First Pictures, Early Concepts - Early Concept Books
First Pictures, Early Concepts - Early Concept Books
First Pictures, Early Concepts - Early Concept Books
The Lion and the Unicorn, Volume 29, Number 3, September 2005, pp.
324-347 (Article)
The earliest picture books read to children are filled with pictures of
common objects—such as a ball, teddy bear, apple, chair, or dog—
printed on cardboard, plastic, wood, or cloth pages. Their titles may refer
to implied users (For Our Child, Baby’s First Book), depicted objects
(First Things, What Is That?), or the books’ pictures (First Pictures,
Pictures for the Little Ones). Sometimes the title stresses the act of
seeing (Come and See!, Look!) or that the book or objects belong to a
young child (My First Picturebook, That Is Mine). These books usually
do not contain much text, often just a single word denoting the objects.
[131.130.169.5] Project MUSE (2024-04-11 21:18 GMT) Vienna University Library
The Lion and the Unicorn 29 (2005) 324–347 © 2005 by The Johns Hopkins University Press
First Pictures, Early Concepts: Early Concept Books 325
Few researchers have shown serious interest in this book type. Marilyn
Apseloff and Perry Nodelman (21–35), and Gunter Kress and Theo van
Leeuwen (21–33) have made some promising attempts, but even Colin
Mills’ “Books for Younger Readers” and the pioneering accounts of
Dorothy Neal White in Books Before Five, Dorothy Butler in Babies
Need Books, and Nicholas Tucker in The Child and the Book hardly take
early concept books into consideration. Their lack of narrative structure
and static representation of everyday things seem to be reasons for the
neglect of this book type in picture book theory.
A closer look reveals that early concept books have prominent
relatives. Johann Amos Comenius’s Orbis Sensualium Pictus (1658) and
Friedrich Johann Justin Bertuch’s twelve-volume Bilderbuch für Kinder,
Picture Book for Children (1792–1830) are similar in that they display
pictures of everyday objects. However, those books were intended for
school children and convey an encyclopedic knowledge of the world.
Early concept books focus on young children and their environment.
They probably originated in the last two decades of the nineteenth
century. For lack of space, we will not venture into the history of early
concept books.
Our present study draws on a small corpus of twenty early concept
books from our private collection. These books come from Western
countries, with the oldest from 1890. Generally, early concept books are
not registered in many bibliographies of children’s literature, with the
exception of art historian Albert Schug, who noted seven early concept
books in a 1988 catalog of an exhibition of five centuries of children’s
books (12). Early concept books published before 1950 are now rare
since most were damaged by frequent use and not saved.
The outline of our study is as follows. To make the object of our study
more accessible, we describe three outstanding early concept books that
display characteristics of this type. We then discuss important visual
properties of early concept books, stressing the by no means trivial
character of the pictures. Next, we show some of the more important
linguistic aspects of early concept books. Finally, we focus on the role of
early concept books in the acquisition of literacy, venturing that they
have more to do with literature than initially perceived. While we rely on
theoretical sources from picture theory, linguistics, and literary theory,
we do not presuppose a fixed theoretical framework. Such a framework
does not currently exist and is a task for the future. Our analysis is guided
by the belief that a developmental approach, one that reflects the growing
cognitive abilities of the child that emerges from rich interactional
settings, will be most fruitful for the study of children’s literature.
326 Bettina Kümmerling-Meibauer and Jörg Meibauer
Figure 1. Shoe. Illustration from Mim Chindli. E neus Bilderbuech für die ganz
Chline (To My Child: A New Picture Book for the Little Ones) by an unknown
illustrator (Zürich: Rascher & Cie, c. 1923)
the right pages, the left pages are empty.4 The realistic photographs are
printed on art paper in a fine, halftone process. The objects are arranged
aesthetically. Generally, they are shown as single objects, sometimes in a
scene, like a still life. Most objects are against a white or black
background, but sometimes they have a realistic background, such as a
carpet. All the objects have light reflections and shadowy surfaces, but
those on a black background do not throw shadows.
