Introduction To Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (Ipartheory) and Evidence
Introduction To Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (Ipartheory) and Evidence
net/publication/303687938
CITATIONS READS
120 2,879
1 author:
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Ronald Preston Rohner on 31 May 2016.
4-1-2016
Recommended Citation
Rohner, R. P. (2016). Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory)
and Evidence. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 6(1). http://dx.doi.org/10.9707/
2307-0919.1055
This Online Readings in Psychology and Culture Article is brought to you for free and open access (provided uses are educational in nature)by IACCP
and ScholarWorks@GVSU. Copyright © 2016 International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology. All Rights Reserved. ISBN
978-0-9845627-0-1
Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory
(IPARTheory) and Evidence
Abstract
Interpersonal acceptance-rejection theory (IPARTheory) is an evidence-based theory
of socialization and lifespan development. It is composed of three subtheories,
each of which deals with a separate but interrelated set of issues. IPARTheory’s
personality subtheory – which is the most highly developed component of the theory
– deals primarily with the pancultural nature and effects of interpersonal acceptance
and rejection. Coping subtheory explores the fact that some individuals are better
able to cope with experiences of perceived rejection than are other individuals.
Finally, IPARTheory’s sociocultural systems subtheory attempts to predict and
explain major causes and sociocultural correlates of interpersonal acceptance-
rejection worldwide. Empirical evidence overwhelmingly supports the theory’s major
postulates and predictions, especially postulates and predictions in personality
subtheory. Emerging evidence about the neurobiological and biochemical risks
posed for the development, structure, and function of the human brain are
beginning to help explain why these postulates and predictions are so consistently
confirmed panculturally.
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-No Derivative Works 3.0
License.
United States. Additionally, it draws from literary and historical insights as far back as two
thousand years. And more importantly, it draws from and helps provide a conceptual
framework for integrating empirical studies on issues of interpersonal acceptance-rejection
that have been published since the end of the nineteenth century, mostly within the United
States. From these sources the theory attempts to formulate a lifespan developmental
perspective on issues surrounding interpersonal acceptance and rejection. Much of this
lifespan perspective is incorporated into IPARTheory's personality subtheory, described
later. First however, we discuss the concepts of interpersonal acceptance and rejection, or
the warmth dimension of interpersonal relationships. At this point, we should remind readers
that in IPARTheory the term parent refers to whoever the major caregiver(s) is/are of a child
– not necessarily to biological or adoptive parents.
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 4
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 6
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
A few years ago I [Rohner] interviewed a high caste Hindu woman about family
matters in India. Another woman seated nearby distracted my attention. The
second woman quietly and carefully peeled an orange and then removed the seeds
from each segment. Her 9-year-old daughter became increasingly animated as her
mother progressed. Later, my Bengali interpreter asked me if I had noticed what
the woman was doing. I answered that I had, but that I had not paid much attention
to it. "Should I have?" "Well," she answered, "you want to know about parental love
and affection in West Bengal, so you should know...." She went on to explain that
when a Bengali mother wants to praise her child – to show approval and affection
for her child – she might give the child a peeled and seeded orange. Bengali
children understand completely that their mothers have done something special for
them, even though mothers may not use words of praise for to do so would be
unseemly, much like praising themselves. (Rohner, 1994, p. 113; see also Rohner
& Chaki-Sircar, 1988).
At this point, we should caution that in everyday American English, the word “rejection”
implies bad parenting and sometimes even bad people. In cross-cultural and multiethnic
research, however, one must attempt to view the word as being descriptive of parents' and
others’ behavior, not judgmental or evaluative. This is true because parents in about 25
percent of the world's societies behave normatively in ways that are consistent with
IPARTheory’s definition of rejection (Rohner, 1975; Rohner & Rohner, 1980). But in the
great majority of cases – including historically in the United States – these parents behave
toward their children in the way that they believe good, responsible parents should behave,
as defined by cultural norms. Therefore, in the context of cross-cultural research on parental
acceptance-rejection, a major goal is to determine whether children and adults everywhere
respond the same way when they experience themselves to be accepted or rejected as
children – regardless of differences in culture, language, race, ethnicity, gender, social class,
or other such defining conditions.
typically parents for infants and children, but include significant others and non-parental
attachment figures for adolescents and adults.
As construed in IPARTheory, a significant other is any person who is uniquely
important to the individual, and who is interchangeable with no one else (Rohner, 2005a).
In this sense, parents and intimate partners, for example, are generally significant others.
But these people also tend to have one additional quality not shared by most significant
others. That is, individuals’ sense of emotional security and comfort tends to be dependent
on the quality of their relationship with their significant others. Because of that fact, these
people are usually the kind of significant others called attachment figures in both
IPARTheory (Rohner, 2005a) and attachment theory (Ainsworth, 1989; Bowlby, 1982; Colin,
1996). The essence of the concept attachment as construed in IPARTheory is captured by
a short poem written by Rohner (2009):
Parents are thus uniquely important to children because the security and other emotional
and psychological states of offspring are dependent on the quality of relationship with their
parent(s). It is for this reason that parental acceptance and rejection is postulated in
IPARTheory to have unparalleled influence in shaping children's personality development
over time. Moreover, according to IPARTheory's personality subtheory, adults' sense of
emotional security and well-being tends to be dependent on the perceived quality of
relationship with adult attachment figures. Thus, acceptance or rejection by an intimate
partner is also postulated to have a major influence on adults' personality and psychological
adjustment.
