Articulo Misinformation
Articulo Misinformation
Articulo Misinformation
cite as: J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci, 2020, Vol. 75, No. 1, 96–103
doi:10.1093/geronb/gbz057
Advance Access publication May 10, 2019
Received: December 22, 2018; Editorial Decision Date: April 26, 2019
Abstract
Objectives: To better characterize the formation of false memories in older adults, we conducted a study using a French
adaptation of the misinformation paradigm from Loftus, Levidow, and Duensing (1992). We aimed to show higher false
memory production in older than in younger adults.
Method: One hundred and four younger adults (18–30 years) and 104 older adults (70–95 years) took part in the study.
Participants were presented with a misinformation paradigm through the viewing of a short video followed by a question-
naire containing misinformation about the film. After a short delay (45 min), they performed a recognition task.
Results: Contrary to our hypothesis, the results analyzed with a Welch t test did not reveal a greater misinformation effect
in older adults than in younger adults. Results were reanalyzed using the equivalence test which indicated that younger and
older adults are statistically equivalent and not statistically different.
Discussion: The equivalence test helped to clarify the contradictory results of the literature. Furthermore, such results show
the interest to reconsider misinformation effect in aging.
Keywords: Aging, Equivalence test, False memories, Misinformation effect
Our frenetic digital world makes it possible to disseminate widely than young adults. So, are older people more recep-
a multitude of information very quickly via a wide variety tive to misleading information? In light of this type of study,
of social networks, with an ever-increasing amount of er- one may ask what kind of memory trace of misleading in-
roneous information. This fake news or alternative facts formation is retained and whether older people are more
abound on the web and have become a routine phenom- inclined to integrate misleading information.
enon. For example, during the 3 months preceding the To memorize is to reconstruct reality rather than re-
U.S. presidential election in 2016, an analysis by Buzzfeed member information veridically. A new memory is a cre-
(Silverman, 2016) reported that the 20 most popular fake ation that implies reality but also our beliefs, knowledge
news items were shared 87,11,000 times while the 20 most about the world, and old memories. Thus, memories are
important political facts were shared 73,67,000 times. In a vulnerable to distortions (e.g., Dehon, 2012; Schacter,
recent study, Guess, Nagler, and Tucker (2019) investigated Guerin, & St Jacques, 2011). Distortions are a normal phe-
Facebook sharing of fake news and examined the charac- nomenon that can occur in adults and are amplified in older
teristics of those who shared it. They observed those over adults (e.g., Lövdén, 2003; Schacter, Koutstaal, & Norman,
65 years disseminated these alternative facts much more 1997). Studying distortions consists of considering the
© The Author(s) 2019. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America. All rights reserved. 96
For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected].
Journals of Gerontology: PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCES, 2020, Vol. 75, No. 1 97
adequacy between the original event and the memory of a recognition test after a variable delay. Today, the score
the event. One way to study the malleability of memory is calculation method is based on errors at the recognition
to investigate false memories. The term “false memories” test, considering both the number of errors after receiving
refers to wrongly but sincerely remembering events distant neutral and misleading information. A misinformation ef-
from reality (Roediger & McDermott, 1995). Thus, false fect is obtained when the errors are more numerous after
memories contain some distortion of real events. receiving misleading information than after receiving neu-
Two main paradigms are used to study the produc- tral information (Roediger & Geraci, 2007). Thus, the mis-
tion of false memories in a laboratory setting. On the one information effect is considered as a type of false memories
hand, in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm (DRM; obtained under the specific conditions of this paradigm.
Deese, 1959; Roediger & McDermott, 1995), participants Several explanations have been developed to account
study lists of words (e.g., slipper, sock, heel) semantically for the misinformation effect observed in younger adults
and the misleading information share a global meaning. postevent questions or neither. One unexpected finding
Verbatim makes the original information unique and dif- was that older adults were less susceptible than younger
ferent from the misleading information. Both gist and adults to the misinformation effect. The authors assumed
verbatim retrieval of misleading information support the that the final memory test (a source monitoring test) could
production of false recognition, while verbatim retrieval explain the greater misinformation effect in younger adults.
of original information rules out false recognition. It is ac- Indeed, the source-recognition test encourages participants
knowledged that older adults tend to base their answers on to examine specific details and promotes source informa-
global representation of events (Brainerd & Reyna, 2015). tion identification in older adults. According to the authors,
Without access to the verbatim, it is very difficult to distin- without this type of test, older adults’ answers would be
guish the original event from the misleading event. If older based on familiarity and misleading information would
adults have no access to the verbatim of the original event, be as familiar as the original information. Additionally,
Blanchard, & Vézina, 1990) and the French version of the participants performed the final recognition test. To con-
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II, Beck, Steer, & Brown, clude, they were thanked for their participation and fully
1998) revealed no signs of depression in the older adults debriefed.
