Construct Walkerwinter
Construct Walkerwinter
net/publication/6883224
CITATIONS READS
154 12,475
2 authors:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
Constructivist methods and Existential therapy: exploring how clients change in therapy from a constructivist perspective View project
All content following this page was uploaded by Beverly M Walker on 20 May 2014.
1
School of Psychology, University of Wollongong, Wollongong 2522, Australia;
email: [email protected]
2
School of Psychology, University of Hertfordshire, Hatfield AL10 9AB and Barnet,
Enfield and Haringey Mental Health NHS Trust, United Kingdom;
email: [email protected]
453
ANRV296-PS58-18 ARI 17 November 2006 1:32
METHOD . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 460
criminations, known as constructs, are bipo-
Grid-Based Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . 460
lar, with both poles necessary for understand-
Nongrid-Based Methods . . . . . . . . . . 462
ing of the discrimination. “Hot” cannot be un-
APPLICATIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 463
derstood except in relationship to “cold.” Our
Clinical Applications. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 463
actions reflect the application of these con-
Educational Applications. . . . . . . . . . 466
struct poles, as we behave in accordance with
Organizational Applications . . . . . . . 467
our constructions. Contrasts are central to an
Other Applications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 467
understanding of change in that the contrast
CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 467
(or opposite pole) to the current way of seeing
the world is the most readily available alter-
native, and changed behavior will reflect this,
at least initially.
INTRODUCTION The processes that relate to the develop-
Personal construct: Although many psychological approaches ment, application, and modification of this
a bipolar from 50 years ago have waned in impor- construing are central to understanding dif-
discrimination tance, personal construct psychology (PCP) ferences between individuals and facilitating
between elements of
was ahead of its time (Mischel 1980). Subse- change, rather than merely the content of con-
an individual’s world
quent theoretical movements have advocated struing per se. Critical to the construct sys-
PCP: personal
themes overlapping with the originally radi- tem’s development are processes such as the
construct psychology
cal position George Kelly articulated in The Validation Cycle, in which ways of making
Psychology of Personal Constructs (1955/1991). sense of the world are tested and revised or
“Scientometric” studies of PCP indicate buttressed, as in Kelly’s metaphor of the per-
that the approach developed “an international son as a potential scientist, and the Creativ-
base of support enjoyed by few psychologi- ity Cycle, which describes how we can cre-
cal theories” (Neimeyer et al. 1990, p. 17). ate something new by successively loosening
References to it are made in nearly half of the ways we construe, then tightening up to a
the volumes of the Annual Review of Psychology defined outcome. The hierarchical nature of
between 1955 and 2005. For example, Tyler construing, with constructs employed more
(1981) describes the publication of Kelly’s widely being known as superordinate, relative
book as a “landmark event in the opening to- to those with more specific applicability, the
ward individuality,” remarking, “Although it subordinate, is also important in determin-
took a number of years for the full signifi- ing change. Although Kelly (1955) formally
cance of Kelly’s theory to become apparent, presents some of his theory in a fundamen-
there is now a worldwide network of com- tal postulate and 11 corollaries, these do little
to convey the spirit of the theory and omit ner and style of his telling, by a rhetoric of hu-
central features, such as the emphasis on con- mor and disrespect, tradition and innovation,
structive alternativism, whereby we can always that he employs to speak a frame of mind into
Sociality: the
construe things differently. Concepts they en- life” (Mair 1989, p. 4). His papers are full of construing of
compass include commonality (shared con- narratives, both personal (Kelly 1962, 1978) another person’s way
struing) and individuality, thereby spanning and cultural (Kelly 1969a,d). Today such an of viewing their
both nomothetic and idiographic approaches, approach seems less controversial, as narra- world
as well as sociality (relationships based on con- tive approaches to psychology are increasingly Repertory grid: a
struing others’ construing). However, some prominent (Mair 1988, 1989). technique for the
assessment of the
know PCP best for its methodologies, espe- Although The Psychology of Personal Con-
structure and content
cially the repertory grid. This method exam- structs (Kelly 1955) focuses more centrally of a construct system
ines the ways a person’s constructs apply to on problematic construing, subsequent pa-
aspects of the world, termed “elements.” pers explore optimal functioning, arguing
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
Our focus is on how Kelly’s theory, and that this requires an Experience Cycle in-
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007.58:453-477. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
determines it” (Kelly 1969d, p. 73). Kelly struct encompasses a wider range of elements
(1969e) goes further in alerting us to the ways than the more subordinate, but rather that
language channels our stance to the world. We constructs differ in their range of implica-
commonly use the indicative mood, implying tions, with those constructs having a larger
that the relationship we propose is inherent number of implications (both superordinate
in the nature of the subject of our sentence, and subordinate) considered more meaning-
namely that something has certain properties. ful. (For example, for most people the con-
Kelly suggests adopting an invitational mood, struct of “good-bad” would carry many more
by which the listener is invited to imagine that implications than a more concrete construct
this is but one possibility, thus fostering the such as “symmetrical-asymmetrical.”) Hinkle
adventurousness that Kelly advocates for sci- reworks Kelly’s notion of threat, which fo-
entific thinking and everyday living. cuses on awareness of wide-ranging immi-
For a clinical psychologist of his era, Kelly nent change in core constructs, to awareness
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
factors, and in 1962 he extends this to an un- the total number of predictive implications of
derstanding of shared features of construing the personal construct system” (p. 25). Simi-
within different nationalities. His focus is on larly, whereas Kelly views anxiety as awareness
how we make choices by using constructs that, that occurrences are not able to be accom-
being bipolar, provide alternatives for move- modated by one’s current construct system,
ment. He proposes that we are enmeshed Hinkle writes of awareness of the lack of
within a cultural decision matrix, which at implications for the constructs that one
the same time presents choices and limits the confronts.
extent of choice available. He highlights the Hinkle argues that people choose to see
impact of exploring the construing of a dif- themselves in terms of that pole of a con-
ferent culture—“looking through glasses that struct they anticipate will be more likely to
are not your own can permanently affect your extend the implications of their system, and
eyesight” (Kelly 1962, p. 90)—and considers thereby the meaning of their lives. Conse-
implications of this position for international quently, people resist movement to unelab-
understanding. orated poles (anxiety) or reduced implicative-
Thus, far from The Psychology of Personal ness (threat). The higher a construct is in the
Constructs being a bible for future PCP work, hierarchy, the greater are its implications and
Kelly sets an example of active elaboration of its resistance to change from one pole to the
his position. We consider below the extent to other. Hinkle demonstrates findings consis-
which that challenge has been grasped. tent with this theoretical elaboration.
