2018 Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Controversy Developmental Mechanisms and Multiple Levels of Analysis
2018 Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Controversy Developmental Mechanisms and Multiple Levels of Analysis
2018 Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Controversy Developmental Mechanisms and Multiple Levels of Analysis
291
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
Contents
INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 292
FACTS AND CONTROVERSY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 293
ETIOLOGY AND UNDERLYING MECHANISMS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 295
Causal Factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 295
Mechanisms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 297
DEVELOPMENTAL PATHWAYS AND COMORBIDITY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 299
GIRLS AND WOMEN WITH ATTENTION DEFICIT
HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 300
SURGING US PREVALENCE: WHAT ARE THE REASONS? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 303
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 307
INTRODUCTION
The neurodevelopmental condition called, in recent decades, attention deficit hyperactivity dis-
order (ADHD) spans the cognitive, behavioral, and affective domains of (a) attentional processing
and executive function (EF), (b) response inhibition and impulsive behavior, and (c) overactive
motor output. It is difficult to imagine a more controversial diagnostic entity than ADHD has
become in the late twentieth and early twenty-first centuries (Hinshaw & Scheffler 2014). Indeed,
rates of diagnosed prevalence have soared in the United States, while debates rage over the use
of stimulant medication to control the behavior of fidgety, possibly bored schoolchildren and
enhance the performance of success-seeking adults. The condition has become a fulcrum point
related to highly debated issues—for example, overreliance on biological models of causation,
faulty educational systems, and the role of the pharmaceutical industry in fueling expanded use of
medication treatments both nationally and internationally.
In the relatively limited space afforded to authors of Annual Review of Clinical Psychology articles,
I aim to present core facts and debunk relevant myths, integrate data on the neurobiological and
psychosocial factors related to the etiology and maintenance of this form of problematic behavior,
discuss crucial issues related to ADHD in girls and women, and highlight the essential need
for integrative models of both causation and intervention strategies. Throughout, I emphasize
processes related to developmental psychopathology (see Cicchetti 2016, Hinshaw 2017) with
the goal of fostering appreciation for the dynamic interplay of risk and protective factors related
to ADHD and its impairments across the life span. Indeed, like all forms of mental disorder,
ADHD incorporates developmental extremes of key spectra of behavior, cognition, and affect.
Additionally, this diagnostic syndrome is subject to equifinality, whereby multiple causal forces
may converge on parallel symptom displays, and multifinality, given that early markers are not
inevitably associated with later impairment (Cicchetti & Rogosch 1996). Optimal treatments
involve combinations of pharmacologic and psychosocial modalities that reduce core symptom
domains, promote essential competencies, and enhance resilient outcomes (Hinshaw & Arnold
2015).
At the outset, I highlight that ADHD far transcends its stereotypic presentation as problems
of fidgeting and distractibility in middle-class, elementary school–aged boys who are parented
permissively and who contend with stifling classrooms. In reality, ADHD symptoms and impair-
ments often reveal themselves during the preschool years and, more often than not, extend into
adulthood. The core symptoms are expressed in a wide range of social and academic contexts
292 Hinshaw
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
(particularly those that emphasize routine or are particularly challenging), and they are linked
with a wide range of impairments, including marked problems with interpersonal relationships;
high risk for accidental injury; and liability for major issues with substance use, risk-taking behav-
ior, unplanned pregnancy, and self-harm across the life span. The constituent behavior patterns
afflict females as well as males and are salient for diverse socioeconomic and ethnic groups. Causal
factors involve clear neurobiological mechanisms interacting and transacting with maladaptive
environmental forces. In short, when diagnosed accurately, ADHD is all too real and debilitating
in its developmental consequences, spanning diverse personal, familial, and cultural contexts. The
clinical reality of ADHD, as well as its often-severe ramifications, is unmistakable.
and (b) hyperactive/impulsive patterns (Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 2013). These behavioral dimensions
are associated with, yet partially independent of, such externalizing patterns as oppositionality and
aggression (Ahmad & Hinshaw 2016, Hinshaw 1987). Diagnosis is made when these symptoms
lie at the far end of the normative continuum and yield impairment in essential life functions.
There is no magic symptom-related cutoff point beyond which ADHD absolutely exists and short
of which it does not. Indeed, like nearly all forms of psychopathology, the constituent behaviors
are spectrum-related phenomena; dimensional accounts are usually superior to categorical models
with respect to scientific validity.
These patterns of attention and behavior came to the notice of the educational and medical
communities over two centuries ago, at the end of the Enlightenment, when educational attainment
became a universal ideal and when, subsequently, compulsory schooling for children swept through
Europe and the United States (see Barkley 2015). Given the strongly heritable nature of ADHD-
related symptoms, it is quite possible that the goal of universal education revealed the propensities
for a subset of youth to show core problems in adapting to a culture of increasingly high academic
expectations (Hinshaw & Scheffler 2014). In the United States, diagnosis transcends racial and
ethnic categories (Visser et al. 2014) and has noteworthy prevalence in girls and women.
Belying the myth that ADHD is an exclusively US phenomenon, the prevalence of ADHD
among children and adolescents is strikingly similar around the world, at least in nations with
compulsory education (Polanczyk et al. 2007, 2014; see also Faraone et al. 2003). Modal rates
include 5–7% of youth. The factors that matter most in explaining cross-national variation in
prevalence rates are methodological and clinical ones, such as the specific diagnostic classification
used by a given nation [e.g., the International Classification of Diseases (World Health Organ.
2004) versus the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)] and whether parent
report alone, versus parent-plus-teacher report, is mandated for assigning a diagnosis (Polanczyk
et al. 2007). Indeed, one key puzzle is the presence of far higher diagnosed prevalence in a few
selected nations, like the United States, potentially related to such environmental forces as edu-
cational policies and pharmaceutical advertisements (see discussion in the section titled Surging
US Prevalence: What Are the Reasons?). Overall, ADHD exists far beyond first-world, affluent
societies, as its symptoms and consequences are felt worldwide.
Indeed, the largely consistent rate of ADHD diagnosis around the globe provides circumstan-
tial evidence for the contention that the underlying symptom domains reflect a neurobiological
propensity for inattentive, unfocused, and impulsive behavior patterns that emerges when chil-
dren are placed in classroom settings, attempting to attain skills (e.g., literacy) for which the
human brain clearly did not evolve. From this perspective, the confluence of biological vulner-
ability with psychosocial and cultural forces produces the phenomenon termed ADHD, with
compulsory education serving as the triggering social factor. All-biological or all-cultural per-
spectives are therefore reductionist and short-sighted.
At their extremes, problems with inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity often yield devas-
tating consequences. Core impairments include academic underachievement (regardless of the
presence of co-occurring learning disorders), emotion dysregulation, social ineptness, high risk
for accidental injury, and a cascading set of associated problems throughout the life span. Indeed,
prospective longitudinal investigations reveal that ADHD symptoms typically incur major negative
consequences for years to come, in domains—such as problematic substance use, propensity for
self-injury, engagement in risky behaviors, display of poor health-related habits, and major prob-
lems with consistent employment—that are essential for individuals, families, and even national
economies (Hinshaw & Scheffler 2014). In addition to the direct expenditures for assessment and
treatment per se, the impairments related to ADHD incur massive long-term indirect costs linked
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
to juvenile justice, special education, and substance abuse treatment during childhood and ado-
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
294 Hinshaw
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
key arguments on sound scientific footing. For book-length compendia on this issue, the reader
is referred to Banaschewski et al. (2018) and Barkley (2015); for a state-of-the-art chapter, the
reader is referred to Nigg (2017).
Causal Factors
A thorough explanation of risk factors and vulnerabilities for ADHD could easily span a book-
length account (see Nigg 2006 for a fine example; see also Thapar et al. 2013). Indisputable
evidence exists for the substantial heritability of ADHD in childhood and adolescence, regarding
both categorical taxa and underlying symptom dimensions. Most estimates center on values near
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
0.75 (e.g., Burt 2009). With clinically diagnosed cases, the heritability appears to be even higher,
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
approaching 0.90 (Larsson et al. 2014). Thus, ADHD is far more heritable than major depression,
falling in the range of other highly heritable conditions like schizophrenia and bipolar disorder and
approaching that of autism spectrum disorders. Notably, estimates of the heritability of ADHD
in adults appear substantially lower. Yet an informative review (Brikell et al. 2015) reveals that the
key reason for this apparent discrepancy pertains to the predominant use of self-report ratings of
ADHD symptoms as the basis for most adult estimates. With cross-informant ratings or clinical
diagnoses, adult heritability figures are nearly as high as those for youth.
It is essential to emphasize, however, that heritability signifies the proportion of variance of
a given trait or condition ascribed to genes versus environmental influences, within a given pop-
ulation at a given point in time. It does not signify that any single genes are responsible for the
condition; indeed, for all psychiatric conditions with measurable heritability, combinations of a
number of genes acting in concert generate vulnerability. Moreover, substantial heritabilities do
not signify that environments lack importance for shaping individual trajectories. Furthermore,
fascinating work in genomics reveals shared genetic vulnerability across a range of major psychi-
atric disorders (Cross-Disord. Group Psychiatr. Genet. Consort. 2013). Moreover, heritabilities
may not be uniform across socioeconomic spectra, as clearly shown with respect to IQ scores
(Turkheimer et al. 2003). Finally, rising heritability estimates across the lifespan for many key
traits or disorders reveal that genes and environments are engaged in active interplay through-
out development, exemplifying the important processes of (a) gene–environment correlation and
(b) gene–environment interaction (Beauchaine et al. 2017, Rutter et al. 2006). It is clearly the case
that the genetic liability for various ADHD-related symptom clusters is potentiated by a range of
environmental and cultural factors (Beauchaine & Hinshaw 2016). Epigenetic forces are highly
likely to come into play, as exemplified by a recent report that individuals with ADHD who were
exposed prenatally to acetaminophen for several weeks underwent differential patterns of gene
methylation than did those exposed for shorter time periods (Gervin et al. 2017; for suggestive
additional data related to ADHD, see Wilmot et al. 2016).