The concept personality is defined in personality subtheory (Rohner, 2005a) as an
individual's more or less stable set of predispositions to respond (i.e., affective, cognitive,
perceptual, and motivational dispositions) and actual modes of responding (i.e., observable
behaviors) in various life situations or contexts. This definition recognizes that behavior is
motivated, influenced by external (i.e., environmental) as well as internal (e.g., emotional,
biological, and learning) factors, and usually has regularity or orderliness across time and
space. IPARTheory's personality subtheory postulates that the emotional need for positive
response from significant others and attachment figures is a powerful and culturally invariant
motivator. When children do not get this need satisfied adequately by their major caregivers
– or adults do not get this need met by their attachment figures – they are predisposed to
respond both emotionally and behaviorally in specific ways. In particular – according to the
subtheory – individuals who feel rejected by significant others are likely to be anxious and
insecure. In an attempt to allay these feelings and to satisfy their needs, persons who feel
rejected often increase their bids for positive responses, but only up to a point. That is, they
tend to become more dependent (see Figure 2). The term dependence in the theory refers
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 8
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
to the internal, psychologically felt wish or yearning for emotional (as opposed to
instrumental or task-oriented) support, care, comfort, attention, nurturance, and similar
behaviors from significant others. The term, as used in IPARTheory, also refers to the actual
behavioral bids individuals make for such responsiveness. For young children, these bids
may include clinging to parents, whining, or crying when parents unexpectedly depart, and
seeking physical proximity with them when they return.
In IPARTheory, dependence is construed as a continuum, with independence defining one
end of the continuum and dependence the other. Independent people are those who have
their need for positive response met sufficiently so that they are free from frequent or intense
yearning or behavioral bids for succor from significant others. Very dependent people, on
the other hand, are those who have a frequent and intense desire for positive response, and
are likely to make many bids for responses. As with all the personality dispositions studied
in IPARTheory, humans everywhere can be placed somewhere along the continuum of
being more or less dependent or independent. According to the theory, much of the variation
in dependence among children and adults is contingent on the extent to which they perceive
themselves to be accepted or rejected by significant others. Many rejected children and
adults feel the need for constant reassurance and emotional support.
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 10
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
emotionally upset – perhaps tearful or angry – when confronted with stressful situations that
accepted (loved) people are able to handle with greater emotional equanimity. All these
acutely painful feelings associated with perceived rejection tend to induce children and
adults to develop a negative worldview. That is, according to IPARTheory, rejected persons
are likely to develop a view of the world – of life, interpersonal relationships, and the very
nature of human existence – as being untrustworthy, hostile, unfriendly, emotionally unsafe,
threatening, or dangerous in other ways. These thoughts and feelings often extend to
people's beliefs about the nature of the supernatural world (i.e., God, the gods, and other
religious beliefs) (Batool & Najam, 2009; Rohner, 1975, 1986) (see IPARTheory's
sociocultural systems subtheory, below).
Negative worldview, negative self-esteem, negative self-adequacy, and some of the
other personality dispositions described above are important elements in the social-
cognition or mental representations of rejected persons. In IPARTheory, the concept of
mental representation (Rohner, 2005a) refers to an individual's more-or-less organized but
usually implicit conception of existence, including conception of things that individuals take
for granted about themselves, others, and the experiential world constructed from
emotionally significant past and current experiences. Along with one's emotional state –
which both influences and is influenced by one's conception of reality – mental
representations tend to shape the way individuals perceive, construe, and react to new
experiences, including interpersonal relationships. Mental representations also influence
what and how individuals store and remember experiences (see also Baldwin, 1992;
Clausen, 1972; Crick & Dodge, 1994; Epstein, 1994).
Once created, individuals' mental representations of self, significant others, and the
world around them tend to induce them to seek or to avoid certain situations and kinds of
people. In effect, the way individuals think about themselves and their world shapes the way
they live their lives. This is notably true of rejected children and adults. For example, many
rejected persons have a tendency to perceive hostility where none is intended, to see
deliberate rejection in unintended acts of significant others, or to devalue their sense of
personal worth in the face of strong counter-information. Moreover, rejected persons are
likely to seek, create, interpret, or perceive experiences, situations, and relationships in ways
that are consistent with their distorted mental representations. And they often tend to avoid
or mentally reinterpret situations that are inconsistent with these representations.
Additionally, rejected children and adults often construct mental images of personal
relationships as being unpredictable, untrustworthy, and perhaps hurtful. These negative
mental representations are often carried forward into new relationships where rejected
individuals develop a fear of intimacy – finding it difficult to trust others emotionally (Phillips
et al., 2013). They are also likely to become hypervigilant and hypersensitive – and to
overreact – to any slights or signs of emotional undependability. This process is often
referred to as rejection sensitivity (Downey & Feldman, 1996; Ibrahim, Rohner, Smith, &
Flannery, 2015). Because of all this selective attention, selective perception, faulty styles of
causal attribution, and distorted cognitive information processing, rejected individuals –
especially children – are generally expected in IPARTheory to self-propel along qualitatively
different developmental pathways from accepted or loved people.
Many of these effects of perceived rejection are also found in developmental trauma
disorder (DTD; van der Kolk, 2010) and in complex posttraumatic stress disorder (Complex
PTSD or simply CPTSD; Courtois, 2004). These are conditions where youths experience
repeated trauma – especially interpersonal trauma – over an extended period of time and
developmental periods. Included among the shared effects of perceived rejection, DTD and
CPTSD are issues of hypervigilance, anxiety, often self-hatred, problems with interpersonal
relationships, and suicidality, among several others. Additionally, issues of depression and
substance abuse, discussed later, are also implicated in perceived rejection, DTD, and
CPTSD.
The pain of perceived rejection is very real (MacDonald & Leary, 2005). In fact, brain
imaging (fMRI) studies reveal that specific parts of the brain (i.e., the anterior cingulate
cortex, and the right ventral prefrontal cortex) are activated when people feel rejected, just
as they are when people experience physical pain (Eisenberger, 2012a, 2012b, 2015;
Eisenberger & Lieberman, 2004; Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, 2003; see also Squire
& Stein, 2003). Moreover, Fisher, Aron, and Brown (2005) found that different regions of the
brain are activated among adults who are happily in love versus those who had been
recently rejected by their partners.