More recent elaborations of the notion
of the hierarchical organization of personal
Theory of Construct Implications constructs include the clarification of possi-
In our survey of PCP contributors, the doc- ble types of implicative relationships between
toral dissertation of Kelly’s student, Hinkle constructs (ten Kate 1981) and the develop-
(1965), exploring why and how people change ment of a model from set and graph theories
(and especially why they do not change), fig- (Chiari et al. 1990).
ured the most extensively. Hinkle’s theory of
construct implications concentrates less on
the way constructs apply to elements of the The Self
person’s world and more on how constructs The unitary notion of a self as the essence of
interrelate. It is based on organization, rather the person is rejected by Kelly. He consid-
than content, of construing. For Hinkle, it ers that self is one pole of a construct, which
is not so much that a superordinate con- in turn is construed. His position entails the
relevance of contrast and the view that the PCP as a Social Psychology: Beyond
construction of self occurs in comparison with the Individual
others. Additionally, our sense of self is con-
Although PCP is commonly classified as a
structed by our understanding of others’ views
personality theory, a case can be made for
of us (Bannister 1983).
it being a social psychology. Consideration
One example of the examination of pat-
of the person-in-relation (Walker 1996) is
terns of construing linking self and others
evident in numerous aspects of Kelly’s the-
(Adams-Webber 1990) is a research pro-
orizing, including the importance of “so-
gram centering on the “golden section hy-
ciality” and “commonality,” people being
pothesis,” whereby individuals apply the pos-
“validating agents” for the testing of construc-
itive poles of their constructs to others
tions, and dependency. (See Kalekin-Fishman
and assign themselves and others to the
& Walker 1996 and Stringer & Bannister 1979
same poles of constructs approximately 62%
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
colleagueship in which two personal scientists tionalized (Walker et al. 1988). Consistent
develop an enduring collaboration with re- with Kelly’s predictions, people with rela-
spect to one another’s important life projects” tively undispersed dependencies have fewer
(p. 129). Those marriages with which the constructs about dependency than those with
partners are satisfied are characterized by more dispersed dependencies, and they dif-
partners’ similar use of constructs (particu- fer in the type of construing employed. De-
larly when these are superordinate) and better velopmental trajectories of dependency based
capacity to predict each other’s use of super- on Kelly’s views concerning transition have
ordinates. A taxonomy of disordered relation- also been elaborated (Chiari et al. 1994),
ships from a PCP perspective has been pro- and this position has been differentiated
posed (Neimeyer & Neimeyer 1985). At least from the integration of PCP with attachment
one disordered relationship cannot be under- theory.
stood readily from other positions: “negative
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
writings, which emphasize the utility of these positions concern whether or not reality
constructions rather than correspondence to exists independently of our knowing it. Closer
reality. Also relevant is Dewey’s rejection to the realist pole is epistemological construc-
Personal construct
of many dualisms often taken for granted tivism, which assumes that there are many psychotherapy: a
and Mead’s emphasis on interaction with differing interpretations of external reality. therapeutic approach
others as integral to selfhood (Butt 2006). This contrasts with hermeneutic construc- that aims to facilitate
Kelly’s position regarding language is related tivism, which focuses on subject/object inter- reconstruing
by McWilliams (1996) to Korzybski’s (1933) dependence. Opinions differ on which form of Constructivism:
General Semantics approach, which argues constructivism best characterizes Kelly’s the- approaches that
consider that
for the elimination of the verb “to be” in En- ory. Minimum realism, whereby some fea-
individuals construct
glish, resulting in greater awareness of re- tures of reality are assumed to be independent their knowledge of
sponsibility for our constructions. Remarks of our experiences, may be linked to Kelly’s the world
such as “she is X” are replaced by “she be- views that truth can be approached (Stevens
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
haves like an X,” or “she gives the appearance 1998). Others express concern that PCP
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007.58:453-477. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
of an X,” statements more conducive to the may “sink without trace” if subsumed under
facilitation of change. the constructivist umbrella (Fransella 1995,
Turning to contemporary psychological p. 131).
approaches, despite Kelly (1969f) being at
pains to point out that his is not a cogni-
tive theory, frequently he is seen as the pre- Testing the Theory
cursor of the “cognitive revolution.” Several Although personal construct theory is rela-
of the references to PCP in the Annual Re- tively abstract, some key assumptions may be
view of Psychology are in the context of it being formulated in testable ways. Research sup-
a pioneering cognitive approach, such as the portive of the theory is reviewed in other sec-
statement that Kelly was “the first truly cogni- tions of this article (see also Adams-Webber
tive personologist” (Wiggins & Pincus 1992, 2003), but we focus here on whether constru-
p. 496). Debts to Kelly are acknowledged ing should be viewed as bipolar, as this is a
by leading cognitive therapists, although per- key differentiator between PCP and other ap-
sonal construct psychotherapy can be dif- proaches. In practice (at least in most methods
ferentiated from at least the original “ra- of repertory grid administration), construing
tionalist” versions of these approaches both is widely treated as dimensional rather than di-
theoretically and empirically. However, more chotomous. The issue of whether construing
recent cognitive approaches, showing greater is universally bipolar (having two contrasting
concern with processes (as opposed to the poles), rather than unipolar (as in a single con-
content) of cognition, arguably are more com- cept) or multipolar (one pole having a number
patible with PCP. Nevertheless, several per- of opposite poles), is more important since it
sonal construct theorists (e.g., Epting 1984) links to the central issue of change. If a feature
consider PCP more aligned with the human- of the world, such as oneself, is viewed as lying
istic tradition, albeit a “rigorous humanism” on one pole of a construct, if one chooses or
(Rychlak 1977). is forced to change, the opposite pole is the
PCP’s relationships to postmodernism perceived alternative.