One risk factor for ADHD is low birth weight (Nigg 2006). In fact, the lower the weight at
birth, the higher is the risk for ADHD-related symptoms, as well as for several additional learning
and motor problems. In addition, evidence is building that exposure to prenatal toxins—including
alcohol and, potentially, nicotine—can and does spur ADHD-related behavior patterns. In the case
of alcohol, a mother’s consistently high levels, or binges, can precipitate fetal alcohol syndrome,
typically involving facial dysmorphisms and intellectual disability, yet lesser amounts may trigger
fetal alcohol effects, which include core ADHD symptoms (Doyle et al. 2017). In the case of
nicotine, more complex interactions between genetic propensity and direct toxic effects may be
involved (Thapar et al. 2009).
A common stereotype is that ADHD results from overly permissive or other kinds of mal-
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
adaptive parenting styles. Except in cases of clear neglect or other forms of maltreatment, little
evidence supports this contention. At the same time, bidirectional and reciprocal effects are clearly
operative (see Bell 1968). Thus, parents of children with difficult temperaments and/or early signs
of impulsive, irritable behavior tend to respond with unsuccessful attempts to manage such be-
havior patterns (Beauchaine & McNulty 2013). The development of coercive parenting styles
(Patterson 1982) is highly likely to increase the child’s initial behavioral propensities and promote
antisocial activity (or, more often in girls, self-harm). Thus, socialization influences, though re-
active or maintaining, are still highly important. In addition, given the strongly heritable nature
of ADHD-related symptoms, it is likely that the biological parents of a diagnosed child will share
propensities for problems in attentional and impulse control, planning and judgment, and emotion
regulation. The result, too often, can be a fully transactional cycle of genetic vulnerability fueled
by discordant, even explosive, family interactions.
Can positive parenting styles protect children with ADHD? Hinshaw et al. (1997) found that
boys with ADHD whose parents showed a strongly authoritative parenting style, incorporating
high warmth plus strong limits, had unexpectedly high rates of social competence in terms of
positive peer sociometric appraisals. Intriguingly, the typically developing comparison boys in
this research program showed no such protective effect (for an additional example of positive
parenting practices related to outcomes of young children with ADHD, see Healey et al. 2011).
Of course, as with all family studies lacking genetically informative designs, it could be argued
that shared genes comprise the third variable driving both positive parenting practices and the
child’s social competence. Yet landmark investigations by Harold et al. (2013a,b) utilizing adoptive
families, in which passive gene–environment correlation was removed from the picture, revealed
that (a) early ADHD-related child behaviors induced hostile parenting responses, and (b) such
parenting practices independently predicted the maintenance of the children’s behavior patterns
years later. In short, despite the strongly heritable nature of ADHD behavior patterns, family
socialization influences are extremely salient.
Beyond parenting, interactions and transactions between impulsive child styles and such con-
textual forces as unsupportive classrooms, peer rejection, and toxic neighborhood environments
can also serve to propel a pernicious developmental course (e.g., Beauchaine & McNulty 2013,
Jennings & Perez 2017; for an example of the protective influence of friendships, see Cardoos
& Hinshaw 2011). Trait impulsivity, characterized by the preference for immediate rewards and
exemplified by high risk taking as development unfolds, is strongly heritable. Children demon-
strating this characteristic tend to reside in less structured and stimulating schools and homes, seek
high-risk contexts, and, in turn, be propelled to behavioral extremes by the responses they receive
from peers and adults in these settings, exemplifying the processes of passive, active, and evocative
296 Hinshaw
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
Mechanisms
This section discusses underlying processes and mechanisms related to ADHD. First, does ADHD
involve deficits in attention? This is a deceptively complex question, given (a) the several forms
of attention that exist (e.g., covert attention, selective attention, sustained attention, and atten-
tional capacity, each with different neurological underpinnings; see Gazzaniga et al. 2014) and
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
(b) research revealing that, except for the apparently preserved functions of covert attention, in-
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
dividuals with ADHD may well show deficits in several of these forms (Barkley 1997) Still, it
is mistaken to think that people with ADHD have inherent and cross-situationally consistent
attention-related deficits. Instead, evidence supports the contention that ADHD can sometimes
manifest as hyperfocus—the inability to detach attention from a particularly compelling stimulus
(e.g., a videogame)—meaning that ADHD is better conceptualized as a deficit in the regulation
of attention to varying environmental demands. In fact, an overriding finding is that ADHD is
associated with high levels of intraindividual variability in attentional (as well as other cognitive)
processing (see Kofler et al. 2013). In other words, ADHD-related performance is erratic and in-
consistent. Neuroimaging research suggests strongly that individuals with ADHD show intrusions
of resting-state and default-mode daydreaming neural networks when attention-focused networks
should be engaged (see Raichle & Snyder 2007). Furthermore, in a fascinating recent investigation,
both youths and adults with ADHD-related symptoms appeared to show, rather than daydreaming
or mind-wandering per se, a blank thought pattern during attentional lapses (Van den Driessche
et al. 2017). This provocative area of research goes beyond the constraints of the present review
but is well worth considering with respect to the underlying neurobiology of ADHD.
Second, a core extension of this line of reasoning is that ADHD comprises problems, beyond
problems with attention itself, with a range of higher-order EFs, including response inhibition,
planning, working memory, interference control, and error correction (see Brown 2013). As a
result, difficulties with time management and organizational skills also characterize many individ-
uals with ADHD. Recent, compelling work (Karalunas et al. 2017) reveals that, over and above
other EFs or other potential endophenotypes, developmental changes in working memory were
correlated with changes in ADHD symptom trajectory over time, highlighting the importance
of working memory for ADHD. In addition, Nigg et al. (2017) recently showed that a polygenic
risk index (summing across several vulnerability alleles) predicted ADHD, as mediated by both
working memory and arousal regulation variables, again demonstrating the complexity of causal
pathways to ADHD. Even though the EF model may help to explain the wide variety of per-
formance issues incurred by individuals with ADHD, a significant percentage of people with the
diagnosis do not exhibit EF-related performance deficits as measured by objective neuropsycho-
logical tests (Willcutt et al. 2005). The underlying mechanisms of ADHD in these cases may
have to do more with motivational processes, as discussed later in this section, or other potential
cognitive factors. Once again, equifinality is operative with respect to ADHD.
Modifying the EF model, Barkley (1997) contended that fundamental deficits in response in-
hibition comprise the core cognitive and neuropsychological issue, at least for those individuals
marked by high levels of impulsivity, although not for those with the more purely inattentive pre-
sentation of the disorder. In other words, if individuals cannot, for crucial milliseconds, suppress
a previously rewarded, prepotent response, they will not be able to engage any of the other EFs
needed for optimal performance in school, at work, or within relationships. From this perspec-
tive, then, deficits in response inhibition, characterizing trait impulsivity, are viewed as prior to
problems with other forms of EF in explaining ADHD.
A third major contention is that the core deficit involves insufficient intrinsic motivation. In
short, the heritable underlying mechanism comprises insufficient arousal, related to hypodopamin-
ergic functioning, leading to a constant need for stimulation, an aversion to delayed rewards, and
a need for high levels of reinforcement to motivate performance (for elaboration, see Beauchaine
& McNulty 2013; for strong neurobiological evidence along these lines, see Volkow et al. 2009,
2010). As a result, difficult and/or tedious tasks are anathema to vulnerable individuals, who may
require behavioral contingencies and/or dopamine-enhancing medications like stimulants to en-
hance behavioral and cognitive performance.
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
The perceptive reader will have noted the potential for integration across these seemingly
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
disparate mechanistic accounts. Indeed, extending earlier calls for recognizing both top-down, or
executive, and bottom-up, or motivational, accounts of ADHD, Sonuga-Barke et al. (2010) propose
several differing, yet interrelated, pathways to ADHD (see also Sjowall et al. 2013, Thorell 2007).
Once again, disparate underlying mechanisms are highly salient.
Can neurobiological findings provide coherence? A fascinating program of research from Shaw
and colleagues (for initial findings, see Shaw et al. 2006, 2007) reveals that children with ADHD
show a delay in the maximum thickness of the prefrontal cortex of approximately three or more
years, from 6 to over 9 years of age. Moreover, the expected thinning of this cortical area is also
delayed during adolescence, and cortical size shows substantial correlations with relevant ADHD
symptoms (Shaw et al. 2011). In addition, a plethora of functional imaging investigations implicate
frontostriatal connectivity as highly related to ADHD symptom patterns. Even so, Castellanos &
Proal (2012) caution that multiple areas and pathways are likely to be implicated in ADHD-related
behaviors, far beyond the frontostriatal model per se.
The key lesson from this necessarily brief overview is the necessity of invoking multiple levels
of analysis to begin an adequate account of ADHD and its manifestations and impairments.
