Zlib - Pub Personality Tests and Assessments
Zlib - Pub Personality Tests and Assessments
Personality Tests
and Assessments
Philip E. Vernon
Psychology R evivals
O r i g i n a l l y p u b l i s h e d in 1 9 5 3 th is b o o k p ro v i d e d th e first c o m
p re h e n s iv e a c c o u n t o f m e t h o d s o f p e rs o n a lity ass essm en t by a
B ritish a u th o r . It starts w i t h a s h o r t survey o f p e rs o n a lity th e o ry ,
p o i n t i n g o u t th e difficulties in an y m e t h o d o f te s ti n g or assess
m e n t . N e x t it d esc rib es t h e w eak nesses o f t h e c o m m o n in te rv ie w
m e t h o d . ( T h r o u g h o u t th e e m p h a s is is on m e t h o d s w h ic h are
usab le in e d u c a t io n a l o r v o c atio n al g u i d a n c e a n d se lectio n , n o t on
m e t h o d s w h ic h are m a i n ly o f scientific in te re st.) T h e r e a f t e r it takes
u p each m a in ty p e o f te c h n i q u e - tests based on p h y s i q u e o r p s y
c h o lo g ica l m e asu res, on expressive m o v e m e n t su ch as g e s t u r e s and
h a n d w r i t i n g , tests o f b e h a v io u r ( i n c l u d i n g W a r Office Selection
Board ‘h o u se p a r t y ’ m e t h o d s ) , r a t in g s an d ra t in g scales, q u e s
tio n n a ir e s, a n d so-called p ro je c tiv e te c h n i q u e s . T h e e v id e n c e for or
a g a in s t each tes t o r m e t h o d is su rv eyed a n d n u m e r o u s references
p ro v i d e d for re le v a n t lite r a tu re . I l lu s tra tiv e e x c erp ts are g iv e n o f
m a n y o f th e m o re p r o m i s i n g tests, a n d so m e p ic to ria l il lu s tra tio n s .
B ritish w o rk in th is field a t th e t i m e is covered c o m p l e te l y , a n d an
a t t e m p t is m a d e to p ro v i d e a fair s u m m a r y o f th e m a i n c o n t r i b u
tio n s o f A m e ric a n a n d o t h e r p sy c h o lo g ists o f th e day.
This page intentionally left blank
Personality Tests
and Assessm ents
P h ilip E. V e r n o n
Routledge
Taylor & Francis Group
R
L O N D O N AN D NEW YORKI
First p u b li s h e d in 195 3
by M e t h u e n & C o Ltd
T h is e d itio n first p u b li s h e d in 2 0 1 4 by R o u tl e d g e
27 C h u r c h R o a d , H o v e , B N 3 2F A
S im u lta n e o u s ly p u b l i s h e d in th e U SA an d C a n ad a
by R o u tl e d g e
7 11 T h i r d A v e n u e , N e w Y o rk , N Y 1 0 0 1 7
A ll r i g h ts reserved. N o p a rt o f th is bo o k m ay be re p r in te d or re p ro d u c e d
o r u tilise d in any form o r by any elec tronic, m e c h a n ic a l, or o t h e r m ean s,
now k n o w n o r h ereafter in v e n t e d , in c l u d i n g p h o to c o p y i n g a n d r e c o r d in g ,
or in an y in f o r m a t io n sto rag e o r retrie val sy s tem , w i t h o u t p e rm is s io n in
w r i t i n g from th e p u b lish e rs .
P u b lis h e r ’s N o te
T h e p u b li s h e r has g o n e to g r e a t le n g t h s to e n su re th e q u a li ty o f th is
re p r in t b u t p o in t s o u t t h a t so m e im p e rfe c tio n s in th e o rig in al copies may
be a p p a re n t.
D is c la im e r
The p u b l i s h e r has m a d e every effort to trace c o p y r i g h t h o ld e rs and
w e lc o m e s c o rre sp o n d e n c e from th ose th e y have b een u n a b le to con tac t.
IS B N : 9 7 8 - 0 - 4 1 5 - 7 1 6 6 5 - 9 (h b k )
IS B N : 9 7 8 - 1 - 3 1 5 - 8 7 9 5 3 - 6 (eb k)
PERSONALITY TESTS
AND
ASSESSMENTS
BY
P H IL IP E. VERNON
PROFESSOR OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY IN THE
INSTITUTE OF EDUCATION, UNIVERSITY OF LONDON
1-4
CATALOGUE N O . 0 2 / 5438/36
R E P R IN T E D B Y L IT H O G R A P H Y I N G R E A T B R IT A IN B Y
JA R R O L D A N D S O N S L T D . N O R W IC H
TO
GORDON A LLPO RT
This page intentionally left blank
FOREWORD
OST of my own work in the field of personality assessment
M was done between 1928 and 1985, a t which tim e such
rapid developments were taking place th a t it seemed hardly
possible to encompass them in a book. However, I attem pted
to provide a system atic survey of verbal tests, questionnaires
and ratings in a monograph, which was published as a Report
of the Industrial H ealth Research Board in 1988. This has
long been out of print, and Chapters V II-IX of the present
volume cover much the same ground. I am grateful to the
Medical Research Council and to the Controller of Her
M ajesty’s Stationery Office for permission to reproduce some
of the material contained in th a t Report.
Since the 1930s, personality testing has become more
stabilized. The exigencies of applied psychology in war-time
showed th a t certain methods could be p u t to immediate use,
under carefully controlled conditions, whereas m any bright
ideas were quite impracticable. I t showed also th a t so-called
‘ clinical ’ methods like the interview and projection techniques,
in spite of their apparent advantages in yielding insight into
the personality as an organized whole, are very subjective and
untrustw orthy tools for vocational purposes. Thus the tim e
is now ripe for an overall appraisal of the various approaches to
personality assessment, for noting the more solid achievements
and the m ost promising lines for further development, and for
dismissing the unsuccessful.
The 1940s have also seen some decline of interest in the
assessment of individual personalities, and a greater emphasis
on the psychology of people’s behaviour in social groups. B ut
however true it may be th a t the individual person behaves
differently according to the structure of the group of which
he is a member, the problem of assessment still remains. The
results of most investigations in general and social psychology
are still affected by personality differences among th e people
being studied, and these need to be measured. The employer
or teacher, and the educational or vocational psychologist,
still wish to find out the character, social and emotional quali
ties, the attitudes and interests, of prospective pupils, students,
vii
viii Personality Tests and Assessments
and employees. While this book tries to cover all such types of
assessment, it does not deal specifically with the diagnosis of
abnormal personalities, nor discuss clinical methods in detail.
Some use is made of the evidence from tests of neurotic and
psychotic patients, but chiefly in order to prove the worth
of the tests among normal adults and children.
