Sava Popa 2011
Sava Popa 2011
net/publication/284610489
CITATIONS READS
52 4,810
2 authors, including:
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Florin Alin Sava on 11 January 2016.
ABSTRACT
This paper reports two independent studies investigating personality types using
cluster analysis based on the five-factor personality model. In the first study, we
focused on identifying the most appropriate cluster number by checking the
replicability of two to eight clusters across two random halves of a nationally
representative Romanian sample. The second study established the external
validity of the more replicable three-cluster and five-cluster solution by studying
their relations to specific self-reported behaviors. The results indicate good
validity for a five-cluster solution describing the following personality types:
resilient, undercontrolled, strain, overcontrolled, and passive. The findings are
discussed in relation to a suggested classification based on degrees of self-control
and of adaptation / flexibility.
*
Corresponding author:
E-mail: [email protected]
360 F. A. Sava, R. I. Popa
Aken, 2001; Asendorpf & Van Aken, 1999; Avdeyeva & Church, 2005;
Barbaranelli, 2002; Block, 1995; de Fruyt, Mervielde, & van Leeuwen, 2002;
Herzberg & Roth, 2006, Pulkkinen, 1996; Robins, John, Caspi, Moffitt, &
Stouthamer-Loeber, 1996). Almost all these attempts refer back to Block and
Block’s (1980) personality typology.
The best-known classification of personality types, based on the Big-Five
model, identifies only three gender-indifferent personality types (Asendorpf & Van
Aken, 1999; de Fruyt, Mervielde, & van Leeuwen, 2002, Robins et al., 1996). This
typology differentiates between personality types described as resilient,
overcontrolled, and undercontrolled. This classification is based on Block and
Block’s proposal (1980), which focused, in one dimension, on Ego resiliency (the
degree of an individual’s flexibility and adaptation towards the demands of the
environment) and in another on Ego control (the intensity with which impulses and
wishes are expressed – measured along an axis that has self-discipline at one end
and impulsivity / explosive behavior at the other).
The specific typology for the resilient personality is indicated by scores
above the average for all the Big-Five dimensions, except for neuroticism - a
dimension in which resilient people obtain low scores. The typology for the
overcontrolled personality typically involves below average scores for extraversion
combined with above average ones for the traits of neuroticism and
conscientiousness, the other two trait dimensions being less relevant for describing
this type. Finally, the profile of the undercontrolled personality type is usually
characterized by low agreeableness and conscientiousness in combination with high
levels of neuroticism and extraversion.
However, this typology suffers from shortcomings, concerning
(a) differences between studies in the number of identified clusters,
(b) inappropriate labeling of clusters, (c) differences in cluster-analytic methods,
and (d) disregard for possible cultural differences. We will address these four issues
in turn.
Although the most commonly proffered solutions suggest a three-cluster
typology, Pulkkinen (1996) describes seven personality types (with twelve
personality subtypes) while a recent study using a nationally representative German
sample suggests a five-cluster solution (Herzberg & Roth, 2006).
Even those studies that provide a three-cluster solution seem to suffer from
a labeling problem. One conclusion of Herzberg and Roth (2006), in their review of
the literature, is that in many instances prototypes with differently detailed profiles
have been assigned the same names as used in previously described work,
apparently in order to align the new findings to a three-cluster solution. For
instance, the undercontrolled type as described by Asendorpf et al. (2001) had a
high level of neuroticism, a very high level of extraversion and an average level of
agreeableness, whilst the same type label was used by de Fruyt, Mervielde, and van
Leeuwen (2002) for a type having an average level of neuroticism and lower levels
In our first study, we focused on identifying the most appropriate cluster number
starting from Block’s (1980) typology, as well as establishing a proper label for
each cluster in a Romanian sample.
As Asendorpf (2001) affirmed, the consistency of personality types across
different studies is far from being perfect, mainly because different studies may
differ in (i) language, (ii) culture, (iii) selectivity and size of the sample,
(iv) instrument of trait assessment, (v) judge (self or others), and (vi) method of
deriving types.
In order to look for the impact of five out of six above-mentioned aspects
(we based our study entirely on self-report measures) and to overcome the previosly
mentioned limitations: (a) we conducted the analysis on a nationally representative
population sample in order to be in a position to extract the best cluster number,
especially from the point of view of replicability; (b) we used a more liberal cut off
value for Cohen's kappa for identifying the most appropriate number of clusters as
more appropriate when studying a large, heteregogenous sample; (c) we conducted
our study in a non-English-speaking country, using an indigenous instrument to
measure the Five-factor model of personality, and (d) we addressed the issue of
labeling the resulting clusters based on a more objective basis, by computing their
degree of similarity with a typical personality type (profile) as resulted from
literature review related to Block and Block’s (1980) model.
