Ingles McNaugton, 2022

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 23

Europe PMC Funders Group

Author Manuscript
Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.
Published in final edited form as:
Trends Neurosci. 2022 July 01; 45(7): 550–562. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2022.04.006.
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

Construction of complex memories via parallel distributed


cortical-subcortical iterative integration
Neil McNaughton1,
Seralynne D. Vann2
1Department of Psychology and Brain Health Research Centre, University of Otago, POB56,
Dunedin, New Zealand
2School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Park Place, Cardiff CF10 3AT, UK

Abstract
The construction of complex engrams requires hippocampal-cortical interactions. These include
both direct interactions and ones via often-overlooked subcortical loops. Here, we review the
anatomical organization of a hierarchy of parallel ‘Papez’ loops through the hypothalamus that
are homologous in mammals from rats to humans. These hypothalamic loops supplement direct
hippocampal-cortical connections with iterative re-processing paced by theta rhythmicity. We
couple existing anatomy and lesion data with theory to propose that recirculation in these loops
progressively enhances desired connections, while reducing interference from competing external
goals and internal associations. This increases the signal-to-noise ratio in the distributed engrams
(neocortical and cerebellar) necessary for complex learning and memory. The hypothalamic nodes
provide key motivational input for engram enhancement during consolidation.
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

Keywords
Memory; iterative processing; anterior thalamic nuclei; cerebellum; mammillary bodies;
supramammillary nuclei; theta

What is memory for?


Memory is phylogenetically old. Important aspects of human memory are ancient: they
are present in simple invertebrate circuits [1]; well-developed in fish; and strongly
homologous in birds [2]. Therefore, human cortical memory has emerged out of conserved
[3] fundamental subcortical memory systems. While basic memory systems are present
across species, memory capabilities have expanded with evolution, requiring a more energy-
expensive brain. However, neural memory did not evolve to simply store data; its adaptive
functions are linked to motivation. The bringing to mind of past experience, and of past

This work is licensed under a CC BY 4.0 International license.


Correspondence to: Neil McNaughton; Seralynne D. Vann.
Correspondence: [email protected] (Neil McNaughton); [email protected] (Seralynne D. Vann).
Declaration of interests
The authors declare no conflicts of interests in relation to this work.
McNaughton and Vann Page 2

goal-subgoal sequences [4], generates current goals and the means to achieve them. Yet,
many assume that memory control is in ‘cold’ neocortex rather than in ‘hot’ limbic [5-8]
cortex; and certainly not in ultra-hot ‘survival circuit’ [9-12] subcortex. We think these
assumptions need to be revisited.

Here we argue that formation of even the most data-focused engrams1 in the cortex
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

depends on a set of highly conserved nested cortical-subcortical-cortical closed loops that


are essentially a set of parallel Papez circuits [14]. These loops support iterative processing
– paced by inhibitory ‘theta’ rhythmicity – and are positioned to add motivational bias.
Critically, wherever activity is blocked within these loops, the outcome is similar neural
and behavioral dysfunction. The same Papez architecture can be seen across amniotes
and, likely, monotremes and Theria [15], with perhaps a single equivalent loop even in
fish [16-19]. However, there is also evolutionary progression. Relative to other mammals
and primates, the most recent of the diencephalic Papez-like closed-loop components
appears larger in humans alongside the relatively expanded telencephalon [20]. This Papez
architecture is at least partially distinct from, and more unidirectional than, the many other
interactive loops that use a “hierarchical system of brain oscillations” [21] to support more
global processing including the “emergence of cognition from action” and alternative event
predictions (see Fig. 1 in [22]).

How and why do old brain areas control recent ones?


A key to goal-oriented engram processing lies in the motivation-biased base of the brain.
The hypothalamus is a surprisingly important node in mnemonic loops [23]. At only 2%
of brain volume in rodents (0.3% volume in humans) [24,25], it is nonetheless key to a
vast range of functions. It controls not only low-level autonomic and homeostatic functions
[26], but (often overlooked) high-level cognitive ones [27]. It is small and so, must exert
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

wide-ranging diffuse control – not supply detailed computation. Importantly, hypothalamic


nuclei can add emotional bias to the loop processing of engrams – held in areas such as the
cortex – and so have a major adaptive impact on memory.