Though there are numerous and valuable reviews of parts of
the field of personality assessment, and these are listed in the
short bibliography a t the end, no book dealing with the whole
of it appears to have been published since Symonds’s Diagnosing
Personality and Conduct in 1981, and none a t all by a British
author. (Special mention should be made of R. B. Cattell’s
Description and Measurement o f Personality and H. J . Eysenck’s
Dimensions of Personality; b u t both of these are primarily
outlines of the writers’ own contributions.) I have tried to
cover the majority of British publications, but the American
literature is by now so vast th a t I have had to be highly
selective. My judgments as to w hat to leave out are doubtless
open to question, but I believe th a t sufficient references are
supplied in the footnotes or the bibliography to enable the
research worker to follow up any topic th a t I appear to have
slighted. The book is written mainly for psychology students,
but it avoids technicalities as far as possible and will, it is
hoped, be useful to the intelligent layman who wishes to know
what psychology can contribute to the im portant problem of
personality assessment. Chapter I deals with fundamental
theoretical and methodological m atters ; it may,, if preferred,
be read last rather than first. The final chapter sums up the
practical implications of the survey.
My interest in personality psychology was first aroused by
the writings of Gordon W. Allport, and I owe more than I can
say to the stimulus of working with him during 1980-1981 and
1987. Thus I have ventured to dedicate this to him, although
I have diverged from his views in many respects. I am
particularly grateful also to Mark A. May and Henry A.
Murray for their inspiration, and for the help th a t they gave
with my studies while I was in America.
Acknowledgements are also due to the following for permission
to quote illustrative items from published tests :—
Dr. E. A. Doll, The Training School, Vineland, New Jersey-—
The Vineland Social Maturity Scale. Dr. D. A. Laird, Colegate
Foreword ix
University, Hamilton, N.Y.— Personal Inventories B2 and C8.
Professor T. F. Lentz, Washington University, St. Louis, Mo.—
C-R Opinionaire. Dr. E. J . Shoben and Genetic Psychology
Monographs—Scale of Parental Attitudes to Child Adjustment.
Dr. Lydia Jackson, London—Test o f Family Attitudes. F. H. and
G. W. Allport and Houghton Mifflin Co., Boston, Mass.—
Allport’s A -S Reaction Study and Allport-Vernon Study of
Values. Stanford University Press—Strong’s Vocational Interest
Blank, Bernreuter’s Personality Inventory, and Willoughby’s
Emotional Maturity Scale. Bureau of Publications, Teachers
College, Columbia University, New York—G. B. Watson’s Test o f
Public Opinion and Mailer’s Character Sketches. C. H. Stoelting
Co., Chicago, 111.—Woodworth’s Personal Data Sheet and
Pressey’s X -0 Test.
P.E.V.
This page intentionally left blank
CONTENTS
I. Introduction 1
II. The Interview : Its Reliability and Validity 20
III. Physical Signs of Personality 82
IV. Expressive Movements 40
V. Simple Behaviour and Cognitive Tests 68
VI. Miniature and Real Life Situation Tests 88
VII. Ratings and Judgments of Personality 101
VIII. Self-Ratings and Personality Questionnaires 122
IX. Measurement of Attitudes and Interests 144
X. Projection Techniques 170
XI. Conclusions and Future Developments 199
Short Bibliography of Suggested Reading 207
Index of Authors 209
Index of Subjects 215
xi
This page intentionally left blank
I
Introduction
N bringing up children wisely, and guiding them into suitable
Iaccount
educational careers and occupations, we have to take
of their personality qualities. The doctor will tell us
their physical capacities and defects ; school examinations and
psychological tests will give us a t least an approximate
indication of their educational abilities, intelligence and
aptitudes along special lines. B ut many a child with high
intellectual qualifications a t 11 years does not fulfil his promise
in a secondary grammar school through lack of perseverance,
weak academic interests, or emotional instability. Other
children who are dull according to tests, or in their school work,
develop into worthy members of society owing to their sound
personalities. In Personnel Selection in the British Forces 1
Dr. Parry and the writer have described the success of psycho
logical methods of allocating recruits to jobs in the Services
during the Second World War. B ut there were many individuals
who did very much better, or less well, in some employment
than had been predicted, largely because of the difficulties of
making accurate personality assessments. For example, a little
progress was made—but far too little—in diagnosing the men
with poor morale or neurotic tendencies who were a liability
to the army, or the potential leaders who would make good
officers.
Cannot the psychologist, then, devise some tests of person
ality, analogous to tests of intelligence and other abilities,
which could be applied by the teacher, the vocational or
personnel officer, or others interested in the future of a pupil
or employee ? Such tests would be of the utm ost value too to
Child Guidance Clinic workers, and to those concerned with
abnormal personalities such as delinquents and criminals,
neurotics or the insane. Much unhappiness and failure, not
only in school or employment, but also in marriage, might be
1 Cf. Bibliography. N .B .—Other references not listed in footnotes will
be found in the Bibliography at the end of the book.
X
a Personality Tests and Assessments
obviated by scientific personality testing. B ut though an
enormous amount of research and experiment on tests has, in
fact, been carried out by psychologists during the past thirty
years or so, the answer to our question is indubitably negative.
We shall see in this book th a t many personality qualities can
be measured or diagnosed fairly effectively, but th a t the methods
are far too elaborate and time-consuming, or far too dependent
on the skill and experience of the psychologist, to be generally
applicable for any practical purpose, or to be used by anyone
not specially trained. True, it is possible to suggest some
improvements on the unreliable methods th a t the average
layman habitually employs. Moreover, the outlook is more
hopeful in certain fields, such as the measurement of interests
and attitudes. B ut in the writer’s opinion it is safer to be
pessimistic regarding the future of personality testing in general,
and one of the objects of this chapter is to explain why.
PERSONALITY TRAITS
EXTRAVEP . INTROVERT
(SOCIAL) (UNSOCIAL)
Persis Neurotic,
tence Unstable
UNDEPENO-
ABLE
Fig. 1.— Diagram of Relations between Main Personality Dimensions.
PERSONALITY TYPES
RE X . T A B T 1 . I T Y AND VALIDITY
3
22 Personality Tests and Assessments
valid—the extent to which the method predicts, or correlates
with, some external criterion. As shown in Chap. I, the validity
of a test or interview can be investigated : (a) by its correlations
with other samples of behaviour or opinion (e.g. ratings) which
are presumed to cover the same tra it or q u a lity ; (b) by its
correlation with a trait-composite or factor ; (c) by its capacity
to discriminate between groups known on other grounds to
differ in personality, such as different types of neurotic patients,
or successes and failures in a job.
Reliability is used to indicate the trustworthiness or stability
of the test itself, ap a rt from its representativeness or capacity
for predicting anything else. Different kinds of tests or
judgments possess different kinds of reliability and unreliability,
and it will be as well to list some of the main ones. In sub
sequent chapters the type th a t is mentioned should usually
be clear from the context.
