Claude Germain 2018

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 30

The Neurolinguistic

Approach (NLA) for


Learning and Teaching
Foreign Languages
The Neurolinguistic
Approach (NLA) for
Learning and Teaching
Foreign Languages:

Theory and Practice

By

Claude Germain
The Neurolinguistic Approach (NLA) for Learning and Teaching
Foreign Languages: Theory and Practice

By Claude Germain

This book first published 2018

Cambridge Scholars Publishing

Lady Stephenson Library, Newcastle upon Tyne, NE6 2PA, UK

British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data


A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library

Copyright © 2018 by Claude Germain

All rights for this book reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced,
stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means,
electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, without
the prior permission of the copyright owner.

ISBN (10): 1-5275-1581-8


ISBN (13): 978-1-5275-1581-9

The translation of this book, by MacfarLingua Language Services, is


based on the manuscript entitled L’approche neurolinguistique (ANL)
pour l'apprentissage-enseignement des langues étrangères – Théorie et
pratique provided by Claude Germain.
TABLE OF CONTENTS

Foreword .................................................................................................. viii

Acknowledgments ....................................................................................... x

Note ........................................................................................................... xii

Abbreviations ........................................................................................... xiii

Part One: Theoretical Foundations of the NLA

Chapter One ................................................................................................. 3


Five Lessons from the Neurosciences
First Lesson: The Absence of a Direct Connection Between
Declarative Memory and Procedural Memory ................................. 3
Second Lesson: The Complexity/Flexibility of the Brain and Two
Neuronal Mechanisms: conscious (vocabulary) and Unconscious
(lexicon)............................................................................................ 9
Third Lesson: Focusing on the Meaning or on the Task to Develop
Implicit Competence ...................................................................... 11
Fourth Lesson: The Importance of “Transfer Appropriate Processing”
(TAP) and the Limbic System’s Role ............................................ 11
Fifth Lesson: Intake and Individual Cognitive Benefits of Social
Interaction ...................................................................................... 13

Chapter Two .............................................................................................. 16


The NLA’s Five Fundamental Principles
First Principle: Distinguishing between Two Grammars, Internal
(Implicit Competence) and External (Explicit Knowledge) ........... 16
Second Principle: Literacy (Primacy of Orality) and Pedagogy
of the Sentence (Importance of the Lexicon) .................................. 18
Third Principle: Emphasising the Meaning of Messages and Use
of Project-Based Pedagogy ............................................................ 22
Fourth Principle: TAP and Motivation: Authenticity of (Oral)
Communication and the (Written) Document ................................ 24
Fifth Principle: Social Interaction ........................................................ 25
vi Table of Contents

Chapter Three ............................................................................................ 31


The NLA and Other Approaches
The “Grammatical Paradox” ................................................................ 31
Reassessing Language Teaching’s General Reference Models ........... 32
The NLA and the CEFR’s Action-Oriented Approach:
A Comparison ................................................................................ 35
Comparison with Some Other Teaching Approaches .......................... 44
Language Textbooks ............................................................................ 47

Part Two: The NLA in Practice: Frequently Asked Questions

Chapter Four .............................................................................................. 54


The Predominance of Acquiring Oral Skills in the NLA
Acquiring and Teaching Internal Grammar ......................................... 54
Vocabulary and Lexicon Acquisition................................................... 64
Teaching Oral Comprehension and Phonetics ..................................... 80

Chapter Five .............................................................................................. 82


Learning and Teaching Reading, Writing and Culture with the NLA
Learning and Teaching Reading .......................................................... 82
Teaching External Grammar ................................................................ 90
Learning and Teaching Writing ........................................................... 92
The Spoken and the Written .............................................................. 107
Teaching and Appropriating Culture ................................................. 117

Part Three: Implementation of the NLA

Chapter Six .............................................................................................. 126


The NLA and Intensive French
The Canadian Linguistic Context ...................................................... 126
First Experience Outside Canada: Implementation
at the South China Normal University (SCNU) ........................... 146

Part Four: Some Outcomes of the NLA

Chapter Seven.......................................................................................... 154


NLA Results: Evaluation
Evaluations at the End of a Teaching Unit......................................... 154
Quantitative Evaluation of Learning .................................................. 155
Some Testimonials from Learners ..................................................... 182
The NLA as a Universal Approach .................................................... 185
The Neurolinguistic Approach (NLA) for Learning vii
and Teaching Foreign Languages

Chapter Eight ........................................................................................... 187


Training Teachers in the NLA
Effects of Teaching on Learning ........................................................ 187
Training Second-language Teachers .................................................. 195