Two adjacent posterior hypothalamic areas are particularly linked to cognition: the
mammillary bodies (MB) and supramammillary area (SuM). Each was initially seen as
homogenous – but both have three distinct, matching, parts [28]. The six parts differ in
detailed anatomical connectivity and, at first sight, functions. Discrete lesions, targeting of
specific connections, and genetic models (particularly in SuM) have separated contributions
from subregions as well as structures [29-34]. For example, the medial MB and lateral SuM
both contribute to hippocampal activity during REM sleep [35,36]. Lateral and medial MB
lesions both impair performance on spatial memory tasks, but the pattern of impairments
is different [30,37]. Lateral SuM is thought to have a greater role in spatial learning
and memory [33] with medial SuM being more biased towards inhibitory learning [38].
Nonetheless, all six parts have similar roles in the integration of input from ascending
activating (‘arousal’) systems and in the rhythmic pacing of processing in, e.g., the Papez

1We use the word engram to refer to any one of the distributed Hebbian cell assemblies that are thought to be basic units of memory
[13].

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 3

circuit [14]. As noted above, SuM and MB are physically adjacent, and we suggest that they
are also computationally similar – but as key nodes in distinct loops that have more engaged
subcortex and cortex, respectively (see Figure 1). They provide subcortical motivational
input into parallel circuits that support hippocampo-cortical long-loop interactions.

Memory and Emotion?


Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

Papez [14] initially put forward his eponymous hypothalamically-mediated hippocampus-


cortex-hippocampus circuit as the basis of emotion [23]. However, to later researchers, the
role of the hippocampus in amnesia (particularly obvious in Henry Mollaison [39]) made
the circuit seem more relevant to memory. However, memory versus emotion is a false
dichotomy, given that goals require both, and hippocampal damage alters emotion [40-42].
Indeed, the hippocampus is among the main structures controlling the level of stress (and
other) hormones [43-45]. Current descriptions of memory, and its processes, need to better
reflect interdependence with emotion.

Lesions at any point within the Papez circuit (or its sub-loops) can impair memory.
While the severity and specificity of memory impairment can vary according to site of
pathology, a similar pattern of impairments can be seen throughout the system. Importantly,
in both humans and rodents, many aspects of memory remain intact, e.g., in simple item
discrimination tasks and procedural tasks [46,47]. Rather than affecting a particular type
of memory [48], impairment usually requires that any type of paradigm have sources
of interference – as when cues are combined into spatial features, complex objects, or
temporal contingencies [e.g., 49,50]. But Papez circuit structures are also all implicated
in stress, anxiety, and emotion – all can produce anxiolytic effects in standard tests of
anxiety including approach-avoidance conflicts [51]. (Consistent with this, Henry Mollaison
appeared to be unusually lacking in anxiety; J. Ogden, pers. comm.) Benzodiazepine
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

anxiolytics can produce amnesia; the high density of benzodiazepine receptors within both
the MB and SuM could contribute to these amnestic effects [52,53].

The MB and SuM receive external representations via inputs descending from limbic,
temporal, and prefrontal cortices. But their key role is integrating these representations
with ascending somatic inputs. For instance, both regions have cells strongly responsive to
running speed and they moderate hippocampal speed-cell function [54-56]. They are also
able, via inputs from the dorsal tegmental nucleus, to provide wider hippocampo-cortical
circuits with vestibular input that is crucial for spatial memory (including hippocampal theta
rhythm and ‘place fields’ [57,58]).

Lower-level input to memory circuits is not functionally trivial. Simple peripheral vestibular
receptor damage disrupts emotion and memory: it is associated with hippocampal atrophy
and may be a risk factor for dementia [59]. Other low-level inputs (including from
the ventral tegmental nucleus) provide sensory, motor, autonomic, and arousal-related
information and control the frequency of hippocampal theta pattern activity. The theta
pattern, per se, is important for neural plasticity [60] and spatial learning [61]. But, its
disruption does not change the basic organisation of place fields [62], unlike disruption of

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 4

head direction pathways in the antero- or lateral-dorsal thalamus [63,64]. The theta system
also responds strongly during threat-induced freezing [65].