The strongly heritable nature of the core symptom dimensions clearly implicates substantial,
yet complex, genetic vulnerability, with a number of neurotransmitter systems (including but
not limited to dopaminergic pathways) involved. Early in development, those children with high
trait impulsivity—who are likely to have shown difficult temperament and poor effortful control
(Rothbart 2007) in the initial years of life—begin to encounter coercive, ineffective discipline
styles, fueling an escalation of hard-to-manage behaviors (for the development of the theory of
coercive family processes, see Patterson 1982). In contrast, the strongest behavioral correlate and
predictor of academic underachievement is inattention, rather than trait impulsivity. Teachers and
parents alike may become frustrated by or develop low expectations for children with substantial
inattention and disorganization, fueling additional transactional patterns. Overcrowded schools,
ever-earlier demands for academic performance, high-risk neighborhoods, and varying cultural
expectations for conformity and attainment comprise even higher levels of analysis in terms of the
interactive processes that are involved in the development of ADHD.
Models such as the Research Domain Criteria (see Cuthbert & Insel 2013, Insel et al. 2010)
comprise a twenty-first-century attempt to transcend the static diagnostic categories of the DSM
by encompassing multiple domains of cognitive and motivational functioning embedded within a
multiple-levels-of-analysis approach ranging from basic neural circuits to higher-order psychoso-
cial processes. This model is highly reminiscent of the calls from developmental psychopathol-
ogists, for over a generation, to take into account bidirectional and transactional processes
(Achenbach 1982; Cicchetti 1984, 1990; Sroufe & Rutter 1984).
298 Hinshaw
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
elementary education? The answer is a qualified “yes,” if assessments are thorough and evidence
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
based (see Am. Acad. Pediatr. 2011). Indeed, the American Academy of Pediatrics guidelines are
predicated on the supposition that accurate, early identification may prevent years of impairment
and suffering later in the life course. The more difficult question is whether even earlier indicators,
during infancy and toddlerhood, are valid. Given the developmental normativeness of dysregulated
and obstreperous behaviors in many young boys, the risk for false-positive diagnostic errors is great.
A major challenge is to ascertain whether early markers of difficult temperament, as well as other
indicators of trait impulsivity, can accurately predict clinical levels of ADHD.
The elementary school years comprise the period when most youth with ADHD come to
clinical attention. Core impairments include academic performance issues (see review in Barkley
2015); a strong propensity for peer rejection, which can occur quite quickly, particularly when
impulsivity characterizes the behavioral presentation of ADHD (Erhardt & Hinshaw 1994; see also
Hoza et al. 2005); continued parenting stress and family discord (e.g., Gordon & Hinshaw 2017a,b);
and the emergence of comorbid psychiatric disorders—particularly externalizing conditions like
oppositional defiant and conduct disorders, as well as learning disabilities, and anxiety- and mood-
related conditions. The impairments, as well as the challenges for families and teachers alike, are
often considerable.
By adolescence, with the increasing demands of secondary school for organization and indepen-
dence, ADHD-related challenges with EFs can be quite costly in terms of academic independence
and performance. Indeed, the vast majority of children with ADHD continue to show high levels
of symptoms and impairments throughout adolescence and even beyond (see Biederman et al.
2010a). Moreover, the progression of comorbidities for the most symptomatic youth includes
elevated risk for delinquency (particularly when earlier signs of aggression have been present),
substance abuse (particularly for boys), and self-injury and suicidal behavior (particularly for girls,
as discussed in the next section). Key references for these comorbidities include the work of Molina
et al. (2009) and Lee & Hinshaw (2004), as well as the reviews of Barkley (2015) and Hinshaw &
Scheffler (2014). Clearly, given the many challenges of the adolescent years in general, the kinds
of impairments and comorbidities linked to ADHD often become more severe.
Despite the prevailing belief, a few decades ago, that ADHD (or, as it was then called, hy-
peractivity or hyperkinesis) vanished before adulthood, a host of longitudinal evidence reveals
that, in a majority of cases, symptom levels—particularly inattention and problems with impulse
control, as motor overactivity per se typically transmogrifies into mental, as opposed to physical,
restlessness—and related impairments remain high (for early accounts, see Huessy 1974, Weiss
& Hechtman 1993, Wender 1995; for long-term prospective data, see Klein et al. 2012). In a
thoughtful and provocative review, Asherson et al. (2016) emphasize the strong clinical impor-
tance, as well as neurobiological reality, of ADHD during the adult years. Interpersonal issues,
emotion dysregulation, and job-related performance decrements continue to plague many adults
with ADHD.
Two lessons emerge from this whirlwind tour of ADHD-related symptoms and impairments
across the life span. First, what is often characterized as comorbidity—technically signifying the
joint presence of two or more independent disorders in the same individual at the same time
(Caron & Rutter 1991)—may in fact be an artifact of overlapping symptoms within existing
nosologies or may even reflect a developmental unfolding of increasingly severe complications
that result from a given vulnerability. In other words, the extraordinarily high levels of apparent
comorbidity between ADHD and a wide range of additional psychiatric conditions (for a seminal
review, see Angold et al. 1999; see also Newcorn et al. 2004 for information on the impact of
comorbidity on developmental outcomes) could actually reflect, in a number of cases, unfolding
ontogenic processes that emanate from transactions between strongly heritable trait impulsivity
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
and challenging, even toxic, family, school, peer, and neighborhood environments (Ahmad &
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Hinshaw 2017, Beauchaine & Hinshaw 2016, Beauchaine & McNulty 2013). Such unfolding
processes often exemplify heterotypic continuity, the progression of developmentally unfolding
manifestations of gene–environment correlations and/or interactions into different behavioral
manifestations of an underlying trait (see, for example, Dodge & Rutter 2011).
In other words, it is not immediately apparent that trouble with paying attention and regulating
classroom behavior during kindergarten and the early elementary grades would necessarily lead
into serious substance use problems, placement in juvenile hall, or escalating patterns of self-
destructive behavior. Yet these kinds of longitudinal progressions characterize many children who
display early ADHD. Descriptive, too-often static classification systems like the DSM are not well
equipped to comprehend such essential developmental paths (see Hinshaw 2017 for elaboration).
The field is too often left with a range of multiply diagnosed individuals for whom underlying
causal mechanisms may be obscured.
Second, to understand this kind of unfolding process, a multiple-levels-of-analysis approach
is essential (Cicchetti 2008). Again, the strongly heritable trait of impulsivity, reflecting products
of genetic and epigenetic processes, yields highly maladaptive outcomes when the development
of structural and functional brain regions and pathways fails to meet age-related expectations (see
Shaw et al. 2006, 2007; Castellanos & Proal 2012). These outcomes are particularly likely when
difficult parent–child interactions, unhealthy peer relationships, and unequipped school settings
potentiate and further shape the behavioral manifestations of underlying biological propensities. In
short, as emphasized throughout this review, ADHD lies at the confluence of genetic vulnerability
and environmental potentiation.
300 Hinshaw
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
By adulthood, however, the sex ratio narrows, signaling that (a) women are more revealing of
relevant symptoms than men; (b) women tend to present with inattentive forms of the condition,
which are more likely to persist than those marked exclusively by impulsivity and hyperactivity;
and/or (c) potential adult-onset cases of ADHD are more likely to exist in females (for selected
literature on this latter, controversial topic, see Agnew-Blais et al. 2016, Moffitt et al. 2015).
A relevant conceptual issue is the gender paradox (see Eme 1979, 1992). The contention of
this paradox is that, for the sex or gender with lower prevalence of a given condition—such as
males in regards to major depression—the disorder, when it exists, will be more severe, requiring a
greater genetic vulnerability or accumulation of other risk factors to lead to its onset (for relevant
information regarding ADHD, see Gaub & Carlson 1997, Gershon 2002). Despite this theoretical
interest, however, the vast majority of the research literature on ADHD pertains to boys and men.
Over twenty years ago, our research group made a conscientious effort to understand ADHD in
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
girls, including its development into adulthood. The relevant investigation, called the Berkeley
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Girls with ADHD Longitudinal Study, has contributed to the literature on girls and women with
ADHD (see also Biederman et al. 2010b).
Details of sample ascertainment, along with initial findings, were discussed by Hinshaw (2002),
as well as Blachman & Hinshaw (2002) and Zalecki & Hinshaw (2004). In short, we investigated
girls with carefully diagnosed ADHD—both the inattentive presentation and the combined (inat-
tentive plus hyperactive/impulsive presentation)—as a part of naturalistic, ecologically valid sum-
mer programs when they were in elementary school, along with an age- and ethnicity-matched
comparison group of typically developing girls. From our baseline assessments and from data col-
lected during the summer programs, we determined that the ADHD sample displayed statistically
and clinically significant deficits across parent, teacher, staff, peer, and objective-test informants,
spanning every domain measured (e.g., academic and peer-related impairments, relational aggres-
sion, friendship quality, use of special educational services, self-perceptions, family functioning),
even with strict statistical adjustment for demographic variables and IQ (we deployed these co-
variates in our follow-up data analyses, discussed below in this section, as well). Thus, we found
ADHD in girls to be associated with a plethora of real-life problems. In addition, their EFs, mea-
sured via objective neuropsychological tests, were significantly worse than those of the comparison
girls, with effect sizes in the medium range (Hinshaw et al. 2002).