(Botella 1995) and constructivism (Mahoney Researchers have compared how a set of
1988) have also been discussed. There are al- elements is rated on each pole of a construct,
ternative constructions of the latter relation- and have found that although such ratings are
ship, with writers proposing differing cate- negatively correlated, they are by no means
gories of constructivism. For example, Chiari perfectly dichotomous. One of these studies
& Nuzzo (1996) locate different positions on included the variation of allowing some par-
a dimension contrasting realism and idealism; ticipants to specify additional contrasts for
such areas as empathy and power balance in Construct-construct grids. In two proce-
relationships. These include the completion dures linked to Hinkle’s (1965) theory of
not only of individual grids, but also grids as implications, both grid axes are formed by
it is imagined that others will complete them, constructs. In the implications grid, individ-
grids completed together with a partner, and uals are asked, for each construct, if they
grids using both the individual’s and family were to change from one pole to the other,
members’ constructs (Feixas et al. 1993). The on what other constructs they would be
personal meaning of death has been explored likely to change. A variation involves ask-
in an extensive research program utilizing a ing the person, for each construct pole, what
form of grid termed the “threat index,” in other poles would characterize an individ-
which death threat is assumed to be indicated ual so described (Fransella 1972). Implica-
by discrepancies in how constructs are applied tion grids may be analyzed by mapping the
to the element “death” and to the self and ideal implicative relationships between constructs
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
self (Neimeyer & Epting 1992). and counting the number of implications for
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007.58:453-477. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Interview methods. Content analysis scales parties (Theodoulou 1996). This methodol-
have been applied to verbalizations, gener- ogy has potential uses in other areas.
ally obtained from open-ended interviews,
by Viney & Caputi (2005). Of these scales, Visual techniques. There is increasing
which concern a range of emotions, sociality, awareness that verbal symbols of constru-
psychosocial stages, and experiences of con- ing can be supplemented by the nonverbal.
trol, cognitive anxiety is most closely linked Ravenette’s (1999) work with children, which
to Kellian concepts. Nevertheless, the over- pioneers the use of drawings (and their op-
all framework of Viney’s research program is posites), is influential and may be extended
informed by PCP, and her approach enables to use with adults, as in the exploration of
important problems to be explored less intru- choices confronting women dealing with the
sively than by more formal techniques. menopause (Foster & Viney 2006).
Interview methods for children have been A procedure in which a person is asked to
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
developed by Ravenette (2003), for example, take a series of photographs to answer the
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007.58:453-477. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
to explore their “troubles” (in PCP terms, ex- question “Who am I?” provides a measure
periences of invalidation). There are also in- of the degree of constriction (drawing in of
terview procedures for the elicitation of core the boundaries of application) of the construct
constructs (Leitner 1985) and to explore the system, reflected in the range of categories of
links between family members’ constructions the resulting photographs (Bailey & Walker
of others and their actions (Feixas et al. 1993). 2003). Consistent with Kelly’s theorizing, de-
pression is associated with a more constricted
Construct content analysis. Despite PCP’s system (Hanieh & Walker 2007).
emphasis on process, content has not been ig-
nored, and various methods have been devel-
oped to categorize constructs elicited by any APPLICATIONS
of the methods described above (Green 2004).
Clinical Applications
One of the first such systems, consisting of 22
categories (e.g., forcefulness), most with sub- Kelly’s two-volume magnum opus evolved
categories (e.g., high, low), was provided by from the handbook of clinical procedures he
Landfield (1971). Problems with this system wrote for his students, and the clinical field is
have been addressed by Feixas et al. (2002) in a that in which PCP has been most widely used.
scheme for the classification of personal con- The primary advances are in the understand-
structs using categories of moral, emotional, ing of disorder and of therapy, with the devel-
relational, personal, intellectual/operational, opment and evaluation of new approaches to
values/interests, existential, and concrete de- personal construct psychotherapy.
scriptors, each with subcategories.
Disorder. Kelly (1955) views disorder as
Questionnaires. Because of PCP’s idio- “any personal construction which is used re-
graphic emphasis, questionnaires have rarely peatedly in spite of consistent invalidation”
been used, with two exceptions. The Personal (p. 831). His classification of disorders into
Construct Inventory (Chambers & O’Day those of construction and of transition con-
1984) provides measures of processes of con- tains several inconsistencies, and it has been
struing and has been revised based on its psy- suggested that the notion of disorder car-
chometric properties (Watson et al. 1997). ries mechanistic implications antithetical to
Statements from newspapers illustrative of Kelly’s philosophy (Walker & Winter 2005).
types of construing have been used to develop Kelly’s definition of disorder implies a
a questionnaire on economic attitudes that failure to complete the process of experi-
differentiates supporters of different political mentation described in his Validation and
Experience Cycle. The earlier the blockage who differentiate it from paranoia in terms of
in the Experience Cycle, the more severe the the earlier age at which major invalidation has
likely resulting disorder (Neimeyer 1985a). been experienced, and Gara et al. (1989), who
The failure to test construing adequately has associate it with unelaborated (poorly devel-
been termed “nonvalidation” (Walker 2002), oped) self-construing.