Based on these characteristics we will provide further evidences related to
the opportunity to continue considering the Block and Block’s (1980) typology a
reference framework for interpreting the personality types derived from a five-
factor model of personality.
Finally, considering arguments by Herzberg and Roth (2006) and by Hair,
Anderson, Tatham, and Black (1998), we did not only study the replicability of
cluster solutions, but also checked in a second study how well cluster membership
of participants predicted specific behaviors. This would provide evidence that the
different uncovered types are predictive of real life differences between types.
STUDY 1
METHOD
Participants
The randomly selected sample of 1,073 participants was representative of the
Romanian population in terms of distribution of gender, age, ethnographically
significant regions, and milieu (urban vs. rural). However the questionnaire was
administered only to a population of participants between 16 and 60 years of age
and who had at least ten years of formal education. The percentages obtained for
each sample category were: 49.3% (urban) and 50.7% (rural); 26.5% (16-25 years),
34.2% (26-40 years), and 39.3% (41-60 years). The gender distribution was 50.1%
males and 49.9% females. The wider context of the research had as its main
objective the establishing of national representative norms for the DECAS
Personality Inventory through the use of a stratified multi-stage probability design
as a sampling procedure. Thus, in the first stage explicit stratification was employed
by using several strata variables that are usually taken into account when running
national surveys (e.g., geographical region, the milieu, the population size for the
cities etc.). This measure allows a random selection of independent samples for
each stratum. During the second stage, a simple random sample of localities was
drawn in order to cover all combination of strata (e.g., cities with less than 50,000
inhabitants from Transylvania region). The next stage involved a systematic
probabilistic sample procedure to select households from the selected locations
(e.g., every fifth household), by using multiple randomly chosen starting points to
cover both center and the suburbs of the selected locations. Finally, the last step
used gender and age as strata, in order to reproduce the proportions of these
demographics in accordance with data provided by the last national census. In this
last step, one person per selected household was asked to respond, based on a
simple probabilistic procedure (e.g., interviewing the person who has celebrated his
or her birthday more recently). The design of the sampling procedure and the
implementation of the field survey (collection of data) were subcontracted by two
specialized survey companies.
Instruments
All participants in the study were tested using the DECAS Personality Inventory
and a questionnaire addressing demographic characteristics. DECAS (Sava, 2008)
is a 95 item test which solicits dichotomic (“true” / “false”) answers to items
equally distributed among five content scales: openness; extraversion,
conscientiousness, agreeableness and emotional stability (as opposed to
neuroticism) that have been developed based on the factor analysis framework . The
internal consistency of the scales within this sample ranges from .70 for
conscientiousness to .75 for emotional stability, and the six week test-retest stability
coefficients range from .79 to .91, the later being obtained on a smaller convenience
sample. The questionnaire also comprises three validity scales measuring social
desirability, random responding, and acquiescence. The validity of the DECAS
scales was confirmed in a study that included, among others, the DECAS, the BFQ
(Caprara, Barbaranelli, & Borgogni, 2008), Goldberg's IPIP items (Goldberg,
1999), and the NEO-FFI (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Table 1 reports the mean and the
range of the correlations between corresponding scales in the different instruments
to measure the Big Five, the left column referring to correlations of the five DECAS
scales with the corresponding scales in the other instruments, and the right column
Table 1.
Correlations of the DECAS with other instruments (BFQ-2, IPIP, NEO FFI) and
correlations of the other instruments among each other (mean N – 163)
Procedure
The first step consisted in eliminating invalid cases from the sample. The two
criteria for considering a case as invalid were: (1) cases having a social desirability
score over two standard deviations above the average; (2) unusual cases
(“outliers”), at a p < .00 level based on Mahalanobis distance statistics. The second
step consisted of creating standardized z scores following a procedure described in
Urbana (2004), specific to situations in which the shape of data distribution is not
normal. However, such a procedure goes against the suggestion made by Asendorpf
et al. (2001) who explicitly recommend analyzing the raw scores in order to
increase the chance of replicating a three-cluster solution. This recommendation
was challenged by Costa et al. (2002), who consider it a potential source of
methodological artifact; they also state that a cluster analysis solution based on z-
standardized data is more appropriate when calculating kappa indices.
Results
For processing the data SPSS (SPSS 11.0, 2001) was used for selecting the
optimum number of clusters.
Table 2.
