Goldenberg 2021
Goldenberg 2021
Goldenberg 2021
552
Perspectives on Science 553
and thereby dictated its solution. The problem has been framed as a conflict of
science versus ignorance, the former unproblematic and the latter entirely flawed.
I propose that while we can accept that the public is prone to misunderstand-
ing science and failing to appreciate relative risk, these characteristics do not
explain vaccine hesitancy. Instead, I will draw from social scientific research
into parental attitudes regarding vaccination as well as information sourced
from vaccine-critical parent advocacy websites to reveal an alternate framing
of the question of vaccine safety. What gets described as “public rejection of
1.1. Outline
After offering a brief background of the notorious Lancet study that inner-
vated contemporary vaccine hesitancy over the MMR vaccine, specifically,
and other vaccines more generally, I will turn to the task of reframing vac-
cine hesitancy from its current characterization as emblematic of public
misunderstanding of science. To do this, I will first outline the standard
characterization of and response to vaccine hesitancy consistently presented
in the health and science literature, which, I will show, presumes an igno-
rant public. I will then challenge this account of the vaccine hesitancy
problem and deny that the public is ignorant. I will demonstrate how
the official response has thereby been misguided and numerous opportu-
nities to correct the problem through effective public outreach have been
missed. I will then reframe the problem of vaccine hesitancy as a problem
of trust and strained expert-lay relations due to poor communication prac-
tices. I close by briefly situating my analysis of vaccine hesitancy within a
vigorous line of criticism of the “knowledge deficit model” for explaining
conflicts between science and the public.
2. Some commentators still acknowledge that there were precipitating factors leading
up to the explosive reaction to Wakefield et al.’s 1998 study. Fitzpatrick, for instance, notes
that a few years prior to Wakefield and colleagues’ first suggestion of an MMR-autism link,
“there were already signs that MMR was in trouble” (2004a, p. 11). The UK had expe-
rienced its first decline in MMR vaccine uptake (from a 92 percent average to roughly
91 percent) in 1997 following bad press over the 1994 “Operation Safeguard” school immu-
nization program that offered a combined measles-rubella vaccine. There had also been
publicity in 1995 for Wakefield and colleagues’ earlier work suggesting a link between
measles or the measles vaccine and inflammatory bowel disease (Fitzpatrick 2004a,
pp. 11–12).
556 Public Misunderstanding and Vaccine Hesitancy
the study (Editors of the Lancet 2010); London Times investigative reporter
Brian Deer revealed that Wakefield had fabricated his data and exposed
this fraud in a 3-part report commissioned by and published in the British
Medical Journal (Deer 2011a; 2011b; 2011c). At each point of damning
revelation of impropriety and serious scientific misconduct, public officials
anticipated a sea change in public attitudes towards vaccination; this atti-
tudinal shift never materialized. To illustrate, a May 2013 USA Today
headline read, “Measles Surge in U.K. Years after Flawed Research”
5. For an analysis of the influence of Jenny McCarthy on vaccine hesitancy, see Largent
2012, pp. 138–48.
558 Public Misunderstanding and Vaccine Hesitancy
6. This thesis permits the increasingly popular option among parents of a modified or
“alternative” vaccine schedule rather than indiscriminate rejection of all vaccines. Some
vaccines could be eliminated, combined vaccines could be unbundled, and vaccines could
be introduced more slowly. See for instance, “Dr. Bob’s Alternative Vaccine Schedule” pro-
moted by best-selling author and physician, Dr. Robert Sears (Sears 2007).
Perspectives on Science 559
Taking on both the MMR-autism thesis and the alternative thesis that
autism is caused by the mercury-based preservative, thimerosol, found in
vaccines with inactivated viruses (such as polio and pertussis), the authors
review twenty epidemiological studies that uniformly fail to make an autism-
vaccine association. They highlight the reliability of the findings and the
significance of these studies’ convergent conclusion. They note that
[t]hese studies have been performed in several countries by many
capacity and found it to far exceed the roughly two dozen vaccine antigens
that he or she receives as part of routine childhood vaccination. Knowing
that antibodies, the component of the immune system most capable of pro-
tecting against infection, are made by B cells, and that B cells make anti-
bodies against only one epitope (an immunological unit), the calculation
can be made by estimating the “number of B cells in the bloodstream
[against] the average number of epitopes contained in a vaccine, and the
rapidity with which a sufficient quantity of antibodies could be made
7. Those estimates and the detailed calculations are reviewed in Offit et al. 2002.
8. Offit made the now-infamous 100,000 vaccines argument in response to Dr. Sears’s
claim that the combination-MMR booster should be withheld until age five when the im-
mune system is more mature. Offit hoped to prove, to the contrary, that vaccines given in
the first year of life induce an excellent immune response. Instead the 100,000 vaccines
comment raised the ire of many vaccine hesitators and deniers, who perceived Offit as being
insensitive and uncaring towards vulnerable children. See Huff 2012.
