Scale For Time and Space Experience in Anxiety (STEA) Phenomenology and Its Clinical Relevance

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 37

Journal Pre-proof

Scale for time and space experience in anxiety (STEA):


Phenomenology and its clinical relevance

Cheng-Ju Lu, Josh Goheen, Angelika Wolman, Lorenzo Lucherini


Angeletti, Filipe Arantes-Gonçalves, Dusan Hirjak, Annemarie
Wolff, Georg Northoff

PII: S0165-0327(24)00710-9
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2024.04.099
Reference: JAD 17531

To appear in:

Received date: 17 January 2024


Revised date: 5 April 2024
Accepted date: 29 April 2024

Please cite this article as: C.-J. Lu, J. Goheen, A. Wolman, et al., Scale for time and
space experience in anxiety (STEA): Phenomenology and its clinical relevance, (2023),
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2024.04.099

This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such
as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is
not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting,
typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this
version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production
process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers
that apply to the journal pertain.

© 2024 Published by Elsevier B.V.


Journal Pre-proof

Scale for time and space experience in anxiety (STEA):


Phenomenology and its clinical relevance

Cheng-Ju Lu a, Josh Goheen b, Angelika Wolman b, Lorenzo Lucherini Angeletti b,


Filipe Arantes -Gonçalves c, Dusan Hirjak d, Annemarie Wolff b, Georg Northoff b

a
School of Medicine, College of Medicine, National Yang Ming Chiao Tung
University.
b
Institute of Mental Health Research, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada
c
Department of Psychiatry, Clínica Psicodinâmica, Lisbon, Portugal
d
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health,

of
Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany

ro
Correspondence to: G. Northoff, Institute of Mental Health Research, University of
Ottawa, Ottawa, ON K1Z 7K4, Canada. -p
[email protected]
re
Abstract
Anxiety is a pervasive emotional state where, phenomenologically, subjects often report
lP

changes in their experience of time and space. However, a systematic and quantified
examination of time and space experience in terms of a self-report scale is still missing
na

which eventually could also be used for clinical differential diagnosis. Based on
historical phenomenological literature and patients’ subjective reports, we here
introduce, in a first step, the Scale for Time and Space Experience of Anxiety (STEA)
ur

in a smaller sample of 19 subjects with anxiety disorders and, in a second step, validate
its shorter clinical version (cSTEA) in a larger sample of 48 anxiety subjects. The main
Jo

findings are (i) high convergent and divergent validity of STEA with both Beck Anxiety
Inventory (BAI) (r = 0.7325; p < 0.001) and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (r =
0.7749; p < 0.0001), as well as with spontaneous mind wandering (MWS) (r = 0.7343;
p < 0.001) and deliberate mind wandering (MWD) (r = 0.1152; p > 0.05), (ii) statistical
feature selection shows 8 key items for future clinical usage (cSTEA) focusing on the
experience of temporal and spatial constriction, (iii) the effects of time and space
experience (i.e., for both STEA and cSTEA scores) on the level of anxiety (BAI) are
mediated by the degree of spontaneous mind wandering (MWS), (iv) cSTEA allows for
differentiating high levels of anxiety from the severity of comorbid depressive
symptoms, (v) significant reduction in the cSTEA scores after a therapeutic intervention
(breathing therapy). Together, our study introduces a novel fully quantified and highly
valid self-report instrument, the STEA, for measuring time-space experiences in anxiety.
Journal Pre-proof

Further we develop a shorter clinical version (cSTEA) which allows assessing time
space experience in a valid, quick, and simple way for diagnosis, differential diagnosis,
and therapeutic monitoring of anxiety.

Keywords: Anxiety, Time and space experience in anxiety, Phenomenology of


anxiety, Clinical differential diagnosis, Depression, Mind wandering

1. Introduction

Anxiety is a pervasive emotional state which is manifest as a transdiagnostic feature


across various psychiatric disorders, entangling psychological, physiological, and

of
behavioral components to shape the overall anxious experience. To substantiate the
impact of psychological and physiological hallmark characteristics of anxiety on

ro
behavioral correlates, recent clinical and phenomenological studies focused on the
subjective experience of anxiety. Some authors, employing the Zimbardo Time
-p
Perspective Scale (ZTPS) (Zimbardo and Boyd, 1999), have displayed alterations in
subjects suffering from anxiety in their time perspective with respect to the experience
re
of present, past and future (Anagnostopoulos and Griva, 2011; Åström et al., 2014;
Griffin and Wildbur, 2020; Kooij et al., 2018; Zimbardo and Boyd, 1999).
lP

These alterations are reflected in phenomenological reports on the subjective


na

experience of anxiety such as "quickening subjective time" (Aho, 2020), "weakening


perception of the past and the future" (Doerr-Zegers, 2016; Minkowski et al., 1970),
"engaging in fatalistic thinking" (Doerr-Zegers, 2016; Minkowski et al., 1970), and
ur

"heightened perception of external non-psychological objects with specific temporal


structures" (Berenskötter, 2020; Reuther, 2014). Further, individuals with pathological
Jo

anxiety also report abnormal experience of space in terms of a perceived shrinking of


the lived-space (i.e., the totality of space that an individual reflectively lives and
experiences) (Bollnow, 1961; Fuchs, 2007; Woodgate et al., 2021), as manifest in an
abnormal peripersonal space/PPS, i.e., the space surrounding the body that all the
physical interactions with the objects occur, Rabellino et al. (2020)) and other
individuals (personal space/PS, Chiara and Gian Domenico (2013)).

Despite the strong evidence for alterations in the experience of time and space in
anxious subjects, a valid quantified self-report questionnaire is still lacking in the
current literature. Therefore, the first aim of this study is to address this gap in the
current literature. For that purpose, we developed a series of questions for self-reporting
of subjective experience of time and space in anxiety and exploratorily probed them in
Journal Pre-proof

a smaller sample of patients with anxiety disorders, undergoing slow breathing therapy.
We coined our scale “Scale for Time and Space Experience of Anxiety” (STEA).
Considering that anxiety frequently co-occurs with depression (Brown et al., 2001;
Lamers et al., 2011; Olfson et al., 1997), we validated the STEA by including standard
self-report instruments like Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) (Beck et al., 1988) and Beck
Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al., 1961). Thus, validating the STEA was our
second specific aim in this exploratory study. As third aim of the study, we investigated
whether the STEA score would change during therapeutic intervention by breathing.

Patients with anxiety disorders commonly experience an escalation in particularly


negative thoughts (Spinhoven et al., 2015; Watkins, 2008) and exhibit concerns

of
regarding future events (Borkovec et al., 1998). At the same time, they are strongly
distracted from their external environmental context (Bar-Haim et al., 2007; Eysenck,

ro
1988; Mathews and MacLeod, 2005; Najmi et al., 2012). This suggests close
relationship of anxiety with subjects’ thoughts like in mind wandering also known as
-p
"daydreaming", reflecting the "the dynamic nature of thought" (Singer, 1966).
re
Recent studies classified mind wandering into two types: spontaneous
(unintentional) mind wandering and deliberate (intentional) mind wandering (Carriere
lP

et al., 2013; Christoff et al., 2016) with especially the former being closely related to
anxiety (Christoff et al., 2016; Seli et al., 2019). The exact relationship of spontaneous
na

mind wandering with anxiety, including its abnormal time and space experience,
remains unclear, though. Addressing this is the fourth specific aim of our study. For that
purpose, we, in a fourth aim, also included two scales for probing mind wandering:
ur

spontaneous scale (MWS) and mind wandering: deliberate scale (MWD) from (Carriere
et al., 2013) in order to relate that, by using mediation models, to the STEA, the BAI,
Jo

and the BDI. Finally, a fifth aim of our study consisted in using statistical feature
selection to test for a shorter version of the STEA, e.g., clinical or cSTEA, in a larger
sample to make it more feasible for clinical application. Presupposing the framework
of Spatiotemporal psychopathology(Northoff et al., 2023a) (we ultimately intend to use
the cSTEA as screening instrument for clinical diagnosis of anxiety including its
differential diagnosis from the often comorbid depression and schizophrenia.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