All of our prospective assessments have yielded retention rates nearing 95%, removing large
attrition rates as sources of bias. By our initial, 5-year follow-up, when the girls were in their early-
to-mid-teenage years, significant impairments persisted in all domains we investigated, including
EFs, as well as signs of disordered eating (Hinshaw et al. 2006, 2007; Mikami et al. 2008). Thus,
ADHD was not a transitory phenomenon for the affected girls, as its symptoms and impairments
clearly lasted into adolescence. In fact, even in cases for which clinical symptoms dipped below
clinical thresholds for diagnosis, impairments typically persisted. Additionally, whereas adolescent
outcomes varied considerably across the ADHD sample, distressingly few girls showed clearly
resilient functioning and positive adjustment (Owens et al. 2009).
When we subsequently followed the sample into early adulthood, fascinating yet clinically dis-
tressing patterns crystallized. As expected, academic, peer-related, self-concept-relevant, familial,
and neuropsychological impairments persisted (see Hinshaw et al. 2012, Miller et al. 2012a). Ex-
cept for a few selected outcomes, including antisocial behavior and peer rejection (for which girls
with early impulsivity showed worse outcomes), few effects emerged when we attempted to predict
impairment from the childhood designation of ADHD types (now termed presentations). In other
words, girls with both the combined and inattentive forms of ADHD were at high risk for nearly
every measure of impairment we examined, including EF impairment. Moreover, predictions from
childhood EF deficits to key young-adult functional outcomes were robust (Miller et al. 2012b,
2013). Somewhat surprisingly, risk for substance use problems was not above that of the norma-
tive comparison group, and the eating-related pathology evident during adolescence for the girls
with ADHD no longer remained a significant area of concern, given relatively high rates of binge
eating and body-image distortion among the comparison young women (Hinshaw et al. 2012).
In addition to repeating key measures—essential for longitudinal research—we measured new
domains of outcome during our young-adult follow-up. First, the girls with childhood ADHD
were at high risk for experiencing intimate partner violence (Guendelman et al. 2016a). Second,
striking findings emerged for the outcome of self-harm, including both nonsuicidal self-injury
(NSSI)—including cutting, burning, and self-mutilation without the intent of ending one’s life—
and actual suicide attempts. As detailed by Hinshaw et al. (2012) and Swanson et al. (2014), girls
who had been diagnosed in childhood with the combined presentation of ADHD—that is, those
with noteworthy hyperactivity/impulsivity in addition to inattention—showed far higher rates of
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
both NSSI and suicide attempts than both those with the inattentive presentation and those in
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
the comparison group. Indeed, 51% of the girls with the combined presentation of ADHD were
engaging in moderate to severe NSSI, and over 22% had made at least one suicide attempt, over
twice as many as the girls with the inattentive presentation and the comparison girls in the case of
NSSI and three times as many in the case of suicidal behavior. In these results, early impulsivity,
rather than inattention, was the key risk factor.
Moreover, Guendelman et al. (2016b) found that, among the girls with ADHD who had
also experienced maltreatment earlier in life—physical abuse, sexual abuse, or neglect (or some
combination)—fully one-third had made a suicide attempt by our 10-year follow-up. These results
are parallel to those for another highly heritable condition, bipolar disorder, for which the presence
of childhood maltreatment predicts comorbid disorders, a worse course, and heightened risk for
suicide attempts (see Brown et al. 2005). In both instances, heritable risk is accentuated by trauma
with respect to clinically important outcomes such as attempted suicide.
Additional investigations have examined adolescent mediators of the predictive association
between childhood ADHD and young-adult self-injury. Swanson et al. (2014) discovered that,
during early-to-mid-adolescence, comorbid externalizing behavior patterns, in addition to deficits
in response inhibition (revealed from an objective neuropsychological test), partially mediated the
risk for severity of NSSI by early adulthood. Yet only comorbid internalizing behavior patterns
(e.g., anxiety, depression) mediated the risk for suicide attempts. Also, in the domain of peer
relationships, Meza et al. (2016) found that peer victimization (physical and/or verbal, as reported
by participants) partially mediated the link between early ADHD-related cognitive and behavioral
patterns and NSSI. Yet peer rejection (as reported by participants’ teachers) partially mediated
the link to subsequent suicide attempts. In short, behavioral and emotional, neuropsychological,
and peer-related factors play key roles in pathways to self-injury for the vulnerable population of
girls with ADHD.
Regarding our 16-year follow-up, conducted when participants were in their mid-to-late 20s,
space allows only headline coverage (see Owens et al. 2017). Most saliently, even for cases in which
relevant symptoms abated to the point where ADHD was no longer detectable, impairments
persisted in important domains of functioning. For example, regardless of the persistence of
ADHD symptoms, risk for unplanned pregnancy was substantially higher in the girls diagnosed
with ADHD years before (nearly 44%) than in the comparison group (10%). In addition, markers
of educational attainment (objective test scores as well as years of education and highest degree
attained) were well behind those of the comparison sample regardless of ADHD persistence.
Yet for most other outcomes, including comorbid psychopathology, self-injury, and occupational
functioning, the worst impairments were evident for the young women whose ADHD-related
symptoms had persisted over time. In all, adult impairments were substantial.
302 Hinshaw
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
Consistent with the ontogenic process perspective, Owens & Hinshaw (2016) also found that a
childhood measure of neurocognitive vulnerability, indexed from EF deficits plus teacher-reported
cognitive problems, reliably predicted the presence, 16 years later, of conjoint internalizing and
externalizing symptoms, over and above ADHD behavior patterns per se. In addition, in keeping
with a multiple-levels-of analysis approach, processes measured during adolescence and early
adulthood—particularly low self-control and poor delay of gratification (intraindividual level),
peer problems (social level), and low educational attainment (school level)—mediated the links
from early neurocognitive vulnerability to comorbid psychopathology in adulthood. Parenting
stress factors (family level) played a moderating role, such that the individual- and school-level
mediator paths were strongest in the presence of low family distress. Once again, equifinal pathways
appear to exist—and ascribing predictive effects of early ADHD to any one level of analysis would
fail to do justice to the complexity of the multilevel processes involved.
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
In sum, girls with ADHD often show a difficult developmental course. Risk for self-injury—
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
intensified by the presence of early maltreatment and mediated, during adolescence, by comorbidi-
ties, deficits in response inhibition, and maladaptive peer relationships—is substantial. Persisting
academic and vocational problems, as well as strong likelihood of unplanned pregnancy, are other
core outcomes. Multiple levels of analysis are needed to comprehend relevant developmental
pathways.
treatment is reimbursed. Third, a Supreme Court decision on Supplemental Security Income led
to the inclusion of ADHD, if accompanied by documented, severe impairments, as a covered
condition. The key conclusion is that, if a given diagnosis can lead to accommodations and/or
benefits, it will be sought.
In their discussion of forces outside the United States that are alleged to spur ever-higher
diagnosis of psychiatric conditions like ADHD, Conrad & Bergey (2014) posit that the concepts
of medicalization and globalization are central. Indeed, they discuss efforts from pharmaceutical
firms (see also Schwarz 2016), the growth of advocacy groups that pressure government insurers
to cover medication treatment, and the increased online presence of ADHD—including symptom
checklists and algorithms for self-diagnosis.
Not only has diagnosed prevalence surged in the United States, as well as abroad (see Hinshaw
et al. 2011), but major variation is readily apparent across regions and states (for an international
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
example, see Madsen et al. 2015). In the United States, such variation is dramatic, as nearly
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
threefold differences in rates of diagnosed prevalence across regions and states exist. Specifically,
the South and Midwest show far higher rates of ADHD diagnosis (and, in general, of medication
treatment for ADHD) than the Far West (Visser et al. 2014). In states like North Carolina,
Arkansas, and Indiana, an alarming rate of nearly one in three adolescent boys has been diagnosed
with ADHD at some point. Yet in California and Nevada, rates are far lower, consistent with
averages in the rest of the world.
The search for relevant factors to explain such discrepancies has a robust history in a research
area termed small-area variation (Wennberg & Gittelsohn 1973). Their formulation addressed
variability in diagnosis and especially treatment of medical conditions, positing that such varia-
tion was influenced far less by actual disease distribution than by physician beliefs about specific
indications for treatments for the condition in question. One potential explanation for the discrep-
ancies in ADHD diagnosis would be demographic factors, such as ethnic variation, which have
been related to diagnosis rates (with Hispanic families traditionally low with respect to ADHD
diagnosis). Yet adjusting for ethnic differences across states and regions did little to alter the dra-
matic differences in rates of ADHD diagnosis (Fulton et al. 2015, Hinshaw & Scheffler 2014).
That is, despite the far greater concentration of Hispanic families in California (a low-diagnosis
state) than in North Carolina (an extremely high-diagnosis state), adjusting statistically for this
demographic difference still left nearly twice as many diagnosed youth in the North Carolina. Our
research team also examined whether such well-established factors as the regional or state density
of health professionals might underlie the marked difference in ADHD diagnoses, as it does for
other mental health and physical health conditions. Yet concentrations of pediatricians or child
specialists was not significantly associated with youth ADHD diagnoses in the NSCH.
A shift in focus to educational policy proved revelatory. Specifically, changes in US policy
in recent decades provided the opportunity for a natural experiment related to the provision of
incentives to schools and school districts regarding student scores on high-stakes standardized
testing (Fulton et al. 2015). In brief, following marked concern over diminishing standardized
test scores in the United States, which began in earnest in the 1980s—and which often focused
on public schools with large numbers of low-income youth—many states began to implement
consequential accountability legislation. This legislation made public school districts accountable
for their students’ levels of achievement, such that in the absence of gains, negative consequences
would ensue (e.g., newspaper accounts, cuts in public funding to given districts). Such direct,
output-focused incentives and punishments had the goal of more immediate and dramatic test
score increases than previous input-focused policies (e.g., lowering of classroom size).