1. The agreement between two or more persons in their
judgm ents of, or decisions about, the personalities or traits of a
group of individuals or ‘ subjects This applies to interview
assessments, to diagnoses of m ental patients by psychiatrists,
to ratings, and to interpretations of personality based on
projection tests, or expressive movements.
2. The agreement between two or more observers in recording
specified types of behaviour ; also the agreement between two
or more scorers in sorting the responses of subjects to projection
tests into the same categories (apart from any interpretation of
the behaviour or the responses).
8. The agreement between scores or ratings received by the
same subjects when the test, interview, or other method is
repeated. Any method can be investigated in this way, b u t it
is seldom done, both because the subjects’ responses may differ
when they meet a situation a second time, and because person
ality qualities themselves are adm itted to be somewhat unstable
or liable to fluctuation. This may be denoted as repeat
reliability.
4. The agreement between scores derived from one half of
the test and those from the other half (e.g. the corrected odd-
even technique), or the consistency of responses to all the items
(Kuder-Richardson technique). The comparison of scores on
two alternate forms of a test given on different occasions is
interm ediate between Nos. 8 and 4. So is the method some
The Interview : Its Reliability and Validity 28
times applied to ‘ time-sampling ’ (p. 94), where records of
behaviour on alternate days over a considerable period may be
inter-correlated. This type of reliability—better termed
consistency—chiefly applies to objective tests and question
naires, and to certain projection test scores (e.g. Rorschach).
I t can be studied too when a judge gives ratings on several
items presumed to cover the same trait, as in third-person
questionnaires (p. 109).
5. A number of tests such as the Haggerty-Olson-Wickman
Behavior Schedules, Kent-Rosanoff word association, Strong
Interest Blank, etc. are scored by the resemblance of the
subject’s responses to those of a given group (mental patients,
people in different vocations, etc.). Such scores tend to be
unstable unless the standardization group was very large. In
this instance, reliability can best be established by w hat is
called cross-validation. Two groups of, say, m ental patients
are used, and two scoring keys developed which differentiate
them from normal persons. Group I is then scored on Group
I l ’s key, and vice versa. The reliability of the keys is shown by
the extent to which each group is differentiated from normals
by the other’s key. I t is generally recognized nowadays th a t
the same kind of procedure should be applied in all validatory
studies, particularly of batteries of tests. Validities established
within a single group only are often very unreliable.
T a b le I
Correlations
2 yr. 4 yr.
Paper qualifications only, judged by 2 psychologists . •17 -22
Paper qualifications -f 1 hour’s interview •15 -25
Judgments based on paper qualifications + a number of
objective tests . . . . . . •27 •28
Separate objective tests only : Miller Analogies •17 •80
Certain scores on Guilford-Martin Personality Inventory •22 •16
Strong Interest Blank, scored for psychologist . •25 •20
Strong Interest Blank, scored for clinical psychologist . -32 •16
Judgments based on above data -f a series of projection tests •29 ■26
Separate projection tests, each given by an independent
tester
Rorschach Inkblots . . 12 •05
Thematic Apperception . '1 1 •12
Sentence Completion (group test) . . 19 •21
D itto + a further intensive interview . . -27 •26
Judgments o f a team based on all the above . -24 ■30
D itto after observing 4 Situations 1 tests (final prediction) . -20 •38
Situations alone judged by 3 independent observers . -27 ■22
T able I I
CONCLUSIONS
4
88 Personality Tests and Assessments
into the somatotypes of officer candidates and air-force pilots,
but there seems to be no evidence th at they are of any value in
selecting men of suitable temperament. Like Kretschmer’s
types, somatotypes probably depend considerably on age.
EXPRESSIVE MOVEMENTS
5
54 Personality Tests and Assessments
treatm en t of neurotic and unstable patients largely on th e
practice of progressive m uscular relaxation. H e describes
an instrum ent—th e neurovoltm eter—for sum m ating action
potentials, and thus m easuring th e to ta l contraction tendencies
of, say, th e relaxed arm . As yet, however, there is no direct
Straight lines
Although the tester can base his judgments on the full range
of expressive movements, including conversation, the method
has serious limitations. The subject is in an unusual situation,
and there is no guarantee that he will react to the problems set
before him in the same manner as he habitually reacts in daily
life. Particularly in testing adults, it is difficult to produce a
natural, spontaneous, atmosphere. I t is only too easy, also,
to jump to conclusions from slight evidence, to misinterpret
facial, vocal, or other expressions, or to be biased by chance
resemblances to acquaintances, by general like or dislike of the
testee, or by hasty first impressions, etc. On the other hand
the psychologist usually has the advantage of coming fresh to
each testee, uninfluenced by previous knowledge of him.
Clearly everything depends on his impartiality, experience, and
intuitive skill.
In one experiment by the w riter,1 25 students were observed
by 3 testers (2 of them quite inexperienced) in three different
performance test sessions. The average agreement between
their impressions, determined by the m atching of personality
sketches, was represented by a correlation of -72. They also
rated the subjects on Practical Intelligence, Quickness, and
Impulsiveness, Extraversion-Introversion, and Emotional
Stability. The average inter-correlation of -56 shows a con
siderable am ount of variation either in the subjects’ behaviour
or in the testers’ interpretations. Nevertheless the correlations
of the summed judgm ents with composite measures of the same
traits averaged -50 (-48 to -61), showing a very promising
validity.
The method can be made still more useful by including a wide
variety of tests which will stimulate the subjects to display more
significant behaviour. Ordinary individual intelligence, educa
tional and performance tests are hardly provocative enough. It
can also be developed in such a way as to yield quantitative
indices at least of certain traits, and thus reduce the amount of
subjective judgment. A good example of this is the Q or Quality
score in the Porteus Mazes,* based on the number of times the
subject crosses or touches the printed lines, cuts corners, starts
to go up wrong turnings, lifts his pencil, etc. Porteus states
1 Vernon, op. cit., p- 49.
* Porteus, S. D ., Qualitative Performance in the Maze Test. Vineland,
N .J. : Smith Printing House, 1942.
GENERAL RATING SCALE FOR QUALITATIVE OBSERVATIONS
DURING TESTING AND INTERVIEWING
Name................................................ Date Examiner
ACTIVITY
Excited, restless, unable to keep still Impulsive
Quick and vivacious
Stable
Calm and deliberate Cautious
In e rt and listless Inhibited
Poses, m otor a ttitu d es............................................................................................
T ic s .............................................. N ail-biting.................... Twitchings
Fiddling with m a te ria l.............. Clothes............... H ands............... Feet
Peculiar expressions....................................Excessive wrinklings.......................