To validate STEA, 19 subjects with 13 females and 6 males are included in this
Journal Pre-proof

exploratory study. As for the validation of the clinical version of STEA, i.e., cSTEA,
48 subjects with 29 females and 19 males are included. All the subjects are
participants from an ongoing study of breathing therapy for anxiety. The therapy
involved the participants performing slow breathing protocols (~ 6 breaths per minute)
twice daily for six weeks. Recent literature has shown similar breathing therapy reduce
levels of anxiety (Balban et al., 2023; Gitler et al., 2022; Sevoz-Couche and Laborde,
2022; Wang et al., 2020) while reducing the degree of mind wandering (Blum et al.,
2019; Nashiro et al., 2022). Among the subjects included in the study for the validation
of STEA, 13 of them were clinically diagnosed as anxiety disorder (GAD, SAD, PD)
and served as main group in the ongoing study. Among the subjects included in the
study for the validation of the cSTEA, 31 of them were clinically diagnosed as anxiety

of
disorder and served as main group in the ongoing study. These subjects showed a wide
range of anxiety from moderate to high levels as shown in the BAI scores. Almost all

ro
subjects except one were on additional pharmacological therapy mostly serotoninergic
drugs while several subjects also received cognitive behavioral therapy. The remaining
-p
subjects were included as healthy control group in the ongoing trial. The total group of
subjects for the validation of STEA ranged in age from 19 to 68 years old (M = 41.47;
re
Me = 40; SD = 13.65). Distributions of age in males (M = 43.50; Me = 41.5; SD = 9.89)
and females (M = 40.54; Me = 38; SD = 15.35) show no significant difference by
lP

student t-test (t = -0.4295; p = 0.6730). As for the subjects for the validation of cSTEA,
the total group range in age from 17 to 77 years old (M = 38.33; Me = 38; SD = 15.41).
na

Distribution of age in males (M = 36.14; Me = 34; SD = 15.03) and females (M = 41.68;


Me = 40; SD = 15.78) show no significant difference by student t-test (t = -1.2260; p =
0.2265).
ur

2.2. Scale construction


Jo

The question set for the scale is constructed according to previous studies about the
alteration of time (Aho, 2020; Anagnostopoulos and Griva, 2011; Åström et al., 2014;
Berenskötter, 2020; Doerr-Zegers, 2016; Griffin and Wildbur, 2020; Kooij et al., 2018;
Minkowski et al., 1970; Reuther, 2014; Rinaldi et al., 2017; Zimbardo and Boyd, 1999)
and space (Rabellino et al., 2020; Woodgate et al., 2021) experience in anxiety disorders.
Furthermore, the prevalent co-occurrence of anxiety disorders and depression reveals
overlapping characteristics in the experience of time within both conditions
(Anagnostopoulos and Griva, 2011; Fuchs, 2005, 2013; Griffin and Wildbur, 2020;
Kooij et al., 2018; Minkowski et al., 1970; Rinaldi et al., 2017; Thönes and Oberfeld,
2015; van Beek et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2021; Zimbardo and Boyd, 1999). Similarly,
we also included questions about space experience in our scale which can also be
Journal Pre-proof

observed in depression (Barrett et al., 2016; Coll-Florit et al., 2021; Hou et al., 2021;
Kunzendorf et al., 2011; Riskind et al., 2014; Sass and Pienkos, 2013) for exploratory
investigation. In addition to referring to existing literatures, we also sought expert
opinion from one psychiatrist (GN) working in the field of anxiety research. 27 items
for time experience and 17 items for space experience are formulated (See Table. 1 and
Table. 2). The listed items are included in the final survey in a random manner in order
to minimize the “order effect.” The items were rated in a visual analog scale format (0
– 10, with incremental units of 1) (see Appendix 1).

Table 1. Construction and description of time experience items


Items Description Reference

of
I think the current Based on the assumption that (Gu et al., 2020; Lake and
situation is quite perception of uncertainty is Labar, 2011; Penrod,

ro
uncertain involved in perception of 2001)
I think events temporal dynamics, we further
-p
occurring right diversify the interpretation of
now are perception of uncertainty by
re
predictable elucidating the difference in
I think what will temporal categories – present,
lP

happen in the future, and transition.


future is quite
na

uncertain.
I can tolerate
unpredictable
ur

future events.
I am uneasy about
Jo

the changes
I can handle the
alteration of
current
situation.
I think time passes Perception of the speed of (Aho, 2020; Fuchs, 2013;
fast passage of time. The Mioni et al., 2023;
I feel that my thought interpretations are diversified Pollatos et al., 2014;
or behaviour based on the concepts of Thönes and Oberfeld,
are accelerated interoception and 2015; Van Hedger et al.,
I feel that everything exteroception. 2020; Wittmann, 2022;
surrounding me Zhang et al., 2018)
Journal Pre-proof

is fast
I think that time
passes slowly
I feel that everything
near to me
slows down
It is hard for me to
catch on other’s
tempo
I think there is gap Sometimes being interpreted (Fuchs, 2005)
between present as “trapped in present,” it

of
and future. suggests the inability to
I think there is gap normally perceive past and

ro
between present future in anxiety. In other way
and past. of interpretation, it involves
-p
Comparing with the discontinuity of time
future and past, perception on the temporal
re
present is the dimension from present
only thing toward past and future.
lP

concrete to me
I think everything The fatalistic thinking is also (Anagnostopoulos and
na

about me is suggested to be a feature in Griva, 2011; Åström et al.,


unchangeable individuals with anxiety. We 2014; Doerr-Zegers, 2016;
Everything, including hold that fatalistic thinking or Griffin and Wildbur,
ur

the future, is out attitude are also related to time 2020; Minkowski et al.,
of control. perception to lifetime, 1970; van Beek et al.,
Jo

My life is involving the perception of 2011; Zimbardo and


determined. past, present, and future. Boyd, 1999)
I often perceive Unbalanced time perspectives (Anagnostopoulos and
something bad in past-negative orientation Griva, 2011; Åström et al.,
happened before and future orientation are 2014; Griffin and
to be far away observed in individuals with Wildbur, 2020; Kooij et
from me. anxiety. Based on this, we al., 2018; Rinaldi et al.,
I feel that the further diversify the 2017; van Beek et al.,
negative events interpretations by terms of 2011; Wang et al., 2021;
in the past have perception of past, present, and Zimbardo and Boyd,
influenced me a future. 1999)
lot.
Journal Pre-proof

Myself right now has


long distance to
myself in the
past.
To me, the future is
distant.
I often think about
the future.
It seems that myself
in the future will
not be the same

of
as myself right
now.

ro
I am often Under phenomenology and the (Berenskötter, 2020;
consciously concept of attachment theory, Reuther, 2014)
-p
aware my the sense of being in every
heartbeat moment is acquired implicitly
re
I often focus on through the engagement in
things with a environment. We hold that this
lP

monotonous also serves for time perception


rhythm and may alter in anxiety. To
na

I often do behaviors construct the items, we further


with a certain specified the interpretation
speed into certain interoceptive or
ur

exteroceptive events.
Table 2. Construction and description of space experience items
Jo

Items Description Reference


I have the feeling that Loss of depth perception. Or it can (Hou et al.,
objects become also be comprehended as a form of 2021;
flatter. constriction of space perception. We Kunzendorf et
I have the feeling that diversify the interpretation with terms al., 2011)
my self becomes of object (exteroceptive), myself
flatter. (interoceptive), and others (social).
I have the feeling that
others become
flatter.
I think the space around In addition to constriction of space (Coll-Florit et
me is enclosed or perception, these items further capture al., 2021; Sass
Journal Pre-proof

bounded. the boundness of space perception and Pienkos,


I think I am confined. between individual and surroundings. 2013)
I think that other things
are out of reach or
distant.
I think the distance
between me and
external objects or
other people is
long.
I often perceive objects In addition to the constriction and the (Barrett et al.,

of
or other people boundness of space perception, these 2016; Hohwy,
approaching me items further capture the synchrony of 2016; Riskind

ro
lately or, saying, space perception, i.e., the ability to et al., 2014)
not quickly. engage with surroundings.
-p
My perception to the
surroundings is
re
blurred or dull.
I have the sense of
lP

separation or
seclusion from the
na

surroundings.
I think I am locked-in
from the external
ur

world.
I have the feeling that I Peripersonal space (PPS) refers to the (Rabellino et
Jo

perceive more space perception of reachability from al., 2020)


things, including self toward surroundings or from
hearing things, surroundings toward self.
seeing things, etc.
I can quickly perceive
what is
approaching me.
The world is getting Under phenomenology and ecological (Fuchs, 2007;
more enclosed and psychology, lived-space is a spatial Woodgate et
surrounding me. and social relationship between al., 2021)
I have the feeling that I individual and surroundings. It
am getting closer to involves the concept of responsivity
Journal Pre-proof

the edge or end of and exchange with the surrounding.


