
ht
In this section, we briefly review how a clinician goes about gathering information beyond
yr
test data, or how different types of information can be used to answer diagnostically and
op
functionally relevant questions. Specifically, this section reviews the general process of
reviewing records, gathering information in face-to-face2 interviews with patients and col-
C
Records Review
Ideally, a clinician would have the opportunity to review patient records prior to the patient
arriving for the evaluation so as to begin formulating clinical hypotheses. For providers
2 Typically,
and preferred in most settings, interviews are conducted face-to-face with the patient, though tele-
medicine options are becoming more commonplace.
The Why, What, and How of Interviewing 11
that work outside of an institutional setting, this typically requires some foresight, includ-
ing working with patients to identify the custodian(s) of the most salient records and
securing written permission to obtain such records. Additionally, since there is consider-
able variability in the ease versus difficulty of securing records from different sources,
some practitioners may need to contact other health care providers several weeks prior to
the appointment with their patient, due to a considerable lag many hospitals and clinics
experience when responding to record requests. In contrast, some settings lend themselves
to easy access to patients’ records. For example, practitioners that work in institutional
settings (e.g., Veterans Affairs hospitals, major medical centers) and see internally referred
s
patients readily have access to the patient’s shared electronic medical records, as long as
es
the patient’s other health care needs have been handled by the same institution. Although
in many clinical situations only medical records are reviewed, in some cases records from a
Pr
variety of sources are made available. Table 1.4 provides an overview of the common types
of information that different record sources have to offer.
rd
All types of records listed in Table 1.4 have the potential to contribute to answering
questions about current and premorbid functioning, as well as specific diagnostic and
lfo
contextual considerations. For example, school or work records, especially if spanning a
number of years, offer information about the premorbid level of functioning, and a sudden
ui
change in school/work performance could potentially provide insight about the timing of
G
the onset of functional difficulties. Similarly, legal records may help clarify whether crimi-
nal behavior can be attributed to a particular diagnosis, or whether it was present premor-
e
bidly. In the same vein, such records may help pinpoint the onset of functional changes,
evidenced by a sudden late-life onset of shoplifting, car accidents, or sexual misconduct.
Th
TABLE 1.4. Overview of Relevant Record Types for Determination of Premorbid and Current Functioning
23
|
tests | Incident reports | EMT/ED reports
| Attendance | Traffic offenses | Physician notes
ht
|
• Department of Corrections
relevant | Procedure (e.g., surgery)
(jail/prison)
op
summaries
• Work | Convictions/sentences
| Application form, résumé, etc. | Admission/processing/ • Outpatient
C
Last, medical and psychiatric records often provide discrete diagnosis-specific informa-
tion (e.g., neuroimaging, psychotic episode), but may also help pinpoint the onset of a
functional change (e.g., a sudden increase in falls or accidents reported in the emergency
department records), or may again help determine whether reported difficulties are linked
to a suspected diagnosis or whether they may have been present premorbidly (e.g., chronic
headaches, psychiatric hospitalizations).
s
Typically, a neuropsychological interview begins with a thorough and detailed inquiry into
es
the reason for the current evaluation, including a detailed history of the present illness or
the cognitive/functional difficulties that prompted the evaluation. Additionally, a detailed
Pr
inquiry into how, or whether, present functioning reflects a change from a historical or pre-
morbid baseline takes place early on in the interview. These inquiries are then followed by
rd
gathering additional contextual information that allows the clinician to place the current
symptoms and complaints into a broader context, allowing a more nuanced interpretation.
lfo
As is the case in any clinical interview (i.e., outside of clinical neuropsychology), skilled
clinicians follow up a patient’s responses with clarifying questions. For example, vague
ui
statements, such as “I don’t drink too much” or “I did great in school,” need to be followed
up with questions that gather specific information about, say, the actual number of drinks
G
consumed per day or the actual grades the patient earned in school. Similarly, abstract
e
statements, such as “I am depressed” or “I have an anxiety disorder,” need to be followed
up with questions about specific symptoms, as patients often come with preconceived
Th
(and often inaccurate) notions about what their symptoms signify. It is also important to
remain mindful of the patient’s cognitive status and their ability to comprehend verbal
23
communication. Asking complex, multipart questions may need to give way to simple
questions with concrete answers in order to elicit information effectively.