and it may be regarded as a strategy to avoid A single profound invalidation may lead to
reconstruing. It may be associated with an im- fragmentation of construing, as indicated in
balance in the use of the pairs of processes that studies associating post-traumatic stress dis-
Kelly considers central to construing, e.g., order with unelaborated construing of the
loosening versus tightening and dilation ver- traumatic event (Sewell 1997). This leads to
sus constriction. Although, optimally, there “constructive bankruptcy” (p. 209), in which
is a cyclical and balanced interplay of con- the person cannot relate the trauma to other
trasting processes, disorders may involve the life experiences. Childhood sexual abuse is
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
virtually exclusive use of one process. How- one trauma that has received considerable at-
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007.58:453-477. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
ever, the distinction between optimal func- tention from a PCP perspective (Erbes &
tioning and disorder extends beyond individ- Harter 2002).
ual processes to such aspects of interpersonal In contrast to schizophrenic thought dis-
relationships as the “egalitarian outlook,” order, a pattern of tight construing may char-
which combines an acceptance of individuality acterize individuals with anxiety disorders
with cooperativeness and lack of oppression (Winter 1985). There is evidence that those
(Warren 1992), and “sociality” (Leitner & diagnosed as agoraphobic may constrict their
Pfenninger 1994). worlds to avoid anxiety associated with inter-
Kelly (1955) and subsequent personal con- personal conflict, the agoraphobic’s constru-
struct theorists (Raskin & Lewandowski 2000) ing of which tends to be at a low level of aware-
criticize psychiatric nosology for its tendency ness (Winter 1989).
to lead to preemptive construing, in which In one of Kelly’s (1961) own elaborations,
a person is seen solely in terms of a diag- he distinguishes different types of suicidal
nostic label. Nevertheless, much post-Kellian acts. Suicide as a “dedicated act,” as in many
clinical research considers clients defined by cases of suicide bombings, is “designed to val-
conventional psychiatric diagnostic categories idate one’s life . . . to extend its essential mean-
(Button 1985), partly to facilitate commu- ing” rather than to terminate it (Kelly 1961,
nication with a community steeped in such p. 260). Kelly differentiates this from “mere
constructions. One of the first such research suicide,” which may occur in circumstances of
programs associated schizophrenic thought realism (certainty) and indeterminacy (chaos).
disorder with loose construing, specifically In the former, “the course of events seems
when psychological constructs are applied to so obvious that there is no point in waiting
people (Bannister 1962, Bannister & Salmon around for the outcome,” whereas in the lat-
1966). Evidence was provided that this may ter, “everything seems so unpredictable that
result from serial invalidation of constru- the only definite thing one can do is to aban-
ing (Bannister 1965), and the Grid Test of don the scene altogether” (Kelly 1961, p. 260).
Schizophrenic Thought Disorder (Bannister This taxonomy has been extended to en-
& Fransella 1966) was developed as a diagnos- compass nonfatal acts of deliberate self-harm
tic instrument. Bannister’s research spawned (Neimeyer & Winter 2006). Some of this re-
numerous studies, and although some pro- search associates high suicidal intent with con-
posed alternative explanations for his results, striction of the construct system, although the
none was entirely convincing (Winter 1992). findings are not entirely consistent.
Schizophrenia has also been explored from a PCP perspectives have been provided on
PCP perspective by Lorenzini et al. (1989), numerous other clinical problems, including
& Rivers 1975, Neimeyer 1988a). This in- however, that Piaget’s view of reality is more
volves a phase of rotating dyads in which absolutist than is Kelly’s.
group members briefly interact with every Personal construct approaches to educa-
other member concerning a provided topic, tional psychology have also been developed,
followed by a plenary phase in which members and the implications of PCP for the process
discuss these dyadic experiences. One partic- of learning have been elaborated.
ular variant of personal construct psychother-
apy emphasizing the intimate client-therapist Children’s construing. A series of studies,
relationship, rather than techniques, is Leit- mostly using grids (Salmon 1976), provides
ner’s (1988) experiential approach. He argues evidence for elaboration of construing during
that such relationships pose the danger of in- childhood. Mancuso (2003) views child devel-
validation of core constructs and may there- opment as the construction of self-guiding an-
fore be terrifying; this is less likely if an op- ticipatory narratives, essentially stories based
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
tion and separateness, is maintained between ing their actions. He identifies, and provides
therapist and client. training in, the characteristics of expert par-
Although conventional outcome research ents, those who most facilitate their children’s
has been regarded by some personal con- development. One way in which a parent may
struct psychotherapists as incompatible with attempt to develop a child’s construing is by
constructivism, there is a growing evidence reprimand, which Mancuso considers a pos-
base for this form of therapy with a wide sible outcome of rule violation, the invalida-
range of client groups (Viney et al. 2005). tion of a person’s construction of an event by
Meta-analysis of the studies concerned, which another. Invalidation by a child of an adult’s
compare the outcome of personal construct construction of how a learner should behave
psychotherapy with either no treatment, stan- may lead to the child being construed pre-
dard care, or other types of intervention, has emptively in terms of a diagnosis such as at-
indicated effect sizes comparable to those tention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. This is
in cognitive-behavioral and psychodynamic likely to impair the child’s ability to develop a
therapies (Viney et al. 2005). This research construction of self as a competent learner.
is complemented by numerous uncontrolled,
single-case, and/or qualitative studies. Educational psychology and learning.