MOVEMENT
Fluent and graceful
Accurate and well-controlled Quick stride and movements
Angular and awkward Slow stride and movements
Clumsy
PHYSIQUE AND BEARING
Impressive in bearing Healthy looking, well developed and nourished
Satisfactory impression
Unimpressive I Unhealthy, feeble physique
Forceful, efficient, energetic, upright posture and gait
Slouching gait
Weak, Inefficient movements and bearing
Plump (pyknic)proportions Florid
Well and symmetrically proportioned
Thin (asthenic) Pale
PERSONAL APPEARANCE AND EXPRESSION
A ttractive and good-looking (positive reaction)
Pleasant Sensual.....
Uninteresting, indifferent attractiveness
Ugly and repulsive (negative reaction) Effeminate
Strong expressiveness of face and gestures Frank
Expressionless I Secretive
Quick and strong sense of humour Cheerful, optimistic
Slow b u t sure
Unable to see humour
IDepressed, melancholy
S E L F -A S S E R T IO N
P o m pous a n d o verbearing
C om placent D ecisive
S elf-confident a n d possessed
W avering
S elf-critical a n d d ep re cato ry
E m b a rrassed , b ashful, self-conscious C o n trasuggestible
A nxious, ap prehensive
Subm issive, re tirin g S uggestible
C O -O P E R A T IV E N E S S
W illing to co -o p erate in ev e ry r e s p e c t; en ters In to sp irit
R eserved a n d form al
C onstrained a n d suspicious, ou tsid e th e situ a tio n
S u rly a n d hostile
S crupulous, p u n c tu a l a n d reg u la r in a tte n d a n c e a n d ap p licatio n
In d u strio u s
E asy-going, indifferent
L az y an d irre g u la r
A L E R T N E S S A N D C O N C E N T R A T IO N
In te llig e n tly a tte n tiv e , w ide-aw ake
C o n c en tra ted
A bsent-m inded
E asily d istra c te d , in a tte n tiv e
T E S T R E A C T IO N S : P L A N N IN G
A n aly tical
S erious b u t u n sy ste m a tic P ro fits b y p a s t ex perience
T rial a n d e rro r
H a p h a z a rd R e p ea ts sam e m istak es
E M O T IO N
W ild a n d u n re stra in e d em o tio n al beh av io u r a n d rem a rk s
W ilful a n d childish reactio n s, capricious
Som e loss of sclf-control a n d o v e rt em otion
H u m o ro u s a n d u n concerned
Serious, philosophical
R epressed a n d in h ib ited
S P E C IA L C H A R A C T E R IS T IC S
66 Personality Tests and Assessments
that delinquents and adult criminals make an average of two
to three times as many such errors as normal controls, but this
has not yet been independently confirmed. Foulds,1 for
example, finds few differences between neurotic and normal
adults, though different types of neurotics show some char
acteristic differences, particularly in speed of maze performance.
A series of measurements indicative of emotional reactions to
psychomotor and performance tests, together w ith rating scales,
were devised by Biesheuvela and his associates in selecting
South African Air Force pilots during the last war. I t is claimed
th a t these helped in choosing men w ith suitable personalities
as well as aptitudes. However, the evidence is equivocal, for
similar methods tried out in the USAAF gave very meagre
correlations of 1 to -2 with the criterion of passing vs. failing
pilot training. In fact they were scarcely superior to judgm ents
based on appearance alone.3 Similarly M. D. Vernon 4 attem pted
to develop methods of gauging liability to breakdown under
stress. Subjects worked a t a dotting machine (cf. p. 85) a t
high speed for several minutes, and a t other tests likely to
induce strain, and records were made of their performance in
successive half-minutes. From the upward or downward, or
irregular trend, and from qualitative observations, a somewhat
subjective rating of stability was reached. B u t no good evidence
of the predictive value of these ‘ trend tests ’ is available.
O ther more objective techniques are described in Chap. VI.
Even in group paper-and-pencil tests of abilities there is
some scope for individual differences in manner of performance,
which may express the testees’ personalities without their
being aware of it. Some people check multiple-choice answers
only when they are certain, others guess more wildly. Guilford
and Lacey 6 have shown that a consistent error-score factor
(distinct from ability at the tests as such) may be extracted,
1 Foulds, G. A., ‘ Temperamental Differences in Maze Performance.
Part I. Characteristic Differences Among Psychoneurotics ’. Bril. J .
Psychol., 1851, 42, 209-217.
• Biesheuvel, S., ‘ An Observational Technique of Temperament and
Personality Assessm ent’. Nat. Inst. Personnel Res. Bull., 1949, 1, No. 4.
• Guilford, J. P., and Lacey, J. I., Printed Classification Tests. Army
Air Forces Aviat. Psychol. Prog. Res. Rep. No. 5. Washington, D.C. :
U.S. Government Printing Oflice, 1947.
4 Cf. Vemon and Parry.
4Op fit.
Expressive Movements 67
6
70 Personality Tests and Assessments
patients tend to do better on tests scored for speed, dysthym ics
on tests scored for accuracy.
In the intellectual, as distinct from the motor, field it is quite
difficult to separate speed from * power ’, i.e. from general
intelligence. However, such tests as those mentioned below
usually yield a factor for fluency of m ental associations
(Spearman’s / ) , over and above g and v factors, which may have
some significance for personality. Here also some investigators
sub-divide it into more specialized types.1 Examples of fluency
tests used by Cattell, Stephenson and Studm an, Thurstone,
Eysenck, Rogers, and others 2 include :
W riting as many words as possible in a m inute beginning
w ith the letter S ; or words ending with ‘ tion ’ ;
W riting as m any names of animals, birds, plants, as
possible, in a minute each ;
W riting adjectives to describe a ho u se; listing names of
round o b je cts;
Giving associations to inkblots ;
Speed of free word association, or numbers of words in
continuous association ;
Suggesting objects which could be inserted a t a certain spot
in a picture.
Normal speed of reading, and productivity in w riting com
positions, or in building words from a given set of letters, have
also been used ; and several of th e above tests have been
applied orally. I t has been shown th a t manic patients score
higher in / than melancholics, and Cattell 3 claims th a t his
battery of w ritten tests scores as highly as -6 with assessments
of ‘ surgency ’ among normal subjects. Probably this is
largely attributable to halo (cf. p. 5), for no one else has
approached this figure. Petrie * found th a t / tests do not
differentiate between hysterics and dysthymics, and an extensive
and thorough research by Rogers s gave negative results. He
1 Cf. Vernon, op. cit.
1 Cattell, R. B., A Guide to Mental Testing. London : University of
London Press, 1930. Stephenson, W., Studman, G. L., et. al., ' Spearman
Factors and Psychiatry '. Brit. J . Med. Psychol., 1934, 14, 101-135.
Thurstone, L. L., *Primary Mental Abilities ’. Psychometr. Monogr., 1938,
1. Rogers, C. A., A Factorial Study of Verbal Fluency and Related Dimen
sions of Personality. Ph.D. Thesis, University of London, 1952.