Descriptive indicators for different solutions, ranging from 2 to 8 clusters
Table 2 presents the main findings from analyzing three sets of data: (a) the
cluster solution based on replicating the findings from sub-sample A on sub-sample
B (cross-validation); (b) the cluster solution based on replicating the findings from
sub-sample B on sub-sample A (double cross-validation); (c) the cluster solution
resulted from a k-means analysis of the entire sample (N = 1019) taking as starting
points the means of the cluster seeds for the two sub-samples.
We can notice from table 2 that only three of the solutions (two, three and
five clusters) have kappa values for the entire sample solution greater than the pre-
established level of .50, although the three-cluster solution has a wider range for
kappa values if sub-samples are taken into account (from .43 to .63).
Figure 1.
Prototype expressed in z scores for the five identified personality types
(N - Neuroticism; E - Extraversion; O - Openness; A - Agreeableness;
C - Conscientiousness)
Five-cluster solution
Strain type. The second cluster includes 17.2% of the participants, a total of 179
persons. None of the existing literature-derived profiles meet the criteria to justify a
claim of sufficiently close similarity with this cluster. The specific traits, on a Five-
Factor personality model, indicate a prototype with disharmonic features: scores
above the average for O, E, N and C, and scores below average (e.g.: z < -0.50) on
the A scale.
Resilient type. Cluster three includes 21.4% of the participants representing 222
cases. The distinctive traits of these people are found in high scores on all five
scales of the Big-Five model except for neuroticism. The label used to name this
typology is consistent with previously described profiles (both with the resilient
type in a three-cluster(r = .84, d = .86) and (r = .81, d = 1.22) with the resilient in a
five-cluster typology). However this cluster is also rather similar to the confident
(well-adjusted) type as proposed by Herzberg and Roth (2006) (r = .72, d = .89).
Passive type. The fifth cluster also accounts for 232 cases, representing 22.3%
observations. No previously reported profile seems to fit this type well. However
the closest profiles in terms of the two criteria mentioned above are the reserved
type in Herzberg and Roth’s (2006) five-cluster solution (r = .44, d = 1.10) and the
resilient type in our literature-based three-cluster solution (r = .45, d = 1.51). On the
other hand, this cluster could also be seen as an opposite type for the strain type
(r = - .97, d = 3.27 these being the highest absolute values obtained from all
possible paired comparisons). On a Five-Factor personality model, the prototype
profile indicates high scores for A and low scores for dimensions N, E, and C.
Figure 2.
Prototype expressed in z scores for the three identified personality types
(N - Neuroticism; E - Extraversion; O - Openness; A - Agreeableness;
C - Conscientiousness)
Three-cluster solution
Resilient type. From the profile analysis of the three-cluster solution, in the first
group we may find the prototype corresponding to the resilient in the five-cluster
classification. This cluster comprehends 29.6% of the participants (308 cases). The
specific traits of these people involve high scores in E, A, O, average scores for C,
and a low N score. This profile fits well with the literature-based profiles for a five-
cluster solution; thus for resilient and confident types r = .77, d = 1.37, and r = .78,
d = .77, respectively. This profile is also similar to the one described as the resilient
type in the three-cluster literature-based solution (r = .78, d = .81).
The psychological profile for this three-cluster type is similar to the resilient type
presented in the five-cluster solution. Seen from a five-cluster solution viewpoint
96% of five-cluster resilients, 36% of five-cluster undercontrolleds and 14% of
five-cluster passives are individuals who, on a three-cluster solution, would be
categorized as three-cluster resilients.
Strain type. The second cluster invites direct comparison with the strain type in the
five-cluster classification, which included 332 participants (32.0% of those
questioned). In this segment of the population we can observe that the prototype
shows scores above the average for E, C and N, and below average for A. The
psychological description is similar to that presented in the five-cluster solution for
the strain type. None of the previous literature-based profiles satisfied the statistical
criteria to justify a potential overlap with this cluster. From a structural perspective
this cluster contains over 99% of the strain individuals, 41% of the undercontrolled
and 32% of the overcontrolled type as identified in the five-cluster solution.
Passive-overcontrolled type. This cluster contains those individuals identified as of
passive type falling within the overcontrolled category. This type comprehends
39.4% of the participants, totaling 399 cases. From the Five-Factor personality
model perspective, this prototype shows scores below average for E and N, and
slightly below average for O and C. The significance of this cluster can be better
deduced from the composition of the group tested. On a five-cluster solution
analysis, as described above, this heterogeneous type includes approximately 86%
of the five-cluster passive types, and almost 67% of the members of the
overcontrolled type, along with 23% of the undercontrolled type. None of the
previous literature-based profiles fitted the standards to justify a potential overlap
with this cluster.