In addition to research using fMRIs, results of both animal and human studies suggest
that emotional trauma in childhood may affect brain structure and function in other ways.
For example, evidence shows that emotional neglect in childhood may be a significant risk
factor for cerebral infarction in old age (Wilson et al., 2012). Moreover, perceived rejection
and other forms of long-term emotional trauma are often implicated in the alteration of brain
chemistry (Ford & Russo, 2006). The effect of these and other neurobiological and
neuropsychological changes may ultimately compromise children's central nervous system
and psychosocial development (Ford, 2005). On the positive side, however, Luby et al.
(2012) found that the early experience of maternal nurturance among preschoolers is
strongly predictive of larger hippocampal volume among the same children at school age.
These results are important because the hippocampus is a region of the brain that is central
to memory, emotion regulation, stress modulation, and other functions – all of which are
essential for healthy social and emotional adjustment.
It is perhaps for reasons such as these that over 550 studies involving tens of
thousands of participants cross-culturally and major American ethnic groups consistently
show that about 60 to 80 percent of the children and adults measured so far respond as
personality subtheory predicts (Ki, 2015; Rohner, 2016c). In fact, no adequate study
anywhere in the world – across cultures, genders, ages, geographic boundaries, ethnicities,
and other defining conditions – has failed to show the same basic trend that children’s and
adults’ mental health status tends to be impaired in direct proportion to the form, frequency,
severity, and duration of rejection experienced (see Figure 3). Some adults who came from
loving families, however, also display the constellation of psychological problems typically
shown by rejected children. These people are called "troubled" in IPARTheory; many are
individuals who are in rejecting relationships with attachment figures other than parents. This
fact helps confirm IPARTheory's expectation that, for most people, perceived rejection by
any attachment figure at any point throughout the lifespan effectively compromises the
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 12
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
IPARTheory's coping subtheory deals with the question of how some rejected individuals
are able to withstand the corrosive drizzle of day-to-day rejection without suffering the
negative mental health consequences that most rejected individuals do. Theoretically and
empirically, the coping process is the least well-developed portion of IPARTheory. As is true
for most other bodies of research on the coping process (Ki, 2015; Somerfield & McCrae,
2000; Tamres, Janicki, & Helgeson, 2002), little is yet known with confidence about the
mechanisms and processes that help answer coping subtheory's basic question.
Nonetheless, it seems clear that in order to understand the coping process – indeed the
entire acceptance-rejection process – one must adopt a multivariate, person-in-context
perspective. This perspective has three elements: self, other, and context. Specifically, the
multivariate model of behavior employed in IPARTheory states that the behavior of the
individual (e.g., coping with perceived rejection) is a function of the interaction between self,
other, and context. "Self" characteristics include the individual's mental representations
along with the other internal (biological) and external (personality) characteristics discussed
earlier. "Other" characteristics include the personal and interpersonal characteristics of the
rejecting other, along with the form, frequency, duration, and severity of rejection. "Context"
characteristics include other significant people in the individual's life, along with social-
situational characteristics of the person's environment. A specific, but so far not well tested,
hypothesis coming from this perspective states that, all other things being equal, the
likelihood of individuals being able to cope with perceived interpersonal rejection is
enhanced by the presence of at least one warm, supportive attachment figure in their life.
IPARTheory's emphasis on mental activity – including mental representations – leads
us to expect that specific social cognitive capabilities allow some children and adults to cope
with perceived rejection more effectively than others. These capabilities include a clearly
differentiated sense of self, a sense of self-determination, and the capacity to depersonalize
(Rohner, 1986, 2005a). More specifically, coping subtheory expects that the capacity of
individuals to cope with rejection is enhanced to the degree that they have a clearly
differentiated sense of self, one aspect of which is a sense of self-determination or self-
efficacy. Self-determined individuals believe they can exert at least a modicum of control
over what happens to them through their own effort or personal attributes. Other individuals
may feel like pawns: they feel as though things happen to them because of fate, chance,
luck, or powerful others. Individuals with a sense of self-determination are believed in
IPARTheory to have an internal psychological resource for minimizing some of the most
damaging consequences of perceived rejection.
Similarly, individuals who have the capacity to depersonalize are believed in the theory
to be provided with another social-cognitive resource for dealing with perceived rejection.
Personalizing refers to the act of "taking it personally," that is, to reflexively or automatically
relate life events and interpersonal encounters to oneself – interpreting events egocentrically
in terms of oneself, usually in a negative sense. Thus, personalizers are apt to interpret
inadvertent slights and minor acts of insensitivity as being deliberate acts of rejection or
other hurtful intentions. This, too, is related to rejection sensitivity discussed earlier.
Individuals who are able to depersonalize, however, have a psychological resource for
dealing in a more positive way with ambiguities in interpersonal relationships. All three of
these social cognitive capabilities are thought in IPARTheory to provide psychological
shields against the more damaging effects of perceived rejection. However, these attributes
themselves tend to be affected by rejection, especially rejection occurring during the
formative years of childhood. This complicates the task of assessing the independent
contribution that each attribute might make in helping children and adults cope with
perceived interpersonal rejection.
It is important to note here that the concept coper in IPARTheory's coping subtheory
refers to affective copers versus instrumental copers. Affective copers are those persons
whose emotional and overall mental health is reasonably good despite having been raised
in seriously rejecting families or despite being seriously rejected by other attachment figures
throughout the life span. Instrumental copers, on the other hand, are rejected persons who
do well school, in their professions, occupations, and other task-oriented activities, but
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 14
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
whose emotional and mental health is impaired. Instrumental copers maintain a high level
of task competence and occupational performance despite experiencing serious rejection in
childhood. Many prominent personalities in history have been instrumental copers. Included
among them are such personages as Louis "Satchmo" Armstrong, John Stuart Mill, Richard
Nixon, Edgar Allen Poe, Eleanor Roosevelt, Babe Ruth, and Mark Twain, among many,
many others (Goertzel & Goertzel, 1962; Howe, 1982). Biographies and autobiographies of
these individuals reveal that even though they were successful instrumental copers, they
were not affective copers. All appear to have been psychologically distressed in ways
described in IPARTheory's personality subtheory.