Conclusion ............................................................................................... 200


Why Adopt the NLA

Appendices

Appendix A ............................................................................................. 208


Origins of Intensive French

Appendix B.............................................................................................. 210


The First IF Classes in Canada

Appendix C.............................................................................................. 212


Intensive Learning of French—the Bulgarian Case

Appendix D ............................................................................................. 214


Sample Project

Bibliography ............................................................................................ 217


FOREWORD

Twenty years already! It was the spring of 1997 when my colleague Joan
Netten and I began our discussions about the best way to improve the state
of French-as-a-second-language learning among Anglophone
schoolchildren in Canada. Over time, the Intensive French (IF) curriculum
we designed, developed, and gradually implemented across Canada’s
provinces and territories changed shape. When it was implemented at
China’s South China Normal University (SCNU) for young Chinese
university students enrolled in a specialised French-language programme,
it became known as the Neurolinguistic Approach (NLA).
Soon after, as the NLA spread to neighbouring areas in Asia (Taiwan,
Hong Kong, Japan), an increasing number of stakeholders in the area of
learning and teaching second or foreign languages (L2/FL) expressed the
desire to learn more about the origins of this new approach for adults.
Pressure grew for the publication of a work, accessible to all, which would
set out the foundations of the NLA as well as its results.
To celebrate the NLA’s 20th anniversary, that work has now been
produced and is in your hands.
Over the years, Joan Netten has had to concentrate primarily on the
evolution of IF among young students in Canada, while I have focused
increasingly on the implementation of the NLA at the adult level and in an
international context. While recognising her role in the NLA’s evolution, I
am the sole author of this book.
The present work is first and foremost addressed to current L2/FL
teachers, as well as to students of language education pedagogy, future
language teachers, and future second-language teaching theorists. I hope
that it will also interest language-teaching theorists, as a fair number of the
issues addressed are liable to call into question many “received ideas”,
particularly about the relationships between language learning and
language teaching.
The book is divided into four major parts. Two of these are geared
towards researchers, students in language teaching, and experts in second-
language teaching interested in the approach’s theoretical foundations
(Chapters 1, 2, and 3) as well as in the results obtained (Chapters 7 and 8).
Two other parts are more directly addressed to language teachers
interested in the approach’s pedagogical applications (Chapters 4 and 5)
The Neurolinguistic Approach (NLA) for Learning ix
and Teaching Foreign Languages

and the NLA’s origins and implementation, in Canada, as well as in Asia,


and elsewhere in the world (Chapter 6).
Broadly speaking, the two more “theoretical” parts frame the more
“practically” oriented parts (the second part [Chapters 4 and 5] and the
third part [Chapter 6]). The latter are presented in a Frequently Asked
Questions format and include graphics and illustrations to facilitate
understanding.
It should be noted that in the more practical parts of the work, the
questions answered in the Frequently Asked Questions are those that I
have most often been asked, either by students in language-education
programmes or by teachers at conference lectures I have given or training
sessions I have facilitated in various countries. As I formulated my
answers for this book, I realised that, in the second and third parts, I could
no longer use quite the same writing style I was used to. For instance, the
constant use of text boxes may prove as disconcerting to some readers as it
was for me when I began writing this book. These boxes allowed me to
occasionally offer useful complementary information about the
development of my ideas, as well as anecdotes, testimonials from teachers,
tips and tricks, transcriptions from videos, and so forth. I also determined
that frequent recourse to illustrations and figures taken from the large
number of slides I have used over the years in presentations on the NLA
would make certain parts of the book not only more attractive, but also
more accessible.
The Neurolinguistic Approach (NLA) for Learning and Teaching
Foreign Languages is thus both “scholarly” and “non-technical,” and will,
I hope, provide food for thought for all.

Claude Germain, Professor Emeritus


Université du Québec à Montréal (UQÀM), Canada
Professor Emeritus
South China Normal University (SCNU)
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I would like to extend my sincerest and warmest thanks to:


— the 13 students of the 2016-17 graduating class of the Master 1
Didactiques des langues, FLÉ (Français langue étrangère),
Technologies Éducatives (Language Education, French as a
Foreign Language, Educational Technologies), of the Université
d’Angers, who sent me 53 questions that I hope I have adequately
answered: Kani, Marie-Edith, Camille, Salam, Kristell, Élodie,
Burcu, Mouna, Lidia, Diana, Maud, Nora, Anne-Sophie, Anne,
Mélanie, and their teacher, Delphine Guedat-Bittighoffer;
— David Bel, Director of the French Department of the South China
Normal University (SCNU), who enabled the NLA to be launched
in China, with the constant support of the faculty’s Dean, Wu Jianli
(Amy);
— the FFL (French as a Foreign Language) teachers of the French
Department of the South China Normal University (SCNU), as well
as their many students whom I’ve had an opportunity to observe in
the classroom twice yearly over a six-year period;
— the FFL (French as a Foreign Language) teachers of the French
Department of Nanhai Experimental High School in Foshan City,
China, as well as their many students, whom I was also
occasionally able to observe;
— Ching-Hsin Chang, Director of the Department of European
Languages of Da-Yeh University (Taiwan) and NLA teacher, for
her collaboration and her permission to let me observe her FFL
classes;
— NLA teacher Elham Mohammadi of the To Be Language Institute
(Tehran), for allowing me to observe her classes by video;
— the 30 student teachers who participated in the NLA training
session organised by Olivier Massé in Tokyo, Japan, in March
2015, for their numerous pertinent questions;
— Gladys Benudiz, Romain Jourdan and Raymond LeBlanc, for
agreeing to review my manuscript;
The Neurolinguistic Approach (NLA) for Learning xi
and Teaching Foreign Languages

— Clément Beaudoin, President of Myosotis Presse, for rising to the


challenge of publishing, in French, the first book on this topic:
L’approche neurolinguistique (ANL) : Foire aux questions
(Myosotis Presse, 2017);
— my partner Louise, for her endless patience and unfailing
understanding.

Apologies and my thanks also to all those whom I may have forgotten to
mention.
NOTE

While the Neurolinguistic Approach (NLA) for the teaching of second and
foreign languages may be applied to all language-learning classroom
situations, the numerous examples of student work provided in the
discussion that follows come mostly from French-as-a-second/foreign-
language classrooms. The NLA’s principle of authenticity makes it
primordial that such examples genuinely illustrate the issues that can arise
in the classroom, and as the NLA was originally developed for and applied
to learning French at the school level, FSL/FFL classrooms naturally
presented the widest pool of examples to choose from.
These examples of original student work are accompanied in the text
by English translations which attempt to provide equivalent errors to those
found in the original French.
Unless otherwise indicated, translations of quotations from sources
published in languages other than English have been provided by the
translators. Any errors or omissions in the translations are the
responsibility of the translators.
ABBREVIATIONS

The following abbreviations are used throughout this book:

L1 First or dominant language

L2 Second language

FL Foreign language

DELF Diplôme élémentaire de langue française: a certificate


provided after successfully passing a French language
exam. The DELF assesses language proficiency based on
a scale developed by the CEFR.

CEFR Common European Framework of Reference for


languages

NLA Neurolinguistic Approach

IF Intensive French: refers to a French-as-a-second-


language program delivered in a number of schools in
Canada. The program offers up to 300 hours or second-
language instruction during a five-month period of the
school year
PART ONE

THEORETICAL FOUNDATIONS OF THE NLA

Firstly, it is important to mention that in second-language pedagogy, all


research begins with an issue from the field. In the late 1990s, the issue
from the field with which we were faced was that of the low levels of
communicative skills shown by the majority of Anglophone Canadian
schoolchildren learning French (with the exception of those in French
Immersion classes). We consequently needed to examine both theoretical
and foundational scholarship to discover potential solutions. In other
words, we needed to delve into the relevant data from the various
disciplines involved in language pedagogy.
After a while, it became clear that the greatest difficulty needing to be
addressed was the lack of an adequate relationship between
acquiring/learning a language and the ways in which it was taught. To a
large extent, it seemed to us that this difficulty could be ascribed to the
fact that the multiple disciplines involved in language learning/teaching
existed in highly compartmentalised silos. We therefore focused our
attention on the various disciplines providing links between what is known
about how L2/FLs are acquired/learnt and how they can be taught. It was
at this point that the then-new area of neuroscience research struck us as
potentially being able to shed new light on how we might conceptualise
relationships between L2/FL acquisition/learning and teaching strategies
used in the classroom.
As we examined research in the various disciplines that contribute to
second-language pedagogy, the neurosciences yielded up Paradis’s
neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism (1994, 2004, 2009). Over time, the
neurosciences provided us with five major lessons or conceptual data
models which all struck us as pertinent to our goal of improving results in
acquiring/learning languages.
The first chapter deals with the five lessons or teachings that provided
us with the foundations for the development of the neurolinguistic
approach (NLA). Building on these theoretical foundations, we devised
the five fundamental principles of the NLA; these are discussed in the
second chapter. In order to highlight the originality of the NLA, in the
2 Part One

third chapter I compare the approach to some others, in particular the


action-oriented approach advocated in the Common European Framework
of Reference for Languages (CEFR, 2001). It is worth noting that we
designed the NLA in 1997 and began to test it in the classroom in
September 1998, some years before the CEFR was published.
CHAPTER ONE