An analysis of demographic data allows us to reach similar conclusions
concerning the association of the three personality types with: (1) education level -
χ² (8) = 45.17, p < .00; (2) rural vs. urban milieu - χ² (2) = 22.77, p < .00; and
(3) exact location of residence - χ² (2) = 15.89, p < .00.
We can observe a higher incidence of the resilient type among university
graduates and a lower frequency of this type in the section of the sample with less
than a high school level of education. A higher percentage of the resilient type is to
be found in the urban milieu and a higher percentage of the passive-overcontrolled
and strain types in the rural milieu. As for the more precise location of residence in
the urban area, the resilient type can be found more often in the central areas of
cities, while the strain type appears more often in the suburbs.
Finally, in order to obtain a better description of the specific characteristics
of each type discussed in this study and to see how well does the Block and Block’s
(1980) model can provide a meaningful interpretation for the resulted clusters, we
mapped various personality types using a multidimensional scaling approach. More
specifically, we have plotted on the same chart types derived from four different
sources: (1) our five-cluster and three-cluster solutions (Ro5 and Ro3); (2) Herzberg
and Roth’s five-cluster solution obtained using a German, nationally representative
sample (Ger5); (3) Avdeyeva and Church’s first study (2005) that used NEO PI-R
and provided a three-cluster solution to data from a non-western, Philippine sample
(Phil3); (4) a literature based, three-cluster solution (Std3). This last was obtained
by aggregating the findings from seven studies, including 11 different analyses
involving a total of more than 5000 people, conducted in a variety of western
countries by various researchers. The aggregated results were derived by averaging
data presented in Herzberg and Roth’s (2006) Table 1.
Figure 3.
A multidimensional scaling map containing several proposed prototypes
A visual inspection of how the various personality types plot on the map
suggests two different possible classification systems: (1) one (inside arrows)
reliant on a classical perspective that takes a U-shape perspective on personality
types (Asendorpf et al., 2001), considering the levels of resilience (flexibility)
(resilient type at the top) and of control (undrecontrolled at the bottom left and
overcontrolled at the bottom right) as the main axes along which to categorize
various personality types, in accordance with Block and Block’s (1980) theory;
(2) one that adopts a complementary perspective which takes into account the
degree of behavioral internalization (externalization) and the degree to which
healthy psychological resourcefulness is available to allow for adaptive coping (for
a discussion that links mental disorders to internalization, externalization, and
personality, see Krueger 1999 or Krueger, McGue, & Iacono, 2001).
DISCUSSION
The objective of the first study was to identify the number of clusters and assign to
them the most appropriate labels by which the Romanian adult population might be
segmented, based on a typology derived in relation to the five-factor personality
model that take into account the Block and Block’s (1980) typology model. The
results of the cluster analysis indicate two possible solutions: one with three and the
other with five clusters. Both solutions have an acceptable level of stability,
showing acceptable replicability under a double cross-validation procedure.
The five-cluster solution based on the five-factor personality replicates a
series of personality types already well known in the research literature. This
solution combines the five-factor model of personality with Block’s typology: the
undercontrolled type (with insufficient control of the impulses), the overcontrolled
type (with excessive control of impulses) and the resilient type. However two
additional types can be characterized, inviting further refinement in their
definitions. For the present we have labeled them the strain type [comparable to
Friedman’s (1996) A type and to Block and Block’s (1980) brittle undercontrolled
type] and the passive type [somewhat similar to Friedman’s (1996) B type and to
Block and Block’s (1980) resilient overcontrolled type]. The strain type was also
found as more prone to possess irrational beliefs and maladaptive schemas that
might trigger psychopathological problems (Sava, 2009). Another interesting
finding is that these two newly distinguished clusters seem to be antagonistic. In
addition there is no correspondence between our strain type and the remaining two
types (confident and reserved) found in the German representative sample, but there
is some correspondence between the passive types described in our study and
Herzberg and Roth’s reserved type (compared with their reserved type our passive
type people are more reserved, less active, and relatively confident in their actions).
When comparing the two possible solutions provided in this study, it may
be observed that our three-cluster solution reproduces two of our five clusters,
specifically the resilient and the strain types. The third cluster, which has been
labeled the passive-overcontrolled type, seems in fact to be formed mainly as a
combination of two clusters (the two independent types being the passive and the
overcontrolled). Finally we should note that the undercontrolled type in a five-
cluster solution cannot be simply mapped onto a three-cluster solution; membership
of this type becomes partitioned in an almost equal way between the three types of
the three-cluster solution when a reassignment is made. Even though the three-
cluster solution seems to be the most favored solution for investigators using the
Five-Factor model (Herzberg & Roth, 2006), we can observe a partial
correspondence between our three-cluster types and the classical typology of Block
(1995). Based on the results from this study it would be a fallacy of nomenclature to
label, for instance, the passive-overcontrolled type as overcontrolled on the basis of
its specific profile characteristics. So, while the resilient type is practically identical
as a psychological profile with that seen in other studies and the strain type seems to
be a particular subtype of the theoretical undercontrolled type, the passive-
overcontrolled type is not similar to clusters described in other research, even
though certain similarities and correspondences do exist.