Even though the mental health status of affective copers is reasonably good, it is
generally not as good as that of individuals coming from loving (accepting) families or in
other emotionally satisfying relationships – but it does tend to be significantly better than
that of most individuals coming from rejecting families or seriously rejecting adult
relationships (Ki, 2015). Over time, from childhood into adulthood, however, all but the most
severely rejected and psychologically injured individuals are likely to have enough positive
experiences outside their families of origin to help ameliorate the most damaging emotional,
cognitive, and behavioral effects of parental rejection. Thus, given the ordinary resilience
characteristic of most people most of the time (Masten, 2001, 2014) – in combination with
successful psychotherapy, positive work experiences, satisfying intimate relationships, and
other emotionally gratifying experiences – adults who were rejected in childhood are often
better adjusted emotionally and psychologically than they were as children under the direct
influence of rejecting parents – though they tend not to have as positive sense of well-being
as adults who felt loved all along. That is, important sequelae of rejection are apt to linger
into adulthood, placing even affective copers at somewhat greater risk for social, physical,
and emotional problems throughout life than persons who were loved continuously. This is
especially true if the rejection process in childhood seriously compromised the individual's
ability to form secure, trusting relationships with an intimate partner or other adult attachment
figure.
The first empirical study to assess key features of the affective coping process was
conducted by Ki (2015). That study was based on 11,946 adults from 10 nations. Almost
17% of the respondents in the study reported having been seriously rejected in childhood
by one or both parents. Nonetheless, 40% of the respondents fit the conceptual and
operational definition of affective copers. According to Ki, significantly more women than
men tend to be affective copers. At this point, it is unclear why this gender difference exists,
except that women everywhere appear to seek emotional support from others more often
than do men (Tamres et al., 2002). Beyond this, it is worth noting that both male and female
copers tend internationally to report significantly more maternal acceptance than paternal
acceptance (Ki, 2015). However, male copers report experiencing significantly more
maternal acceptance in childhood than do women, and female copers report experiencing
more paternal acceptance in childhood than do men. Nonetheless, remembrances of both
maternal and paternal acceptance in childhood tend to be significant and independent
predictors of male copers’ psychological adjustment in adulthood. For women, on the other
hand, an interaction between remembered maternal and paternal acceptance in childhood
are significant predictors of adjustment. That is, female copers’ remembrances of paternal
acceptance in childhood moderate the relation between remembered maternal rejection and
women’s psychological adjustment. Similarly, female copers’ remembrances of maternal
acceptance in childhood moderate the relationship between remembered paternal rejection
and women’s adjustment. Alternatively stated, the psychological adjustment of female
copers who experienced serious maternal rejection in childhood improves to the extent that
they remember their fathers as having been a loving (accepting) parent. But under the
condition of high paternal rejection in childhood, the intensity of the relation between
remembered maternal rejection in childhood and female copers’ psychological
maladjustment intensifies. The same pattern appears when the moderator is maternal
acceptance.
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 16
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
IPARTheory initially focused on parental acceptance and rejection. But in 1999, it went
through a paradigm shift from parental to interpersonal acceptance and rejection. More
specifically, the central postulate of what was then called PARTheory stated that perceived
parental rejection is associated with the specific cluster of personality dispositions noted in
personality subtheory. The reformulated postulate now states that perceived rejection by an
attachment figure at any point in life tends to be associated with the same cluster of
personality dispositions found among children and adults rejected by parents in childhood.
Now the theory and associated research is focused on all aspects of interpersonal
acceptance-rejection, including but not limited to parental acceptance-rejection, peer and
sibling acceptance-rejection, teacher acceptance-rejection, grandparent acceptance-
rejection, acceptance-rejection in intimate adult relationships, parent-in-law acceptance-
rejection, and acceptance-rejection in other attachment relationships throughout the
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 18
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
lifespan. Despite this paradigm shift in theory and research-focus, the theory continued to
be known as PARTheory until 2014 because that label had become so widely recognized
internationally.
However, by 2014, overwhelming empirical evidence supported the theory’s basic
postulate that children and adults in many classes of relationships other than parent-child
relationships understand themselves to be cared about (i.e., accepted or rejected) in the
same four ways that children do in parent-child relationships. Additionally, evidence
supported the postulate that individuals in these non-parental relationships tend to respond
to perceptions of acceptance-rejection in the same way that children do when they perceive
themselves to be accepted or rejected by their parents. In view of these facts, the name of
the theory was officially changed to IPARTheory on June 25, 2014 (Rohner, 2014) at the 5th
International Congress on Interpersonal Acceptance and Rejection.
The first empirical study to test IPARTheory's reformulated postulate was conducted
in 2001 (Khaleque, 2001; Rohner & Khaleque, 2008). That study examined the impact of
perceived acceptance-rejection by intimate male partners on the psychological adjustment
of 88 heterosexual adult females in the United States. This path-breaking study sparked
great international interest, so much so that the study was conducted in more than 15
countries worldwide. One of the earliest of these studies (Parmar & Rohner, 2005) dealt with
79 young adults in India. There, the authors found that the less accepting both men and
women perceived their intimate partners to be, the worse was their psychological
adjustment. Simple correlations also showed the expected positive correlation between
adults' psychological adjustment and remembered maternal and paternal acceptance in the
childhood of the adults. However, results of multiple regression analysis showed that partner
acceptance was the strongest single predictor of men's psychological adjustment, though
this relation was partially mediated by remembered paternal (but not maternal) acceptance
in childhood. For women, on the other hand, both partner acceptance and paternal (but not
maternal) acceptance were approximately coequal as predictors of psychological
adjustment.