Personal construct educational psychology
was pioneered by Ravenette (1999), who de-
Educational Applications veloped methods for elicitation of children’s
Another major area of application of PCP is construing. These exemplify such features of
the educational setting. Although Kelly ran a PCP as the credulous approach (taking the
traveling service to schools, he devotes little client’s views at face value), the emphasis on
attention in his writing to the process of the contrast, and that assessment itself may pro-
development of construing. However, PCP mote reconstruction. Ravenette stresses that
has subsequently been elaborated as a psy- problems in which the educational psycholo-
chology of personal growth, and both similar- gist is asked to intervene are generally not pre-
ities and differences have been noted with Pi- sented by children themselves but by adults
aget’s epistemological position (Salmon 1970, interacting with them. He considers that such
Soffer 1993). For example, both approaches problems occur “when a person cannot make
view the individual as an active experimenter, sense out of an event, and feels that he should”
and both focus on structural aspects of devel- (Ravenette 1988, p. 103; italics in original).
opment rather than the content of the per- Resolution is as likely to require reconstru-
son’s view of the world. It has been suggested, ing by an adult as by the child. For example,
he describes how a boy was no longer con- lection procedures, employee induction, per-
strued by his teacher as a “problem child” but formance appraisal, facilitation of decision-
rather as simply “having reading difficulties” making, team building exercises, exploration
when the teacher understood that the boy’s of communication, conflict resolution, and
withdrawal from her had invalidated her core analysis of how managers and employees
construction of herself as a caring person. view a company’s work (Brophy et al. 2003,
Some of Kelly’s post-1955 publications Coopman 1997, Jankowicz 1990). The results
indicate the importance of personal mean- are often fed back to participants, allowing the
ings in the learning process (Kelly 1970, planning of change processes such as retrain-
1979, 2003), and this theme has been contin- ing programs, which in turn may be evaluated
ued (Pope & Denicolo 2001). For example, by grids. A particularly helpful outcome may
Thomas & Harri-Augstein (1985) promote be the facilitation of construing not only of
self-organized learning, in which people are individuals’ own construction processes, but
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
helped to reflect upon their learning processes also those of other stakeholders in the or-
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007.58:453-477. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Organizational Applications
An area of application of PCP that has grown Other Applications
with the recognition of the inherently social The work of post-Kellian personal construct
nature of the theory is its utility in organiza- theorists shows the theory to have a range
tional and business settings, extending beyond of convenience, or of useful application, ex-
the construct system of the individual within tending far beyond the clinical, educational,
the organization to that of the organization and organizational settings. Other spheres in
itself. which it is applied include the forensic set-
ting, politics, sport, the arts, anthropology, re-
The individual within the organization. ligion, accounting, and artificial intelligence
Personal construct work with individuals in (Fransella 2003a, Fransella & Thomas 1988,
organizations includes personal development, Horley 2003, Kalekin-Fishman & Walker
coaching, counseling (Brophy et al. 2003, 1996, Scheer & Sewell 2006).
Fransella et al. 1988), and vocational guid-
ance (Savickas 1997). It relies heavily upon,
but is not limited to, the use of grids (Stewart CONCLUSIONS
& Stewart 1981). A feature of PCP is reflexivity, the ability to
turn its concepts upon itself. How, then, has
The organization. Personal construct as- the theory fared in the past half-century when
sessment methods are also used in areas rel- evaluated in personal construct terms?
evant to broader concerns of organizations, Kelly (1955, p. 22) sets out a list of “de-
including market research, job analysis, se- sign specifications for a psychological theory
of personality,” namely that it should have an the viability of Kelly’s ideas. While there have
appropriate focus and range of convenience, been attempts to integrate the theory with,
be fertile, produce testable hypotheses, be or subsume it within, other approaches, there
valid, have generality, be amenable to op- has also been resistance to such developments.
erational definition, be modifiable and ulti- This itself supports Kelly’s view that people,
mately expendable, avoid the problems asso- even personal construct theorists, are threat-
ciated with assumptions of mental energy, be ened by the prospect of their core constructs
able to account for the choices that people being expendable. On the other hand, some
make, and recognize individuality. personal construct theorists regard the assim-
The appropriateness of the focus of con- ilation of the theory into other perspectives
venience of PCP on the “psychological re- and disciplines a natural stage in the develop-
construction of life” (Kelly 1955, p. 23) is ment of a successful theory, and one to which
particularly demonstrated by post-Kellian its followers should aspire (Neimeyer 1985).
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
elaborations of personal construct psy- The lack of concepts of mental energy has
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007.58:453-477. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
chotherapy. The diversity of its other appli- not hampered the theory’s explanatory power,
cations indicates that its range of convenience and its notion of choice has received empir-
extends throughout human psychology, in- ical support and enabled the understanding
cluding areas in which it was initially not well of the often surprising, and sometimes appar-
elaborated. These applications, and the new ently self-defeating or destructive (Neimeyer
approaches and research programs generated & Winter 2006, Winter 2006), choices that
from PCP, also demonstrate its fertility. The people make. Finally, the idiographic applica-
theory has produced numerous hypotheses tion of the theory to such areas as individual
that have been subjected to experimental test, choice is complemented by nomothetic ap-
and the support that these have received indi- plications, such as the derivation and testing
cates something of the theory’s validity (per- of hypotheses concerning the construing of
haps a surprising concept for Kelly to employ, particular groups.
but one that essentially refers to predictive The elaboration of PCP can therefore be
efficiency). The abstractness of the theory’s considered to demonstrate that the theory
constructs enables them to have a generality meets Kelly’s design specifications, perhaps
extending far beyond the clinical realm and too well in that it has yet to become expend-
the historical context in which they were orig- able! He concludes the second volume of his
inally developed. These concepts have also book with the hope that the “unique pair of
been operationally defined, often using assess- spectacles” through which the reader has been
ment methods developed from PCP. That the offered a look will be found to fit (Kelly 1955).
theory itself, as opposed to the approaches The extent of the subsequent fit in extremely
and techniques devised from it, has been sub- diverse fields may be far greater than even
ject to little or no modification may attest to Kelly could have anticipated.
SUMMARY POINTS
1. Personal construct psychology anticipated numerous subsequent developments in
psychology.
2. It is not, as sometimes described, merely a cognitive psychology of the individual, but
is concerned with the whole person, including the person in relation to others.
3. Particular areas in which the theory has been developed include implicative relation-
ships between constructs, self-construing, social relationships, emotions, relationships
with other theories, and research testing aspects of the theory.
4. There has been considerable adaptation of Kelly’s repertory grid and self-
characterization methods for the assessment of construing; several other assessment
methods have also been developed.