the world or events. The deformation of lived-space may
occur in certain mental illness.
I tend to avoid going to Under attachment theory, the sense of (Rabellino et
spaces that is being is acquired implicitly through al., 2020)
unfamiliar to me. the engagement in environment. This
I tend to stay in spaces may alter in mental illness.
that is familiar to
me.
2.3. Assessment of anxiety, depression, and mind-wandering

of
The Beck anxiety inventory (BAI) (Beck et al., 1988), Beck depression inventory (BDI)
(Beck et al., 1961), mind wandering: spontaneous mind wandering scale (MWS)

ro
(Carriere et al., 2013), and mind wandering: deliberate mind wandering scale (MWD)
(Carriere et al., 2013) were included to assess, respectively, the degrees of anxiety,
-p
depression, spontaneous mind wandering, and deliberate mind wandering of the
participants. These instruments were assessed before breathing therapy as well as after
re
three weeks of ongoing breathing therapy. Finally, at the timepoint when this study
ended, 1 of the subjects was assessed 6 times of the measurements. 1 of them was
lP

assessed 5 times. 1 of them was assessed 4 times. 6 of them were assessed 3 times. 1 of
them was assessed 2 times. 9 of them were assessed 1 time. Thus, 44 data points in total
na

were assessed in this study. Moreover, 9 of the subjects completed the breathing therapy
before the end of this study. The measurements from these subjects were further used
for pre- and post-therapeutic analysis.
ur

2.4. Statistical analysis


Jo

2.4.1. Assessment of Reliability and Validity


We involved Cronbach’s 𝛼 to assess the reliability of STEA and its time experience
subscale (T-STEA) and space experience subscale (S-STEA). We also conduct the
analysis for assessing validity of STEA. Two kinds of validity, i.e., convergent validity
and divergent validity, are assessed in this study. Convergent validity refers to how
closely an instrument is correlated with other instruments with similar construct.
Divergent validity indicates that the measurement of the instrument doesn’t correlate
too strong with other instruments measuring similar traits but with distinct constructs.
For the evaluation of convergent and divergent validity of STEA, we involved
Pearson’s correlation coefficient for the validation, measuring the correlation of STEA
with BAI and BDI as well as with both MWD and MWS. Furthermore, to probe the
Journal Pre-proof

sensitivity of STEA to change, we conducted Paired T-test to assess the significance of


score changes of STEA, T-STEA, S-STEA, BAI, and BDI before and after three weeks
of breathing therapy.

2.4.2. Mediation Analysis and Structural equation modeling


In the mediation analysis and structural equation modeling, data were managed and
analyzed using a Python package – semopy (Igolkina and Meshcheryakov, 2020). Since
our data sample is small, we also included unweighted least squares (ULS) whose
efficacy has been validated (Tenenhaus, 2008). Furthermore, to assess the significance
of the models, Sobel test (Sobel, 1982) was used to evaluate the significance of the
mediation pathway. Also, for the validation of interaction models in SEM analysis,

of
several variables including CFI, GFI, NFI, TLI, and RMSEA, are used to indicate the
quality of the proposed model.

ro
2.4.3. Feature Selection and the Validation of Short version of STEA for clinical
use, the cSTEA.
-p
In order to reduce the number of items for developing a short version of the STEA for
re
clinical use, we used feature selection, a process of selecting most relevant
features/variables for construction of model, for this purpose. The data were managed
lP

and analyzed using a machine learning package in Python – Scikit-Learn (Pedregosa et


al., 2012) – and the Lasso method (Tibshirani, 1996) was involved for feature selection.
na

The constant 𝛼 , which multiplies the L1 term, the sum of values of parameter
coefficients in the model, and controls the strength of regularization, was set to be 1 in
our analysis. For the reference data, BAI scores were taken as the predicting values for
ur

the model fitting.


Jo

To assess the validity of the short version of the STEA, we calculated Spearman’s
correlation coefficient for the ordinal correlation analysis of ranked grouping data.
Furthermore, Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve, a plot that depicts the
diagnostic ability of a classifier in varied thresholds, and calculation of area under the
curve (AUC) were also used to assess the degree of separability of diagnostic positive
examples and diagnostic negative examples in the short STEA version (see Appendix
2).

3. Results

3.1. Scale reliability and convergent and divergent validity


Journal Pre-proof

We first focused on testing the reliability as well as validity of the STEA which is based
on the subjects’ data before therapy of all 19 participants (n = 19, including both anxious
and depressed subjects). Subsequently, the supplementary evaluation encompasses all
data points in the time series of all participants (44 data points) (See supplementary
material). We first analyzed the reliability of STEA by including Cronbach’s 𝛼 as our
index of reliability. All three Total STEA ( 𝛼 = 0.9497 ), STEA Time Experience
Subscale (T-STEA) (𝛼 = 0.8930), and STEA Spatial Experience Subscale (S-STEA)
(𝛼 = 0.9510) showed good reliability according to the index.

To test for validity, we correlated the STEA (also with T-STEA and S-STEA) with
established scales like BAI, BDI, MWD and MWS (Table 3). Overall, the significant

of
correlations of STEA and its subscales with BAI and BDI indicate good levels of
convergent validity. This is further supported by its significant relationship with

ro
spontaneous mind wandering which, in contrast, was not observed with deliberate mind
wandering. Albeit tentatively, the latter finding of non-significant relationship with
-p
deliberate mind wandering suggests first evidence for divergent validity of STEA.
Moreover, these results validity applied to the total STEA as well as to its two subscales,
re
time and space. Together, this suggests good convergent and, albeit tentatively,
divergent validity of the STEA.
lP

Table 3. Correlation analysis for the validity of STEA


STEA T-STEA S-STEA BAI BDI MWD MW
na

S
STEA - - - - - - -
ur

T- 0.9633*** - - - - - -
STEA *
Jo

S- 0.9517*** 0.8343*** - - - - -
STEA * *
BAI 0.7325*** 0.7402*** 0.6575** - - - -
BDI 0.7749*** 0.8248*** 0.6478** 0.8641*** - - -
* * *
MWD 0.1152† 0.1677† 0.0446† 0.0906† 0.1046 - -

MWS 0.7343*** 0.6641** 0.7484** 0.5860** 0.5372 0.3464 -
* * †
† : insignificant ; * : p < 0.05 ; ** : p < 0.01 : *** : p < 0.001 : **** : p < 0.0001
3.2. Relationship of STEA with BAI and MWS
Journal Pre-proof

To investigate the relationship of STEA with BAI and MWS, we utilized mediation-
structural equation modeling to test our hypothesis that MWS would serve as mediator
between STEA and BAI. The alternative model to our hypothesis includes spontaneous
mind wandering as independent variable, time or space experience as mediator, and
anxiety as dependent variable.

Our findings show that, spontaneous mind wandering significantly mediates the effects
of both time experience ( 𝑎 = 1.522; 𝑏 = 1.035; 𝑆𝐸𝑎 = 0.376; 𝑆𝐸𝑏 =
0.386; 𝑆𝑜𝑏𝑒𝑙 𝑡𝑒𝑠𝑡 𝑠𝑡𝑎𝑡𝑖𝑠𝑡𝑖𝑐 = 2.234; 𝑝 = 0.025 ) (Fig. 1(A)) and space experience
( 𝑎 = 2.226; 𝑏 = 1.269; 𝑆𝐸𝑎 = 0.696; 𝑆𝐸𝑏 = 0.414; 𝑆𝑜𝑏𝑒𝑙 𝑡𝑒𝑠𝑡 𝑠𝑡𝑎𝑡𝑖𝑠𝑡𝑖𝑐 =
2.212; 𝑝 = 0.027) (Fig. 1(B)) on the severity of anxiety symptoms (BAI). The power

of
of the tests is assured (> 0.8) as our sample size (44 data points) is larger than the
suggested minimum sample size (nmin = 42 data points) according to (Fritz and

ro
Mackinnon, 2007). In contrast, the alternative model that considers time and space
experience as mediators for the effect of spontaneous mind wandering on anxiety did
-p
not show statistical significance (see supplementary material Fig. S1(A) and (B) for
alternative mediation model of time and space experience respectively).
re
lP
na
ur
Jo

Fig. 1. Mediation models show that time (A) and space (B) experience (STEA) affect
the severity of anxiety symptoms (BAI) as mediated by spontaneous mind wandering
(MWS). The variable “time experience” is measured by the 27 temporal items in the
STEA scale. The variable “space experience” is measured by the 17 spatial items in
the STEA scale. As for other variables, “spontaneous mind wandering” is measured
Journal Pre-proof

by the 4 items in MWS. The variable “anxiety” is measured by the 21 items in BAI
scale. (About the setup of the modeling and the mediation analysis, please see 2.4.2.
Mediation Analysis and Structural equation modeling)The alternative model with
time and space experience mediating the effects of spontaneous mind wandering on
anxiety did not yield any significance (see Supplementary material).