In the context of goal processing, neocortex (particularly the interaction of anterior and
posterior neocortex) can maintain representations of the external world but, we argue, would
need the ascending inputs from the base of the Papez circuit to add key ‘contextual /
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

emotional / internal’ information. In particular, if episodic memory (and mental time travel)
depends on the cell assemblies originally postulated by Hebb [66], spatial and temporal
direction could be added by SuM/MB theta-rhythmic control during circuit processing. The
need for internal direction inputs for event processing would explain the importance of
simple vestibular input for ‘memory’. The ascending inputs to SuM would therefore inject
position/emotion information into the base of the Papez circuit, while also having more
direct connections to higher levels including the hippocampus.

Why is memory controlled by iterative loops?


The original circuit envisioned by Papez [14], while capturing the essence of a key
unidirectional loop around the various regions, does not give an accurate representation
of the multiple parallel Papez-architecture closed loops that it encompasses (Box 1; Figure
1) and that all have their lowest nodes in SuM/MB. The length and number of these closed
loops likely explain the fact that the range of frequencies of the (usually synchronous)
theta rhythmicity covers the round trip time from cortex to subcortex and back for these
circuits [67,68]. One of the shortest is SuM➔hippocampus➔MB➔SuM; where iteration
has been clearly demonstrated and directional control of the rhythmicity shown to vary
with its acceleration and deceleration [see 69]. Multiple loops also fit with suggestions
that the hippocampus uses “big loop” iteration2 for not only episodic memory but also
“integration of information across experiences” [70, p. 1342] and likely other forms of
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

inferential processing.

On first glance, the circuits give an impression of redundancy. For each indirect two-
or three-synapse connection in a single pathway, there is usually a direct single synapse
connection – with the direct and indirect paths often starting as collaterals of the originating
neurons [71].

Why would this be, computationally? These multi-level connections provide a means for
multi-level processing. Each pathway is one part of a hierarchical onion-like layering, where
a simple direct first pass through subcortical ‘survival circuits’ [9] (evolutionarily early,
conserved, and more likely linked to encoding and engram formation [72-74]) is followed
by progressively more complex indirect cortical processing (evolutionarily late, expanding,
and likely linked to recall, consolidation, reconsolidation, and perhaps more recently
even imagination [75-77]). In evolutionary terms this subcortical/cortical hierarchy allows
integration of fast but reflexive with slow but sophisticated processing [78] – achieving,
phylogenetically, the most efficient processing across a range of task urgencies.

2The circuits can be referred to as recurrent but, computationally, ‘recursion’ is a different process than iteration.

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 5

We think there are two, linked, issues here. The prime issue for an evolved system is a form
of Cocktail Party Problem. How to separate signal (situation-specific, not necessarily loud)
from noise (which may be loud and also situation-related) via motivational bias. You must
detect what you most need not what is most salient. This is analogous to the classic figure-
background problem, most easily solved in perceptual systems [79,80], where within-circuit
iteration is computationally advantageous [79]; but with added active motivational filtering.
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

Second, is the issue of recall. How is just one item retrieved against a background of similar
competing remembered items? For both issues there is a need to prevent percept-level
interference and catastrophic forgetting [48].

We suggest that each of the known parallel loops operates to separate key percepts and
engrams from interfering associations and alternatives in much the same iterative way as
a figure is separated from its ground [79] but by motivational filtering (at the SuM/MB
nodes). Current active memory – one of the earliest stages of processing – is known to hold
information without modification by simple iteration in frontal-posterior loops (Figure 2).
In contrast, we suggest that Papez-architecture loops through MB/SuM modify engrams via
iterative reprocessing. This iterative reprocessing progressively enhances active circulation
of target stimulus components, while suppressing active circulation of interfering stimulus
components, through application of a motivational filter. This would, in the first instance,
reduce confusing competing associations from non-target external stimuli.