Figure 2 is typical of the photographs in this book. Three sheets of
paper of child’s scribbles and a pencil are lying on top of each other. The
objects are against a black background, which contributes to their
isolation. Light reflections are on the pencil, while shadows are discern-
ible on the sheets. Even if the objects are static, their composition evokes
a kind of liveliness and spontaneity, as if a child pushed them apart
afterward. The sheet at the top is not presented as a whole object since it
bleeds into the edge of the photograph. This photograph shows an image
within an image, creating a meta-image with the scribbles on the sheets
of paper.
This analysis contradicts Steichen’s claim of objectivity and neutrality
of the photographs. Every photograph involves some degree of subjec-
tive choice through selection, arrangement of objects, photographic point
of view, lighting, and framing. Roland Barthes in Camera Lucida
differentiates between the denotative and connotative meaning of photo-
[131.130.169.5] Project MUSE (2024-04-11 21:18 GMT) Vienna University Library
Figure 2. Paper and pencil. Photograph by Edward Steichen from The First
Picture Book (New York: Fotofolio, 1991; first published 1930). © 1930, 1958.
Reproduced with kind permission by Carousel Research Inc. on behalf of
Joanna T. Steichen.
Figure 3. Ball. Illustration from Erste Bilder (First Pictures) by Dick Bruna
(Ravensburg: Otto Maier Verlag Ravensburg, 1987; first published 1973).
© 1969, Mercis bv. Reproduced with kind permission by the publisher.
are depicted in two out of three books. The objects can be categorized.
Toys are found in all three books. Two of the books contain objects from
the categories of food, household items, hygiene articles, clothes, dishes,
and furniture. Animals, another popular category in these types of books,
are in one of the sample three. While Mim Chindli shows a naturalistic
approach, Steichen’s photographs reveal the influence of New Realism.
Bruna’s abstract style is connected to pictograms and continues to
influence early concept books.
Most of the man-made objects—such as balls, shoes, dishes, or
dolls—have changed through the years. For instance, balls differ by size,
color scheme, and pattern. The changing fashion of clothes and shoes
reflects their cultural history. When are these objects important enough to
put in early concept books? In older concept books, objects such as a
coffee grinder, drum, or ribbon are often shown, but not in later books.
Objects such as a computer, television, and cordless phone replace them.
Although these three early concept books differ, they also have striking
similarities. A compilation of the essential features of early concept
First Pictures, Early Concepts: Early Concept Books 331
the empty background. The proportions are striking. The objects are
much smaller than those they represent. Because the background is
empty, a sense of proportion is unclear. Objects seem to be the same size,
even though they may have different actual sizes; a chair is bigger than
an apple.
A concise description of early concept books reveals that objects in
these books are not shown naturally. The actual objects are not outlined
in black, nor are they in bright rich colors without modulations. They do
not have just one surface or seem to float in an empty space. Because of
this stylized representation, most of the modern concept books can be
characterized by a certain degree of abstraction.6 Whether this style is
appropriate to the imperfect pictorial perception of young children
cannot be determined, due to the lack of studies. The supposed simplicity
of pictures in early concept books turns out to be problematic since even
these show an astonishing complexity.
Adults give young children early concept books assuming that pictures
can be understood more easily and directly than words. We want to show
that the understanding of pictures requires a knowledge of competencies
that have to be acquired. To understand that an image, which seems
simple and straightforward, is complex and artificial one must remember
that all pictures demand some kind of interpretation simply because there
is no picture that contains as much visual information as an object. This
[131.130.169.5] Project MUSE (2024-04-11 21:18 GMT) Vienna University Library
leads to the conclusion that even young children must possess some
abilities when interpreting pictures in early concept books. Four basic
skills of perception are necessary to understand these images.
Children must learn to distinguish a figure from the background. They
have to realize that the depicted object is relevant, rather than the
unicolored background. The world flows past in a succession of half-
perceived images, but here children must focus on one object. This
tendency is underlined by framing objects with a black line and the
emptiness of the background. Children are guided to focus their attention
on the objects whose contours are emphasized by black lines. Black lines
around depicted objects also appear in coloring books to mark objects to
be colored; the background serves as a negative space, as in early
concept books.