FIVE LESSONS FROM THE NEUROSCIENCES

This chapter deals with the five lessons that we drew from the
neurosciences in order to improve L2/FL learning/teaching in school
settings. These lessons constitute the theoretical basis for the NLA. Our
approach builds on data produced by recent research in cognitive
psychology, particularly the neurosciences, as well as the design of an
approach in the development of literacy1 skills specific to L2/FL, all from
a neuroeducational2 perspective (as we will see in detail further on).
The neurolinguistic approach (NLA) is a new paradigm, that is, a new
way of conceiving the relationships between appropriating (acquiring
and/or learning) and teaching a second or foreign language (L2/FL), which
aims at creating optimal conditions, in a classroom setting, for
spontaneous communication and successful social interaction.
The NLA was developed by two Canadian scholars: Claude Germain,
a Francophone Québécois, Emeritus Professor at the Université du Québec
à Montréal (UQÀM) and Emeritus Professor at the South China Normal
University (SCNU), and his Anglophone colleague Joan Netten, Member
of the Order of Canada and Honorary Research Professor at Memorial
University of Newfoundland (MUN).

First Lesson: The Absence of a Direct Connection between


Declarative Memory and Procedural Memory
Our principal neuroscience reference source is Paradis’s neurolinguistic
theory of bilingualism (2004). We should note, however, that ten years
earlier, he had already introduced the major elements of his theory in an

1
Literacy is here defined as the ability “to use the listening, viewing, speaking,
reading and writing strands of language(s), and other ways of representing […] to
think, learn, and communicate effectively” (Government of Newfoundland and
Labrador, 2002). For a more elaborate definition, see next chapter.
2
By neuroeducation, we refer to the study of cerebral mechanisms as applied to
education.
4 Chapter One

important chapter entitled “Neurolinguistic Aspects of Implicit and


Explicit Memory: Implications for Bilingualism” (Paradis 1994), which he
contributed to Nick Ellis’s collective work Implicit and Explicit Learning
of Second Languages.
Paradis introduces his neurolinguistic theory with a fact long known to
psychologists, namely the distinction between two types of memory:
declarative memory and procedural memory.
Declarative memory deals with facts or conscious knowledge: for
instance, recalling what we ate for breakfast, the name of Canada’s capital,
a grammatical rule, etc. Procedural memory, on the other hand, is the
memory involved with skills, such as the ability to swim, to drive a car, to
speak, to communicate or interact with language, etc. (see also Contreras
Asturias 2016).
Any explicit or conscious knowledge is sustained by declarative
memory; any implicit competence or unconscious skill is sustained by
procedural memory.
However, the main fact revealed by the empirical data provided by
scans of the human brain is the absence of any direct connection between
the two types of memory. According to Paradis, in language, as in any
other cognitive activity, there is no direct connection between declarative
memory and procedural memory.
This, at least, is what is revealed by the research Paradis discusses,
which looked at bilingual individuals suffering from Alzheimer’s disease
and bilingual individuals suffering from aphasia (difficulty in forming
sentences). In the first case, it is declarative memory that is affected, while
morphosyntax (which relates to procedural memory) is unaffected. In the
case of those suffering from aphasia, the reverse was essentially true:
morphosyntax was affected, while ability to use skills linked to declarative
memory was retained.
Brain scans of both the Alzheimer’s patients and the aphasia patients
reveal that completely different neuronal mechanisms are affected by their
respective conditions.
To illustrate how an aphasic person’s language troubles can manifest
themselves, here is the transcription of a video extract of a conversation
between an adult aphasic patient (A) and his therapist (T):3
T: Tell me what was this thing with your leg, last week or the week
before.

3
Source:https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=f2IiMEbMnPM&list=PLtUz1usdFE0
uU_PwD1WNsE0uO6NJIvOug
Five Lessons from the Neurosciences 5

A: eh… no good… eh… ache… and… eh… eh… eh… eh… [long
pauses]… knees… and ankles… eh…
T: What did you do about it?
A: eh… home… doctor… and… legs… eh… eh… walking… no good…
As can be seen, this aphasic person can only use vocabulary words
(verbs, nouns, adjectives), without any hope of forming statements that are
syntactically acceptable: no good, ache, knees, and… ankles, home,
doctor, and… legs, walking, no good… Brain scans of this aphasic patient
would show that it was the neuronal systems associated with his
procedural memory that were affected. His declarative memory, however,
remains intact.