An initial engram would usually be a simple cell assembly [66]. Both consolidation and
repeated experience will then add additional components to this original engram and
generate distributed engram ensembles [13]. The MBs provide theta pattern input that
guides plastic engram formation, both in the hippocampus and the cortex [35,81,82].
Functionally, damage to the MBs, and other regions within the basic Papez circuit, is
associated with relative impairments in recollective memory while ability to discriminate
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

whether simple items have been previously experienced (i.e., familiarity) is left intact [e.g.,
49]. These dissociations are often couched within dual-processing models: two functions
that are distinct and dissociable. However, we suggest that this pattern of impairments
instead reflects differences in the current configuration of the cell assembly coding the
engram, not on distinct processes. That is, for complex episodic information, activation
of the single correct associative representation is necessary for recall. In contrast, the
activation by perceptual input of a more impoverished engram would be sufficient to detect
a previously experienced item. As such, a nascent partial engram would be detectable as
familiar even when the final full engram is not yet sufficiently developed to be recalled
against the background of interference. Although it is implicit in the ideas of consolidation
and reconsolidation, we think that engrams are rarely seen as both unitary and dynamic
to this extent. But iteration [79] (particularly via replay [83-86]) combined with more
basic interactions between Hebbian learning and single-cell homeostasis [87] can solve the
problem of retaining viable (albeit labile) memories in a dynamic world.

Anatomically, there are multiple loops. Each of these loops operates at its own hierarchical
neural level (note the increase in loop nodes as one goes from ADT to AVT to AMT in
Figure 1). The brain uses parallel reflexive versus slow complex processing in many systems
[78] to balance urgency against clarity. Papez looping can start in a shorter loop, and then

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 6

be accompanied by processing in progressively longer loops (Figure 1). With sufficient


time (via consolidation or repeated experience) this would selectively enrich the engram by
allowing more distributed cell assemblies. Such enrichment could involve extension of the
time across which traces can be maintained [88], or later, extension of the components of
the engram with more experience, or consolidation, or reconsolidation [86], during wake or
sleep [35]. Simple repetition of information in each loop as in active memory would enhance
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

the strength of connections of a cell assembly through conventional Hebbian processes


[89,90] for both noise and signal. By contrast, active iterative reprocessing (combining
general enhancement with selective suppression of motivationally unwanted connections
[91,92]) should allow complex (e.g., temporally and episodically related) engrams to
become stabilized in the cortex. So, improvement in signal-to-noise ratio would not just
be through reduction in noise but also through creation of a richer signal through expansion
of the engram by progressively more sophisticated loops. Consistent with this, lesions of the
mammillothalamic tract in rats reduce expression of plasticity markers in the retrosplenial
cortex [81,93], reduce neurogenesis and spine density in the hippocampus, and reduce
long-term grey-matter changes observed in both hippocampus and cortex following spatial
training [35].

What triggers iterative processing?


Cortex-subcortex loop-related integration is an adjunct to the distinct cortex-cortex looping
that maintains active memory. Active memory is refreshed by loops in which frontal and
posterior cortex can be seen as simple relays. (The refreshment cost is born by the fact
that information needs to be temporarily maintained for adaptive function.) But cortical-
subcortical loops go beyond, and should not be confused with, the simplicity of loops
underlying active memory. The temptation is to see prefrontal cortex (highly expanded in
humans relative to other species) as a be-all and end-all. This is an (anthropocentric) error.
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

“To give to our prefrontal cortex the role of the autonomous origin of all our
decisions and actions leads inevitably to an infinite regress that should be avoided
(“What agency controls the prefrontal cortex? What other agency controls that
one?”…and so on ad infinitum). The only reasonable solution to the quandary is
to place the prefrontal cortex in the perception-action cycle, where the action can
originate anywhere, including the cerebral cortex, prefrontal or other.” [94, p. 7]