Another basic skill is realizing that lines, points, and colors have
meanings insofar as they are inseparable parts of the depicted object,
even if, in reality, it is not outlined in black. Lines do not just separate
figures from ground; they can do more, such as suggesting textures or
darken colors. Artists also use hatchings and light reflections, which
indicate a source of light and shadows. These artistic techniques are
conventional. Children must learn how to read them in artwork. Nodelman
First Pictures, Early Concepts: Early Concept Books 333
While playing this pointing and naming game, the adult asks the child
what the picture shows; the child has to answer correctly. If the child
gave the wrong answer, the parent might correct her. This pattern is
variable and extendable; for example, the child or the adult might make
animal noises. If children make mistakes, they are corrected. Looking at
picture books helps children in vocabulary acquisition. This is in line
with usage-based approaches to language learning suggested, by Michael
Tomasello in Constructing a Language.
Katrin knows that the depicted object is called a crocodile. What is
unclear is whether she is already able to obtain from this particular
picture and generalize about all crocodiles and whether she already has
some knowledge about crocodiles.
The process of acquiring the meaning of a single word is complex. We
will look at the word apple in detail because pictures of apples seem standard
in early concept books. In a more systematic and elaborate fashion, the
relevant aspects of the word apple may be illustrated as in figure 4.
We assume that children know a word when they have acquired a
consistent mapping between form and concept. A form-concept mapping
is called a lexeme, an element permanently stored in the child’s mental
First Pictures, Early Concepts: Early Concept Books 335
lexicon. (There are other elements in the lexicon besides words, such as
idioms). A concept is the set of properties of a lexeme that makes
reference to a referent possible. The referent (or denotatum) is the entity
a speaker refers to while saying a lexeme. When saying, “This is an
apple,” the speaker refers to a specific apple. Characteristics of an
APPLE are that it can be eaten, is a fruit, is round, has a stem, grows on
trees, and is red, yellow, or green. Conceptual knowledge allows us to
categorize things, to decide whether a given object is correctly referred to
with the lexeme apple or not. Conceptual knowledge gradually develops.
Even adults may have difficulties categorizing a fruit as an apple
versus a tomato. Research in prototype semantics has shown that
prototypes, the best examples of a category, are crucial for categoriza-
tion. A chair seems to belong in the category “furniture” rather than a
bench, and robins are in the category “bird” rather than penguins (Taylor,
38–55). In language acquisition, prototypes seem relevant for conceptual
development and lexical acquisition (Murphy, 317–84).
In categorical hierarchies such as “furniture,” “chair,” “kitchen chair,”
the category of “chair” is prototypical. Categories belonging to this
salient level are basic-level categories. These are more accessible than
other categories, and seem to play a prominent role in lexical acquisition
(Clark 51). Before learning the words kitchen chair and furniture, the
child probably will acquire the word chair.
Pictures are two-dimensional visual representations of referents. Pho-
tographs depict a concrete referent, while drawings depict a prototypical
referent. We make a further distinction between a picture and a mental
picture. A drawing depicts a red apple, but an individual may mentally
imagine a yellow apple. Knowledge of a concept and of a mental picture
336 Bettina Kümmerling-Meibauer and Jörg Meibauer
What types of pictures are found in early concept books, and are there
linguistic explanations for these choices?
In the following table, we have listed pictures in twenty early concept
books, published between 1890 and 2000, and organized them into
conceptual classes. The objects are ranked according to their frequency
of appearance in the twenty books. Almost all early concept books have
balls. This survey is far from representative, but it gives a good
impression of typical objects.