By the early 2000s, 30 US states had enacted such consequential accountability legislation,
with the highest concentration in the South, which had already shown high rates of ADHD
304 Hinshaw
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
diagnosis at the time of the first NSCH in 2003. Fortuitously for our research design, at just this
time, the educational reform package called No Child Left Behind (NCLB) became law, with
implementation starting in the 2002–2003 school year—precisely the initial year for inclusion of
NSCH’s ADHD-related questions. Via this legislation, the remaining 20 states (plus the District
of Columbia) were immediately mandated to implement consequential accountability.
Of course, our research team did not randomly assign consequential accountability to the states
in question, but with fixed-effects statistical models and rigorous covariates, we had an intriguing
contrast in place. In a follow-up 4 years later, as we had hypothesized, for the most impoverished
youth in the 21 states with NCLB-mandated accountability beginning in 2002–2003 (i.e., youth
concentrated in public, Title I–backed schools), rates of ADHD diagnoses surged compared to
those in the states that had enacted such legislation previously. Indeed, by the second wave of the
NSCH in 2007, for children in families within 200% of the federal poverty level, rates of ADHD
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
diagnoses escalated by nearly 60%, compared to a 10% increase for comparable children in those
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
states that had enacted consequential accountability some years before. No such effect emerged
between these two groups of states for either (a) middle-class and more affluent youth or (b) those
in private schools, as such schools are not accountable under NCLB.
What are the relevant mechanisms resulting in these findings? First, school districts would, in
all likelihood, be incentivized to refer their lowest-scoring youth for ADHD-related assessment
to facilitate medication and perhaps educational interventions. Second, and more nefariously,
districts might push for ADHD diagnoses of low-achieving youth, given that a special education
designation would remove that child’s scores from the district’s overall average. Evidence exists
that, before it was banned several years later, precisely this gaming of the system was practiced in
some locales (see also Hinshaw & Scheffler 2018).
Of course, the originators of NCLB had no interest in ADHD, as this legislation was an attempt
to lift school achievement nationwide. Thus, the ramifications linked to ADHD comprise what
are called unintended effects or unintended consequences. As a follow up, it is important to note
that, between 2007 and 2012 (the second and third waves of the NSCH survey data), the Obama
administration had begun to dismantle NCLB and replace it with a different set of incentives for
high-performing schools. Not surprisingly, the dramatic difference in rates of increase of ADHD
diagnoses for poor versus wealthier children vanished during this time period (Fulton et al. 2015).
Finally, additional state legislation in the United States at the beginning of the twenty-first
century has also affected rates of ADHD diagnosis. For example, as elaborated by Fulton et al.
(2015), several states implemented laws designed to curtail the rights of schools to insist on med-
ication treatment for youth with ADHD. Analysis of the NSCH database revealed that, in such
states, rates of ADHD diagnosis remained nearly flat from 2003 to 2012, whereas in the remaining
states, rates surged. Once again, policy appears to be a significant factor in explaining variation in
ADHD diagnostic prevalence.
Overall, educational practices and policies play an important role in explaining differences in
rates of ADHD diagnosis for children of different social classes and in driving ever-higher rates
of diagnosed prevalence. As reviewed above, academic performance pressures may constitute a
precipitating factor related to increases in the rates of diagnosis of ADHD through the unintended
consequences of consequential accountability statutes. At the same time, other laws—directly
intended to reduce ADHD medication pressures—result in lowered rates of ADHD diagnosis.
Does evidence exist for any actual increase in the true prevalence of ADHD? Nigg (2006)
posited that the increased presence of neonatal intensive care units, which have greatly increased
survival rates of low-birth-weight infants, may in fact increase the true prevalence of conditions
like ADHD. Still, increases in diagnosed prevalence, linked to educational policies or other so-
cial forces, appear to be far greater. I highlight once again that non-evidence-based diagnostic
assessments (e.g., a few minutes in a pediatrician’s office) are undoubtedly linked to these phe-
nomena. That is, false-positive diagnoses may well emerge from evaluations that fail to include
rating scales, thorough developmental histories, differential diagnoses, appropriate use of cogni-
tive and achievement tests, and the like. Overly quick and cursory assessments can also trigger
underdiagnosis related to ADHD, i.e., when the clinician in question fails to obtain ecologically
valid data from the individual’s school, social, or vocational settings and concludes, on the basis of
an informal office assessment, that the individual could not exhibit ADHD. Overall, if more strin-
gent diagnostic standards were taught, accepted, and reimbursed, much of the critique of current
diagnostic practices related to ADHD could be eliminated—and, I posit, school accountability
legislation would probably not result in increased ADHD diagnoses for disenfranchised children.
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
TREATMENT STRATEGIES
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
An entire review could be devoted to the controversial and essential issues related to intervention
for ADHD. In this highly selective overview, the vast literature in this area is barely touched on.
Although many forms of treatment for ADHD are touted, only two truly evidence-based
forms of intervention exist, each having received scrutiny in literally hundreds of controlled trials:
(a) medication, chiefly stimulants but also noradrenergic agents, and (b) behavioral (for youth)
and cognitive behavioral (for adults) forms of psychosocial treatment. For children and adoles-
cents, behavioral intervention chiefly involves management interventions with parents, consulta-
tion with teachers, and structured forms of social skills training with youth themselves. The overall
goals are to provide extrinsic rewards for successive approximations of important academic, be-
havioral, and social skills. For adults, extremely promising work reveals that organizational and
time-management skills as well as emotion regulation can be learned via cognitive behavioral in-
tervention strategies (e.g., Solanto 2011), although the data are far skimpier than those for children
and adolescents regarding traditional behavioral interventions. A recent meta-analysis summarizes
the voluminous controlled research on pharmacologic and psychosocial interventions for ADHD
(Catalá-López et al. 2017).
Among potentially promising forms of intervention, neurofeedback has not, in recent trials,
compared favorably to careful control conditions (e.g., sham feedback; see Schönenberg et al.
2017). Dietary interventions, formerly viewed as a panacea before controlled trials yielded far
more pessimistic findings, may yield some improvements in some cases, particularly with respect
to restriction of dyes and additives, though not in the same range of effect size as medications
or behavioral interventions (see Nigg et al. 2012). Relatively recent controlled trials of aerobic
exercise show promising results (Cerrillo-Urbina et al. 2015). Specific cognitive training—most
saliently, to enhance working memory—has been advocated, but recent reviews suggest poor
transfer of skills to real-world tasks (e.g., Mawjee et al. 2015, Melby-Lurvåg & Hulme 2013).
There are simply not enough data regarding mindfulness interventions, practices of martial arts,
or exposure to green environments, despite advocacy for these treatments. More dubious forms of
intervention, including chiropractic treatments, cerebellar stimulating exercises, blue-green algae,
and homeopathy, and other non-evidence-based interventions, are best avoided.
Stimulant medications provide reduction of relevant symptoms in a clear majority of cases,
as attested by a vast literature of controlled trials, yielding medium, large, or very large effect
sizes on average. They are generally superior to noradrenergic medications; both classes are
superior to placebo (Catalá-López et al. 2017). Even large-scale, relatively long-term investigations
attest to this finding (MTA Coop. Group 1999). Indeed, medication treatment of ADHD is
more successful, on average, than pharmacologic intervention for any other child or adolescent
psychiatric condition. Side effects can be a real concern but are often managed with adjustments
to dose levels and their timing.
306 Hinshaw
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
Yet placebo-controlled medication interventions cannot last for years. Despite the inher-
ent problems in investigating outcomes for any class of interventions based on naturalistic
investigations—in which various selection biases may well exist—accumulating evidence from
prospective interventions reveals the lack of long-term sufficiency of medications as a sole treat-
ment for ADHD (e.g., Swanson et al. 2017). Indeed, many, if not most, youth with ADHD stop
taking medication during the teen years, and longitudinal investigations reveal no conclusive
evidence that receipt of medications during childhood or adolescence slows trajectories toward
substance use disorders or antisocial behavior (Molina et al. 2009, 2013). Yet population-based
studies reveal that, during periods of receipt of ADHD medications, risk for substance abuse and
serious accidents is reduced, often substantially (e.g., Chang et al. 2014, Quinn et al. 2017). In the
long run, pharmacologic intervention alone is rarely adequate for complex, comorbid cases or for
enhancing the skills and competencies so clearly needed to counter the long-range impairments
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
linked to ADHD. Indeed, in a reconsideration of findings from trials such as the Multimodal
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Treatment Study of Children with ADHD, Hinshaw & Arnold (2015) have emphasized the need
to intervene with parents, schools, and even wider communities; to emphasize skill building as
much as symptom reduction; and to gear intervention toward amelioration of key life impairments.
In short, combination treatments appear optimal for most individuals with ADHD if the objec-
tives are to go beyond symptom reduction per se. Moreover, a number of comorbid conditions
(e.g., anxiety disorders, depression, disruptive behavior disorders, learning disabilities) require
interventions that far exceed ADHD medications.
Huge controversy surrounds the use of stimulant medications as performance enhancers for
the general population. In brief, despite continued advocacy for cognitive enhancement (neuroen-
hancement) of people without ADHD via stimulant medications (for a recent argument, see Lyon
2017), Smith & Farah (2011) and, in an experimental investigation, Ilieva et al. (2013) contend
that the effects of stimulants on actual cognitive enhancement (beyond simple alertness) are not
strong. Moreover, and the risk for developing dependence on stimulants in the general population
is considerable. These issues keep the topic ensnared in controversy.