From such a graphical representation as the one provided in Figure 3 we
can appreciate the richness and the diversity of types assigned to the same class. For
instance, we are able to notice at least three different undercontrolled subtypes: the
strain type (N+, E+, A-), closer to the choleric temperament or to borderline
personality disorder (self-regulating failure due to negative affect), the prototypical
undercontrolled type (E+, C-), closer to the demonstrative, impulsive-hedonistic
type and to histrionic personality disorder (self-regulating failure due to excessive
reward seeking, immediate positive stimuli), and the undercontrolled type obtained
in the German sample (N+, A-, C-), more similar to the antisocial personality (self-
regulating failure due to inconsistent aims and lack of perseverance). It is worth
noting that all three of these personality disorders belong to the B cluster according
to the DSM IV classification (Widiger & Frances, 2002). Additionally, it can be
seen that the three undercontrolled subtypes are plotted rather closed to each other,
which implies they have a more similar profile than other types / subtypes of
personality. This chart could also provide a clue that differences in the profile of
various prototypes which were given an identical name actually represent various
subtypes of a theoretical type, which have been obtained solely on statistical basis
due to the characteristics of the samples which were analyzed. But most important,
the chart suggest that we are dealing with fuzzy types and not with discrete types,
due to several possible subtypes that might result due to variance in sample
characteristics of various studies.
Unfortunately, a limitation of this analysis is that no independent measures
of resiliency and control were included in the study; further research would
therefore be needed to validate such a taxonomy that suggests that personality types
based on the five-factor model of personality could be classified on their self-
STUDY 2
The high level of heterogeneity found in the third cluster, the passive-
overcontrolled type, might lead us to expect that to discard the five-cluster solution
in favour of the three-cluster solution would result in a loss of valuable information
about the behavior of the adult members of a specific type.
In order to verify in what manner a five-cluster solution may furnish
additional information of practical relevance we carried out a second study, a non-
experimental one, to test whether specific associations exist between the various
identified types and certain self-reported behavioral preferences. This procedure
was aimed at external validation of the cluster solutions in the first study. For
instance, Caspi et al. (1997) found that undercontrolled people tend to engage more
frequently in health-risky behaviors. Similarly, Atkins and Hart (2008) looking at
data from a longitudinal project found that those who were identified as
undercontrolled at 5 or 6 years of age were more likely than those resilient or over-
controlled to have sexual intercourse before the age of 16 and they were more likely
to report they had hurt someone seriously at least once in the past year at the age of
12 (Atkins, 2007). Moreover, a significant inverse association was found between
likelihood of childhood unintentional injury and the overcontrolled prototype score
(Atkins and Matsuba, 2008), while children identified as resilients at 5 or 6 years of
age were more likely than children characterized by the overcontrolled and
undercontrolled personality types to volunteer 8 and 10 years later in adolescence
(Atkins, Hart, & Donnelly, 2005). Finally, Vollrath, and Torgersen (2008) showed
that types combining high extraversion and low constraint (a personality profile that
is similar to the one for the undercontrolled type in our five cluster solution),
engage in more risky health behaviors such as tobacco cunsumption or a higher
number of new sexual parteners. So, unless a shift of the analysis from the three-
cluster solution to the five-cluster solution can be found to offer additional
information related to the self-reported behaviors of the people surveyed we should
consider the more parsimonious solution, the three-cluster one, to be better. On the
other hand, if segmentation of the population into five psychological types could be
shown to have a superior predictive value then that would provide support for the
view that the five-cluster solution is to be preferred.
METHOD
Participants
Data used in the second study was collected from 150 participants in the course of a
routine annual psychological examination that was obligatory for compliance with
work safety legislation. After application of the screening procedures described in
study one, 133 valid observations remained for analysis. The resulting sample
comprised 94 males and 39 females, with ages between 18 and 59 (M = 41,
SD = 11.4), classified according to three levels of education: those who had not
completed high school (46%), those who had completed high school (45%), and
those with university degrees (9%).