(3), toothbrush (3), hair slides (2), ribbon (2), bathtub, brush, washbasin,
toothpaste, washing bowl
clothes: shoes (14), mittens (2), hat (2), cap (2), pullover (2), socks
(2), trousers, bib, coat, pajamas
dishes: bowl (11), mug (10), spoon (10), eggcup (5), silverware (4),
glass (4), milk bottle (3), knife (2), salt cellar (2), penknife, cup, teapot,
soup cup
household items: telephone (5), basket (3), umbrella (3), bunch of
keys (2), clock (3), flowerpot (2), vase (2), bucket (2), saucepan (2),
scissors (2), bowl (2), matches (2), thread, watering can, hammer, coffee
grinder, broom and dustpan, coins, candlestick, nails, needle, purse, lock,
matchbox, torch, hot-water bottle, pliers, pincushion, thimble, padlock,
key rack
animals: cat (4), dog (3), fish (2), bird (2), duck, fly, rabbit, chicken,
cow, pig, wasp
furniture: crib (5), high chair (4), coat rack, wooden bench, lamp,
table, children’s chair
vehicles: car (3), train, plane, pram, ship
other items: pacifier (4), book (3), flower (3), watch (2), tree, letters,
house, toddler, medicine bottle, package, sun, pencil and paper, black-
board, newspaper
The early concept books Mim Chindli and The First Picture Book are
unusual in that they have more than ten pictures, most books have less.
This may be because of restricted cognitive capacities of young children
or lower production costs. Authors seem to strive for a mix of pictures
representing objects that belong to different conceptual classes: the
concentration on just one conceptual class—cars or animals—would lead
to a different book type.
Some objects have a meta-representative quality. The picture may
represent a toy animal, which, in a simplified way, represents a real
animal (Moerk 549). Sometimes pictures are ambiguous representations of
a real or toy animal, such as the dog in Bruna’s Erste Bilder. Many objects
have pictures on them, such as a cup with a picture of a chicken on it.
Based on this list, it is tempting to argue that the illustrators make
proposals as to what should constitute a prototype for the child. An apple
may be a prototypical fruit even for an adult, but a crib is certainly a
prototypical furniture only for a child. This is similar to the prototypicality
of shoes and telephones. In addition, it seems authors do not always
represent basic categories. For example, a high chair is not a basic
category, although it is relevant to a child. There is some evidence that
authors try to give conceptual information that is interesting from a
338 Bettina Kümmerling-Meibauer and Jörg Meibauer
children use the word bear to refer just to a specific teddy bear (Barrett
372).
Early concept books tell children that there is a conventional, adult
usage of expression that they should adapt. If an apple will repeatedly be
called apple, balls should probably not be used for apple. The informa-
tion about concepts is encoded in the pictures. Nodelman observes that
stereotypical pictures “operate somewhat like dictionary definitions of
objects—they express the essence of the type of object represented rather
than the specific nature of any given one such object” (27). If this is
correct, there is a correspondence between the prototypicality of the
depicted object (the apple) and the prototypicality of the way the object
is depicted (a picture of an apple).
When early concept books are read by children and parent together,
children acquire concepts that are pictorially mediated. They enlarge
their vocabulary that refers to objects in their surroundings and learn to
avoid mistakes.
relevant concept. Even if young children cannot read, they can grasp the
difference between type and visual representation of an object. As noted
by Apseloff, an early confrontation with printed letters emphasizes skills
of observation that lead to reading (65).
Adults can choose labels for represented objects when showing early
concept books without words. For example, an illustration of a stuffed
bear might be labelled bear, teddy bear, or just teddy, or an image of a
dog could be referred to as dog, doggie, or bow wow. Examining these
books with single words finds that those that function as basic level
categories are preferred, thereby stressing the prototypical properties of
the represented object. In comparison, particular terms such as robin
(instead of bird) or beagle (instead of dog) are not used. However,
Nodelman regards this convention merely as an act of labelling objects
(31), whereas Kress and van Leeuwen claim that the printed words serve
to “bestow similarity and order on the diverse heterogeneous world of
images” (23). Single words do not constitute a narrative, but they can be
regarded as a way of interpreting the illustrations and as a way to
encourage adults to weave their own story.
In contrast to Nodelman’s claim that pictures in early concept books
are not linear, it is striking how the objects unfold on a doublespread and
that they are often thematically connected to each other. This occurs
through similarity, contrast, or relationships.