3.3. Feature Selection for short clinical version of STEA, the cSTEA

To extract those items most strongly associated with anxiety and to create a shorter
scale for clinical use, we employed Lasso regression as our feature selection method,
with the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) as the reference for feature selection (see Fig.

of
2 for the performance of Lasso model). Our analysis resulted in an 8-item scale,
comprising six items of time experience and two items of space experience, which all

ro
exhibited a high level of association with anxiety (see Table. 4) (see also Appendix 2).
This is further supported by replicating the mediation model findings for the shorter
-p
eight-item-version of the STEA, the cSTEA (with c standing for clinical), as described
above for the total STEA (See supplementary material Fig. S2).
re
lP
na
ur
Jo

Fig 2. Performance of Lasso model in predicting BAI scores from cSTEA scores.

Table 4. Items of cSTEA


Items Positive/Negative
I am uneasy about the changes. Positive
I often consciously aware my heartbeat. Positive
I can tolerate unpredictable future events. Negative
Comparing with the future and the past, the present is the only Negative
thing concrete to me.
My life is determined. Negative
Journal Pre-proof

My self right now has a long distance to my self in the past. Negative
I think the space around me is enclosed or bounded. Positive
I think I am locked-in from the external world. Positive
Caption for Table 4. The “positive/negative” stands for the positive or negative
correlation of the item with anxiety, i.e., the positive/negative items in cSTEA. These
are determined from the results of previous correlation analysis. In this case, the
calculation of the cSTEA score would be:
(Total of positive items scores) – (Total of negative items scores)
Under the 10-point scale, the possible cSTEA scores are ranging from -40 to 40.

of
3.4. Validation and mediation models of the cSTEA

ro
To validate our cSTEA scale, we repeated the the correlation analysis and the mediation
analysis in a larger group of subjects (See 2.1.Participants for more information).
-p
Measurement of BAI, BDI, MWS, and MWD were also included in this correlation
analysis. The results of the correlation analysis are shown in Table 5. The results of the
re
meditation analysis are shown in Fig 3. (B). Overall, the spontaneous mind wandering
significantly mediates the effects of time-space experience, which is measured by
lP

cSTEA, on the severity of anxiety ( 𝑎 = 0.264; 𝑏 = 0.665; 𝑆𝐸𝑎 = 0.0485; 𝑆𝐸𝑏 =


0.1972; 𝑆𝑜𝑏𝑒𝑙 𝑡𝑒𝑠𝑡 𝑠𝑡𝑎𝑡𝑖𝑠𝑡𝑖𝑐 = 2.8658; 𝑝 = 0.0042 ) and depression ( 𝑎 =
na

0.259; 𝑏 = 0.568; 𝑆𝐸𝑎 = 0.0487; 𝑆𝐸𝑏 = 0.1952; 𝑆𝑜𝑏𝑒𝑙 𝑡𝑒𝑠𝑡 𝑠𝑡𝑎𝑡𝑖𝑠𝑡𝑖𝑐 =


2.5544; 𝑝 = 0.0106), while deliberate mind wandering insignificantly mediates the
effects of time-space experience on anxiety 𝑎 = 0.110; 𝑏 = 0.409; 𝑆𝐸𝑎 =
ur

0.0592; 𝑆𝐸𝑏 = 0.1617; 𝑆𝑜𝑏𝑒𝑙 𝑡𝑒𝑠𝑡 𝑠𝑡𝑎𝑡𝑖𝑠𝑡𝑖𝑐 = 1.4990; 𝑝 = 0.1339 ) and depression
( 𝑎 = 0.106; 𝑏 = 0.647; 𝑆𝐸𝑎 = 0.0591; 𝑆𝐸𝑏 = 0.1605; 𝑆𝑜𝑏𝑒𝑙 𝑡𝑒𝑠𝑡 𝑠𝑡𝑎𝑡𝑖𝑠𝑡𝑖𝑐 =
Jo

1.6388; 𝑝 = 0.1013). In addition, a hypothesis-driven SEM analysis is also included


in this part (Fig 3. (A)) in order to show the central role of time space experience in
connecting anxiety, depression, and mind-wandering with each other.

Table 5. Correlation analysis for the validity of cSTEA


cSTEA T- S- BAI BDI MWD MW
cSTEA cSTEA S
cSTEA - - - - - - -
T- 0.8494** - - - - - -
cSTEA **
S- 0.6281** 0.1229† - - - - -
cSTEA **
Journal Pre-proof

BAI 0.6026** 0.4568** 0.4598** - - - -


** ** **
BDI 0.5451** 0.3350** 0.5311** 0.7689** - - -
** * ** **
MWD 0.1904† 0.0044† 0.3516** 0.2814** 0.3602** - -
* *
MWS 0.4865** 0.3373** 0.4176** 0.4990** 0.4423** 0.3523* -
** * ** ** ** **
† : insignificant ; * : p < 0.05 ; ** : p < 0.01 : *** : p < 0.001 : **** : p < 0.0001

of
ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo

Fig 3. (A) Here, we suggest a hypothesis-driven SEM analysis for testing the
relationship of time-space experience as in cSTEA with anxiety as in BAI, depression
as in BDI, and mind-wandering as in MWS and MWD. Our hypothesis is that time-
space experience may serve a more fundamental role compared with anxiety,
depression, and mind-wandering. The figure represents the significant interactions of
cSTEA, with the other variables measured by the scales mentioned earlier (for all
interactions, p-value < 0.0001). This indicates a key role of time-space experience in
Journal Pre-proof

of anxiety. (CFI = 0.9701; GFI = 0.8755; NFI = 0.8755; TLI = 0.9635; RMSEA =
0.0525); (B) The mediation models that were significant in Fig 1. are repeated in the
larger sample with cSTEA. Similarly, spontaneous mind-wandering, rather than
deliberate mind-wandering, partially mediates the relationship of time-space
experience (measured by items in cSTEA) with anxiety (measured by items in BAI)
and depression (measured by items in BDI). This suggests a fundamental role for time
space experience among the different measures of anxiety, e.g., BAI, BDI and MWS.

3.5. Capacity of cSTEA for differential diagnosis of anxiety and depression

of
In order to test the capacity of cSTEA for differentially assessing anxiety and the often
comorbid depression, the total group of anxiety subjects was divided into four

ro
subgroups by median cut on BAI and BDI scores. The median score of BAI in our total
group of subjects is 15. As for BDI, the median score is 22.5. According to the original
-p
articles, BAI score 15 is close to the distinction (16-17) between moderate and severe
anxiety vs minimal and mild anxiety (Beck et al., 1993). The BDI score of 22-23 is
re
close to the cut-off score (25) reflecting the distinction between moderate to severe
depression vs minimal to mild (Beck, 1973).
lP

After grouping the data in this way, we divided the subjects into four groups: High
na

anxiety with High depression (h/h), High anxiety with low depression (h/l), Low
anxiety with High depression (l/h), and Low anxiety with Low depression (l/l). These
four groups are represented in the box plots in Fig 4.(A) and the student t test is
ur

conducted to test for significant differences between the four groups. Significant
differences were obtained for h/h vs l/h (t = 4.0345; p = 0.0002) and h/h vs l/l (t =
Jo

6.7955; p < 0.0001), while no significant differences was shown h/h vs h/l (t = 1.9503;
p = 0.0568) and l/h vs l/l (t = -0.3954; p = 0.6944).