Instruments
Two sets of collected data are relevant: the DECAS Personality Inventory and the
Bio-data. This latter refers to responses to seven questions examining self-reported
behavioral aspects: (1) general state of health (alcohol and tobacco consumption,
perception of state of health) and (2) childhood upbringing and general lifestyle (the
strictness with which they were raised in childhood, the regularity of their practice
of sport; exercise of voting rights; frequency of change of employment).
The bio-data was measured either nominally or ordinally (with only a few
categories). For instance, in relation to smoking habits, the alternative answers to
the question: “How many cigarettes do you usually smoke per day?” were a) none, I
do not smoke b) up to 10 cigarettes per day; c) more than 10 cigarettes per day.
Procedure
The instruments were applied in groups of 15 to 25 participants. The remaining 133
valid cases were cluster analyzed directly by a k-means nonhierarchical method,
using as cluster seeds the values found in our first study that had been conducted on
a national representative sample.
Results
First, we checked whether the resulting clusters in the second study were similar to
those obtained in the first study. We found support for the existence of a similarity
between the profiles obtained in the first study and those observed in the second
(e.g. the similarity between the strain type in the five-cluster solution obtained in
the first study and the strain type for the five-cluster solution in the second study
was .90 in terms of Euclidian distance dissimilarity and .83 in terms of profile
similarity correlation). For all cases, the similarity between a specific type obtained
in the first study and the corresponding type obtained in the second study met the
acceptance criteria [Pearson correlations (r) between two profiles beyond .70 and
Euclidian distances (d) less than 1.50)], a result showing a good replicability of the
types.
Table 3.
Comparison of health, psychological and social status for various prototypes (values refer to
percentages)
** for ps < .01, * for ps < .05, and † for ps < .10
R – Resilient; S – Strained; PO – Passive-Overcontrolled; U – Undercontrolled;
O – Overcontrolled; P – Passive
For illustrative purposes, the Cramer’s V values indicating various intensities for effect sizes
are: V = .10 (small effect); V = .30 (medium effect); and V = .50 (strong effect) (Ferguson,
2009; Sava and Maricutoiu, 2007).
DISCUSSION
GENERAL DISCUSSION
The purpose of the research reported in this paper is to validate, internally and
externally, a typology of personality based on the dimensions of the five-factor
model, measured in a large sample representative of the Romanian population. The
results show that five types can be identified and this solution has been shown to
have both external and internal validity.
The first three discussed types (resilient, overcontrolled and
undercontrolled types) are consistent findings of research studies of personality
typology based on the Big-Five model (Asendorpf & Van Aken, 1999; Barbaranelli,
2002; Block, 1995; de Fruyt, Mervielde, & van Leeuwen, 2002; Herzberg & Roth,
2006). However the only two studies conducted using nationally representative
samples (the present study and Herzberg & Roth’s 2006 study) converged on the
view that a five-cluster solution may possibly furnish a better typology based on the
five-factor personality model. One possible explanation for this is that when using
national representative samples which are more heterogeneous more clusters can
emerge, reflecting the higher degree of type heterogeneity.
Fruyt, Mervielde, and van Leeuwen (2002) have argued, research in this
area should aim to go beyond simple replication of the three major personality types
described by Block (1995) and should try to identify new personality types, beyond
those already described. Hertzberg and Roth (2006) were among the first authors to
propose an extension of personality prototypes beyond the big-three types to
include confident and reserved types also. In the present study we obtained two
additional antagonistic types: the strain type and the passive type, where the later
has some correspondence with the reserved type described by Herzberg and Roth
(2006) and to a resilient subtype which was entitled - well adapted - found by
Boehm, Asendorpf, and Avia (2002).
Based on psychological similarities between various profiles, as presented
in figure 3, we may tentatively reply to a question raised by Avdeyeva and Church
(2005) that is, whether resiliency and control are actually orthogonal dimensions as
suggested by Block and Block (1980) or whether the relationship between resilience
and control is in fact quadratic (e.g., optimal resiliency is associated with an optimal
level of control) as has been argued by Asendorpf et al. (2001). It would appear that
all types based on a five-factor personality model can be delineated by combining
various degrees of control and resilience, treated as orthogonal dimensions. For
instance, the reserved, the passive and the passive-overcontrolled types can be seen
as exhibiting at different degrees of intensity the type described by Block and Block
(1980) as resilient overcontrolled.