An Anti-Chomskyan Position
This means that Paradis’s position (2004) is resolutely anti-Chomskyan.
According to Chomsky (1965), the brain includes a Language Acquisition
Device (LAD), that is, a specific mechanism for language acquisition, pre-
programmed by universal grammar and distinct from all other types of
cognitive activity.
Many neuroscientists, by contrast, Paradis among them, hold that
language works like any other human cognitive activity. It follows that
there is no LAD, no genetic mechanism containing a universal grammar.

Declarative Memory is not transformed


into Procedural Memory
However, though procedural memory and declarative memory call on
discrete neuronal systems, it does not follow that declarative knowledge
can transform itself into “procedural knowledge.” This is in fact the
subject of debate among cognitive psychologists. On one side, some (such
as Griggs, Carol and Bange 2002) believe that even if these two types of
memory call on discrete neuronal systems, there might nevertheless be a
transformative process allowing declarative knowledge to become
procedural knowledge. Those who hold this position do not hesitate to
speak of “knowledge proceduralisation” or know-how.
Paradis, on the other hand, contends that declarative knowledge cannot
transform itself into procedural knowledge and, on this basis, refuses to
speak of procedural knowledge or knowledge proceduralisation, which,
according to him, would be a contradiction in terms, or even an
impossibility. Rather, what happens is that metalinguistic knowledge is
acquired in parallel, by dint of hearing and using a language structure to
6 Chapter One

which one has paid attention (Paradis 2009). Paradis’s position is therefore
completely different from the preceding one.
For instance, in the case of a patient suffering from aphasia, one can
state that explicit knowledge is not transformed into an implicit
competence, that is to say a skill underlying spontaneous communication.
Otherwise, one might suppose that, if there existed a direct connection
between these two discrete neuronal systems and a transformation of the
knowledge associated with one into that associated with the other, this
would mean that what can be learnt can affect what is acquired, and vice
versa. It would be as if knowing the explicit rules of grammar would, ipso
facto, allow one to use them unconsciously. We know, however, that they
can only be used explicitly and in a controlled manner.

Indirect Influence of the Declarative on the Procedural


It should be noted that in the normal course of language use, the
declarative may indirectly influence the procedural. One has only to think
of what happens when, in an everyday conversation, we suddenly use the
wrong word. For instance, let us suppose I want to say, “Yesterday
evening, we went to the theatre,” but instead I say, “Yesterday evening, we
went to the cine–” interrupting myself in the middle of the word “cinema.”
In such a case, I might say, “Oops! I mean, to the theatre.” This “Oops”
can only come from what Krashen (1981) called a sort of monitor (or
piloting) in our brains that supervises, so to speak, everything we say.4
And given the extreme speed of one’s verbal flow, this sort of mistake is
quite understandable.
But in such a case, it must be agreed that conscious knowledge has an
indirect influence on the unconscious ability to communicate. This
underscores that when using language, recourse to conscious knowledge
presupposes the pre-existence of a skill.5 There is therefore no question of
a connection between an unconscious skill and conscious knowledge.
From this point of view, the dual originality of Paradis’s theory resides
in its demonstration that, firstly, there is no transformation of conscious
knowledge into unconscious skills and, secondly, that human language

4
This does not mean that the NLA is simply an application of Krashen’s theory.
To learn about the important distinction between the two approaches, see the next
chapter.
5
Moreover, as Paradis (2004) remarks, it is always possible for the implicit to be
made explicit (as in linguistic studies, for instance), but this never involves the
transformation of the implicit into the explicit.
Five Lessons from the Neurosciences 7

functions in the same way as any other non-linguistic human activity, such
as, for example, swimming.