5 Op cit. * See Eysenck, Bibliography. 6 Op cit.
Sim ple Behaviour and Cognitive Tests 71
applied a large variety of fluency tests and personality ratings
to a representative group of 14-year-olds. His oral fluency tests
did show some relation to ‘ surgent ’ traits, b u t his w ritten
ones none a t all. I t has been shown by Bousfield and Sedgwick1
th a t the production of responses in a typical fluency te st tends
to follow an exponential curve, and the constants for such a
curve m ight be identified with the total reservoir of the subject’s
responses and with their rate of exhaustion. Rogers found no
evidence th a t these more analytic fluency measures gave any
better correlations with personality traits.
PERCEPTUAL TESTS
JUNE d o w n e y ’s w il l -t e m p e r a m e n t tests
PERSISTENCE TESTS
EMOTIONAL STABILITY
7
86 Personality Tests and Assessments
highly. The validity of this particular score requires con
firmation ; it may be th at, as in Morgan and H ull’s Maze test,
observations of m anner of reacting to difficulties would be
more useful. The fact th a t such motor co-ordination tests as
rail-walking and manual dexterity are done badly by neurotics
(cf. p. 79) gives some support.
Cattell’s 1 C.M.S. (Cursive Miniature Situation) test is a much
more elaborate version of dotting, ;n which the subject crosses
out or encircles various kinds of lines and patterns which pass
before him at a rapid rate. If he kee ps his head and chooses
the most profitable sets of lines, he can raise his score consider
ably. Possibly the task is a bit too sophisticated except for
intelligent and/or experienced subjects. But Cattell claims
remarkably good differentiation between psychotic, delinquent,
and normal adults. Unfortunately the scoring is so tedious
th at no one else has tried to confirm the value of the test.
Probably there is scope for a test intermediate in complexity
between the McDougall and the Cattell, which could be scored
by electric counters. Kehr in Germany, and Freeman 2 in
America, have described discriminatory reaction tests which
likewise put the subject in ‘ stress ’ situations, and which appear
to have given good results. Freeman compares the perform
ances under difficult, with those under easy conditions, and
measures the time taken to recover efficiency after the stress
period.
Instability of performance at learning tests is also promising.
Thus B all3 finds that the learning curves of unstable or neurotic
boys at a high-relief finger maze are much more irregular than
those of normals. R e y 4 describes similar results with a learning
problem based on a kind of formboard which can be solved
only by trial and error. Neither test as yet gives a quantitative
1 CatteU, R. B., 4 An Objective Test o f Character-Temperament ’.
J . Gen. Psychol., 1941, 25, 59-73.
' Kehr, T., 4 Versuchsanordnung zur experimentellen Untersuchung
einer kontinuierlichen Aufmerksamkeitsleistung ’. Zsch. f. ang. Psychol.,
1916, 11, 485—479. Freeman, G. L., 4 Suggestions for a Standardized
44 Stress ” Test ’. J . Gen. Psychol., 1945, 82, 3-11.
5 Ball, R. J . , 4 An Objective Measure of Emotional Instability ’. J . Appl.
Psychol., 1929, 18, 226-256.
4 Rey, A . , 4 D ’un proc6d<5 pour ^valuer l’£ducabilit£ ’. Arch, de Psychol.,
1934, 24, 297-337. Cf. also Zangwill, O. L., 4 Some Clinical Applications
of the Rey-Davis Performance Test ’. J . Ment. Set., 1946, 92, 19-84.
M iniature and Real Life Situation Tests 87
score for instability, and inspection of the curves needs to be
supplemented by qualitative observations of the children’s
reactions to difficulties. But a more practicable and more
objective test might be developed along these lines. Thiesen
and Meister 1 report an experiment on maze learning under
frustrating conditions—namely, no solution possible and
criticisms by the tester. Alterations in blood pressure and
psychogalvanic response during stress appeared to relate to
inability to tolerate frustration, and to school adjustment in
general; but only 10 children were tested. Mirror drawing is
another learning task which has, at various times, been alleged
to show freedom from perseveration, or emotional control, or
adventurousness vs. timidity, or yet other traits. Actually it
correlates moderately with intelligence; and in a research by
the writer a score based on the number of errors during the
first four trials correlated better with a trait-composite of
* impulsiveness ’ than with one of ‘ emotionality ’.
SUGGESTIBILITY
LEVEL OF ASPIRATION
1 Cf. Wilson, N. A. B., * The Work of the Civil Service Selection Board ’.
Occup. Psychol., 1948, 22, 204-212. Vemon, P . E ., ‘ The Validation o f
Civil Service Selection Board Procedures ’. Occup. Psychol., 1950, 24,
75-95.
* Fraser, J. M., ‘ New-Type Selection Boards in Industry ’. Occup.
Psychol., 1947, 21, 170-178.
100 Personality Tests and Assessments
In one or two education areas, groups of 11-year-old children
who are borderline candidates for gramm ar school places have
been collected a t a convenient centre for a day, and observed a t
a variety of tasks not unlike the Army leaderless-group tests—
group games, constructional, imaginative, and dram atic
activities. The teachers and psychologists who watch them
apparently reach an agreed judgm ent quite readily as to which
children show most initiative, co-operation, and other desirable
personality qualities ; but again there is no evidence y et as to
how far this predicts anything relevant to gram m ar school
success. Here, too, there is some danger of children behaving
unnaturally, through a sense of the importance of the occasion,
or as a result of coaching by schools or parents. One would
have thought th a t a week or a m onth's trial period in an actual
gram m ar school, with a teacher specially trained to observe
their social and intellectual adjustm ents, would be more
diagnostic; or indeed th a t a system of interm ediate schools
before a final decision is reached a t 13 years would be even
more effective.
In conclusion : these group procedures do not constitute
personality tests. They are likely to be somewhat superior to
the conventional interview method of assessing people, because
they provide a more prolonged and varied set of situations in
which to observe and interpret. B u t they are ju st as dependent
as the interview on the skill, experience, and im partiality of
the observer, and they should be applied w ith all the more
caution because they engender in the observers an undue
measure of confidence in the accuracy of their judgments.
One m ight expect them to be superior also to the observation
of behaviour a t performance or other tests, described in Chap.
IV, because they bring out social reactions of the candidates to
their fellows, instead of only to the tester. B ut this is a dubious,
and as yet unsubstantiated, advantage since it also means th a t
the situation is more complex, less standardized, more a p t to
stim ulate self-consciousness and playing a part.
VII
Ratings and Judgments of Personality
H E object of the rating method is to draw on the knowledge
T th a t a person’s associates have acquired about him, and to
turn this into numerical estimates of his standing on various
personality traits. Let us look first a t the acquisition of such
knowledge. As soon as we meet a person we jump to con
clusions about him. We interpret his features and expressive
movements, and any actions we see or words we hear, and
arrive a t a kind of picture or schema of his personality as a
whole. Our further contacts, observations, and conversations,
help to fill in and extend, sometimes to modify, this schema.