However we should also point out the real difficulties that exist in
replicating any standard typology of personality that is based solely on cluster
analysis. As Costa et al. (2002) have noted, the same familiar cluster names have
been used in presentation of the results from various studies so as to conform to the
nomenclature in reports of previous findings even though the Five-Factor
personality profile pattern was not similar for the specific types being so labeled. A
possible solution to this limitation is proposed by Herzberg and Roth (2006). They
suggested that the cluster analysis should only be applied to representative
population samples, as the initial step to extract the appropriate number of clusters
in a given country. Then the next step would be to assign the participants to a
specific type on the sole basis of population parameters (rather than on the basis of
any new sample), using discriminant functions as a statistical method rather than
cluster analysis. This approach is intended enhance cluster replicability and reduce
the possibility of labeling error. However, regardless the cluster solution found in a
study, it seems that all resulted personality types can be interpreted as a specific
combination of the two self-regulation processes: the degree of resilience and the
degree of control. In our case, the strain type shares some characteristics of the
undercontrolled type, but the former has even a lower degree of resilience.
Similarly, the passive type shares many characteristics of the resilient prototype, but
is less assertive and active.
A further point needs to be made concerning these two new clusters,
namely the strain type and the passive type. These types seem to be novel, with no
previous findings fitting the personality profiles associated with them. We are
therefore left with a need to consider whether our finding of two novel types might
be accounted for due to features peculiar to this study. The two features concerned
are: the indigenous (DECAS) personality measure used and the specific Romanian
cultural context. Since the DECAS personality inventory has been previously
validated as an adequate measure for use in investigating the standard five-factor
personality model, it is more likely to be the cultural context that is the source of the
two previously unremarked clusters. In this regard, Adamesteanu (2008) affirmed
that there are two types of Romania (the urban – forward-looking and culturally
orientated to the West, which is modernized, and the rural – which is less
developed, and more traditional and archaic). Post hoc analysis has revealed that a
significantly higher percentage of people categorized as strain type live in a rural
milieu (61%) and a similar tendency is seen in regard to the passive type. A
contrasting situation has been found for the more commonly recognized clusters, in
particular for the resilient type and for the undercontrolled type, of whom
respectively 59% and 60% live in an urban milieu. Based on the above figures, it
may be the case that these two novel types, which go beyond the Big-Three
typology, manifest as a consequence of sensitivity of the prototype patterns to the
cultural context.
Another limitation is related to our decision to conduct our analysis without
taking into account the potential implications of gender differences that some
previous findings (e.g., Pulkkinen, 1996) suggest may be significant. We based our
decision on technical grounds, being primarily interested in ensuring an appropriate
sample size to overcome most of the technical limitations that would have been
involved had we run a cluster analysis on a smaller sample.
A possible additional limitation of the study is the decision to use an
alternative instrument for the Big Five, one that may underestimate the type
equivalence across cultures. However, when we compare our results with those
obtained in the German population (Herzberg & Roth, 2006), we can see that we
have replicated a five-cluster solution rather well. This resulted even though we
employed a different means of operationalizing the five factor personality model as
a strategy to increase the construct validity of these findings, in particular the
finding that a five-cluster solution might be a better solution than a three-cluster
solution when using nationally representative samples.
Another limitation is the decision to extract the clusters solely on the five
global scores which affects the reliability of the results (Mandara, 2003). The use of
another instrument such as the NEO PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992), which provides
scores for facets in addition to the five global dimensions, might have offered a
better way to develop reliable clusters. However, in this research we found a very
good replication of the five clusters obtained on a Romanian representative sample
in the second study that was done on an independent sample consisting of 150
adults. This finding supports the suggestion made by Herzberg and Roth (2006) that
in order to get replicable clusters in subsequent studies based on convenient
samples, it is recommended to use as cluster seeds those values that represent
population parameters resulted from a probabilistic representative sample.
In a similar vein, this paper provided limited opportunities for discussing
the personality types derived from the Big Five model through the lenses of other
theoretical approaches [see for instance Cloninger (1994); Thomas, Chess, & Birch
(1970)]. For instance, the concept of novelty seeking proposed by Cloninger (1994)
can be theoretically linked to the undercontrolled type, while harm avoidance seems
to be a less developed trait among people perceived as resilient. Future studies
should empirically explore the link between the five-factor model derived typology
and other relevant theories regarding personality and temperament.
Despite these limitations, using personality types does have its utility in
that the convenient labels used to summarize combinations of traits make sense to
many practitioners as well as to naïve specialists working in human resources and in
clinical contexts, even if such labels might have less predictive power than an
approach using traits (Costa et al., 2002; Roth & von Collani, 2007). In an
analogous way the four personality types proposed by Gough (1987, as cited in
Pitariu et al., 2006) do not have as much predictive power as the California
Personality Inventory’s twenty folk scales on which those types are based.