More recently, similar findings were reported in eight other international studies
including in: Colombia and Puerto Rico (Ripoll-Núñez & Alvarez, 2008); Finland (Khaleque,
Rohner & Laukala, 2008); India (Parmar & Rohner, 2008); Japan (Rohner, Uddin,
Shamsunaher, & Khaleque, 2008); Korea (Chyung & Lee, 2008); Kuwait (Parmar, Ibrahim,
& Rohner, 2008); Turkey (Varan, Rohner, & Eryuksel, 2008), and the United States (Rohner,
Melendez, & Kraimer-Rickaby, 2008). Finally, a 2010 meta-analysis of 17 studies (discussed
later) showed that adults’ perceptions of their intimate partners’ acceptance tended in all
studies to correlate strongly with the psychological adjustment of both men and women
(Rohner & Khaleque, 2010). Collectively, these studies suggest the possibility of a universal
relationship between adults’ mental health status and their perceptions of acceptance-
rejection by adult intimate partners as well as by parents in childhood.
The second major issue to be studied following the reformulation of IPARTheory in
1999 dealt with the relative contribution of perceived teacher versus parental (maternal and
paternal) acceptance and behavioral control to the psychological adjustment, school
conduct, and academic achievement of school-going youths (boys and girls) within six
nations cross-culturally (Rohner, 2010). These nations included Bangladesh, Estonia, India,
Kuwait, the Mississippi Delta region of the United States, and Turkey. Results of analyses
exposed enormous gender and sociocultural variability in patterns of predictors assessed
with each of the three outcome variables studied. As expected from IPARTheory, however,
predictors of variations in youth’s psychological adjustment were much more stable. More
specifically, both perceived teacher acceptance and parental (maternal and paternal)
acceptance were significantly correlated with the adjustment of both boys and girls in all
nations where this relationship was studied. Results of multiple regression analyses,
however, showed that perceived teacher acceptance mediated to a large degree the
relationship between perceived parental acceptance and youth’s psychological adjustment
in Bangladesh and India. On the other hand, these analyses showed that only perceived
parental acceptance made independent or unique contributions to student’s psychological
adjustment in Kuwait and Estonia.
The first cluster consists of two types of studies. The first involves quantitative
psychological studies using techniques such as interviews, behavior observations, and
self-report questionnaires, most often the Parental Acceptance-Rejection Questionnaire
(PARQ) (Rohner, 2005b), the Parental Acceptance-Rejection/Control Questionnaire
(PARQ/Control) (Rohner, 2005c), and the Personality Assessment Questionnaire (PAQ)
(Rohner & Khaleque, 2005b). Along with many other self-report questionnaires (see Box 1),
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 20
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
these measures were developed to help researchers and practitioners study issues of
interpersonal acceptance-rejection. Extensive information about the reliability and validity of
many of these measures and others for international use and among American ethnic
groups is already available in the Handbook for the Study of Parental Acceptance and
Rejection (Rohner & Khaleque, 2005a) and elsewhere (e.g., Gomez & Rohner, 2011,
Martorell & Carrasco, 2014; Senese, Bacchini, Miranda, Aurino, & Rohner, in press).
References to over 4,400 quantitative psychological studies using these and related
measures may be found in Rohner (2016c). There, interested students and researchers may
do keyword searches to locate articles and books dealing with specific topics of special
interest to them.
Among these many articles are more than 120 studies throughout the Arab world
alone (Ahmed, Rohner, Khaleque, & Gielen, 2016). These and other studies support
IPARTheory’s expectation that perceived parental rejection in childhood and rejection by
attachment figures in adulthood tend to be associated with a host of psychological,
behavioral, physical-health, and other problems, including externalizing and internalizing
behaviors, loneliness, various forms of psychopathology including personality disorders,
risky sexual and other behaviors, suicide and suicidal ideation, among many others.
Perceived parental acceptance in childhood and acceptance by attachment figures in
adulthood, on the other hand, has been found in these studies to be associated with many
psychological, behavioral, emotional, and other benefits, including altruism, positive life
satisfaction, psychological hardiness, positive scholastic achievement, feelings of emotional
security, and social responsibility. Additionally, remembrances of parental acceptance in
childhood have been shown to be an overall buffer in adulthood against the development of
biomarkers indicating a proneness for many negative physical-health outcomes such as
cardiovascular disease (Carroll et al., 2013).
Meta-analyses, the second type of study in the quantitative psychological-studies
category, summarize and synthesize results of a collection of these discrete psychological
studies. In all, 11 such meta-analyses testing central aspects of IPARTheory have been
completed. They are based on 551 quantitative reports representing an aggregated sample
of 125,437 respondents in 31 countries on every continent except the Antarctic.
Collectively, these meta-analyses confirm IPARTheory’s central postulate that both
maternal and paternal acceptance panculturally predict the psychological adjustment of both
boys and girls. Additionally, the meta-analyses show that all seven of the personality
dispositions featured prominently in IPARTheory’s personality subtheory are panculturally
associated with children’s perceptions of parental acceptance. It is worth noting, too, that
children’s perception of parental warmth/affection, hostility/aggression, and
indifference/neglect – along with overall perceived acceptance – each panculturally predicts
significant variations in children’s overall psychological adjustment as well as the seven
personality dispositions measured on the PAQ.
It is not only variations in parents’ love that panculturally predict children’s adjustment
and behavior, but so too do other significant people in children’s lives. For example, as noted
in the 10th meta-analysis (Ali, Khaleque, & Rohner, 2015) children’s perceptions of their
teachers’ acceptance tend to predict panculturally children’s overall psychological
Box 1
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 22
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
both men and women. Adults’ remembrances of paternal acceptance, however, do not
appear to correlate as expected with dependence. Beyond this, it is important to note that
both men’s and women’s psychological adjustment tends to be panculturally associated with
their perceptions of their intimate partners’ acceptance in the same way that adults’
adjustment is associated with remembrances of parental acceptance-rejection in childhood.