Learning Cannot be Transformed into Acquisition


That which is conscious, Paradis specifies, cannot become something
unconscious, just as knowledge cannot become a skill, and that which is
explicit cannot become an implicit competence. These involve completely
discrete neuronal mechanisms, one of which cannot transform itself into
the other: (conscious) learning cannot transform itself into (unconscious)
acquisition, nor become an acquisition. The salient parts of Paradis’s
argument, which was first introduced in his A Neurolinguistic Theory of
Bilingualism (2004), were developed in more detail in his Declarative and
Procedural Determinants of Second Languages (2009).
In order to further distinguish the nature of these two memories, it is
important to note that neuroscience research strongly suggests there is as
great a difference between procedural memory and declarative memory as
there is between a process and a product.
If one is wondering what sort of process is involved in procedural
memory, it is, in a sense, a set of patterns or traces left in the brain by dint
of frequent use. Looked at this way, procedural memory consists of
regularities rather than rules. These regularities are like patterns or
“pathways,” rather like a path in a field created by passing numerous times
in more or less the same spot (as is so well shown in the great
Impressionist artist Monet’s painting reproduced below).
According to Paradis (2004), frequent use of verbal language is
required to develop implicit competence. “What serves as input for
implicit competency is the frequency with which particular linguistic
constructions are encountered, irrespective of their surface form” (Paradis
2009, 80). Nick Ellis (2011) also supports this point of view.6

6
Nick C. Ellis is a specialist in language acquisition and a professor of psychology
and linguistics at the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, United States. He has
served as publisher for the prestigious American journal Language Learning. (He
must not be confused with Rod Ellis, an internationally renowned British specialist
who is also recognised in the area of language acquisition.)
8 Chapter One

The Development of Implicit Competences:


A Process and Regularities

Fig. 1-1

Furthermore, when it comes to language, declarative memory seems to


be more of a product allowing the storage of vocabulary and verb
conjugations, as well as formal rules such as grammar rules.

In summary, the following can be learnt from Paradis’s


neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism:
Implicit competences, underpinned by procedural memory, and
explicit knowledge, underpinned by declarative memory, are two
discrete aspects of neuronal functioning;
There is no direct connection between the two;
Explicit knowledge cannot transform itself into an implicit
competence;
Declarative memory is like a product made up of facts (vocabulary
and verb conjugations) or rules (like grammar rules);
Procedural memory is like a process made up of regularities due to
the frequent use of language structures leaving traces or patterns in
the brain.
Five Lessons from the Neurosciences 9

Second Lesson: The Complexity/Flexibility


of the Brain and Two Neuronal Mechanisms: conscious
(vocabulary) and Unconscious (lexicon)
The brain is a very complex instrument. For instance, contrary to what was
once thought, there is not a language “centre.” The brain’s complexity can
be illustrated by examining certain brain scans. These reveal, as one would
expect, that visual information does not follow the same pathways as aural
information when reaching the brain. Clearly this implies discrete
neuronal connections, which means that when a word (for instance,
hammer) is heard, it does not activate the same neuronal circuits in the
brain as it does when read, spoken, or even thought—as brain scans reveal
(see Fig. 1-2). This explains why we cannot necessarily recognise a word
(such as table) in its written form, even if we are able to use it orally.
Likewise, even if we can read a word (like table), we cannot necessarily
spell it.

Words heard Words seen

Words produced Words thought

Fig. 1-2—Source: Damasio and Damasio 1997, 13

It should be mentioned that the brain is not merely complex, but also
highly flexible; it is neither frozen in time nor entirely genetically
determined. Current research indicates that the brain’s structure is
continually evolving as people learn and interact with their environment.
This is “one of the most fundamental discoveries of modern neuroscience,”
10 Chapter One

according to Masson (2015a).7 For instance, thanks to magnetic resonance


imaging (MRI) techniques, empirical research has demonstrated that as
learners learn to read, their cerebral activity gradually shifts from the front
of the brain, the seat of attention, concentration and short-term memory (in
the prefrontal cortex8), to the top and then the back of the brain (reaching
areas that specialise in such learning), due to the automatisms being
created: “This has the effect of freeing up the prefrontal cortex, and thus
diminishing cognitive overload,” Masson explains (2015a).

Conscious Mechanism (Vocabulary) and


Unconscious Mechanism (Lexicon)
Another aspect of Paradis’s distinction between cerebral mechanisms is
that drawn between vocabulary and lexicon. For Paradis, vocabulary is
made up of all the explicit correspondences between meaning and sound
(or written word), as found in the dictionary (2004, 247). A speaker’s
conscious knowledge of a word’s meaning-sound association can be
demonstrated by their ability to point to a house when asked to show a
house. This therefore represents a piece of conscious knowledge, sustained
by declarative memory. The object pointed to is the word’s referent.
By lexicon, Paradis means all of the correspondences between meaning
and implicit grammatical properties, as much on the phonological level as
the morphological or the syntactic. For instance, in some languages,
gender—whether masculine, feminine, or neuter—is one of a noun’s
grammatical properties. Used in a sentence, a word constitutes a lexical
element endowed with its own implicit grammatical properties. This
distinction is equally valid for verbs: whether a verb is transitive or
intransitive is part of its implicit grammatical properties.
Note that Paradis’s distinction is fully in line with a recent trend in
cognitive linguistics dedicated to construction grammar, which now
insists on the syntax-lexicon continuum rather than their separation; this is
why one sometimes sees discussions of word syntax.9