With similarity, pictures are grouped together from the same concep-
tual class, spoon and mug, spade and bucket, comb and brush, or roll and
banana. Eleven early concept books from our corpus reflect this organi-
zational style throughout. Children are introduced to an understanding of
conceptual classes, preparing for more complex books.
The contrast style of organization has pictures from opposite concep-
tual classes—such as dog and car, ball and shoes, or telephone and teddy
bear. The contrast of conceptual classes constitutes a sort of visual
enigma, stimulating the viewer’s imagination to find out the reason for
the combination, which may be between animate and inanimate or in a
sequence of actions. To go on the playground children have to put on
shoes (ball vs. shoes). This calls into question Apseloff’s second point
that this book type generally does not stimulate the imagination.
This possible form of interaction shows certain similarities of organiz-
ing by relation that is rare in early concept books. Objects are combined
to establish a relation, for example watering can and flower. A more
complex ability is required. Children have to figure out how the objects
belong to each other. How is an apple connected to the apple tree? The
principle of relation is usually realized on the last doublespread of early
342 Bettina Kümmerling-Meibauer and Jörg Meibauer
Jörg Meibauer holds the Chair for German Linguistics at the Johannes
Gutenberg-University in Mainz (Germany). His research interests are in
the grammar-pragmatics-interface, word formation, and language ac-
quisition. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the sixteenth
IRSCL conference in Kristiansand, Norway, in July 2003.
Notes
1
Two editions are recorded. The first one has the subtitle: Es Buech, wo nüd
lad, für die ganz Chline, A Book Which Is Untearable, For The Little Ones
(1910). The second edition in 1923, consists of four, thick, cardboard pages with
some different objects.
2
In the 1991 edition, Edward Steichen’s daughter’s name has changed to
Mary Steichen Calderone. This reissue was published in association with the
Whitney Museum of American Art in New York and included an epilogue by
John Updike.
344 Bettina Kümmerling-Meibauer and Jörg Meibauer
3
According to the Dutch publishing house Mercis, which has the copyright
for this book, Bruna’s book was never published in the Netherlands. Mercis
claimed that the original colors of Bruna’s illustration were changed by the
German publisher Otto Maier Verlag. Since different color versions of Bruna’s
ball exist, this problem is difficult to resolve.
4
This arrangement also occurs in the Swiss German book. The similarity of
design can be regarded as a convention of older concept books.
5
A possible reason for this is the so-called whole object assumption that
suggests that young children assume words refer to whole objects, not to parts or
properties, as proposed by Ellen Markman in Categorization and Naming in
Children.
6
Concerning the abstract style, Tana Hoban’s famous Shapes and Things
(1970) consists of simple, black-and-white silhouettes, which is a further stage
in early concept books.
7
Goldsmith published a comprehensive research report about this topic; she
referred to psychological research that supports the thesis “that what is often
called ‘visual literacy’ cannot be taken for granted” (111). Maria Nikolajeva
provides a summary of relevant research in children’s literature in the Handbook
and Early Childhood Literacy (235–48).
[131.130.169.5] Project MUSE (2024-04-11 21:18 GMT) Vienna University Library
8
This idea already occurs in Nicholas Tucker’s The Child and the Book,
which shows the author’s interest in the cognitive and intellectual development
of children. A first step toward a classification of book types intended for small
children was undertaken in Kümmerling-Meibauer’s article on preschool books,
which will appear in the forthcoming Oxford Encyclopedia of Children’s
Literature.
Works Cited
Tucker, Nicholas. The Child and the Book: A Psychological and Literary
Exploration. Cambridge: Cambridge UP, 1990.
Wagner, Klaus R., and Sybille Wiese, eds. Teilkorpus KATRIN (1;5) im
Dortmunder Korpus der spontanen Kindersprache, erstellt von Annegret
Schwarze (Kindersprache, 11). Essen: Verlag Die Blaue Eule, 1996.
Wernhard, Hermann. Das ist meins. Ravensburg: Otto Maier Verlag, 1971.
_____. Mein erstes Buch. Ravensburg: Ravensburger, 1988.
White, Dorothy Neal. Books Before Five. Wellington: New Zealand Council for
Educational Research, 1954.