5. Personal construct psychology has been applied extensively in the clinical, educational,
and organizational fields, as well as in a wide range of other areas.
6. There is a growing evidence base for personal construct psychotherapy.
FUTURE ISSUES
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
LITERATURE CITED
Adams-Webber J. 1990. Some fundamental asymmetries in the structure of personal constructs.
See Neimeyer & Neimeyer 1990, pp. 49–85
Adams-Webber J. 2001. Cognitive complexity and role relationships. J. Constr. Psychol. 14:43–
50
Adams-Webber J. 2003. Research in personal construct psychology. See Fransella 2003a, pp.
51–58
Bagozzi RP, Dabholkar PA. 1994. Consumer recycling goals and their effect on decisions to
recycle: a means-end chain analysis. Psychol. Market. 11:313–40
Bailey A, Walker BM. 2003. Using psychophotography as a nonverbal measure of constriction.
In Psychological Constructivism and the Social World, ed. GC Chiari, ML Nuzzo, pp.306–12.
Demonstrates
Milan: FrancoAngeli
empirically that a
Balnaves M, Caputi P. 2000. A theory of social action: why personal construct psychology particular type of
needs a superpattern corollary. J. Constr. Psychol. 13:117–34 psychological
Bannister D. 1962. The nature and measurement of schizophrenic thought disorder. disorder,
J. Ment. Sci. 108:825–42 schizophrenic
thought disorder, is
Bannister D. 1965. The genesis of schizophrenic thought disorder: retest of the serial invali-
related to features
dation hypothesis. Brit. J. Psychiatry 111:377–82 of construing.
Bannister D, ed. 1970, Perspectives in Personal Construct Theory. London: Academic
Bannister D, ed. 1977a. New Perspectives in Personal Construct Theory. London: Academic
Bannister D. 1977b. The logic of passion. See Bannister 1977a, pp. 21–37
Bannister D. 1983. Self in personal construct theory. In Applications of Personal Construct Theory,
ed. JR Adams-Webber, JC Mancuso, pp. 379–86. Toronto: Academic
Bannister D, Fransella F. 1966. A grid test of schizophrenic thought disorder. Br. J. Soc. Clin.
Psychol. 5:95–102
Bannister D, Salmon P. 1966. Schizophrenic thought disorder: specific or diffuse? Br. J. Med.
Psychol. 39:215–19
Bell RC. 2000. A psychometric assessment of the bipolarity of constructs in repertory grid data.
See Scheer 2000, pp. 141–49
Bell RC. 2001. Some new measures of dispersion of dependency in a situation-resource grid.
J. Constr. Psychol. 14:227–34
Bell RC. 2004. Predictive relationships in repertory grid data: a new elaboration of Kelly’s
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
Benjafield J, Adams-Webber J. 1976. The golden section hypothesis. Br. J. Psychol. 67:11–15
Bieri J. 1955. Cognitive complexity-simplicity and predictive behavior. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol.
51:263–86
Bonarius H, Holland H, Rosenberg S, eds. 1981. Personal Construct Psychology: Recent Advances
in Theory and Practice. London: Macmillan
Botella L. 1995. Personal construct theory, constructivism, and postmodern thought. See
Neimeyer & Neimeyer 1995, pp. 3–35
Brophy S, Fransella F, Reed N. 2003. The power of a good theory. See Fransella 2003a,
pp. 329–38
Bruner JS. 1956. A cognitive theory of personality. Contemp. Psychol. 1:355–57
Butler R, Green D. 1998. The Child Within: The Exploration of Personal Construct Theory with
Young People. Oxford: Butterworth Heinemann
Butt T. 2006. Personal construct therapy and its history in pragmatism. In Personal Construct
Psychology: New Ideas, ed. P Caputi, H Foster, LL Viney, pp. 20–34. London: Wiley
Butt T, Burr V, Bell RC. 1997. Fragmentation and the sense of self. Constr. Hum. Sci. 2:12–29
Button E, ed. 1985. Personal Construct Theory and Mental Health. Beckenham, UK: Croom
Helm
Chambers WV. 1983. Circumspection, preemption and personal constructs. Soc. Behav.
Personal. 11:33–35
Chambers WV, O’Day P. 1984. A nomothetic view of personal construct processes. Psychol.
Rep. 55:554
Chiari G, Mancini F, Nicolò F, Nuzzo ML. 1990. Hierarchical organization of personal con-
struct systems in terms of their range of convenience. Int. J. Personal. Constr. Psychol.
3:281–311
Chiari G, Nuzzo ML. 1996. Psychological constructivisms: a metatheoretical differentiation.
J. Constr. Psychol. 9:163–84
Chiari G, Nuzzo ML, Alfano V, Brogna P, D’Andrea T, et al. 1994. Personal paths of depen-
dency. J. Constr. Psychol. 7:17–34
Coopman SJ. 1997. Personal constructs and communication in interpersonal and organizational
contexts. See Neimeyer & Neimeyer 1997, pp. 101–47
Crockett. 1965. Cognitive complexity and impression formation. In Progress in Experimental
Personality Research, ed. BA Maher, 3:47–90. New York: Academic
Cummins P. 2003. Working with anger. See Fransella 2003a, pp. 83–91
Davis H, Cunningham C. 1985. Mental handicap: people in context. See Button 1985,
pp. 246–61
Deeter-Schmelz DR, Sojka JZ. 2004. Wrestling with American values: an exploratory investi-
gation of World Wrestling Entertainment as a product-based subculture. J Consum. Behav.
4:132–43
Diamond CPT. 1991. Teacher Education as Transformation. Milton Keynes, UK: Open Univ.
Press
DiLollo A, Manning WH, Neimeyer RA. 2005. Cognitive complexity as a function of speaker
role for adult persons who stutter. J. Constr. Psychol. 18:215–36
Duck S. 1979. The personal and the interpersonal in construct theory: social and individual
aspects of relationships. See Stringer & Bannister 1979, pp. 279–97
Epting FR. 1984. Personal Construct Counseling and Psychotherapy. New York: Wiley
Epting FR, Landfield AW, eds. 1985. Anticipating Personal Construct Psychology. Lincoln: Univ.