But when we are asked to rate him and give him, say, a high
mark for Sociability or a low mark for Dependability, it is not
so much because we have observed any particular pieces of
behaviour which are representative of these traits, as because
we generalize from our total impressions. Sometimes certain
observations stand out in our minds and influence our judg
ments : he may have failed to carry out some commission, so
we call him undependable. B ut usually a whole conglomeration
of more or less unanalysed recollections and emotional reactions
is bound up in any judgment. Earlier conclusions about him
considerably affect later observations; once the schema has
been formed we tend to interpret what we see of him to fit in
with it. Thus the schema is not an objective portrait or
summary of the person. Although it may embody visual
images and verbal descriptions, it also involves an emotional
attitude or sentiment towards him. Landis 1 has studied the
reasons given by raters for their judgments, and pointed out
th a t good or bad reasons have little effect on accuracy. They
tend to be rationalizations, in the psychoanalytic sense, of
whose real origins the rater is largely unaware.
Our own theories of human nature and the meanings we
attach to various traits also affect our judgments. In the
1 Landis, C., 1 The Justification of Judgments \ J . Personnel Res.,
1925, 4, 7-19.
101
8
102 Personality Tests and Assessments
course of our lifetime, our analysis of self, our contacts with
other people, and the books and newspapers we read or the
cinema films and plays that we see, all help to build up in us
a set of stereotypes or stock personalities—the typical athlete,
the aesthete, the absent-minded professor, the pedantic civil
servant, etc. We are very apt to fit each new acquaintance into
one or other of these categories. Actions which fail to conform
are often not noticed. Hence our schemas remain primitive
and far too simple to cover the complexities of the personalities
we actually meet. (An enlightening discussion of the develop
ment of conceptions of people among children and poorly
educated adults is given by W atts.1 The interplay between
our linguistic education and our understanding of people
deserves much more study.)
The result, as mentioned in Chap. I, is the halo phenomenon.
Either our general liking or disliking affects our judgments of
what should be distinct tra its ; or our schema embodies so
strong an impression of one personality type or trait—say
joviality, devotion to work, selfishness, or their opposites—that
we interpret all other behaviour and rate other traits to accord
with this. The subjectivity of judgments of personality is
apparent whether these judgments are expressed in a free
description (witness the varied interpretations of Napoleon’s
or Hitler’s personalities by different authors), or as ratings.
Although the latter are given in more standardized form, and
the rater is usually warned to avoid halo, yet discrepancies
between raters are probably as great as, or greater than,
between biographers because of ambiguities in the interpretation
of traits to be rated, and variations in standards of judgment.
Ratings are therefore best regarded as samples of the ‘ reputa
tion ’ of the subject in the eyes of the rater. They are most
inadequate as sole criteria of a person’s traits, or as the sole
source of data for the scientific study of personality. Yet at
the same time they cover a much wider range of more natural
behaviour than any practicable battery of personality tests or
time-samples, and they have the tremendous advantage of
being applicable without taking up the time of the subjects—
even without their knowing anything about it. In an extensive
research by the writer on a small group of students, the average
‘ W atts, A. F ., The Language and Mental Development of Children.
London : Harrup, 1944.
Ratings and Judgments o f Personality 108
validity coefficient of sets of associates’ ratings, when compared
with trait-composites, was +-60, whereas most of the b etter
objective tests yielded coefficients between -30 and -45. U n
doubtedly, then, ratings are useful, and they should be included
in any approach to the assessment of people, provided th at certain
precautions are observed. Indeed they have probably been more
widely used (in the form of school reports and record cards, merit
ratings in industry, etc.) and more thoroughly studied than any
other psychometric technique except the intelligence test.
I t should be noted th at ratings overlap with many of the
more objective methods described in previous chapters, and
particularly with the expressive methods of Chap. IV. There
we saw that measurements of the speed, extent, pressure, etc., of
expressive behaviour were seldom as diagnostic as judgments
of the behaviour by an impartial observer. (Nevertheless
there is much to justify the argum ent1 that a properly weighted
combination of measurements from such tests as Porteus
Mazes, the Luria apparatus, or some form of dotting or stress
test, would be more accurate than subjective interpretation.)
Time-sampling and group observational methods are kinds of
rating, and the former—when applicable—is superior to the
latter just because it leaves so little to the judgment of the
observer. Some researches based on ratings such as Webb’s,
Burt’s, and Newcomb’s 2 have required the judges to observe
their subjects systematically over a considerable period; and
several rating devices mentioned below try to make the judges
rate more from direct observation than from generalized
recollections. These steps should help to make their schemas
fuller and more impartial, though they certainly do not elim
inate all halo, stereotypy, and bias.
RATING TECHNIQUES
A B C D E
T 24 38 24 7%
A B C D E
8 60 30 7 0%
Here are three people who are very changeable. You can
never depend on them. They are offended and
annoyed very easily.
( 1 )..................... - (2 )----- ------------- --- (8 ).....................~
JUDGING ABILITY5
MULTIPLE TESTS
Testees are not told about these traits, and the questions
are so arranged that they are unlikely to guess that six
deal with carefulness, six with worry, etc. Each question is
answered Yes, Yes ?, 0, No ? or No, or om itted ; and is
scored 4 to 0. Thus there is a possible range of 24 to 0 for
each trait. Naturally the traits overlap considerably, and a
factor analysis by the writer of the scores of 100 students
> Flanagan, J. C., Factor Analysis in the Study of Personality. Stanford,
Cal. : Stanford University Press, 1035.
* Bell, H . M., Adjustment Inventory. Stanford, Cal. : Stanford University
Press, 1934.
* Boyd, W., ‘ A New Personality Test ’. Scot. Educ. J ., 1989, Sept.
lst-1 5 th , 998-999, 1014-1010, 1024-1025.
Self-Ratings and Personality Questionnaires 188
indicated th a t they could be boiled down to three or four
distinct tendencies :
(1) Self-depreciatory and psychoneurotic tendency—a general
factor particularly strong in the scores for Depression, In
stability, Worry, Lack of Self-Control, Shrinking Responsibility,
and Lack of Confidence.
(2) ‘ Care-freeness ’, most marked in Shrinking Responsibility,
Suggestibility, Inability to Concentrate, Lack of Definite
Interests, and in low scores on Worry, Self-Consciousness,
Emotional Thinking, Dissociation, and Tenseness.
(3) ‘ Scrupulousness most marked in Obsessional Careful
ness, Acting Readily w ithout Pressure, Suspiciousness, Self-
Control, and Low Instability, Emotional Thinking, and
Inability to Concentrate.
(4) A sex difference factor.