Together, these findings suggest that the anxiety symptoms provide the main
contribution to the cSTEA scores among the individuals with depression. This means
that the cSTEA really targets the anxiety symptoms themselves rather than the often
comorbid depressive symptoms. Albeit tentatively, this suggests that the cSTEA has the
potential to serve for clinical differential diagnosis of anxiety and depression.

Furthermore, we conducted categorial correlation analysis (with three groups of


subjects: l/l, h/l, h/h) using Spearman’s rho and ROC analysis (on diagnosing anxiety
and anxiety with depression comorbidity). The significant correlation from low anxiety
Journal Pre-proof

group to high anxiety group as well as from low the group without depression
comorbidity to the group with depression comorbidity is indicated (Spearman’s rho =
0.5991; p-value < 0.00001) (Fig 4.(B)). ROC analysis further indicates good
classification accuracy of cSTEA on diagnosing anxiety (AUC = 0.837) and anxiety
with depression comorbidity (AUC = 0.831) (Fig 4.(C)&(D)). Similar AUC scores from
the ROC analysis suggest minimal bias from depression comorbidity in diagnosing
anxiety and predicting the severity of anxiety. Together with the previous results, these
findings show that cSTEA can maintain its accuracy in the diagnosis/prediction of
anxiety with minimal bias from depression comorbidity. This strengthen its potential
role in serving for clinical differential diagnosis of anxiety and depression.

of
ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo
Journal Pre-proof

of
ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo

Fig 4. (A) Box plots of the distribution of cSTEA scores among four groups of
subjects (h/h: High anxiety with High depression; h/l: High anxiety with Low
depression; l/h: Low anxiety with High depression; l/l: Low anxiety with Low
depression. According to the student t test, significant differences hold in h/h vs l/h
and h/h vs l/l, while no differences were obtained for h/h vs h/l and l/h vs l/l. (ns: p >
0.05; *: p < 0.05; **: p < 0.01; ***: p < 0.001; ****: p < 0.0001); (B) The results of
Journal Pre-proof

categorial correlation analysis of cSTEA with three groups of subjects: l/l, h/l, h/h.
The significant correlations are indicated (Spearman’s rho = 0.5991, p-value <
0.00001). (C) and (D) are the ROC curve of cSTEA in diagnosing anxiety (AUC =
0.837) and anxiety with depression comorbidity (AUC = 0.831) respectively.

3.6. Capacity of cSTEA for therapeutic monitoring of anxiety

We finally tested whether the cSTEA is sensitive to therapeutic changes during


breathing therapy by investigating its pre-post therapeutic effects. We’ve collected all
pre- and post-intervention relationships from the data (n = 50) and compared the cSTEA,

of
BAI, and BDI scores before and after standardized breathing therapy (see methods for
details), i.e., the pre- and post-score differences were calculated. The changing of scores

ro
of cSTEA, BAI, and BDI are illustrated in Fig 5.(A). From paired t test (one-tailed),
significant differences between pre- and post-scores of cSTEA (t = 2.1508; p-value =
-p
0.0182), BAI (t = 4.3969; p-value < 0.00001), and BDI (t = 3.0554; p-value = 0.0018)
were obtained. In order to make the pre- and post-score differences comparable between
re
the different scales, we further standardized their values by dividing the respective scale
values with the total score range of the scale. The possible score of BAI or BDI is
lP

ranging from 0 to 63. Thus, the total score range of BAI or BDI is 63. The possible
score of cSTEA is ranging from -40 to 40. Thus, the total score range of cSTEA is 80.
na

Furthermore, the standardized score differences of cSTEA, BAI, and BDI were
compared through correlation analysis (Fig 5.(B)&(C)) The results suggested
ur

significant correlation between standardized pre- and post-cSTEA-score differences


with both BAI (r = 0.6946; p < 0.00001) and BDI (r = 0.6424; p < 0.00001), indicating
Jo

similar effects of the therapeutic intervention on the levels of both anxiety and
depression. Furthermore, the slopes of the linear regression (cSTEA vs BAI: 0.988;
cSTEA vs BDI: 0.9082) suggest similar sensitivity in detecting anxiety and depression
as per BAI and BDI respectively. This hints upon the capacity of the cSTEA for
detecting changes in the level of anxiety during therapy making it a suitable candidate
for serving as marker for therapeutic monnitoring.
Journal Pre-proof

of
ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo

Fig 5. (A) The changes between pre- and post-therapeutic scores of cSTEA, BAI, and
BDI. Each line indicates a single subject. The left end of the lines represents the pre-
therapeutic scores and the right end of lines represents the post-therapeutic scores. (B)
The correlation of the standardized pre- and post-cSTEA score difference ( (post-
intervention cSTEA score -–pre-intervention cSTEA score)/80 ) with the standardized
pre- and post-BAI score difference ( (post-intervention BAI score -–pre-intervention
BAI score)/63 ). (C) The correlation of the standardized pre-and post-cSTEA score
difference with the standardized pre-and post-BDI score difference ( (post-
intervention BDI score -–pre-intervention BDI score)/63 ).

4. Discussion
Journal Pre-proof

We here introduce a novel standardized quantified instrument for measuring time space
experience in anxiety, the STEA, in an exploratory study. We show that the STEA is (i)
valid and reliable, (ii) sensitive to therapeutic improvement, (iii) closely related with
spontaneous mind wandering, (iv) can differentiate between anxiety and the often
comorbid depression, and (v) can be condensed in a short more clinically applicable
form, cSTEA.

We show good convergent validity of the STEA with the self-report scales of anxiety
and depression BAI and BDI. This suggests that abnormal time space perception is
indeed an integral feature of anxiety. However, future studies are warranted that relate
the STEA to more observer-based anxiety and depression scales.

of
On the more psychological level, we demonstrate close relationship of STEA with mind

ro
wandering [see also Christoff et al. (2016) and Scalabrini et al. (2022)]. Importantly, as
shown in our mediation model, the impact of STEA on the anxiety level was not direct
-p
but rather indirect as mediated through the mind wandering. This is in accordance with
the framework by Christoff et al. (2016), who consider the spontaneous, dynamic, and
re
excessive automatic constraints on thought a core feature of anxiety. That, in turn, is
closely related to biased attention and abnormal processing of threat-related stimuli in
lP

individuals with anxiety (Bar-Haim et al., 2007; Eysenck, 1988; Najmi et al., 2012).
na

The selected items of the shorter clinical form, the cSTEA, seem to all revolve around
one underlying core or basic disturbance, namely the perception of time and space as
highly constricted. These include “Comparing with the future and the past, present is
ur

the only thing concrete to me,” “Myself right now has long distance to myself in the
past,” “I think the space around me is enclosed or bounded,” and “I think I am locked
Jo

in from the external world.”. Accordingly, the perception of time is constricted to either
the present or past without any perceived access to the future. Analogously so in the
domain of space which was also perceived as being constricted to the own body and
detached from the environment. Albeit tentatively, we therefore assume that
constriction of the perceived time and space may be considered a potential candidate
for a basic disturbance of anxiety (see Northoff, 2023; Northoff and Hirjak, 2022;
Northoff et al., 2023b for the notion of basic disturbance).

In addition to the easy and simple clinical applicability of the cSTEA in anxiety subjects,
our results also hint towards its potential value for clinical differential diagnosis. We
recently introduced a scale for the time space experience of psychosis, e.g., STEP
(Arantes-Gonçalves et al., 2022). Here, the perception of temporal and spatial
Journal Pre-proof

fragmentation was a key feature which distinguished the psychotic subjects from those
suffering of mood disturbances (Fusar-Poli et al., 2022; Madeira et al., 2016; Röhricht
et al., 2020; Stanghellini and Raballo, 2015). Future studies are needed whether the
perception of temporospatial fragmentation versus perception of temporospatial
constriction allows differentiating psychotic and anxiety symptoms or syndromes.