Nebraska Press
Erbes CR, Harter SL. 2002. Constructions of abuse: understanding the effects of childhood
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
Horley J, ed. 2003. Personal Construct Perspectives on Forensic Psychology. London: Routledge
Jackson SR. 1988. Self-characterization: dimensions of meaning. See Fransella & Thomas
1998, pp. 223–31
Jankowicz AD. 1990. Applications of personal construct psychology in business practice. See
Neimeyer & Neimeyer 1990, pp. 257–87
Jankowicz D. 2003. The Easy Guide to Repertory Grids. London: Wiley
Kalekin-Fishman D, Walker BM, eds. 1996. The Construction of Group Realities: Culture, Society
and Personal Construct Psychology. Malabar, FL: Krieger Publ.
Kelly GA. 1955 (1991). The Psychology of Personal Constructs. New York: Norton/London:
Routledge
Kelly GA. 1961. Theory and therapy in suicide: the personal construct point of view. In The
Cry for Help, ed. M Farberow, E Scheidman, pp. 255–80. New York: McGraw-Hill
Kelly GA. 1962. Europe’s matrix of decision. In Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, ed. RM
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
Mahoney MJ. 1988. Constructive metatheory: I. Basic features and historical foundations. Int.
J. Personal. Constr. Psychol. 1:1–35
Mair JMM. 1977. The community of self. See Bannister 1977a, pp. 125–49
Mair M. 1988. Psychology as story-telling. Int. J. Personal. Constr. Psychol. 1:125–38
Mair M. 1989. Kelly, Bannister, and a story-telling psychology. Int. J. Personal. Constr. Psychol.
2:1–14
Mancuso JC. 2003. Children’s development of personal constructs. See Fransella 2003a, pp.
275–82
McCoy MM. 1981. Positive and negative emotion: a personal interpretation. See Bonarius
et al. 1981, pp. 95–104
McWilliams SA. 1996. Accepting the invitational. In Personal Construct Theory: A Psychology for
the Future, ed. BM Walker, J Costigan, LL Viney, B Warren, pp. 57–78. Melbourne: Aust.
Psychol. Soc.
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
Millis KK, Neimeyer RA. 1990. A test of the dichotomy corollary: propositions versus con-
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007.58:453-477. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Neimeyer RA, Baker KD, Neimeyer GJ. 1990. The current status of personal construct theory:
some scientometric data. See Neimeyer & Neimeyer 1990, pp. 3–22
Neimeyer RA, Epting FR. 1992. Measuring personal meanings of death: twenty years of
research using the Threat Index. In Advances in Personal Construct Psychology, ed. RA
Neimeyer, GJ Neimeyer, 2:121–47. Greenwich, CT: JAI
Neimeyer RA, Mahoney MJ, eds. 1995. Constructivism in Psychotherapy. Washington, DC: Am.
Psychol. Assoc.
Neimeyer RA, Neimeyer GJ, eds. 1987. Personal Construct Therapy Casebook. New York:
Springer
Neimeyer RA, Neimeyer GJ, eds. 1995. Advances in Personal Construct Psychology. Vol. 3.
Greenwich, CT: JAI
Neimeyer RA, Raskin JD. 2001. Varieties of constructivism in psychotherapy. In Handbook of
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
Neimeyer RA, Winter DA. 2006. To be or not to be: personal construct perspectives on
the suicidal choice. In Cognition and Suicide: The Science of Suicidal Thinking, ed. T Ellis,
pp. 149–69. Washington, DC: Am. Psychol. Assoc.
Novak JM. 1990. Advancing constructive education: a framework for teacher education. See
Neimeyer & Neimeyer 1995, pp. 233–55
Pope M. 2003. Construing teaching and teacher education worldwide. See Fransella 2003a,
pp. 303–10
Pope ML, Denicolo PM. 2001. Transformative Education: Personal Construct Approaches to Practice
and Research. London: Whurr
Procter HG. 1981. Family construct psychology: an approach to understanding and
Introduces the
notion of the family treating families. In Developments in Family Therapy, ed. S Walrond-Skinner,
construct system, pp. 350–66. London: Routledge
with properties Procter HG. 2001. Personal construct psychology and autism. J. Constr. Psychol. 14:107–26
similar to
Procter H. 2005. Techniques of personal construct family therapy. See Winter & Viney 2005,
individual
construct systems. pp. 94–108
Raskin JD, Lewandowski AM. 2000. The construction of disorder as human enterprise. In
Constructions of Disorder: Meaning-Making Frameworks in Psychotherapy, ed. RA Neimeyer,
JD Raskin, pp. 15–40. Washington, DC: Am. Psychol. Assoc.
Ravenette AT. 1977. Personal construct theory: an approach to the psychological investigation
of children and young people. See Bannister 1977a, pp. 251–80
Ravenette AT. 1988. Personal construct psychology in the practice of an educational psychol-
ogist. In Working with People: Clinical Uses of Personal Construct Psychology, ed. G. Dunnett,
pp. 101–21. London: Routledge
Ravenette AT. 1999. Personal Construct Theory in Educational Psychology: A Practitioner’s View.
London: Whurr
Ravenette T. 2003. Constructive intervention when children are presented as problems. See
Fransella 2003a, pp 283–93
Reynolds TJ, Gutman J. 1988. Laddering theory, method, analysis, and interpretation. J. Ad-
vert. Res. 28:11–31
Riemann R. 1990. The bipolarity of personal constructs. Int. J. Personal. Constr. Psychol. 3:149–
65
Rogers CR. 1956. Intellectualized psychotherapy. Contemp. Psychol. 1:357–58
Rowe D. 2003. Depression: The Way Out of Your Prison. Hove, UK/New York: Brunner-
Routledge. 3rd ed.