These results indicate the dangers of taking questions a t
their face value. Even when multiple tests are devised to
measure traits distinguished by factor analysis, the resulting
scores tend to be far from distinct. An example of this is
provided by the :
Guilford-Martin Temperament Profile Chart.1 In several
publications Guilford has analysed correlations between
typical items from extraversion-introversion tests, and claimed
to break down this tra it into separate components. Two of his
tests attem p t to measure such factors, n am ely:
10
184 Personality Tests and Assessments
factor as does the Bernreuter Inventory.1 A third test included
in Guilford’s Profile Chart is his Personality Inventory I. This
aims to pick out trouble-makers and paranoid individuals in
business and industry by means of three groups of items
measuring : Objectivity, Agreeableness, and Co-operativeness.
I t has been shown to correlate moderately with ratings of
employees,* b u t whether it would work equally well as a
selection test, when testees are on the defensive, is not known.
Cattell's 16 P .F . Test.3 Perhaps the most am bitious question
naire is the one constructed by Cattell to cover the twelve
‘ source traits ’ of his personality factor analysis, together with
four additional traits (radical-conservative, self-sufficiency,
will-control, and nervous tension). There are two parallel
forms, each containing 187 items. They are designed for college
student level, but a children’s edition is being prepared. The
am ount of overlapping among the sixteen scores is not stated,
and no evidence of validity is so far available.
BIOGRAPHICAL INVENTORIES
DISCUSSION OF PAPER-AND-PENCIL
PERSONALITY TESTS
VALIDITY
1 A scale value will be less than 1 if more tJtian 50% of judges place it in
the first pile. Its median position is then found by extrapolation. The
same applies to statements placed by more than 50% in the last pile.
More often one or other of the quartiles falls outside the graph, in which
case Q is read off from the median and the other quartile instead o f from
both quartiles.
154 Personality Tests and Assessments
scale value vary widely, it is obviously unsatisfactory and
should be elim inated. On a 9-point scale, Q should seldom
exceed 2 0, and an average of less than 1 5 should be
aim ed at.
To make up the final scale, about twenty to thirty statements
are selected which are : (a) fairly evenly spaced throughout
the scale. Thus there might be three items having each value
1 + , 2 + . . . 8 + ; (b ) low in Q ; (c) heterogeneous in content.
The tester must still use his subjective judgment to ensure th a t
the scale contains a variety of expressions of attitude. Thurstone
does not mention this point, but he describes an ‘ index of
similarity ’ or ‘ irrelevance ’, which would tend to reduce
heterogeneity. It is based on the responses to the statements
of a large group of testees, and resembles a correlation between
each statem ent and every other. Actually, few attitude testers
have ever adopted this.
I t m ight be thought th a t the judges' own attitu d e s would
affect the results of their scaling, b u t experim ents prove th a t
this is not so. Possibly it is unwise to apply scales sorted by
adults to children, since the meanings they read into th e item s
m ay differ. The T hurstone m ethod is laborious, b u t it does
yield scales which * go down ’ well w ith m ost subjects. The
fact th a t they need check only a few statem ents, instead of
having to give a Yes, No, or graded response to every item , is
an advantage. A m ajor defect in m any scales is th a t th e scores
are insufficiently reliable. This m ay be due to vagueness or
m ultidim ensionality in the a ttitu d e itself, or to th e small
num ber of item s th a t subjects check. I t is desirable, therefore,
to try ou t a new scale on a group of one or two hundred people,
and to calculate their scores separately on the odd-num bered
and even-num bered items. These two scores should correlate
to a t least -74 if the scale as a whole is to have a corrected split-
half reliability of -85. N ote th a t T hurstone scale units, although
equal-appearing, are no t absolute. I t is n o t legitim ate to
com pare an individual’s score on two or more scales (except
via percentiles); nor can any one score, say 5, be designated as
a neutral attitu d e . The G uttm an ty p e of scale is th e m ost
successful in establishing the point where favourable changes
over to unfavourable.1
1 Cf. Guttm an, L., ‘ On Festinger’s Evaluation of Scale Analysis
Psychol Bull., 1947, 44, 451-465.
Measurement o f Attitudes and Interests 155
MULTIPLE-CHOICE SCALES
DISCUSSION
INTERESTS
M ASCULINITY -F E M IN IN IT Y TESTS
Though relatively little used nowadays, this was not only the
first projection test, but also one of the earliest methods for
exploring mental differences—being developed by Galton in
1879. Usually a list of 50 to 100 stimulus words is read out by
the te ste r; to each word the subject responds with the first
word that comes to mind. He is told not to search about for
particularly apt associations. Many of the associations are
superficial verbal habits—opposites, rhymes, genus-species, etc.
(Jung’s ‘ objective ’ ty p e); e.g. black-white, father-mother.
But a few stimuli may touch on emotional complexes and lead
to personal (Jung’s 4 subjective ’ or ‘ egocentric ’) responses.
Often these are accompanied by signs of embarrassment,
blocking, or laughter, and by a slow reaction time (2 seconds or
more) or complete failure to respond. These so-called complex
indicators draw attention, as it were, to a sore spot in the
personality, which repays fuller exploration. Some testers go
through the list a second time and ask the subject to reproduce
his original responses. Failures of reproduction are also
considered significant.
The first systematic studies of the diagnostic possibilities
of the test among psychoneurotic patients were carried out by
Jung.1 H e also experimented with the psychogalvanic reflex,
whose deflections provide another sign of tension (though
they do not, as W hately S m ith 2 claimed, closely parallel
lengthened reaction times). More fruitful is the Luria technique
of recording voluntary and involuntary muscular accompani
ments (cf. p. 54).
Jung’s list of 100 words is often used, since it contains
stimuli likely to evoke many common complexes; Rapaport,
Gill, and Schafer provide an alternative. Kent and Rosanoff’s 3
list was selected for a different purpose and avoids words likely
to ‘ call up personal experiences ’. C attell4 gives a list suitable
1 Jung, C. G., Studies in Word Association. London : Heinemann, 1918.
* Smith, W. W., The Measurement of Emotion. London : Kegan Paul,
1922.
* Kent, G. H ., and Rosanoff, A. J . , 4 A Study o f Association in Insanity’.
Amer. J . Insanity, 1910-1911, 67, 37-96, 317-390.
4 Cattell, R. B ., A Guide to Mental Testing. London: University*
London Press, 1936.
Projection Techniques 178
for application to children, and another by Boyd (unpublished)
is used a t several Scottish Child Guidance Clinics. ‘ Chain ’
or continuous association tests are sometimes preferred, where
the subject is instructed to say everything th a t comes to
mind in connection with a given stimulus word or words.
Meltzer,1 for example, studied children’s attitudes to their
parents by getting them to ‘ think aloud first about some
innocuous words like ‘ table ’ and ‘ ball ’, then ‘ father ’ and
* m other ’. H e recorded the first ten associations to the
latter.