The perception-action cycle depends on the interaction of anterior with posterior cortex (for
an example, see Figure 2). But a goal requires not only situation (whether a local object or
a more complex context) but also motivation (a neutral object will not be a goal). To some
extent motivation will be supplied by limbic cortex via its circuits with the prefrontal cortex.
However, output from these areas, via the Papez circuit, to the hippocampal formation
then receives hippocampal and hypothalamic processing before returning in a modified
form to the prefrontal cortex via the thalamus [95]. Functional hippocampal output requires
that it receives theta pattern input from the medial septum, dependent on arousal-related
reticular and cerebellar input via areas such as SuM [61]. The passage of the resultant
hippocampal output through the MB appears to depend on similar arousal-related inputs
from the dorsal and ventral tegmental nuclei [96-98]. Thus SuM/MB would provide arousal

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 7

and interoceptive information to boost and bias iterative processing, while prefrontal cortex
would contain the cell assemblies that the process enhances; with increased signal/noise
ratio with each iteration. The result would be truly iterative processing (unlike the simple
echo of active memory) with all nodes in a loop able to adjust their output.

Does iteration affect cerebellum as well as neocortex?


Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

The hippocampus is connected by closed loops to not only neocortical areas but also to the
cerebellum; as are the cerebellum and neocortex [99,100]. The cerebellum’s contribution
to memory was traditionally considered limited to motor learning. In this context, eye
blink conditioning provides a well-studied example of both the cerebellar role in simple
conditioning and its interaction with limbic structures in, e.g., trace conditioning (Figure 3).
However, there is increasing evidence that cerebellum has a more widespread, cognitive and
emotional, role.

One proposal is that the cerebellum has a particular role in goal/reward learning, especially
in novel situations. Cerebellum supports this function using trial-by-trial error-correction,
similar to its contribution to motor learning [101] – here its involvement is akin to
the interference reduction seen with more cognitive engrams. Situation (coupled with
motivation) is a key element of goals. It is most easily understood by experimenters when a
situation reflects a place. The hippocampus, of course, has place (we would say goal) fields
and the cerebellum contributes to the association of hippocampal place fields with objects
by updating the place fields when the objects are re-located [102]. The synchronization
of cerebello-hippocampal interactions is also necessary for appropriate spatial processing
[103]. Overlapping similarities between MBs/SuM and cerebellum include contributions to
hippocampal processes for goal/spatial learning [102], involvement of theta [103], and a
bias for processing temporal information [50,104,105]. The unexpected hippocampal role
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

in eating [40-42] is also echoed by the cerebellum [106]. The direct connections between
MBs/SuM and the cerebellum form additional, remarkably similar, loops that provide further
inputs required for the development of representations to support long-term hippocampal-
dependent learning when, as with trace eyeblink conditioning, this involves the cerebellum.

More general computational features of interest in the cerebellum include [107, for review]:
1) its different implementations of learning at different timescales; 2) its greater involvement
during the first hours after learning; 3) extensive recurrent connections allowing iteration;
and 4) apparent similarity of computations across areas, with differences in functional output
depending on the specific other brain areas providing input and receiving output. All of these
features are reminiscent of extended hippocampal circuits.

“A key difference between the cerebellum and other brain areas is the extraordinary
amount of neural hardware devoted to input preprocessing in the cerebellum, which
is roughly equal to the number of neurons in the rest of the brain combined. Yet the
computational functions that have been attributed to the cerebellar preprocessing
stage are similar to those that have been described for other brain areas —
decorrelation, pattern separation, and the generation of temporal basis sets.” [107,
p. 244]

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 8

Importantly, while already clearly present in species like sharks that have no neocortex, the
cerebellum has steadily developed and expanded in phylogeny. In primates and humans,
in particular, its expansion has been greater than that of the neocortex. Compared to
baboons, human cerebellum is 15% larger than would be expected from the expansion of
neocortex; and this cerebellar expansion is likely to have contributed to human cognitive
evolution through increased technical intelligence, advanced technological capacities, and
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

preadaptation for language [108]. Recent studies have also indicated a role for the
cerebellum in both the perception and the action components of active memory [109].
As such, the perception-action cycle appears to be supported by distributed networks from
neocortex to cerebellum, including Papez-architecture circuits that contribute both directly
and indirectly [99,100].