Finally, we show that the cSTEA scores reduce considerable with therapeutic changes.
Note that the therapeutic intervention was nonpharmacological, i.e., breathing, which
complemented the additional pharmacological and or nonpharmacological therapy
given in almost all of our patients. While future placebo-controlled studies are needed
to directly relate anxiety reducing effects with the breathing therapy itself rather than

of
some unspecific effect factor, the changes in cSTEA values clearly indicate the scale’s
sensitivity to therapeutic improvement. This renders the cSTEA scale a strong

ro
potential candidate to serve as therapeutic monitoring marker.
-p
4.1. Limitations and future directions
re

A primary limitation of this study stems from the relatively small sample sizes utilized
lP

for data collection. This may limit the generalizability and external validity of the
proposed insights in our study. Although the suggested results in this study all show
na

high degrees of statistical significance, future research should consider employing


larger and more diverse samples to further validate and extend the proposed insights,
thus, standing for the robustness of the conclusions. Another limitation of this study is
ur

the lack of statistical ground for the a priori concepts included within the construction
and interpretation of STEA and cSTEA. To further enhance the validity of our
Jo

conceptual analysis in this study, future work on evaluating the proposed concepts by
statistical methods, e.g., factor analysis, clustering analysis, and structural equation
model (SEM), is needed in a larger sample size.

Future studies may also link the time space experience to the changes of the self
which is highly unstable and desynchronized in anxiety (Lucherini Angeletti et al.,
2023) assuming their close relationship (Dixon et al., 2022). This is even more likely
given that the self may be conceived as basis or fundamental function underlying
emotions like anxiety as conceived in the Basis model of self specificity (Northoff
and Stanghellini, 2016). Such basic or fundamental self is supposed to be constituted
by temporal and spatial coordinates and trajectories (Northoff and Stanghellini, 2016)
whose alterations in anxiety may be manifested in abnormal time and space
Journal Pre-proof

experience. Given such fundamental nature of time-space experience, one may then
also assume that it may shape emotion regulation in an abnormal way resulting in
emotion dysregulation as suggested by the group around J.Gross (Dixon et al., 2022;
Keskin et al., 2023; Scalabrini et al., 2024). This is further supported by our
observation of the close relationship of time space experience with spontaneous mind
wandering which, in turn, is known to relate to emotion dysregulation (Scalabrini et
al., 2024).

5. Conclusion

of
We introduce a novel scale for time and space experience in anxiety, the STEA and its
shorter clinical version cSTEA, by showing (i) its high convergent and divergent

ro
validity with related scales, (ii) its propensity to monitor therapeutic change, (ii) its
utility to allow for clinical differential diagnosis, (iv) its close relation with spontaneous
-p
thought or mind wandering, and (v) its clinical utility in a short clinical version, the
cSTEA. Importantly, our results demonstrate the close relationship of both time and
re
space experience with spontaneous thoughts, e.g., mind wandering with the latter
mediating the former’s impact on the severity of anxiety symptoms. This points to a
lP

more fundamental role of time and space with the experience of temporal and spatial
constriction possibly constituting a “basic disturbance” of anxiety as distimguished
na

from the basic disturbances of other mental disorders like MDD and schizophrenia
(Northoff, 2023; Northoff and Hirjak, 2022; Northoff et al., 2023b).
ur

More generally, our results show the theoretical and scientific fruitfulness of a
spatiotemporal framework for psychopathological symptoms like anxiety. This is
Jo

hypothesized and developed in the recently developed Spatiotemporal


Psychopathology (Northoff, 2016a, b, 2022; Northoff and Hirjak, 2022; Northoff and
Hirjak, 2023; Northoff et al., 2023b; Northoff and Stanghellini, 2016). Going beyond
pure research, we tentatively hypothesize that such spatiotemporal framework also
carries major practical, e.g., clinical implications like the cSTEA’s potential usage as
both diagnostic and therapeutic marker of anxiety.
Journal Pre-proof

Appendix 1

Scale for time and space experience in anxiety (STEA)

Time Experience Subscale


1. I think the current situation is quite
uncertain.
2. I think what will happen in the future is
quite uncertain.
3. I am uneasy about the changes.

of
4. I think time passes fast.

ro
5. I think that time passes slowly.

6.
-p
I think there is gap between present and
future.
re
7. I think there is gap between present and
past.
lP

8. I think everything about me is


unchangeable.
na

9. I often perceive something bad


happened before to be far away from
me.
ur

10. To me, the future is distant.


Jo

11. I am often consciously aware my


heartbeat.
12. I think events occurring right now are
predictable.
13. I can tolerate unpredictable future
events.
14. I can handle the alteration of current
situation.
15. I feel that my thought or behaviour are
accelerated.
16. I feel that everything near to me slows
down.
Journal Pre-proof

17. Comparing with future and past, present


is the only thing concrete to me.
18. Everything, including the future, is out
of control.
19. I feel that the negative events in the past
have influenced me a lot.
20. I often think about the future.

21. I often focus on things with a


monotonous rhythm.
22. I feel that everything surrounding me

of
fast.
23. It is hard for me to catch on other’s

ro
tempo.
24. My life is determined. -p
25. My self right now has long distance to
re
myself in the past.
26. It seems that my self in the future will
lP

not be the same as my self right now.


27. I often do behaviors with a certain
na

speed.
Space Experience Subscale
1. I have the feeling that objects become
ur

flatter.
2. I think the space around me is enclosed
Jo

or bounded.
3. I think that other things are out of reach
or distant.
4. I often perceive objects or other people
approaching me lately or, saying, not
quickly.
5. I have the sense of separation or
seclusion from the surroundings.
6. I have the feeling that I perceive more
things, including hearing things, seeing
things, etc.
Journal Pre-proof

7. The world is getting more enclosed and


surrounding me.
8. I tend to avoid going to spaces that is
unfamiliar to me.
9. I have the feeling that my self becomes
flatter.
10. I think I am confined.

11. I think the distance between me and


external objects or other people is long.
12. My perception to the surroundings is

of
blurred or dull.
13. I think I am locked-in from the external

ro
world.
14. I can quickly perceive what is -p
approaching me.
15. I have the feeling that I am getting
re
closer to the edge or end of the world or
events.
lP

16. I tend to stay in spaces that is familiar


to me.
na

17. I have the feeling that others become


flatter.
ur
Jo
Journal Pre-proof

Appendix 2

Clinical Scale for time and space experience in anxiety


(cSTEA)
1. I am uneasy about the changes.

2. I often consciously aware my heartbeat.

3. I can tolerate unpredictable future


events.
4. Comparing with the future and the past,

of
the present is the only thing concrete to
me.

ro
5. My life is determined.

6.
-p
My self right now has a long distance to
my self in the past.
re
7. I think the space around me is enclosed
or bounded.
lP

8. I think I am locked-in from the external


world.
na

References
ur

Aho, K., 2020. Temporal experience in anxiety: embodiment, selfhood, and the
collapse of meaning. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 19, 259-270.
Jo

Anagnostopoulos, F., Griva, F., 2011. Exploring Time Perspective in Greek Young
Adults: Validation of the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory and
Relationships with Mental Health Indicators. Social Indicators Research 106, 41
- 59.
Arantes-Gonçalves, F., Wolman, A., Bastos-Leite, A.J., Northoff, G., 2022. Scale for
Space and Time Experience in Psychosis: Converging Phenomenological and
Psychopathological Perspectives. Psychopathology 55, 132-142.
Åström, E., Wiberg, B., Sircova, A., Wiberg, M., Carelli, M.G., 2014. Insights into
features of anxiety through multiple aspects of psychological time. Journal of
Integrative Psychology and Therapeutics 2.
Balban, M.Y., Neri, E., Kogon, M.M., Weed, L., Nouriani, B., Jo, B., Holl, G.,
Zeitzer, J.M., Spiegel, D., Huberman, A.D., 2023. Brief structured respiration
Journal Pre-proof

practices enhance mood and reduce physiological arousal. Cell Rep Med 4,
100895.
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., van, I.M.H.,
2007. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a
meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull 133, 1-24.
Barrett, L.F., Quigley, K.S., Hamilton, P., 2016. An active inference theory of
allostasis and interoception in depression. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
371.
Beck, A.T., 1973. The diagnosis and management of depression.
Beck, A.T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., Steer, R., 1993. Beck anxiety inventory. Journal
of consulting and clinical psychology.

of
Beck, A.T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., Steer, R.A., 1988. An inventory for measuring
clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol 56, 893-897.

ro
Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., Erbaugh, J., 1961. An inventory for
measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 4, 561-571.
-p
Berenskötter, F., 2020. Anxiety, time, and agency. International Theory 12, 273-290.
Blum, J., Rockstroh, C., Göritz, A.S., 2019. Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback Based
re
on Slow-Paced Breathing With Immersive Virtual Reality Nature Scenery. Front
Psychol 10, 2172.
lP

Bollnow, O.F., 1961. Lived-Space. Philosophy Today 5, 31.