20
tion using lay terminology that does not necessarily map onto neurocognitive domains.
ig
For example, it is quite common for patients to refer to a variety of cognitive difficulties
as “memory problems,” whereas in neuropsychological parlance the patient may actually
yr
tional problem such as common losses of mental set, or a lack of motivation. Alternatively,
it is common for patients to state that their “memory is great” because they remember
C
things as far back as their childhood, though when asked about recent events from earlier
in the day, they may have little recollection. Thus, clinicians must clarify statements per-
taining to cognition, rather than accept them at face value. Because memory complaints
are quite common in a variety of both clinical and healthy populations (Begum et al., 2014;
Kareken et al., 1992; Schmidt et al., 2016), and because memory complaints can reflect dif-
ficulties in other cognitive domains (e.g., executive functioning, word retrieval, processing
speed; Baker, Gibson, Georgiou-Karistianis, & Giummarra, 2018; Minett, Da Silva, Ortiz,
& Bertolucci, 2008; Torrens-Burton, Basoudan, Bayer, & Tales, 2017), Table 1.5 provides a
list of sample clarifying questions as they pertain to typical memory complaints.
The Why, What, and How of Interviewing 13
Memory retention • “When you say you forget things, do you mean you actually don’t remember
versus memory things even if reminded of them, or do you mean that you cannot retrieve
s
retrieval information but recognize it if someone reminds you?”
es
• “Do you find yourself misplacing things and then not being able to retrace
your steps?”
Pr
Memory versus • “When you say you don’t remember what you read in a book or what you saw
attention in a movie, do you mean you don’t recall things no matter how hard you pay
rd
attention to them, or is it that your mind wanders and so you never quite
process what you read in a book or saw on TV?”
lfo
Memory versus • “When you say you can’t remember anyone’s name, do you mean you
ui
word finding have trouble learning names of people you just met, or do you mean you
have trouble recalling names of people you know well but the names
G
spontaneously come to you at a later time?”
• “Is it only people’s names, or do you also have trouble remembering the
e
names of objects and places?”
Th
Memory versus • “When you say you get lost, do you mean you cannot envision a route you
spatial difficulties want to take, or is it that you don’t recall where you intended to go?”
• “Do you have trouble remembering how to get to familiar places that you’ve
23
been to before, or is it only new places that you have trouble with?”
20
Memory versus • “When you say you forgot how to cook, do you mean you literally cannot
executive recall a recipe, or do you mean you get overwhelmed by all the steps and can’t
execute them all in the correct order?”
©
ht
Interviewing a collateral source can be highly informative in most populations, but espe-
yr
cially when working with populations where reduced insight or memory limitations can
op
hinder accurate self-report, such as older adults suffering from a neurodegenerative dis-
order and survivors of brain injury or stroke (especially in acute and postacute inpatient
C
settings). Additionally, some patients may at times purposely choose not to be forthcom-
ing about day-to-day challenges (e.g., those worried about losing their independence or
driving privileges, or fitness for duty evaluations). Conversely, some patients may be prone
to overpathologizing normal cognitive lapses (e.g., the “worried well”; cogniform or soma-
tization disorders), and others may intentionally overreport or embellish symptoms (e.g.,
individuals with potential for secondary gain). In all of these cases, collateral informa-
tion may shed light on actual functionality or factual historical events. Last, in inpa-
tient settings, relevant hospital staff can provide invaluable information about functional
fluctuations (which themselves can be diagnostic), such as discrete periods of alertness
14 Neuropsychological Interviewing of Adults
alternating with an acute confusional state, behavioral problems emerging during family
visits, or periods of agitation emerging when fatigued (e.g., after lunch or after a physical
therapy session). It is important to note that the accuracy of collateral informants must
not be taken for granted. For example, relatives of older patients may at times minimize
the patients’ cognitive difficulties, and other times exaggerate them for a variety of psy-
chological or even secondary-gain reasons.
Behavioral Observations
s
During a neuropsychological evaluation, one needs to be vigilant about all the same
es
behavioral observations that are important during a general clinical intake. Additionally,
there are certain behavioral observations that are unique to neuropsychology (see Table
Pr
1.6). For example, while a general clinical psychologist might describe language as “word
salad,” a clinical neuropsychologist may need to be more discriminating, noting specifi-
rd
cally whether a patient was exhibiting literal versus semantic paraphasias, whether the
patient’s speech was fluent or halting, and the degree to which a patient understood gram-
lfo
mar versus nouns and verbs. Similarly, while a general clinical psychologist might simply
state that the patient had a tremor, a clinical neuropsychologist may need to describe the
ui
type of tremor (e.g., resting, action, or intention), as such specificity can often be impor-
G
tant for a differential diagnosis. Pathognomonic signs (i.e., behavioral signs that repre-
sent clear signals of a particular type of neuropathology) span several domains of behav-
e
ioral observations, including language, motor behavior, social inappropriateness, extreme
impulsivity, arousal and arousal fluctuations, thought processes, and observable memory
Th
issues, many of which can be readily apparent over the course of the interview. When rel-
evant, such signs are detailed further in subsequent chapters.