However, due to their practical utility, they have been widely accepted within the
community of practitioners and are now routinely included in electronically
generated reports. In short, despite the existence of some technical limitations in
their use, personality types have proved user-friendly for the average consumer of
psychological testing without surrendering too much of the predictive power which
is to be found in a dimensional approach. Based on these practical considerations,
we found it useful to look at different personality types on the basis of a five-factor
personality model. The explanatory complexity and ecological validation of such an
approach brings additional benefits, allowing us to move beyond a classical
perspective that remains focused on the one-dimensional treatment of personality.
To summarize our main findings, we can conclude that a choice of five,
rather than three, personality prototypes is more appropriate when seeking a
personality typology based on the five-factor personality model. This finding is
particularly relevant for studies where representative samples are used rather than
for studies based on convenient medium- or small-sized samples. It is also the case
that the five-cluster solution provides a better fit when care is taken to avoid the
bias towards a smaller cluster number solution that some technical decisions can
otherwise generate.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
REFERENCES
Gramzow, R. H., Sedikides, C., Panter, A. T., Sathy, V., Harris, J., & Insko, C. A. (2004).
Patterns of self-regulation and the big five. European Journal of Personality, 18,
367-385.
Hair, J. F. Jr., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate data
analysis (5th Ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Herzberg, P. Y., & Roth, M. (2006). Beyond resilients, overcontrollers and undercontrollers?
An extension of personality prototype research. European Journal of Personality,
20, 5-28.
Huey, S. J., Jr., & Weisz, J. R. (1997). Ego control, ego resiliency, and the fivefactor model
as predictors of behavioral and emotional problems in clinicreferred children and
adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 404-415.
Hunsley, J., Lee, C. M., & Wood, J. M. (2003). Controversial and questionable assessment
techniques. In S. O. Lilienfeld, S. J. Lynn, & J. M. Lohr (Eds.), Science and
pseudoscience in clinical psychology (pp. 39-76). New York: Guilford.
Krueger, R. F. (1999). The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 56, 921-926.
Krueger, R. F., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2001). The higher-order structure of common
DSM mental disorders: internalization, externalization, and their connection to
personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 1245-1259.
Mandara, J. (2003). The typological approach in child and family psychology: A review of
theory, methods, and research. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 6(2),
129-146.
Pitariu, H. D., Iliescu, D., Tureanu, & V., Peleaşă, C. (2006). Inventarul Psihologic
California [California Personality Inventory]. Bucharest: Odyseea.
Pulkkinen, L. (1996). Female and male personality styles: A typological and developmental
analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1288-1306.
Rammstedt, B., Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Borkenau, P. (2004). Resilients,
overcontrollers, and undercontrollers. The replicability of the three personality
prototypes across informants. European Journal of Personality, 18(1), 1-14.
Robins, R. W., John, O. P., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1996).
Resilient, overcontrolled, and undercontrolled boys: three replicable personality
types. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(1), 157-171.
Roth, M., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2007). The resilient type: ‘simply the best’ or merely an
artifact of social desirability? Psychology Science, 49(2), 150-167.
Roth, M., & von Collani, G. (2007). A head to head comparison of big-five types and traits
in the prediction of social attitudes. Journal of Individual Differences, 28(3), 138-
149.
Sava, F. A. (2009). Maladaptive schemas, irrational beliefs, and their relationship with the
Five-Factor personality model. Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral
Psychotherapies, 9(2), 135-147.
Sava, F. A. (Ed.) (2008). Inventarul de personalitate DECAS [DECAS Personality
Inventory]. Timisoara: ArtPress.
Sava, F. A., & Maricutoiu, L. P. (2007). PowerStaTim 1.0. – Manualul utilizatorului
[PowerStaTim 1.0 – The user’s manual]. Timişoara: Editura Universitatii de Vest.
Schnabel, K., Asendorpf, J. B., & Ostendorf, F. (2002). Replicable types and subtypes of
personality: German NEO-PI-R versus NEO-FFI. European Journal of Personality
Special Issue, 16, S7–S24
SPSS (2001). SPSS Base 11.0. User’s guide. Chicago, USA: SPSS Inc.
Thomas, A., Chess, S., & Birch, H. (1970). The origin of personality. Scientific American,
223, 102-109.
Urbana, S. (2004). Essentials of psychological testing. New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons.
Vollrath, M.E., & Torgersen, S. (2008). Personality types and risky health behaviors in
Norwegian students. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49, 287-292.
Widiger, T. A., & Frances, A. J. (2002). Toward a dimensional model for the personality
disorders. In P. T. Costa Jt. and T. A. Widiger (eds.), Personality disorders and the
five-factor model of personality (2nd ed.) (pp. 23-44). Washignton, DC: American
Psychological Association.