The clinical or qualitative applications of free association by
the psychoanalyst or psychiatrist lie outside our scope. The
simplest method of scoring responses is by counting the numbers
th a t fall under various types. Many classifications have been
proposed, b u t Wells and Murphy 2 show th a t there are con
siderable discrepancies when the same responses are classified
by different testers, and th a t there seems to be no correlation
between the types to which a subject is prone and his personality
traits or his neurotic or psychotic syndrome. The same
conclusion probably holds for the diagnostic scheme elaborated
by Rapaport, et al. Other measures which have been widely
investigated include the average or median reaction time, or its
dispersion, the total number of complex indicators, and the
average psychogalvanic response. None of these seems to
correlate donsistently with tests or ratings of emotionality or
other personality traits. A few more suggestive findings
deserve mention. C a n tril3 showed th a t persons with high
scores on the Study o f Values test react more quickly to
stimulus words connected with their values (cf. also Moore and
Gilliland’s use of ‘ aggressive ’ words, p. 90). Fisher and
Marrow 4 found th a t depressed or elated moods, induced in
their subjects by hypnotic suggestion, affected the content and
1 Meltzer, H., ‘ Children’s Attitudes to Parents ’. Amer. J . Ortho-
psychicU., 1985, 5, 244-205.
* Wells, F. L., * Association Type and Personality ’. Psychol. Rev.,
1919, 26, 871-376. Murphy, G., ‘ Types of Word-Association in Dementia
Praecox, Manic-Depressives, and Normal Persons ’. Amer. J . Psychiat.,
1928, 2, 539-571.
* Cantril, H., ‘ General and Specific Attitudes ’. Psychol. Monogr., 1932,
42, No. 192.
‘ Fisher, V. E., and Marrow, A. J., * Experimental Study of Moods
Char, dk Person., 1934, 2, 201-208.
174 Personality Tests and Assessments
the speed of response. Meltzer 1 was able to classify children’s
chain associations fairly reliably under such headings as
pleasant vs. unpleasant tone, attachm ent to parents, level of
socialization, and then to study the kinds of homes in which
healthy and unhealthy attitudes appeared.
An entirely different approach to the quantification of word
associations was put forward by K ent and Rosanoff * in 1910-11.
(This was the prototype of the empirical method of standardiza
tion, later adopted by Haggerty, Olson, and W ickm an; by
Bernreuter, Strong, and others in different contexts.) They
tabulated all the responses of 1000 miscellaneous normal persons
to their special 100-word list, and noted the frequency of each
response. When a new subject takes the same test, the fre
quency values of all his responses are summed to give a measure
of w hat is called his idiosyncrasy (a low score) or commonality
(a high score). Alternative simpler forms of scoring are to
count the number of common responses (i.e. the modal or most
frequent responses of the standardization group), or the number
of individual responses (those not listed in the tables owing to
the infrequency of their appearance). Such tables are, of course,
useful only among people similar in language and background to
the standardization group ; they quickly get out of date, and
would certainly be unsuitable in Britain. Woodrow and Lowell *
prepared a new set for children, incidentally using a w ritten
instead of oral form of the test. O’Connor’s * tables were based
on the responses of 2000 U.S. industrial workers. Probably
the best plan for anyone using the test now is to score individual
responses by reference to his own group of subjects, th a t is, to
count for each subject the number of responses given by no
other subject.
The empirically scored word association test does have some
value as a measure of mental abnormality; for K ent and Rosanoff
showed th a t the average member of their standardization group
gave 7% of individual responses, whereas 247 m ental hospital
patients gave an average of 27%. The present w riter also
obtained fair correlations between individual responses and
other measures of emotionality in a group of normal students.
1 Op cit. • Op cit.
» Woodrow, H., and Lowell, F., * Children’s Association Frequency
Tables Psychol. Monogr., 1916, 22, No. 97.
4 O’Connor, J., Born That Way. Baltimore : Williams <fc Wilkins, 1928.
Projection Techniques 175
Many individual responses, low commonality, slow reaction
time and other complex indicators all overlap to some extent,
bu t their psychological significance is most obscure. Some
writers have identified idiosyncrasy with ‘ autistic thinking
others with introversion or with emotionality, some with
intelligence or originality, others with lack of intelligence, and
so on. B ut for the most part the relations with other tests and
ratings are so inconsistent th at the Kent-Rosanoff technique
appears nowadays to have been abandoned, even by psychia
trists. Additional reasons for this are the lack of satisfactory
scoring tables, and the length of time required for giving the
test and scoring.
Other applications of empirical techniques are Wyman’s
standardization against interest groups (cf. p. 168), and Kelly
and Krey’s 1 even less successful attem pt to measure children’s
character traits through word associations.
STORY-TELLING TESTS
13
182 Personality Tests and Assessments
Lennep’s 1 4-Picture test. Jackson’s * set of 6 pictures, A Test
o f Fam ily Attitudes, appears suitable for use w ith children in
this country (Fig. 6).
Unfortunately the approach to interpretation varies widely
with the theoretical background of the psychologist. Those
1 Unpublished memorandum.
1 Symonds, P. M., ‘ Interpreting the Picture Story (TAT) Method \
Amer. Psychologist, 1947, 2, 288-289.
* Harrison, R., and Rotter, J. B., * A Note on the Reliability of the
Thcmatic Apperception Test J. Abn. Soc. Psychol., 1945, 40, 97-99.
* Cf. Kelly and Fiske, Bibliography. Guilford, J. P., and Lacey, J . 1.,
Printed Classification Tests. Army Air Forces Aviat. Prog. Res. Rep.
No. 5. Washington, D.C. : U.S. Government Printing Office, 1947.
* Harrison, R., ‘ Studies in the Use and Validity of the Thenintie
Apperception Test with Mentally Disordered Patients \ Char, d- Person..
19tO, 9, 122-138.
184 Personality Tests and Assessments
of 87 cases, with a validity of 78. Sen 1 finds the reliability
of assessments by different scorers on B u rt’s qualities to
average only -4; nevertheless such qualities as Observation,
Verbal Ability, Level of Organization and M aturity gave very
promising correlations with follow-up results among high-grade
civil servants. Some attem pts have been made to objectify the
test by providing multiple-choice questions or stories to choose
from, b u t this seems to be a blind alley.
ARTISTIC PRODUCTIONS
AESTHETIC APPRECIATION
PLAY METHODS
14
198 Personality Tests and Assessments
for many of the scores have low reliability, and they overlap
considerably. Indeed when they are factorized they appear to
resolve largely into the same bipolar dimension th a t Eysenck
found. Moreover, the significance of the various kinds of jokes
is dubious. When several judges were asked to classify them
under Cattell’s 11 types, they showed little agreement. Never
theless, the test is both genuinely projective and objective, and
clearly merits further development.
XI
Conclusions and F uture Developments
A RTICLES IN T H E P SY C H O L O G IC A L B U L L E T I N