9. Gerber and Offit (2009) described the three popular vaccine danger theses as “shift-
ing” because, by their account, the anti-vaccine movement has shifted public fear and at-
tention from one theory to another as evidence mounted against any one of them and
threatened to undermine it.
10. This theory of public misunderstanding of the science has been stated explicitly in
some instances. For example, the 2008 New York Times headline, “Measles Cases Grow in
Number, and Officials Blame Parents’ Fear of Autism” (Harris 2008).
Perspectives on Science 561
seem, is a fearful public who is unable to integrate this evidence into their
worldview.
A 2003 editorial on vaccine hesitancy co-authored by members of the
Department of Vaccines and Biologics at the World Health Organization
explicitly endorses this disparaging view of the public (Clements and
Ratzan 2003). The authors describe the British public as “misled and con-
fused” by anti-vaccine misinformation. They write:
11. An ESRC commissioned report on science, the public, and the media, which com-
prehensively reviewed media coverage of the MMR-autism debate in 2,214 newspaper, ra-
dio, and television reports from January to September 2002, and surveyed over 1,000
British residents, came to this same conclusion. See Hargreaves et al. 2003.
12. Offit explained to a reporter for Baby Talk magazine that vaccines are “under fire”
due to their success. He is quoted saying “It’s the natural evolution of a vaccine program”
(Howard 2005).
562 Public Misunderstanding and Vaccine Hesitancy
plateau. In the final stage, vaccine fear continues to rise and immunization
rates fall. Rates of preventable disease then increase, as we see now in mea-
sles, mumps, and pertussis outbreaks in the United Kingdom and mainland
Europe, USA, and Canada. Eventually, we return to something the like first
phase (Chen and Hibbs 1998).
The graphic conveys a passive public, motivated by fear rather than
sound judgment, lacking in the critical thinking abilities needed to, first,
appreciate the long-term benefits of vaccines, second, question the motives
and opinions of dissenters, and third, resist the emotional sway of fear
mongering and empathic connection with devastated parents.
Of course, those same epistemic vices structure vaccine compliance as
well. This has implications for how public health outreach efforts are orga-
nized. Health officials widely recognize the importance of public support in
achieving public health goals, and so the importance of garnering the public
trust and engaging the public are more than academic aspiration or political
promise. Even Clements and Ratzan finish their disparaging assessment of
the “misled and confused” public with a quick nod to the current vogue of
science and the public:
Because of these and other potential problems in communicating
with the public, professionals somehow need to draw them into a
participatory process in any risk ommunication efforts. (Clements
14. Historian of science Naomi Oreskes entertained this option in recent news editorials
on public resistance towards the overwhelmingly-strong climate change consensus offered by
the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change, National Academy of Sciences, American
564 Public Misunderstanding and Vaccine Hesitancy
that some of the previously secure relations of trust between science and the
public that gave consensus statements their epistemic weight in the eyes of
the lay public no longer hold. Our discussion so far has highlighted how gov-
ernment bodies were quick to accept the first explanation.
The scientific and policy establishment’s casting of the public as igno-
rant absolves these institutions of listening to the concerns of anxious par-
ents. I will now challenge the characterization of the public as ignorant or
resistant to science. I will not apologize for the general public’s predictable
Meteorological Society, the American Geophysical Union, and the American Association
for the Advancement of Science. In these writings, she attempted to clarify what the con-
sensus represents (a justified majority opinion rather than unanimous agreement), the rig-
orous analysis with which the climate change conclusion was reached, and why the public
should not be concerned by a few outlier scientists that challenge the consensus (Oreskes
2004a; 2004b).
15. Hobson-West (2007) found this language of individualized needs of the child being
heavily used by British Vaccine-Critical Groups (including JABS) in her interviews with
Perspectives on Science 565
The interviewees and survey respondents, who came from both working-
and middle-class backgrounds and subscribed to a range of political views,
were asked about their perceptions of vaccine safety, where they turned for
advice and support, and finally, how they intended to act on the options of
either vaccinating, not vaccinating, or paying out-of-pocket for an alter-
native (spread-out, reduced, or unbundled) vaccine schedule at a private
clinic.