Borkovec, T.D., Ray, W.J., Stober, J., 1998. Worry: A Cognitive Phenomenon
na

Intimately Linked to Affective, Physiological, and Interpersonal Behavioral


Processes. Cognitive Therapy and Research 22, 561-576.
Brown, T.A., Campbell, L.A., Lehman, C.L., Grisham, J.R., Mancill, R.B., 2001.
ur

Current and lifetime comorbidity of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders in
a large clinical sample. J Abnorm Psychol 110, 585-599.
Jo

Carriere, J.S.A., Seli, P., Smilek, D., 2013. Wandering in both mind and body:
individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Can J
Exp Psychol 67, 19-31.
Chiara, F.S., Gian Domenico, I., 2013. Better Safe Than Sorry? The Safety Margin
Surrounding the Body Is Increased by Anxiety. The Journal of Neuroscience 33,
14225.
Christoff, K., Irving, Z.C., Fox, K.C.R., Spreng, R.N., Andrews-Hanna, J.R., 2016.
Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience 17, 718-731.
Coll-Florit, M., Climent, S., Sanfilippo, M., Hernández-Encuentra, E., 2021.
Metaphors of Depression. Studying First Person Accounts of Life with
Depression Published in Blogs. Metaphor and Symbol 36, 1-19.
Journal Pre-proof

Dixon, M.L., Moodie, C.A., Goldin, P.R., Farb, N., Heimberg, R.G., Zhang, J., Gross,
J.J., 2022. Frontoparietal and Default Mode Network Contributions to Self-
Referential Processing in Social Anxiety Disorder. Cognitive, Affective, &
Behavioral Neuroscience 22, 187-198.
Doerr-Zegers, O., 2016. Temporality and spatiality of anxious experience. European
Psychiatry 33, S40-S40.
Eysenck, M.W., 1988. Anxiety and attention. Anxiety Research 1, 9-15.
Fritz, M.S., Mackinnon, D.P., 2007. Required sample size to detect the mediated
effect. Psychol Sci 18, 233-239.
Fuchs, T., 2005. The phenomenology of body Space and Time in Depression.
Comprendre 15, 108-121.

of
Fuchs, T., 2007. Psychotherapy of the lived space: a phenomenological and ecological
concept. Am J Psychother 61, 423-439.

ro
Fuchs, T., 2013. Temporality and psychopathology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive
Sciences 12, 75-104. -p
Fusar-Poli, P., Estradé, A., Stanghellini, G., Venables, J., Onwumere, J., Messas, G.,
Gilardi, L., Nelson, B., Patel, V., Bonoldi, I., Aragona, M., Cabrera, A., Rico, J.,
re
Hoque, A., Otaiku, J., Hunter, N., Tamelini, M.G., Maschião, L.F., Puchivailo,
M.C., Piedade, V.L., Kéri, P., Kpodo, L., Sunkel, C., Bao, J., Shiers, D., Kuipers,
lP

E., Arango, C., Maj, M., 2022. The lived experience of psychosis: a bottom-up
review co-written by experts by experience and academics. World Psychiatry 21,
na

168-188.
Gitler, A., Vanacker, L., De Couck, M., De Leeuw, I., Gidron, Y., 2022.
Neuromodulation Applied to Diseases: The Case of HRV Biofeedback. J Clin
ur

Med 11.
Griffin, E., Wildbur, D., 2020. The Role of Balanced Time Perspective on Student
Jo

Well-being and Mental Health: A Mixed-Methods Study. Mental Health &


Prevention 18, 200181.
Gu, Y., Gu, S., Lei, Y., Li, H., 2020. From Uncertainty to Anxiety: How Uncertainty
Fuels Anxiety in a Process Mediated by Intolerance of Uncertainty. Neural Plast
2020, 8866386.
Hohwy, J., 2016. The Self-Evidencing Brain. Noûs 50, 259-285.
Hou, W.-l., Yin, X.-l., Yin, X.-y., Guan, L.-y., Cao, J.-q., Tang, Z., Jiang, C.-x., Xu,
D.-w., Yu, X., Wang, J., Jia, Q.-f., Chan, R.C.K., Hui, L., 2021. Association
between stereopsis deficits and attention decline in patients with major
depressive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological
Psychiatry 110, 110267.
Igolkina, A.A., Meshcheryakov, G., 2020. semopy: A Python Package for Structural
Journal Pre-proof

Equation Modeling. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal


27, 952-963.
Keskin, K., Eker, M.Ç., Gönül, A.S., Northoff, G., 2023. Abnormal global signal
topography of self modulates emotion dysregulation in major depressive
disorder. Translational Psychiatry 13, 107.
Kooij, D., Kanfer, R., Betts, M., Rudolph, C.W., 2018. Future time perspective: A
systematic review and meta-analysis. J Appl Psychol 103, 867-893.
Kunzendorf, R.G., Flynn, H., Shedlack, J., Georges, M., Deno, S., Rosa, N., Burns, J.,
2011. Depression, unlike Normal Sadness, is Associated with a “Flatter” Self-
Perception and a “Flatter” Phenomenal World. Imagination, Cognition and
Personality 30, 447-461.

of
Lake, J.I., Labar, K.S., 2011. Unpredictability and uncertainty in anxiety: a new
direction for emotional timing research. Front Integr Neurosci 5, 55.

ro
Lamers, F., van Oppen, P., Comijs, H.C., Smit, J.H., Spinhoven, P., van Balkom, A.J.,
Nolen, W.A., Zitman, F.G., Beekman, A.T., Penninx, B.W., 2011. Comorbidity
-p
patterns of anxiety and depressive disorders in a large cohort study: the
Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA). J Clin Psychiatry 72,
re
341-348.
Lucherini Angeletti, L., Scalabrini, A., Ricca, V., Northoff, G., 2023. Topography of
lP

the Anxious Self: Abnormal Rest-Task Modulation in Social Anxiety Disorder.


Neuroscientist 29, 221-244.
na

Madeira, L., Bonoldi, I., Rocchetti, M., Samson, C., Azis, M., Queen, B., Bossong,
M., Perez, J., Stone, J., Allen, P., Howes, O.D., McGuire, P., Raballo, A., Fusar-
Poli, P., Ballerini, M., Stanghellini, G., 2016. An initial investigation of abnormal
ur

bodily phenomena in subjects at ultra high risk for psychosis: Their prevalence
and clinical implications. Comprehensive Psychiatry 66, 39-45.
Jo

Mathews, A., MacLeod, C., 2005. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders.


Annu Rev Clin Psychol 1, 167-195.
Minkowski, E., Metzel, N., Northwestern, U., 1970. Lived time : phenomenological
and psychopathological studies. Northwestern University Press, Evanston Ill.
Mioni, G., Zangrossi, A., Cipolletta, S., 2023. Me, myself and you: How self-
consciousness influences time perception. Atten Percept Psychophys.
Najmi, S., Kuckertz, J.M., Amir, N., 2012. Attentional impairment in anxiety:
inefficiency in expanding the scope of attention. Depress Anxiety 29, 243-249.
Nashiro, K., Yoo, H.J., Min, J., Cho, C., Nasseri, P., Zhang, Y., Lehrer, P., Thayer, J.F.,
Mather, M., 2022. Effects of a randomised trial of 5-week heart rate variability
biofeedback intervention on mind wandering and associated brain function. Cogn
Affect Behav Neurosci 22, 1349-1357.
Journal Pre-proof

Northoff, G., 2016a. Spatiotemporal psychopathology I: No rest for the brain's resting
state activity in depression? Spatiotemporal psychopathology of depressive
symptoms. J Affect Disord 190, 854-866.
Northoff, G., 2016b. Spatiotemporal Psychopathology II: How does a
psychopathology of the brain's resting state look like? Spatiotemporal approach
and the history of psychopathology. J Affect Disord 190, 867-879.
Northoff, G., 2022. Spatiotemporal Psychopathology - A Novel Approach to Brain
and Symptoms. Noro Psikiyatr Ars 59, S3-s9.
Northoff, G., 2023. From research of mental disorders to clinical application - lessons
from diabetes. J Psychiatry Neurosci 48, E240-e244.
Northoff, G., Daub, J., Hirjak, D., 2023a. Overcoming the translational crisis of

of
contemporary psychiatry - converging phenomenological and spatiotemporal
psychopathology. Mol Psychiatry 28, 4492-4499.

ro
Northoff, G., Hirjak, D., 2022. Spatiotemporal Psychopathology – An integrated
brain-mind approach and catatonia. Schizophrenia Research.
-p
Northoff, G., Hirjak, D., 2023. Integrating subjective and objective-spatiotemporal
approach to psychiatric disorders. Mol Psychiatry.
re
Northoff, G., Klar, P., Bein, M., Safron, A., 2023b. As without, so within: how the
brain's temporo-spatial alignment to the environment shapes consciousness.
lP

Interface Focus 13, 20220076.