7
Steve Masson, founder of the Association pour la recherche en neuroéducation/
Association for Research in Neuroeducation (ARN), which publishes the online
scientific journal Neuroeducation, is a professor in the Département de didactique/
Education Department of the Université du Québec à Montréal (UQAM).
8
See Fig. 1-3 below.
9
To learn more about this trend in cognitive linguistics, see Hoffman and
Trousdale (2013). For a synthesis in French of this current, see Mathieu (2003);
more recent sources include Legallois and François (2006) and François (2008).
Five Lessons from the Neurosciences 11

In summary, the neurosciences teach us that:


the brain is highly complex: neuronal connections are discrete,
according to the various sensory organs involved (sight, speech,
etc.);
the brain is highly flexible; it is not static;
according to Paradis, it is important to distinguish a conscious
mechanism (vocabulary) from an unconscious one (lexicon).

Third Lesson: Focusing on the Meaning or on the Task


to Develop Implicit Competence
To ensure that implicit competence is developed, Paradis explains, it is
necessary to turn one’s attention away from the language form by
concentrating either on the meaning of the messages formulated when
accomplishing a task or concentrating on the task to be accomplished.
What is conscious does not transform itself into something unconscious.
Concentrating on language forms (something conscious) means the
unconscious has little chance of being developed. Hence the importance of
concentrating either on the meaning of the formulated messages or on the
task to be accomplished: “attention can then not focus on all relevant
parameters (phonology, morphosyntax, the lexicon) at the same time. If
attention is selectively concentrated on one of these aspects, the others
must necessarily wait” (Paradis 1994, 400).

In summary, a third lesson to be drawn from Paradis’s linguistic


theory is that
to develop implicit competence, one must concentrate on something
other than the language form, thus either on the meaning of a
message formulated while accomplishing a task or on the task
itself.

Fourth Lesson: The Importance of “Transfer Appropriate


Processing” (TAP) and the Limbic System’s Role
A further neuroscientific dimension, especially in cognitive psychology, is
called “Transfer Appropriate Processing” (TAP). What does this refer to?
According to Segalowitz (2010), the brain records data with all their
context, whether this is a linguistic context such as a sentence or an
extralinguistic context such as gestures, mimicry, etc. In other words, the
more the context in which a language is used resembles the context in
12 Chapter One

which it was learnt, the greater the odds that the user’s communication
abilities will rise to an adequate level. All the evidence suggests that this is
why it is easier to later locate the data in the brain, by referring to the
contextual similarities.

Fig. 1-3—Source: Geschwind and Levitsky, 1968

Moreover, as Paradis notes, neuroscientific research shows that the


desire to communicate—in other words, the starting point for any verbal
communication—is ruled by the limbic system. Without the desire to
communicate, or pleasure in doing so, there is an absence of motivation.
Thus, the usual approach to learning an L2/FL—one centred on
knowledge, verb conjugations, and grammatical rules—provides
practically no emotional reaction related to pleasure or to the desire to
communicate, and the limbic system is not activated. Conversely, when
what is communicated is a real, authentic message, the limbic system is
much more likely to be activated, increasing the motivation to
communicate. It is this motivation that allows the limbic system to be
activated.

In summary, a fourth lesson the neurosciences teach us is that:


to develop implicit competence, learning contexts must be similar
to the usage contexts, both orally and in writing (TAP);
activating the limbic system requires a desire to communicate.
Five Lessons from the Neurosciences 13

Fifth Lesson: Intake and Individual Cognitive


Benefits of Social Interaction
The term interactions refers to reciprocal actions between two or more
learners during which there is a simple exchange of information. The
interactions are qualified as social if they take place in a context where
one individual’s actions can influence the actions of another (as is the case
in discussions about completing a project).
Establishing social interaction as a key principle of the NLA is doubly
warranted, firstly by Vygotsky’s model of the links between social
interaction and individual development ([1934] 1985), and secondly by
Paradis (2004, 2009) and Nick C. Ellis (2005) and their ideas on the role
of social interaction.