The positive effects of behavioral interventions take longer to occur than do those of stimu-
lant medications, and behavioral interventions require considerable effort on the part of parents,
teachers, and individuals with ADHD themselves. Cognitive behavioral treatments for adults also
require time and effort. Yet skill gains and reductions in problem behaviors have clearly been
documented (see Fabiano et al. 2009), and debate continues as to whether medication, rather than
psychosocial alternatives, should be the first-line intervention for ADHD. Except for preschool-
aged children, medication is the default indication in the United States; however, elsewhere, the
consensus is that medication should be tried later (e.g., Nat. Inst. Health Clin. Excel. 2009). Finally,
as noted above, combinations of medication and behavioral interventions appear to offer the best
chance of enhancing competencies and producing normalization of functioning (see also Hinshaw
et al. 2000). Yet research on community-based interventions reveals that pediatric practices are
often suboptimal due to a lack of psychosocial or behavioral treatment options (Epstein et al. 2014).
CONCLUSIONS
The global, impairing, costly, and controversial topic of ADHD mandates multilevel efforts with
respect to scientific research, provision of evidence-based clinical services, and integrative dialog
regarding policy endeavors (Singh 2008). Critics who contend that ADHD is exclusively a social
construct—a child- or patient-blaming label to mask social ills—are as shortsighted as those who
focus exclusively on the pathology of particular neural pathways related to deficient attention or
poor impulse control, without due consideration of the kinds of social and educational contexts in
which self-regulation is expressed. The need for integrative perspectives on ADHD in particular
www.annualreviews.org • Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder 307
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
and developmental psychopathology in general has never been greater (see Hinshaw 2015, 2017;
Hyde 2015; Mash & Barkley 2014; Nigg 2017).
Furthermore, without increased advocacy for utilization of evidence-based standards for assess-
ment and diagnosis, the diagnosed prevalence of ADHD is highly likely to continue to outpace
true prevalence, especially in high-performance societies. Such a continuing trend will expose
many individuals to unneeded and potentially harmful treatments—and it will paradoxically fuel
the stigmatization that already exists related to ADHD in particular and mental disorders more
generally (Hinshaw & Stier 2008, Martinez & Hinshaw 2016).
Indeed, how could a diagnostic category revealing such rapid increases in prevalence as
ADHD—and one that receives the publicity that it does—still be stigmatized? For one thing,
people with ADHD often behave inconsistently and erratically, probably leading many in the
general public to believe that misbehavior is intentional. At a larger level, when such a condition
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
becomes linked in the public’s mind with overdiagnosis and the desire for accommodations (for
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
both children, linked to educational supports, and adults, linked to the attaining of medications),
it may well lose credibility as a legitimate disorder.
This article has emphasized a multiple-levels-of-analysis approach to all phenomena related
to ADHD. Indeed, knowledge is expanding rapidly on fronts ranging from molecular genetics
and epigenetic processes, to brain networks and connectivity, to temperament and the unfolding
of heterotypically continuous behavior patterns, and to a host of socialization forces in homes,
schools, cultures, and nations. The clear need is for scientists and clinicians to span and integrate
across such levels—no easy task, but one that is essential for future scientific efforts and clinical care.
In the end, both enhanced public knowledge regarding the neurobiological validity of ADHD
and humanization of those with the condition are crucial. Indeed, the impairments linked to
ADHD are sufficiently devastating that children, adolescents, and adults dealing with this syn-
drome require the best of evidence-based assessment, intervention, and support. It is incumbent
on those involved in scientific, clinical, and preventive efforts to insist on sound scientific stan-
dards, respectful and multilevel dialogue, thorough diagnostic evaluations, and well-disseminated
clinical and preventive efforts. Without progress on these fronts, impairments and social costs will
only grow.
SUMMARY POINTS
1. ADHD is a controversial yet all too real clinical condition marked by developmentally ex-
treme and impairing symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. The burden
of ADHD spans underattainment in academics and underperformance in vocational en-
deavors, compromised relationships with family members and peers, suboptimal health-
related outcomes, and major outlays across multiple societal systems of education and
health care. ADHD is typically a long-standing behavioral disorder, with impairments
persisting into the adult years.
2. A multiple-levels-of-analysis perspective is essential for understanding causal factors and
developmental pathways. The symptom dimensions related to ADHD are highly heri-
table; other biological factors (e.g., prenatal substance use by mothers, low birth weight,
environmental toxins) are relevant for etiology, as well. Yet discordant family interac-
tions, as well as poor fit with educational environments, can and do maintain symptom
patterns and promote an unfolding spiral of low achievement, externalizing behavior pat-
terns, and—by adolescence and beyond—risk for substance use, self-injury, vocational
problems, and lowered independence.
308 Hinshaw
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
in recent years, apparently well beyond its true prevalence and at rates above those of
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
most of the rest of the world. Increased recognition of the condition is clearly a fac-
tor, but policy changes deeming that an ADHD diagnosis can garner needed services
are also relevant. Furthermore, an unintended consequence of consequential account-
ability legislation, designed to boost school achievement test scores, appears to be an
increase in ADHD diagnoses for public school children in the lowest socioeconomic
strata.
6. Medication (usually stimulants) and behavioral or cognitive behavioral treatments are the
core evidence-based interventions for ADHD. Multimodal treatment, involving combi-
nations of carefully monitored medication and behavioral intervention, is usually optimal
for promoting competencies as well as reducing problem behaviors.
FUTURE ISSUES
1. Despite the considerable heritability of ADHD, the particular genes contributing to
vulnerability are proving elusive, as is the case with all mental disorders. Moreover, the
particular kinds of gene–environment interplays that are relevant to particular symptoms
and impairments remain largely unknown.
2. The heterogeneity inherent in the symptom presentations, underlying mechanisms, and
developmental outcomes related to ADHD developmental signifies that future research
is needed to untangle the separable and combined pathways to this syndrome.
3. Manifestations of and mechanisms related to ADHD in females and in adults remain less
well identified than in males and in youth.
4. Promotion of evidence-based assessment and treatment procedures is a major priority.
At the same time, better translational science is needed to approach the kinds of precision
intervention that are so urgently needed.
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
The author is not aware of any affiliations, memberships, funding, or financial holdings that might
be perceived as affecting the objectivity of this review.
LITERATURE CITED
Achenbach TM. 1982. Developmental Psychopathology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. 2nd ed.
Agnew-Blais JC, Polanczyk GV, Danese A, Wertz J, Moffitt TE, Arsenault L. 2016. Evaluation of the persis-
tence, remission, and emergence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young adulthood. JAMA
Psychiatry 73:713–20
Ahmad SI, Hinshaw SP. 2016. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: similarities to and differences from
other externalizing disorders. See Beauchaine & Hinshaw 2016, pp. 19–37
Ahmad SI, Hinshaw SP. 2017. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, trait impulsivity, and externalizing
behavior in a longitudinal sample. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 45:1077–89
Am. Acad. Pediatr. 2011. ADHD: clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics 128:1007–22
Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: Am.
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
Angold A, Costello EJ, Erkanli A. 1999. Comorbidity. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 40:57–87
Asherson A, Buitelaar J, Faraone SV, Rohde L. 2016. Adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: key
conceptual issues. Lancet Psychiatry 3:566–78
Banaschewski T, Coghill D, Zuddas A, eds. 2018. Oxford Textbook of ADHD. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
Barkley RA. 1997. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying
theory of ADHD. Psychol. Bull. 121:65–94
Barkley RA, ed. 2015. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment. New
York: Guilford Press. 4th ed.
Beauchaine TP, Gatzke-Kopp L, Gizer IR. 2017. Genetic, environmental, and epigenetic influences on be-
havior. See Beauchaine & Hinshaw 2017, pp. 68–109
Beauchaine TP, Hinshaw SP, eds. 2016. Oxford Handbook of Externalizing Spectrum Behaviors. Oxford, UK:
Oxford Univ. Press
Beauchaine TP, Hinshaw SP, eds. 2017. Child and Adolescent Psychopathology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. 3rd ed.
Beauchaine TP, McNulty T. 2013. Comorbidities and continuities as ontogenic processes: toward a develop-
mental spectrum model of externalizing psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 25:1505–28
Beauchaine TP, Shader TM, Hinshaw SP. 2016. An ontogenic processes model of externalizing psychopathol-
ogy. See Beauchaine & Hinshaw 2016, pp. 485–501
Bell RQ. 1968. A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychol. Rev. 75:81–95
Biederman J, Petty C, Evans M, Small J, Faraone SV. 2010a. How persistent is ADHD? A controlled 10-year
follow-up study of ADHD boys grown up. Psychiatry Res. 177:299–304
Biederman J, Petty CR, Monuteaux MC, Fried R, Byrne D, et al. 2010b. Adult psychiatric outcomes of girls
with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: 11-year follow-up in a longitudinal case-control study. Am.
J. Psychiatry 167:409–17
Blachman DR, Hinshaw SP. 2002. Patterns of friendship in girls with and without attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 30:625–40
Brikell I, Kuja-Halkola R, Larsson H. 2015. Heritability of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults.
Am. J. Med. Genet. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 168:406–13
Brown GR, McBride L, Bauer MS, Williford WO, Coop. Stud. Progr. 430 Study Team. 2005. Impact of
childhood abuse on the course of bipolar disorder: a replication study in U.S. veterans. J. Affect. Disord.