Finally, we should observe that men’s remembrances of their mothers’ acceptance
in childhood tend to be associated to a significantly greater extent with the adult sons’
psychological adjustment than do adult daughters’ remembrances of maternal acceptance
in childhood. Adult daughters’ adjustment, however, tends to be influenced by
remembrances of paternal acceptance in childhood to a greater degree than do adult sons’
remembrances of paternal acceptance. Almost all of these conclusions are based on data
using the PARQ and the PAQ. One of the meta-analyses (Khaleque & Rohner, 2002b)
shows that these measures are reliable assessment devices to use in international and
cross-cultural comparative research.
Ethnographic Research
The second cluster of distinctive methods used in IPARTheory consists of three types of
studies based on ethnographic research. The first of these is the ethnographic case study
such as the one done by Rohner and Chaki-Sircar (1988) among women and children in a
Bengali village. Ethnographic case studies employ long-term (e.g., six months to several
years) participant observation procedures within a specific culturally organized community,
along with structured and unstructured observations, interviews, and other such procedures.
Such ethnographic studies produce a context-rich account of the way of life of a people. A
second method within this cluster is the controlled comparison or concomitant variation
study (Naroll, 1968; Pelto & Pelto, 1978; Rohner, 1977). In these studies, investigators
usually locate two or more culture-bearing populations in which one of two conditions is true:
(1) Relevant variables in the sampled populations vary, but other sociocultural factors
remain constant, or (2) relevant variables in the sample population remain constant while
other sociocultural factors are free to vary. Rohner's (1960) comparative study of parental
rejection in three Pacific societies (i.e., a Maori community of New Zealand, a traditional
highland community of Bali, and the Alorese of Indonesia) illustrates the second type of
study in this cluster.
Finally, the holocultural method (traditionally called the cross-cultural survey
method) is the third approach within this cluster (Naroll, Michik, & Naroll, 1976, 1980; Whiting
& Child, 1953). This method is a research design for statistically measuring the relation
between two or more theoretically defined and operationalized variables in a random,
stratified sample of the world's adequately described sociocultural systems. The sources of
data are ethnographic reports rather than direct observations, self-report questionnaires,
interviews, or other such procedures (Rohner et al., 1978). Rohner's (1975) study of parental
acceptance-rejection in 101 well-described non-industrial societies distributed widely
throughout the major geographic regions and culture areas of the world illustrates this type
of study.
Each of these five types of studies contains unique strengths and weaknesses. There are
several strengths of the psychological-study cluster, for example – including valid, reliable,
and precise descriptions of phenomena. Estimates of both central tendency and variability
in data generated in this cluster of methods allow sensitive and powerful statistical
procedures to be employed to tease out subtle effects. A typical weakness of these studies,
however, is the fact that rich contextual data is usually missing. Special strengths of the
methods in the ethnographic research cluster are validity and groundedness. That is,
ethnographic studies produce accounts that are rich in cultural detail and context. Derived
as they are from ethnography, holocultural studies are also grounded, but they have an
important additional strength in that they allow for truly pancultural sampling that takes into
account the full range of known sociocultural variation throughout the world, especially of
non-industrial societies. A weakness of these methods, however, is the fact that measures
coded from ethnography are sometimes imprecise, and therefore may be low in reliability.
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 24
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 26
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
Many newer studies have begun to report differential effects of perceived paternal
versus maternal acceptance on the psychological adjustment of daughters versus sons. For
example, Sultana and Khaleque (2016) found that whereas both maternal and paternal
acceptance made significant and independent contributions to the adjustment of adult sons,
only remembrances of paternal acceptance in childhood made a significant and independent
contribution to adult daughters’ adjustment.
Substantial evidence is also beginning to show that genetic influences on mental
health differ as a function of environmental risk factors such as rejecting parent-child
relationships. For example, South and Jarnecke (2015) reported from a nationwide study of
twins that adults’ remembrances of their fathers’ discipline (punishment and behavioral
control) and affection moderated genetic and environmental influences on internalizing
symptoms of the adults. More specifically, heritability was greatest at the highest levels of
paternal affection and discipline. Adults’ remembrances of their fathers’ affection in
childhood also moderated etiological influences on adults’ alcohol-use problems. In this
context, heritability was greatest at the lowest levels of paternal affection. No moderating
effects were found for mothers.
From all this evidence, it is now clear that paternal acceptance is sometimes a better
predictor of offspring’s psychological and behavioral adjustment than is maternal
acceptance. But until recently, it was not clear why this is true. Conclusions reached in a
recent study of offspring’s (children’s and young adults’) perceptions of each parent’s
(mothers’ versus fathers’) prestige and interpersonal power within the family (Rohner &
Carrasco, 2014) helps to explain why the love-related behavior of one parent (e.g., fathers)
sometimes has a significantly greater impact on offspring’s psychological adjustment than
does the love-related behavior of the other parent (e.g., mothers). Results of research within
the 11 nations in that study revealed that the love-related behaviors of both parents tended
in most countries to make unique (i.e., independent) contributions to the psychological
adjustment of both sons and daughters. This fact notwithstanding, results of analyses also
showed that in many instances, offspring’s perceptions of one parent’s (e.g., fathers’)
interpersonal power and/or prestige within the family tended to moderate the relation
between perceived parental (maternal and/or paternal) acceptance and offspring’s
psychological adjustment. The study of 785 college students in Portugal illustrates this
phenomenon (Machado, Machado, Neves, & Fávero, 2014). There, the authors found that
both men’s and women’s remembrances of maternal and paternal acceptance in childhood
made independent contributions to the psychological adjustment of the young adults. But
the magnitude of the relation between perceived paternal acceptance and daughters’
adjustment intensified significantly the more interpersonal power fathers were perceived to
have relative to mothers. The magnitude of the relation between perceived paternal
acceptance and sons’ adjustment, however, intensified the more prestige fathers were
perceived to have relative to mothers.