The Individual Cognitive Benefits of Social Interaction


As is well known, the great Swiss psychologist Piaget showed, in the
context of his genetic psychology, that a child’s cognitive development
involves successive stages, from the concrete operational to the
hypothetico-deductive, with various other stages in between ([1963]
1980). This is common knowledge.
What is less well known, however, is what explains the transition from
one stage to the next: might it simply be an internal development process,
not involving any external influence? In other words, according to Piaget,
there would be little effect by social interaction on the cognitive
development of an individual. Social development and individual
development would evolve in parallel, without influencing each other.
According to Vygotsky
([1934] 1985), however,
this is not the case: social
interaction, in fact, is
what ensures progress
from one cognitive stage
to the next. Thus, there
would seem to be a close
link between what happens
on the level of social
interactions and what
happens in the brain of the
individual. The direction
Fig. 1-4 of the relationship between
14 Chapter One

the social and the individual would go from the social to the individual.
There is even a causal relationship between social interaction and
individual development.
On discovering this audacious theory of Vygotsky’s, we adopted it, for
it allowed us to explain how acquiring L2/FL in a school setting could
have not only utilitarian aims, but also humanist ones—and could thereby
provide the student with cognitive benefits.

The Role of Interaction: From Input to Intake


Another argument in favour of relying on considerable social interaction
emerges from the distinction, recognised by most psycholinguists,
between input, i.e. an interlocutor’s linguistic contribution, and intake
(what you take in), i.e. what the listener actually retains and understands
from the overall input data uttered by the speaker. In the classroom, the
input is the same for all learners (that is, everything the teacher says), but
intake varies from individual to individual.

Fig. 1-5

As van Lier suggests (1988), it is probably social interaction that leads


to increases in intake quantity and improvements in quality, because that is
what mediates between input (the language introduced) and intake (the
language retained). This allows us to speak of cognitive interaction
between input and intake.
Five Lessons from the Neurosciences 15

To get a better grasp of the above, think of a language teacher giving a


lecture on a grammatical rule to students who just need to listen and take a
few notes. Now imagine a language teacher who, after giving an example
of the rule being applied in a written text, asks the students to form small
groups and find other instances in the text of a similar construction to the
case being studied, and, through induction, derive the rule and its
applications. Despite the voluminous language input from the teacher, it is
probable that intake would be much smaller in the first instance (lecturing
and using a deductive approach) than the second (demonstrating a
practical application and using inductive reasoning).

In summary, a fifth lesson from the neurosciences is that


numerous social interactions are important for improving intake
quantity and quality.

These, then, are the five lessons drawn from our explorations in
neurosciences and which form the NLA’s theoretical foundations:
The absence of a direct connection between declarative memory
and procedural memory;
The complexity/flexibility of the brain and two neuronal
mechanisms: the conscious (vocabulary) and unconscious
(lexicon);
Focusing on the meaning or the task in order to develop implicit
competence;
The importance of “Transfer Appropriate Processing” (TAP) and
the limbic system’s role;
Intake and the individual cognitive benefits of social interaction.

It is from these neuroscientific lessons that the NLA gradually


developed around five fundamental principles, discussed in the next
chapter.
CHAPTER TWO

THE NLA’S FIVE FUNDAMENTAL PRINCIPLES

This chapter presents and explains the NLA’s five fundamental principles,
highlighting each one’s links to the theoretical bases drawn from the
neurosciences, particularly Paradis’s neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism
(2004). This is followed by a table summarising the links between the
principles and their neuroscientific origins, as well as, more specifically,
the links between Paradis’s theory and the NLA. Finally, we consider
neuromyths.
Beginning in the late 1990s, my colleague Joan Netten and I eventually
formulated the following five fundamental principles governing the NLA:
1. Two grammars, internal and external;
2. Literacy and the pedagogy of the sentence;
3. Emphasising meaning and using a project-based pedagogy;
4. Authenticity;
5. Social interaction.

First Principle: Distinguishing between


Two Grammars, Internal (Implicit Competence)
and External (Explicit Knowledge)1
Our approach places very strong emphasis not on ONE single grammar, as
is usually done, but on TWO grammars that must be acquired/learnt: an
implicit, unconscious grammar (for verbal communication), which we call
“internal grammar,” and an explicit, conscious grammar (for written
language), which we call “external grammar.” Given that the internal
grammar is a skill that can only call on unconscious, implicit processes, it
is sustained by procedural memory. External grammar, meanwhile,
consists of a collection of conscious, explicit knowledge and is therefore
sustained by declarative memory. In other words, internal grammar is
acquired but external grammar is learnt.

1
Though the idea of acquiring/learning “two grammars” is ours rather than
Paradis’s, it stems from his theory.

You might also like