Parents, with their copious reflections based on experience and observa-
broad characteristics such as the child’s birth timing, maturity, sleep pat-
terns, and behavior (Yaqub et al. 2014). Some even worried about the pos-
sibility of unknown and undetected “weaknesses” in a child, which can be
understood to signify fear of even a slim chance of serious adverse events
(Leach and Fairhead 2007, pp. 58–9).
These personalized accounts do not align with current scientific under-
standing of immune response. But this effort by parents to figure out their
own children’s risk of adverse events should not be read as ignorance of
Parent advocacy groups like JABS, on the other hand, engage parents on
their own terms. These groups were founded on the belief that parents
know their children best, and thereby have insight into their children’s
health not afforded to physicians and medical scientists (Hobson-West
2007). Members of these groups share tales of having their concerns re-
garding vaccination dismissed by health professionals, and those claiming
that their children were harmed by vaccines were routinely ignored (Evans
et al. 2001; Leach 2005, p. 8; Kirby 2006, pp. 9–31). While some accept
the points where health promotion agencies ought to be engaging its pub-
lic, rather than the current practice of defining the problem and the solu-
tion for the supposedly ignorant public. It should be little surprise that
public outreach efforts are not changing public perception. To interpret
vaccine hesitancy as a misperception of the probabilities of harm is to ig-
nore the normative dimensions of risk assessment. Trivializing public con-
cern as confused “risk perception” also damages public trust, the very
ingredient needed for effective health promotion efforts (Wynne 1998).
The case for trust and epistemic dependency being crucial for the proper
functioning of science has already been well argued with respect to the
internal relations of science (see Hardwig 1985, 1991). Complex scientific
knowledge cannot be produced in isolation by careful self-reliant inquirers,
as there is too much for one person to know. Specialization becomes nec-
essary, thus creating a need for trust in the truthfulness, honesty, and
integrity of the researchers with whom one collaborates.16 The vulnerabil-
ity that this trust places us in (insofar as we can be potentially misled by
8. Conclusion
In this investigation into vaccine hesitancy, I challenged the orthodox
reading of the problem as stemming from public misunderstanding of sci-
ence and anti-science sentiments. I proposed that while the lay public will
suffer from some knowledge deficits with respect to the complexity of vac-
cine science, it is incorrect to assume that this explains vaccine hesitancy,
or that this hesitancy amounts to the public’s rejection of scientific claims.
Instead, concerned parents approach the question of vaccine safety differ-
ently than does the scientific establishment. This realization sheds new
light on why concerted efforts to reform public attitudes towards vaccines
have failed so far. By presuming the public is ignorant of the science, and
thereby directing outreach efforts at educating the public, health outreach
efforts are misdirected. The pervasive and reinforcing assumption that the
public only hesitates because it is ignorant shields science and government
institutions from examining their own practices with respect to earning
and maintaining the public trust. I share Brunk’s (2006) position that
those agencies demonstrate a knowledge deficit of their own when they
evade this self-scrutiny. In rereading the supposedly ignorant public, I
highlight the importance of trust and dialogue for remediating supposedly
intractable conflicts between science and society.
lay expert, and finally, public engagement models. The latter is the most desirable model
by their account.
20. The House of Lords claims to have shifted its focus from public misunderstanding,
articulated in the Bodmer report, to a communicative approach. A 2004 publication reads:
While the themes the Bodmer report deals with are still of crucial importance today—not
least to encourage young people to study and develop an interest in science—things
have moved on since this time. The public understanding of science approach has
been questioned as a deficit model of understanding. The implied relationship that
support for science can be achieved through better communication overlooks the fact
that different groups may frame scientific issues differently. (House of Lords 2004, p. 11)
Perspectives on Science 575
References
Atkinson, Sharyl. 2008. “How Independent are Vaccine Defenders?”
CBSNews.com http://www.cbsnews.com/news/how-independent-are-
vaccine-defenders/
Best, James. 2011. “Vaccination a Victim of its own Success.” Sydney Morning
Herald 20 September 2011. http://www.smh.com.au/lifestyle/life/vaccination-
a-victim-of-its-own-success-20110920-1kixy.html#ixzz2nI7grryV (Accessed
January 3, 2014)
Latour, Bruno. 1987. Science in Action: How to Follow Scientists and Engineers
through Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Layton, David, Edgar Jenkins, Sally Macgill, and Angela Davey. 1993. In-
articulate Science? Perspectives on the Public Understanding of Science and Some
Implications for Science Education. Nafferton: Studies in Education Ltd.
Leach, Melissa. 2005. “MMR Mobilisation: Science and Citizens in
a British Vaccine Controversy.” IDS Working Paper 247. Brighton:
Institute of Development Studies. http://www.drc-citizenship.org/