Northoff, G., Stanghellini, G., 2016. How to Link Brain and Experience?
na

Spatiotemporal Psychopathology of the Lived Body. Frontiers in Human


Neuroscience 10.
Olfson, M., Fireman, B., Weissman, M.M., Leon, A.C., Sheehan, D.V., Kathol, R.G.,
ur

Hoven, C., Farber, L., 1997. Mental disorders and disability among patients in a
primary care group practice. Am J Psychiatry 154, 1734-1740.
Jo

Pedregosa, F., Varoquaux, G., Gramfort, A., Michel, V., Thirion, B., Grisel, O.,
Blondel, M., Prettenhofer, P., Weiss, R., Dubourg, V., Vanderplas, J., Passos, A.,
Cournapeau, D., Brucher, M., Perrot, M., Duchesnay, E., Louppe, G., 2012.
Scikit-learn: Machine Learning in Python. Journal of Machine Learning
Research 12.
Penrod, J., 2001. Refinement of the concept of uncertainty. J Adv Nurs 34, 238-245.
Pollatos, O., Laubrock, J., Wittmann, M., 2014. Interoceptive focus shapes the
experience of time. PLoS One 9, e86934.
Röhricht, F., Eranti, S., Ballerini, M., Mancini, M., Neale, J., Tsoumpris, A.,
Stanghellini, G., 2020. Abnormal Bodily Phenomena in First Episode Psychosis:
A Preliminary Exploratory Cohort Study. Psychopathology 53, 74-83.
Rabellino, D., Frewen, P.A., McKinnon, M.C., Lanius, R.A., 2020. Peripersonal
Journal Pre-proof

Space and Bodily Self-Consciousness: Implications for Psychological Trauma-


Related Disorders. Front Neurosci 14, 586605.
Reuther, B., 2014. On our everyday being: Heidegger and attachment theory. Journal
of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology 34, 101-115.
Rinaldi, L., Locati, F., Parolin, L., Girelli, L., 2017. Distancing the Present Self from
the past and the Future: Psychological Distance in Anxiety and Depression.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 70, 1106-1113.
Riskind, J.H., Kleiman, E.M., Seifritz, E., Neuhoff, J., 2014. Influence of anxiety,
depression and looming cognitive style on auditory looming perception. Journal
of Anxiety Disorders 28, 45-50.
Sass, L., Pienkos, E., 2013. Space, Time, and Atmosphere A Comparative

of
Phenomenology of Melancholia, Mania, and Schizophrenia, Part II. Journal of
Consciousness Studies 20, 131-152.

ro
Scalabrini, A., De Amicis, M., Brugnera, A., Cavicchioli, M., Çatal, Y., Keskin, K.,
Pilar, J.G., Zhang, J., Osipova, B., Compare, A., Greco, A., Benedetti, F., Mucci,
-p
C., Northoff, G., 2024. Feeling out of synchrony: Investigating the vulnerability
of self in subclinical realms. Asian J Psychiatr 91, 103859.
re
Scalabrini, A., Schimmenti, A., De Amicis, M., Porcelli, P., Benedetti, F., Mucci, C.,
Northoff, G., 2022. The self and its internal thought: In search for a
lP

psychological baseline. Conscious Cogn 97, 103244.


Seli, P., Beaty, R.E., Marty-Dugas, J., Smilek, D., 2019. Depression, anxiety, and
na

stress and the distinction between intentional and unintentional mind wandering.
Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice 6, 163-170.
Sevoz-Couche, C., Laborde, S., 2022. Heart rate variability and slow-paced
ur

breathing:when coherence meets resonance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 135,


104576.
Jo

Singer, J.L., 1966. Daydreaming : an introduction to the experimental study of inner


experience.
Sobel, M.E., 1982. Asymptotic Confidence Intervals for Indirect Effects in Structural
Equation Models. Sociological Methodology 13, 290.
Spinhoven, P., Drost, J., van Hemert, B., Penninx, B.W., 2015. Common rather than
unique aspects of repetitive negative thinking are related to depressive and
anxiety disorders and symptoms. J Anxiety Disord 33, 45-52.
Stanghellini, G., Raballo, A., 2015. Differential typology of delusions in major
depression and schizophrenia. A critique to the unitary concept of ‘psychosis’.
Journal of Affective Disorders 171, 171-178.
Tenenhaus, M., 2008. Structural Equation Modelling for small samples.
Thönes, S., Oberfeld, D., 2015. Time perception in depression: A meta-analysis.
Journal Pre-proof

Journal of Affective Disorders 175, 359-372.


Tibshirani, R., 1996. Regression Shrinkage and Selection via the Lasso. Journal of the
Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological) 58, 267-288.
van Beek, W., Berghuis, H., Kerkhof, A., Beekman, A., 2011. Time perspective,
personality and psychopathology: Zimbardo’s time perspective inventory in
psychiatry. Time & Society 20, 364-374.
Van Hedger, K., Faig, K.E., Necka, E.A., Norman, G., 2020. Interoceptive predictors
of time estimation. PsyArXiv.
Wang, X., Li, S., Wu, W., 2020. Effects of medical biofeedback trainings on acute
stress by hybridizing heart rate variability and brain imaging. Multimedia Tools
and Applications 79, 10141-10155.

of
Wang, Y., Hu, X., Han, J., Scalabrini, A., Hu, Y., Hu, Z., Tan, Z., Zhang, J., Northoff,
G., 2021. Time is of essence - Abnormal time perspectives mediate the impact of

ro
childhood trauma on depression severity. Journal of Psychiatric Research 137,
534-541. -p
Watkins, E.R., 2008. Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychol Bull
134, 163-206.
re
Wittmann, M., 2022. How we experience the passage of time: the body, feelings, and
the self. PsyArXiv.
lP

Woodgate, R.L., Tennent, P., Legras, N., 2021. Understanding Youth's Lived
Experience of Anxiety through Metaphors: A Qualitative, Arts-Based Study. Int J
na

Environ Res Public Health 18.


Zhang, X., Lei, Y., Yin, H., Li, P., Li, H., 2018. Slow Is Also Fast: Feedback Delay
Affects Anxiety and Outcome Evaluation. Front Hum Neurosci 12, 20.
ur

Zimbardo, P.G., Boyd, J., 1999. Putting Time in Perspective: A Valid, Reliable
Individual-Differences Metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77,
Jo

1271-1288.
Journal Pre-proof

Author Statement Contributors


C.L., J.G., and G.N. designed the research. C.L. constructed the Scale of time and
space experience in anxiety (STEA). *J.G. (main), A.W., and L.L.A. conducted the
investigation and collected the data. C.L. and G.N. analyzed the data. C.L. and G.N.
wrote the original manuscript. The manuscript was further reviewed and edited by all
the authors. All the authors agreed with the final version of the manuscript.

Fundings
This work is financially supported by Physicians Incorporated Services (PSI) and
Canada Institute of Health Research (CIHR) in Canada. The PSI and CIHR had no
further role in the writing of this commentary and in the decision to submit the paper

of
for publication.

ro
Declaration of competing interest
All authors declared no competing interests for this study. There are no other
-p
disclosures.
re
Acknowledgements
None
lP
na
ur
Jo
Journal Pre-proof

Declaration of competing interest


All authors declared no competing interests for this study. There are no other
disclosures.

of
ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo
Journal Pre-proof

⚫ Anxiety is closely related to constricted time and space experiences.


⚫ The dynamic of thoughts, i.e., mind wandering, may serve as the mediator
between the time and space experiences and anxiety.
⚫ cSTEA allow a valid and simple way to assess clinical phenomenology of
anxiety.

of
ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo

You might also like