(M = 6.69, SD = 5.61) and in the younger adults (M = 6.93,
SD = 6.39). The two groups were matched on Education
(t (206) = 1.86, p = .062, Cohen’s d = 0.258), that is, years Data Analysis
of education from the age of 6 (Myears = 13.72, SD = 2.22 A Welch’s t test was conducted to analyze correct
in the younger adults, Myears = 12.84, SD = 4.28 in the recognitions. Next, an equivalence test was run to statis-
older group). tically prove an absence of age-related difference. Proving
a significant difference between two groups A and B is a
Table 1. Illustration of the Terminology Used by Lakens, Scheel, and Isager (2018)
Conclusion of the Reject H0 Statistically Equivalent and Statistically Statistically Equivalent and Not
Equivalence test Different (SE and SD) Statistically Different (SE ans NSD)
Not reject H0 Not Statistically Equivalent and Not Statistically Equivalent and Not
Statistically Different (NSE and SD) Statistically Different (NSE and NSD)
Note: h0 denotes the null hypothesis in the Student’s or Welch’s t test (i.e. h0: μ = 0 against h1: μ ≠ 0) and H0 denotes the null hypothesis in the equivalence test (i.e.,
d1 or d2 is close to 0. All the analyses were done with R To improve inference about the absence of an age-
software (R Core Team, 2017). related effect on the misinformation effect, we reanalyzed
our data using an equivalence test (Lakens, 2017; Lakens,
McLatchie, Isager, Scheel, & Dienes, 2018; Lakens, Scheel,
Results & Isager, 2018). We chose two previous studies allowing us
to define the smallest and the largest size of the misinforma-
Correct Recognitions tion effect: Auslander et al. (2017) highlighted a significant
First, the accuracy for the neutral questions was examined. misinformation effect for younger adults over older adults,
The correct responses provided for neutral questions at the whereas Roediger and Geraci (2007) showed a greater mis-
recognition test reveal the baseline of participants’ abilities information effect in older adults. Let µ = µolder – µyounger the
at an episodic memory task. There is no statistical differ- misinformation difference mean between older adults and
ence between the older (M = 3.35, SD = 0.77) and younger younger adults. Based on the Auslander and colleagues’
adults correct answers (M = 3.46, SD = 0.72) to neutral study, we can consider that the smallest size of interest is
questions, Welch t test showed t(206) = 1.11, p = .26, d1 = −0.431: they indicated misinformation effect with 50
d = 0.147. Unfortunately, we could not apply the equiva- younger adults and 37 older adult samples. Similarly, based
lence test on these data since neither Roediger and Geraci on the Roediger and Geraci study, we can set the biggest
(2007) nor Auslander et al. (2017) provided a suitable his- size of interest d2 = 0.581 (they built this result on 24 for
toric Misinformation effect. In order to characterize the both sample sizes). The selected sample sizes (n1 = n2 = 104)
misinformation effect, we followed the method proposed should have been large enough to detect this smallest effect
by Roediger and Geraci (2007), which considered the errors size with a t test.
at the recognition test for the neutral and the misleading For the equivalence test, we consider H0: μ < d1 (−0.43)
condition. The rate of errors for the neutral condition or μ > d2 (0.58) against H1: d1 ≤ μ ≤ d2. The first one-sided
may indicate a failure to retrieve the original information test indicates that we can reject effects as small as or smaller
(watched in the film) in memory and serves as a baseline than d1 = −0.43, t(205.99) = 1.65, p = .0498. Furthermore,
for the difficulties in retrieving the original information. It the second one-sided test shows we can reject effects as
is essential to know in what extent the participants have large as or larger than d2 = 0.58, t(206) = −5.64, p < .001.
a memory of the original event (Marche et al., 2002) be- These results indicate that younger and older adults are sta-
fore studying the integration of misleading information in tistically equivalent and not statistically different regarding
memory. In our opinion, this method is based on the most their susceptibility to misinformation.
rigorous and demanding criteria. To test for an age-related
difference in the misinformation effect, we thus computed
the score by subtracting errors in the neutral condition Discussion
from those in the misinformation condition, for younger The aim of the current study was to examine the misin-
and older adults separately. formation effect in aging. First, we examined the accuracy
A Welch t test was then conducted to test our hypothesis of memories for neutral questions in younger and older
of a larger misinformation effect in older than in younger adults to have a baseline of participant’s capacity at an ep-
adults. Contrary to our prediction, our results do not in- isodic memory test. We found no statistical difference in
dicate a significant misinformation effect for older adults correct recognitions between younger and older adults.