89:57–67
Brown TE. 2013. A New Understanding of ADHD in Children and Adults: Executive Function Impairments. New
York: Routledge
Burt SA. 2009. Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology: a meta-
analysis of shared environmental influences. Psychol. Bull. 135:608–37
Cardoos SL, Hinshaw SP. 2011. Friendship as protection from peer victimization for girls with and without
ADHD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 39:1035–45
Caron C, Rutter M. 1991. Comorbidity in child psychopathology: concepts, issues, and research. J. Child
Psychol. Psychiatry 32:1063–80
310 Hinshaw
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
Castellanos FX, Proal E. 2012. Large-scale brain systems in ADHD: beyond the prefrontal-striatal model.
Trend Cogn. Sci. 44:17–26
Catalá-López F, Hutton B, Nuñez-Beltrán A, Page MJ, Ridao M, Macı́as Saint-Gerons D, et al. 2017. The
pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in chil-
dren and adolescents: a systematic review with network meta-analyses of randomised trials. PLOS ONE
12:e0180355
Cerrillo-Urbina AJ, Garcia-Hermoso A, Sanchez-Lopez M, Pardo-Guijarro MJ, Santos Gomez JL. 2015. The
effects of physical exercise in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review
and meta-analysis of randomized control trials. Child Care Health Dev. 41:779–88
Chang Z, Lichtenstein P, D’Onofrio BM, Sjölander A, Larsson H. 2014. Serious transport accidents in
adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the effect of medication: a population-based study.
JAMA Psychiatry 71:319–25
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
Protective Factors in the Development of Psychopathology, ed. J Rolf, A Masten, D Cicchetti, K Neuchterlein,
S Weintraub, pp. 2–28. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
Cicchetti D. 2008. A multiple-levels-of-analysis perspective on research in development and psychopathology.
In Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, ed. TP Beauchaine, SP Hinshaw, pp. 27–57. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
2nd ed.
Cicchetti D, ed. 2016. Developmental Psychopathology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. 3rd ed.
Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. 1996. Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Dev. Psy-
chopathol. 8:597–600
Conrad P, Bergey MR. 2014. The impending globalization of ADHD: notes on the expansion and growth of
a medicalized disorder. Soc. Sci. Med. 122:31–43
Cross-Disord. Group Psychiatr. Genet. Consort. 2013. Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five
major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet 381:1371–79
Curchack-Lichtin JT, Chacko A, Halperin JM. 2014. Changes in ADHD symptom endorsement: preschool
to school age. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 42:993–1004
Cuthbert BN, Insel TR. 2013. Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC
Med. 11:126
Dodge KA, Rutter M, eds. 2011. Gene-Environment Interactions in Developmental Psychopathology. New York:
Guilford Press
Doyle LR, Crocker NA, Fryer SL, Mattson SN. 2017. Exposure to teratogens as a risk factor for psychopathol-
ogy. See Beauchaine & Hinshaw 2017, pp. 277–315
Eme RF. 1979. Sex differences in childhood psychopathology: a review. Psychol. Bull. 86:574–95
Eme RF. 1992. Selective female affliction in the developmental disorders of childhood: a literature review.
J. Clin. Child Psychol. 21:354–64
Epstein JN, Kelleher KJ, Baum R, Brinkman WB, Peugh J, et al. 2014. Variability in ADHD care in
community-based pediatrics. Pediatrics 134:1136–43
Erhardt D, Hinshaw SP. 1994. Initial sociometric impressions of ADHD and comparison boys: predictions
from social behaviors and from nonbehavioral variables. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 62:833–42
Fabiano GA, Pelham WE, Coles EK, Gnagy EM, Chronis-Tuscano A, O’Connor BC. 2009. A meta-analysis
of behavioral treatments for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29:129–40
Faraone SV, Sergeant J, Gillberg C, Biederman J. 2003. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: Is it an American
condition? World Psychiatry 2:104–13
Fulton BD, Scheffler RM, Hinshaw SP. 2015. State variation in increased ADHD prevalence: links to NCLB
school accountability and state medication laws. Psychiatr. Serv. 66:1074–82
Gaub M, Carlson CL. 1997. Gender differences in ADHD: a meta-analysis and critical review. J. Am. Acad.
Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36:1036–45
Gazzaniga MS, Ivry RB, Mangun GR. 2014. Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind. New York: W.W.
Norton. 4th ed.
Gershon J. 2002. A meta-analytic review of gender differences in ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 5:143–54
influences on child ADHD symptoms: revisiting the developmental interface between nature and nurture.
J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54:1038–46
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Harold GT, Leve LD, Elam KK, Thapar A, Neiderhiser JM, et al. 2013b. The nature of nurture: disentangling
passive genotype–environment correlation from family relationship influences on children’s externalizing
problems. J. Family Psychol. 27:12–21
Healey DM, Flory JD, Miller CJ, Halperin JM. 2011. Maternal positive parenting style is associated with
better functioning in hyperactive/inattentive preschool children. Infant Child Dev. 20:148–61
Hinshaw SP. 1987. On the distinction between attentional deficits/hyperactivity and conduct problems/
aggression in child psychopathology. Psychol. Bull. 101:443–63
Hinshaw SP. 1992. Externalizing behavior problems and academic underachievement in childhood and ado-
lescence: causal relationships and underlying mechanisms. Psychol. Bull. 111:127–55
Hinshaw SP. 2002. Preadolescent girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: I. Background charac-
teristics, comorbidity, cognitive and social functioning, and parenting practices. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.
70:1086–98
Hinshaw SP. 2015. Developmental psychopathology, ontogenic process models, gene-environment interplay,
and brain development: an emerging synthesis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 124:771–75
Hinshaw SP. 2017. Developmental psychopathology as a scientific discipline: a twenty-first century perspec-
tive. See Beauchaine & Hinshaw 2017, pp. 3–32
Hinshaw SP, Arnold LE. 2015. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, multimodal treatment, and longitu-
dinal outcome: evidence, paradox, and challenge. WIRES Cogn. Sci. 6:39–52
Hinshaw SP, Carte ET, Fan C, Jassy JS, Owens EB. 2007. Neuropsychological functioning of girls with
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder followed prospectively into adolescence: evidence for continuing
deficits? Neuropsychology 21:263–73
Hinshaw SP, Carte ET, Sami N, Treuting JJ, Zupan BA. 2002. Preadolescent girls with attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder: II. Neuropsychological performance in relation to subtypes and individual
classification. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 70:1099–111
Hinshaw SP, Owens EB, Sami N, Fargeon S. 2006. Prospective follow-up of girls with attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder into adolescence: evidence for continuing cross-domain impairment.
J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 74:489–99
Hinshaw SP, Owens EB, Wells KC, Kraemer HC, Abikoff HB, et al. 2000. Family processes and treat-
ment outcome in the MTA: negative/ineffective parenting practices in relation to multimodal treatment.
J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 28:555–68
Hinshaw SP, Owens EB, Zalecki C, Huggins SP, Montenegro-Nevado AJ, et al. 2012. Prospective follow-up
of girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into early adulthood: continuing impairment includes
elevated risk for suicide attempts and self-injury. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 80:1041–51
Hinshaw SP, Scheffler RM. 2014. The ADHD Explosion: Myths, Medication, Money, and Today’s Push for Perfor-
mance. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
Hinshaw SP, Scheffler RM. 2018. ADHD in the 21st century: biology, context, policy, and the need for
integrative perspectives. In Oxford Textbook of ADHD, ed. T Banaschewski, D Coghill, A Zuddas. Oxford,
UK: Oxford Univ. Press. In press
312 Hinshaw
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
Hinshaw SP, Scheffler RM, Fulton B, Aase H, Banaschewski T, et al. 2011. International variation in treatment
procedures for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: social context and recent trends. Psychiatr. Serv.
62:459–64
Hinshaw SP, Stier A. 2008. Stigma as related to mental disorders. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 4:269–93
Hinshaw SP, Zupan BA, Simmel C, Nigg JT, Melnick SM. 1997. Peer status in boys with and without
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: predictions from overt and covert antisocial behavior, social iso-
lation, and authoritative parenting beliefs. Child Dev. 64:880–96
Hoza B, Mrug S, Gerdes AC, Hinshaw SP, Bukowski JM, et al. 2005. What aspects of peer relationships are
impaired in children with ADHD? J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 73:411–23
Huessy HJ. 1974. The adult hyperkinetic. Am. J. Psychiatry 131:724–25
Hyde LW. 2015. Developmental psychopathology in an era of molecular genetics and neuroimaging: a de-
velopmental neurogenetics approach. Dev. Psychopathol. 27:587–613
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
i24 News. 2016. Number of Israeli kids diagnosed with ADHD double world average: study. i24 News, Jan. 17.
http://www.i24news.tv/en/news/israel/society/99428-160117-number-of-israeli-kids-diagnosed-
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
with-adhd-double-world-average-study
Ilieva I, Boland J, Farah MJ. 2013. Objective and subjective cognitive enhancing effects of mixed amphetamine
salts in healthy people. Neuropharmacology 64:496–505
Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, et al. 2010. Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward
a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 167:748–51
Jennings WG, Perez NM. 2017. Neighborhood effects on the development of delinquency. See Beauchaine
& Hinshaw 2017, pp. 387–403
Karalunas SL, Gustafsson HC, Dieckmann NF, Tipsord J, Mitchell SH, Nigg JT. 2017. Heterogeneity in
development of aspects of working memory predicts longitudinal attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
symptom change. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 126:774–92
Klein RG, Mannuzza S, Olaaagasti MA, Roizen E, Hutchison JA, et al. 2012. Clinical and functional outcome
of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder 33 years later. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69:1295–303
Kofler MJ, Rapport MD, Sarver DE, Raiker JS, Orban SA, et al. 2013. Reaction time variability in ADHD: a
meta-analytic review of 319 studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33:795–811
Lahey BB, Applegate B, McBurnett K, Biederman J, Greenhill LL, et al. 1994. DSM-IV field trials for attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Am. J. Psychiatry 151:1673–85
Larsson H, Chang Z, D’Onofrio BM, Lichtenstein P. 2014. The heritability of clinically diagnosed attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Psychol. Med. 44:2224–29
Lee SS, Hinshaw SP. 2004. Severity of adolescent delinquency among boys with and without attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder: predictions from early antisocial behavior and peer status. J. Clin. Child Adolesc.