The search in IPARTheory for panculturally valid principles of behavior is based on the
assumption that with a scientific understanding of the worldwide consequences,
antecedents, and other correlates of interpersonal acceptance-rejection comes the
possibility of formulating culture-fair and practicable programs, policies, and interventions
affecting families and children everywhere. This research contributes to the goal of culture-
fair programs and policies in that it asks researchers and practitioners to look beyond
differences in cultural beliefs, language, and custom when making assessments about the
adequacy of parenting, and to focus instead on whether individuals' fundamental needs
(e.g., the need for emotional support, nurturance, affection, and the like from significant
others, especially attachment figures) are being met. Social policies and programs of
prevention, intervention, and treatment based on idiosyncratic beliefs at a particular point in
history are likely to prove unworkable for some, and probably even prejudicial for many
minority populations. Policies and programs based on demonstrable principles of human
behavior, however, stand a good chance of working as nations and people change. The
values and customs of a particular sociocultural group, therefore, are not – according to
IPARTheory – the most important criteria to be used to evaluate the adequacy of parenting
and other interpersonal relationships in that group. Rather, the most important question
becomes how accepted and cared-about do children and others perceive themselves to be.
Insofar as children and adults perceive their parents and other attachment figures to be
accepting, then – according to both theory and evidence presented here – it probably makes
little difference for children's developmental outcome how external reporters view parents'
behavior.
It is thoughts such as these that have motivated a great part of IPARTheory research.
Now, after almost six decades of research with tens of thousands of children, adolescents,
and adults in over 60 nations worldwide, and with members of every major American ethnic
group, at least two conclusions seem warranted. First, the same classes of behaviors appear
panculturally to convey the symbolic message that "my parent. . ." (or other attachment
figure) "loves me" (or does not love me, care about me, want me – i.e., rejects me). These
classes of behavior include the perception of warmth/affection (or its opposite, coldness and
lack of affection), hostility/aggression, indifference/neglect, and undifferentiated rejection, as
defined at the beginning of this chapter. Additional support for this conclusion is found in the
work of McNeely and Barber (2010) who asked 4,300 adolescents in 12 nations (from Africa,
Asia, Australia, Europe, the Middle East, and North and South America) to respond to the
following question: “Please list the specific things that your parents/caregivers do that make
you feel like they love you?” Responses to this query confirmed that adolescents themselves
in all cultures studied perceived aspects of emotional support (e.g., expressing affection and
encouragement, and the absence of parental hostility or parental aggression) as being
expressions of parental love or acceptance.
The second major conclusion is that differences in culture, ethnicity, social class, race,
gender, and other such factors do not exert enough influence to override the apparently
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 28
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
References
Ahmed, R. A., Rohner, R. P., & Carrasco, M. A. (2012). Relations between psychological
adjustment and perceived parental, sibling, best friend, and teacher acceptance
among Kuwaiti adolescents. In K. Ripoll-Nuñez, A. L. Comunian, & C. M. Brown
(Eds.), Expanding horizons: Current research on interpersonal acceptance (pp. 1-10).
Boca Raton, FL: BrownWalker Press.
Ahmed, R. A., Rohner, R. P., Khaleque, A., & Gielen, U. P. (2016). Parental acceptance and
rejection in the Arab world: How do they influence children’s development? In U. P.
Gielen & J. L. Roopnarine (Eds.) Childhood and Adolescence: Cross-Cultural
Perspectives and Applications (pp. 121-150). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger/ABC-Clio.
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachment beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44(4), 709-
716. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.709
Akun, E. (2016). The relationship between recollections of parental acceptance-rejection in
childhood and adult psychopathology. Manuscript in preparation.
Ali, S., Khaleque, A., & Rohner, R. P. (2015). Influence of perceived teacher acceptance
and parental acceptance on youth’s psychological adjustment and school conduct: A
cross-cultural meta-analysis. Cross-Cultural Research, 49(2), 204-224.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1069397114552769
Amato, P. R. (1994). Father-child relations, mother-child relations, and offspring
psychological well-being in early adulthood. Journal of Marriage and Family, 56(4),
1031-1042. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/353611
Andry, R. G. (1962). Paternal and maternal roles and delinquency. In M. D. Ainsworth (Ed.),
Deprivation of maternal care (Vol. 14) (pp. 31-41). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health
Organization.
Aronoff, J. (1967). Psychological needs and cultural systems: A case study. Princeton, NJ:
D. Van Nostrand.
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 30
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
underpinnings of physical and social pain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(6), 421-
434. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3231
Eisenberger, N. I. (2015). Social pain and the brain: Controversies, questions, and where to
go from here. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 601-629.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115146
Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: A common neural alarm
system for physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 294-300.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.010
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI
study of social exclusion. Science, 302(5643), 290-292.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1089134
Eldred, C. A., Brown, B. S., & Mahabir, C. (1974). Heroin addict clients' description of their
families of origin. International Journal of the Addictions, 9(2), 315-320.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/10826087409057350
Emmelkamp, P. M. G., & Heeres, H. (1988). Drug addiction and parental rearing style: A
controlled study. International Journal of the Addictions, 23(2), 207-216.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/10826088809085116
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious.
American Psychologist, 49(8), 709-724. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-
066X.49.8.709
Eron, L. D., Banta, T. J., Walder, L. O., & Laulicht, J. H. (1961). Comparison of data obtained
from mothers and fathers on childrearing practices and their relation to child
aggression. Child Development, 32(3), 457-472. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1126211
Fisher, H., Aron, A., & Brown, L. L. (2005). Romantic love: An fMRI study of a neural
mechanism for mate choice. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 439(1), 58-62.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.20772
Ford, J. D. (2005). Treatment implications of altered affect regulation and information
processing following child maltreatment. Psychiatric Annals, 35(5), 410-419.
http://www.healio.com/psychiatry/journals/psycann
Ford, J. D., & Russo, E. (2006). Trauma-focused, present-centered, emotional self-
regulation approach to integrated treatment for posttraumatic stress and addiction:
Trauma adaptive recover group education and therapy (TARGET). American Journal
of Psychotherapy, 60(4), 335-355. Retrieved from http://www.ajp.org
Goertzel, V., & Goertzel, M. G. (1962). Cradles of eminence. Boston, MA: Little, Brown and
Company.