Russell CG, Busson A, Flight I, Bryan J, van Lawick van Pabst J, Cox D. 2004. A comparison
of three laddering techniques applied to an example of a complex food choice. Food Qual.
Pref. 15:569–83
Rychlak JF. 1977. The Psychology of Rigorous Humanism. New York: Wiley
Ryle A. 1990. Cognitive-Analytic Therapy: Active Participation in Change: A New Integration in
Brief Therapy. Chichester, UK: Wiley
Salmon P. 1970. A psychology of personal growth. See Bannister 1970, pp. 197–221
Salmon P. 1976. Grid measures with child subjects. In The Measurement of Intrapersonal Space
by Grid Technique. Volume 1. Explorations of Intrapersonal Space, ed. P Slater, pp. 15–46.
Chichester, UK: Wiley
Salmon P, Claire H. 1984. Classroom Collaboration. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul
Savickas ML. 1997. Constructivist career counseling: models and methods. See Neimeyer &
Neimeyer 1997, pp. 149–82
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
Scheer JW, Sewell KW, eds. 2006. Creative Construing: Personal Constructions in the Arts.
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007.58:453-477. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Giessen: Psychosozial-Verlag
Sewell KW. 1997. Posttraumatic stress: towards a constructivist model of psychotherapy. See
Neimeyer & Neimeyer 1997, pp. 207–35
Sewell KW. 2003. An approach to post-traumatic stress. See Fransella 2003a, pp. 223–31
Sewell KW, Baldwin CL, Moes AJ. 1998. The multiple self awareness group. J. Constr. Psychol.
11:59–78
Slater P. 1977. The Measurement of Intrapersonal Space by Grid Technique. Volume 2. Dimensions
of Intrapersonal Space. Chichester, UK: Wiley
Soffer J. 1993. Jean Piaget and George Kelly: toward a stronger constructivism. Int. J. Personal.
Constr. Psychol. 6:59–77
Stevens CD. 1998. Realism and Kelly’s pragmatic constructivism. J. Constr. Psychol. 11:283–
308
Stewart V, Stewart A. 1981. Business Applications of Repertory Grid. London: McGraw-Hill
Stringer P, Bannister D, eds. 1979. Constructs of Sociality and Individuality. London: Academic
ten Kate H. 1981. A theoretical explication of Hinkle’s implication theory. See Bonarius et al.
1981, pp. 167–75
Theodoulou S. 1996. Construing economic and political reality. J. Econ. Psychol. 17:400–41
Thomas LF, Harri-Augstein S. 1985. Self-Organised Learning: Foundations of a Conversational
Science of Psychology. London: Routledge
Tschudi F. 1977. Loaded and honest questions: a personal construct view of symptoms and
therapy. See Bannister 1977a, pp. 321–50
Tyler LE. 1981. More stately mansions—psychology extends its boundaries. Ann. Rev. Psychol.
32:1–20
Viney LL. 1983. Images of Illness. Malabar, FL: Krieger
Viney LL. 1993. Life Stories: Personal Construct Therapy with the Elderly. Chichester, UK: Wiley
Viney LL, Caputi P. 2005. The origin and pawn, positive affect, psychosocial maturity and
cognitive affect scales: using them in counseling research. Meas. Eval. Couns. Dev. 34:115–
26
Viney LL, Metcalfe C, Winter DA. 2005. The effectiveness of personal construct psychother-
apy: a meta-analysis. See Winter & Viney 2005, pp. 347–64
Walker BM. 1996. A psychology for adventurers: an introduction to personal construct
psychology from a social perspective. See Kalekin-Fishman & Walker 1996, pp. 7–
26
Walker BM. 1997. Shaking the kaleidoscope: dispersion of dependency and its relationships.
See Neimeyer & Neimeyer 1997, pp. 63–97
Walker BM. 2002. Nonvalidation vs. (in)validation: implications for theory and practice. See
Raskin & Bridges 2002, pp. 49–61
Walker BM. 2003. Identity and traveling. In Crossing Borders—Going Places, ed. JW Scheer,
pp. 82–92. Giessen: Psychosozial-Verlag
Walker BM, Ramsey FL, Bell RC. 1988. Dispersed and undispersed dependency. Int. J. Personal.
Constr. Psychol. 1:63–80
Walker BM, Winter DA. 2005. Psychological disorder and reconstruction. See Winter & Viney
2005, pp. 21–33
Warren B. 1998. The Philosophical Dimensions of Personal Construct Psychology. London: Routledge
Warren B. 2003. Pragmatism and religion: Dewey’s twin influences? See Fransella 2003a,
pp. 387–94
Warren WG. 1992. Personal construct theory and mental health. Int. J. Personal. Constr. Psychol.
4:223–37
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
Watson N, Watts R. 2001. The predictive strength of personal constructs versus conventional
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007.58:453-477. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
RELATED RESOURCES
Journals
Journal of Constructivist Psychology
Personal Construct Theory and Practice
Annual Review of
Psychology
Contents
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007.58:453-477. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Prefatory
Cognitive Neuroscience
Animal Cognition
vii
Contents ARI 8 November 2006 21:2
Personality Disorders
viii Contents
Contents ARI 8 November 2006 21:2
Small Groups
by City University of New York, The Graduate Center on 11/01/10. For personal use only.
Ostracism
Kipling D. Williams ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! 425
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007.58:453-477. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Personality Processes
Contents ix
Contents ARI 8 November 2006 21:2
Research Methodology
Mediation Analysis
David P. MacKinnon, Amanda J. Fairchild, and Matthew S. Fritz ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! 593
Analysis of Nonlinear Patterns of Change with Random Coefficient
Models
Robert Cudeck and Jeffrey R. Harring ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! 615
Indexes
Errata
An online log of corrections to Annual Review of Psychology chapters (if any, 1997 to the
present) may be found at http://psych.annualreviews.org/errata.shtml
x Contents