Psychol. 33:705–16
Lyon J. 2017. Chess study revives debate over cognition-enhancing drugs. JAMA 318:784–86
Madsen KB, Ersboll AK, Olsen J, Parner E, Obel C. 2015. Geographic analysis of the variation in the incidence
of ADHD in a country with free access to healthcare: a Danish cohort study. Int. J. Health Geogr. 14:24
Marks AR, Harley K, Bradman A, Kogut K, Barr DB, et al. 2010. Organophosphate pesticide exposure
and attention in young Mexican-American children: the CHAMACOS study. Environ. Health Perspect.
118:1768–74
Martinez A, Hinshaw SP. 2016. Mental health stigma: theory, developmental issues, and research priorities.
In Developmental Psychopathology, Vol 1: Risk, Resilience, and Intervention, ed. D. Cicchetti, pp. 997–1039.
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. 3rd ed.
Mash EJ, Barkley RA, eds. 2014. Child Psychopathology. New York: Guilford Press. 3rd ed.
Mawjee K, Woltering S, Tannock R. 2015. Working memory training in post-secondary students with ADHD:
a randomized controlled study. PLOS ONE 10:e0137173
Melby-Lervåg M, Hulme C. 2013. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol.
49:270–91
Meza J, Owens EB, Hinshaw SP. 2016. Response inhibition, peer preference and victimization, and self-
harm: longitudinal associations in young adult women with and without ADHD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol.
44:323–34
Mikami AY, Hinshaw SP, Patterson KA, Lee JC. 2008. Eating pathology among adolescent girls with attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 117:225–35
Miller M, Ho J, Hinshaw SP. 2012a. Executive functions in girls with ADHD followed prospectively into
young adulthood. Neuropsychology 26:278–87
Miller M, Loya F, Hinshaw SP. 2013. Executive functions in girls with and without childhood ADHD:
developmental trajectories and associations with symptom change. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54:1005–15
Miller M, Montenegro-Nevado AJ, Hinshaw SP. 2012b. Childhood executive function continues to predict
outcomes in young adult females with and without childhood diagnosed ADHD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol.
40:657–68
Moffitt TE, Houts R, Asherson P, Belsky DW, Corcoran DH, et al. 2015. Is adult ADHD a childhood-onset
neurodevelopmental disorder? Evidence from a 4-decade longitudinal cohort study. Am. J. Psychiatry
172:967–77
Molina BSG, Hinshaw SP, Arnold LE, Swanson JM, Pelham WE, et al. 2013. Adolescent substance use in
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
of childhood ADHD, random assignment to childhood treatments, and subsequent medication. J. Am.
Acad Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 52:260–63
Molina BSG, Hinshaw SP, Swanson JM, Arnold LE, Vitiello B, et al. 2009. The MTA at 8 Years: prospective
follow-up of children treated for combined-type ADHD in a multisite study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc.
Psychiatry 48:484–500
MTA Coop. Group. 1999. Fourteen-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 56:1073–86
Nat. Inst. Health Clin. Excel. 2009. Diagnosis and Management of ADHD in Children, Young People and Adults.
London: Br. Psychol. Soc.
Newcorn JH, Miller SR, Ivanova I, Schulz KP, Kalmar J, et al. 2004. Adolescent outcome of ADHD: impact
of childhood conduct and anxiety disorders. CNS Spectr. 9:668–78
Nigg JT. 2006. What Causes ADHD? Understanding What Goes Wrong and Why. New York: Guilford Press
Nigg JT. 2017. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. See Beauchaine & Hinshaw 2017, pp. 407–48
Nigg JT, Elmore AL, Natarajan N, Friderici KH, Nikolas MA. 2016. Variation in an iron metabolism gene
moderates the association between blood lead levels and attention/deficit hyperactivity disorder in chil-
dren. Psychol. Sci. 27:257–69
Nigg JT, Gustafsson HC, Karalunas SL, Ryabinin P, McWeeney S, et al. 2017. Working memory and vigilance
as multivariate endophenotypes related to common genetic risk for ADHD. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc.
Psychiatry. In press
Nigg JT, Lewis K, Edinger T, Falk M. 2012. Meta-analysis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder or
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms, restriction diet, and synthetic food color additives.
J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 51:86–97
Novik TS, Hervas A, Ralston SJ, Dalsgaard S, Rodrigues Pereira R, et al. 2006. Influence of gender on
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Europe—ADORE. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 15(Suppl.
1):15–24
Owens EB, Hinshaw SP. 2016. Pathways from neurocognitive vulnerability to co-occurring internalizing and
externalizing problems among women with and without ADHD followed prospectively for 16 years. Dev.
Psychopathol. 28:1013–31
Owens EB, Hinshaw SP, Lee SS, Lahey BB. 2009. Few girls with childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder show positive adjustment during adolescence. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 38:132–43
Owens EB, Zalecki C, Gillette P, Hinshaw SP. 2017. Girls with childhood ADHD as adults: cross-domain
outcomes by diagnostic status. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 85:723–36
Patterson GR. 1982. Coercive Family Process. Eugene, OR: Castalia
Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. 2007. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD:
a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am. J. Psychiatry 164:942–48
Polanczyk GV, Willcutt EG, Salum GA, Kieiling C, Rohde LA. 2014. ADHD prevalence estimates across
three decades: an updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Int. J. Epidemiol. 43:434–42
Quinn PD, Chang Z, Hur K, Gibbons RD, Lahey BB, et al. 2017. ADHD medication and substance-related
problems. Am. J. Psychiatry 174:877–85
314 Hinshaw
CP14CH11_Hinshaw ARI 29 March 2018 11:18
Raichle ME, Snyder AZ. 2007. A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. NeuroImage
37:1083–90
Rothbart MK. 2007. Temperament, development, and personality. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16:207–12
Rutter M. 1998. Developmental catch-up, and deficit, following adoption after severe global early privation.
English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) Study Team. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 39:465–76
Rutter M, Moffitt TE, Caspi A. 2006. Gene-environment interplay and psychopathology: multiple varieties
but real effects. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47:226–61
Safer DJ. 2016. Recent trends in stimulant usage. J. Atten. Disord. 20:471–77
Schönenberg M, Wiedemann E, Schneidt A, Scheeff J, Logemann A, et al. 2017. Neurofeedback, sham
neurofeedback, and cognitive-behavioural group therapy in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity
disorder: a triple-blind, randomised, controlled trial. Lancet Psychiatry 4:673–84
Schwarz A. 2016. ADHD Nation: Children, Doctors, Big Pharma, and the Making of an American Epidemic. New
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
Visser SN, Danielson ML, Bitsko RH, Holbrook JR, Kogan M, et al. 2014. Trends in the parent-report of
health care provider diagnosed and medicated ADHD: United States, 2003–2011. J. Am. Acad. Child
Adolesc. Psychiatry 53:34–46
Volkow ND, Wang G, Kollins SH, Wigal TL, Newcorn JH, et al. 2009. Evaluating dopamine reward pathway
in ADHD: clinical implications. JAMA 302:1084–91
Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Newcorn JH, Kollins SH, Wigal TL, et al. 2010. Motivation deficit in ADHD is
associated with dysfunction of the dopamine reward pathway. Mol. Psychiatry 16:1147–54
Weiss G, Hechtman L. 1993. Hyperactive Children Grown Up. New York: Guilford Press. 2nd ed.
Wender P. 1995. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Adults. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
Wennberg J, Gittelsohn A. 1973. Small area variations in health care delivery. Science 182:1102–8
Willcutt EG. 2012. The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic re-
view. Neurotherapeutics 9:490–99
Access provided by 2001:8a0:fdde:fd00:7418:7dc0:f225:64c3 on 11/13/23. For personal use only.
Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF. 2005. Validity of the executive function theory
of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol. Psychiatry 57:1336–46
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018.14:291-316. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Wilmot B, Fry R, Smeester L, Musser ED, Mill J, Nigg JT. 2016. Methylomic analysis of salivary DNA in
childhood ADHD identifies altered DNA methylation in VIPR2. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57:152–60
World Health Organ. 2004. International Classification of Diseases. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organ.
10th rev., 2nd ed.
Zalecki C, Hinshaw SP. 2004. Overt and relational aggression in girls with attention-deficit hyperactivity
disorder. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 33:131–43
316 Hinshaw
CP14_TOC ARI 26 March 2018 10:46
Annual Review of
Clinical Psychology
Errata
An online log of corrections to Annual Review of Clinical Psychology articles may be
found at http://www.annualreviews.org/errata/clinpsy