Gomez, R., & Rohner, R. P. (2011). Tests of factor structure and measurement invariance
in the United States and Australia using the adult version of the parental acceptance-
rejection questionnaire. Cross-Cultural Research, 45(3), 267-285.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1069397111403111
Howe, M. J. A. (1982). Biographical evidence and the development of outstanding
individuals. American Psychologist, 37(10), 1071-1081.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.37.10.1071
Ibrahim, D. M., Rohner, R. P., Smith, R. L., & Flannery, K. M. (2015). Adults’ remembrances
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 32
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 34
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.59.8.830
Rohner, R. P. (2005a). Glossary of significant concepts in parental acceptance-rejection
theory. In R. P. Rohner & A. Khaleque (Eds.), Handbook for the study of parental
acceptance and rejection (4th ed, pp. 379-397). Storrs, CT: Rohner Research
Publications.
Rohner, R. P. (2005b). Parental acceptance-rejection questionnaire (PARQ): Test manual.
In R. P. Rohner & A. Khaleque (Eds.), Handbook for the study of parental acceptance
and rejection (4th ed., pp. 43-106). Storrs, CT: Rohner Research Publications.
Rohner, R. P. (2005c). Parental acceptance-rejection/control questionnaire (PARQ/Control):
Test manual. In R. P. Rohner & A. Khaleque (Eds.), Handbook for the study of parental
acceptance and rejection (4th ed.) (pp. 137-186). Storrs, CT: Rohner Research
Publications.
Rohner, R. P. (2009). Attachment: The emotional moon phenomenon. Unpublished
manuscript.
Rohner, R. P. (Ed.) (2010). Teacher acceptance, parental acceptance, and the adjustment,
achievement, and behavior of school-going youth [Special Issue] Cross-Cultural
Research, 44(3).
Rohner, R. P. (2014, June). Parental acceptance-rejection theory gets a new name. Paper
presented at the 6th International Congress on Interpersonal Acceptance and
Rejection, Madrid, Spain.
Rohner, R. P. (2016a, June). International project on the relation between parental rejection
in childhood and adults’ fear of intimacy. Paper presented at the 6th International
Congress on Interpersonal Acceptance and Rejection, Madrid, Spain.
Rohner, R. P. (2016b, June). International project on the relation between remembered
parental rejection in childhood and adults’ loneliness. Paper presented at the 6th
International Congress on Interpersonal Acceptance and Rejection, Madrid, Spain.
Rohner, R. P. (2016c). Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Bibliography. Retrieved from
http://csiar.uconn.edu/wp-content/uploads/sites/494/2014/01/Merged-Bibliography-
03-04-16.pdf
Rohner, R. P., Bourque, S. L., & Elordi, C. A. (1996). Children's perceptions of corporal
punishment, caretaker acceptance, and psychological adjustment in a poor, biracial
southern community. Journal of Marriage and Family, 58(4), 842-852.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/353974
Rohner, R. P., & Britner, P. A. (2002). Worldwide mental health correlates of parental
acceptance- rejection: Review of cross-cultural and intracultural evidence. Cross-
Cultural Research, 36(1), 16-47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/106939710203600102
Rohner, R. P., & Carrasco, M. A. (Eds.). (2014). Special Issue: Parental power and prestige
moderate the effects of perceived acceptance on offspring’s psychological
adjustment. Cross-Cultural Research, 48(3). http://ccr.sagepub.com
Rohner, R. P., & Chaki-Sircar, M. (1988). Women and children in a Bengali village. Hanover,
NH: University Press of New England (Available as e-book from Rohner Research
Publications).
Rohner, R. P., & Frampton, S. (1982). Perceived parental acceptance-rejection and artistic
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 36
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 38
Rohner: Introduction to Interpersonal Acceptance-Rejection Theory (IPARTheory) and Evidence
Wilson, R. S., Boyle, P. A., Levine, S. R., Yu, L., Anagnos, S. E., Buchman, A. S., . . .
Bennett, D. A. (2012). Emotional neglect in childhood and cerebral infarction in older
age. Neurology, 79(15), 1534-1539.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e31826e25bd
Rohner, R. P. (2013a, November). Video lecture: Benefits of affection given & affection
received. Retrieved from http://csiar.uconn.edu/2014/07/23/video-lecture-benefits-of-
affection-given-affection-recieved/
Rohner, R. P. (2013b, November). Introduction to parental acceptance rejection theory: A
video lecture. Retrieved from http://csiar.uconn.edu/2014/02/04/introduction-to-parental-
acceptance-rejection-theory-a-video-lecture/
Discussion Questions
8. Select any widely known theory of personality (e.g., psychodynamic, social learning,
humanistic, existential), and explain how it would deal with the phenomenon of parental
rejection. How is that like and different from the way in which IPARTheory deals with it?
Ronald P. Rohner is Professor Emeritus of Human Development and Family Studies and
Anthropology at the University of Connecticut Storrs, USA. He is also the founding President
and Executive Director of the International Society for Interpersonal Acceptance and
Rejection. Moreover, he is author of interpersonal acceptance-rejection theory, along with
most of the measures used to empirically test the theory. As a result of his lifetime
commitment to cross-cultural research, he received the American Psychological Association
Award for Distinguished Contributions to the International Advancement of Psychology. He
also received the Outstanding International Psychologist Award from the USA for 2008.
http://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/orpc/vol6/iss1/4 40