23
20
Consistent with the APA code of ethics, gathering information beyond test data represents
©
chological contexts. See Figure 1.1 for an overview. Methods unique to different popula-
tions, settings, or referral questions are presented in subsequent chapters.
yr
op
REFERENCES
C
American Psychological Association. (2017). Ethical principles for psychologists and code of conduct.
www.apa.org/ethics/code
Baker, K. S., Gibson, S. J., Georgiou-K aristianis, N., & Giummarra, M. J. (2018). Relationship
between self-reported cognitive difficulties, objective neuropsychological test performance
and psychological distress in chronic pain. European Journal of Pain, 22(3), 601–613.
Begum, A., Dewey, M., Hassiotis, A., Prince, M., Wessely, S., & Stewart, R. (2014). Subjective cogni-
tive complaints across the adult life span: A 14-year analysis of trends and associations using
the 1993, 2000 and 2007 English Psychiatric Morbidity Surveys. Psychological Medicine, 44(9),
1977–1987.
The Why, What, and How of Interviewing 15
TABLE 1.6. Behavioral Domains and Observations of Interest during the Clinical Interview
Domain Observations and signs of interest Neuropsychology-specific implications
Arrival • On time? • Consider possible deficits in
• Accompanied? | Memory
| Prospective memory
| Executive functions
s
|
es
(e.g., major scars, major facial
tattoos and piercings, deformities,
Pr
evidence of self-harm)
Motor functioning • Ambulation and mobility aids • Consider ramifications for test selection
rd
• Hemiparesis and test performance
• Gait and posture • Consider implications for
lfo
• Arm swing recommendations
• Gross motor functioning • Consider implications of gait/posture and
ui
• Fine motor functioning involuntary movements for:
• Involuntary movements (e.g., | Movement disorders (Chapter 7)
G
tremor, chorea, tics) | Stroke (Chapter 8)
• Place | Memory
• Time | Reasoning
• Situation • Consider possible delirium
C
(continued)
16 Neuropsychological Interviewing of Adults
s
es
Thought processes • Logic, linearity • Consider implications for thought disorder
and content • Perseverations (Chapter 16)
Pr
• Coherence • Consider implications for executive
• Concreteness functioning
• Delusions
rd
Mood • Depressed (psychomotor • Consider implications for mood disorder
lfo
retardation, slouched posture, • Consider implications for performance
depressed affect, tears, of highly effortful tests (executive
ui
verbalizations) functioning, memory retrieval)
• Manic (psychomotor agitation, G • Consider implications for processing speed
pressured speech, verbalizations)
| Treatment recommendations
ig
|Veracity of self-report
impact on others |Treatment recommendations
op
| Anosodiaphoria
Chelune, G. J., & Moehle, K. A. (1986). Neuropsychological assessment and everyday functioning.
In D. Wedding, A. M. Horton, & J. Webster (Eds.), The neuropsychology handbook: Behavioral and
clinical perspectives (pp. 489–525). Springer.
Kareken, D. A., Williams, J. M., Mutchnick, M. G., Harter, G., Torres, I., & George, W. E., Jr. (1992).
Self-report of cognitive function after cardiac surgery. Neuropsychology, 6(3), 197–209.
Minett, T. S. C., Da Silva, R. V., Ortiz, K. Z., & Bertolucci, P. H. F. (2008). Subjective memory com-
plaints in an elderly sample: A cross-sectional study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry,
23(1), 49–54.
Russell, E. W., Neuringer, C., & Goldstein, G. (1970). Assessment of brain damage: A neuropsychological
key approach. Wiley-Interscience.
s
Schmidt, J. E., Beckjord, E., Bovbjerg, D. H., Low, C. A., Posluszny, D. M., Lowery, A. E., . . . Rechis,
es
R. (2016). Prevalence of perceived cognitive dysfunction in survivors of a wide range of cancers:
Results from the 2010 LIVESTRONG Survey. Journal of Cancer Survivorship, 10(2), 302–311.
Pr
Sweet, J. J., Klipfel, K. M., Nelson, N. W., & Moberg, P. J. (2021). Professional practices, beliefs, and
incomes of U.S. neuropsychologists: The AACN, NAN, SCN 2020 practice and “salary survey.”
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 35(1), 7–80.
rd
Torrens-Burton, A., Basoudan, N., Bayer, A. J., & Tales, A. (2017). Perception and reality of cognitive
lfo
function: Information processing speed, perceived memory function, and perceived task dif-
ficulty in older adults. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 60(4), 1601–1609.
ui
G
e
Th
23
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C