(M = 0.88, SD = 1.29) over younger adults (M = 1.14, Our results do not match those obtained by other authors
SD = 1.28), t(206) = −1.46, p = .146, d = −0.20. Thus, in sta- (Karpel, Hoyer, & Toglia, 2001; Roediger & Geraci, 2007)
tistical terms, according the Neyman–Pearson principle, the who found a decline in correct recognitions in older adults.
observed data provide no strong evidence against the null However, our results are in line with those obtained by
hypothesis (i.e., no misinformation effect); in other words, some authors who found that older adults were as good as
we have insufficient evidence to reject the null hypothesis. younger adults at recognizing an event (Cohen & Faulkner,
Journals of Gerontology: PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCES, 2020, Vol. 75, No. 1 101
1989; Loftus et al., 1992). These results are not surprising well as younger adults on the correct answers for neutral
given that participants performed a recognition task. They questions) 1 hr after viewing the film. Therefore, we suggest
had to choose between two answers provided in the test. that they have a strong memory trace of the misleading in-
This type of task involves decision-making processes to se- formation (which was presented 45 min before the recog-
lect the right answer and reject the wrong answer. This is a nition test) too. With reference to the fuzzy trace theory, it
less-demanding task than a recall task in which participants is thus possible that the verbatim trace of the original and
must use retrieval strategies to produce a response. As the misleading information was as strong in younger adults
mentioned by Fraundorf, Hourihan, Peters, and Benjamin as in older adults. Hence, younger and older adults could
(2019), the age-related difference in recognition tasks is be confronted with the same difficulty during the recogni-
unclear in the literature, while it is well known that older tion test; they must choose between two pieces of informa-
adults are weaker than younger adults on a recall task. In tion that they have encoded and for which they have an
Bayesian approach could become valuable new tools for Dehon, H. (2006). Variations in processing resources and resistance
psychologists to better understand cognition in adulthood to false memories in younger and older adults. Memory, 14,
and aging. 692–711. doi:10.1080/09658210600648456
Finally, this study shows that older adults are not dramat- Dehon, H. (2012). Illusory recollection: The compelling subjec-
ically impaired on certain cognitive tasks. Although aging tive remembrance of things that never happened. Insights
is accompanied by various increasing cognitive difficulties, from the DRM Paradigm. Psychologica Belgica, 52, 121–149.
doi:10.5334/pb-52-2-3-121
it is important to consider the processes that are preserved.
Dehon, H., & Brédart, S. (2004). False memories: Young and older
Thus, under the conditions of our study, there is no evi-
adults think of semantic associates at the same rate, but young
dence to suggest that the elderly adults are more suggestible
adults are more successful at source monitoring. Psychology and
to or influenced by false information than younger adults.
Aging, 19, 191–197. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.191
These results have a practical application, helping to define
detection in younger and older adults. Journal of Experimental Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, R. C. J. (1995). Fuzzy-trace theory: An
Psychology: Applied, 24, 180–195. doi:10.1037/xap0000138 interim synthesis. Learning and Individual Differences, 7, 1–75.
Johnson, M.-K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). doi:10.1016/1041-6080(95)90031-4
Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28. Reysen, S. (2006). Publication of nonsignificant results: A survey of
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3 psychologists’ opinions. Psychological Reports, 98, 169–175.
Karpel, M. E., Hoyer, W. J., & Toglia, M. P. (2001). Accuracy and qual- doi:10.2466/pr0.98.1.169-175
ities of real and suggested memories: Nonspecific age differences. Roediger, H. L., & Geraci, L. (2007). Aging and the misinformation
The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and effect: A neuropsychological analysis. Journal of Experimental
Social Sciences, 56, 103–110. doi:10.1093/geronb/56.2.P103 Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 321–334.
Lakens, D. (2017). Equivalence tests: A practical primer for t-tests, doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.2.321
correlations, and meta-analyses. Social Psychological and Roediger, H. L., Balota, D. A., & Watson, J. M. (2001). Spreading