Concluding remarks and future perspectives


Construction of complex memory engrams engages widespread cortical-subcortical
networks. Even simple engrams may require extended processing (consolidation,
reconsolidation) under conditions of extinction or reversal. Here, we have focused on the
contribution of two adjacent hypothalamic areas, MB and SuM, to their Papez-architecture
circuits.

We have reviewed data that demonstrate:

1. The presence of closed loops, which provides the capacity for iterative
processing (Figure 1).

2. Iterative looping in, for example, the SuM➔hippocampus➔MB➔SuM circuit


with evidence for driving of the circuit from SuM➔hippocampus during theta
frequency acceleration and from Hippocampus➔MB➔SuM during deceleration
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

[see 69].

3. The importance of the Papez-architecture circuits for interference reduction [e.g.,


49,50].

4. The contribution of the Papez-architecture circuits to the formation of mnemonic


representations in hippocampus and cortex [e.g., 35,110].

5. The integration of motivational and situational information into the circuits at


SuM/MB (see Box 1).

We have combined these known features of the system to suggest that the Papez-architecture
circuits use their known capacity for iteration to progressively adjust signal and noise
[79,80] coded by cell assemblies and so both enhance engrams and reduce interference.
Both MB and SuM have a driving role in the combining of representations of both internal
and external information that is needed to identify and prioritise engrams. Their component
nuclei are positioned so as to allow iteration within, and among, parallel distributed loops.
Iteration provides an ideal mechanism for integrating local and long-range inputs and
so constructing and integrating elements of complex (e.g., episodic) engrams; while also
limiting the effects of external (e.g., competing objects) and internal (competing associative
retrieval) interference.

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 9

While the original Papez circuit has been associated with memory processing for over 80
years [see 111], its precise role has been unclear. Many have seen it as a relay circuit,
passively transferring information; but this ignores the massive energy cost of axons and
nuclei. And to what ends would it simply relay information? The hippocampus and cortex
already have direct connections, what use is an additional loop? And why are there, in fact,
multiple, nested, loops – including links to cerebellum that are similar to those to prefrontal
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

cortex (with cerebellum and cortex also highly interconnected)?

We argue that the extended parallel distributed system of Papez-architecture loops has two
adaptive functions. First, it enables the integration of internal cues - emotional and positional
- into hippocampo-cortical-dependent engrams. Second, the iteration of information around
the circuit allows representations to be fine-tuned and enhanced in terms of detail, while
also increasing the signal-to-noise ratio via a process analogous to figure-ground separation
[79,80]. Iterative reprocessing helps to construct memory representations that have sufficient
contextual information to reduce interference across similar overlapping experiences. But
beyond that, it also provides additional gateways to influence and incorporate wider
networks for learning including the cerebellum. This adds further spatial and temporal
processing with, again, its local iterative looping enabling the formation of distinct,
separable, representations.

Seeing memory networks as extended subcortically beyond primary hippocampal-cortical


interaction is essential if we are to properly model the dynamic widespread neural activity
of memory construction and consolidation (see Outstanding Questions). This extension
poses new experimental challenges. Individual brain structures are just small windows into
the processing and storing of information. Detailed analysis of network level activity is
needed to understand the dynamic interactions across memory circuits that vary with time
and task demand. The greatest insights will be obtained when complex circuit analysis
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

and sophisticated behavioral paradigms are combined. However, at present, advances in


behavioral analyses are not developing at the same speed as the tools needed for circuit
analyses. It is also important to make use of tasks that can tap into cross-species processes,
while capitalizing on the natural behavior of individual species.

One key future methodological challenge is that the subcortical structures of interest are
small and deeply located. This makes it hard to identify neural activity originating from
them in humans. Non-invasive recording of electrical activity (such as with EEG) does not
pick up from deep sources; while techniques such as fMRI are limited in their spatial
resolution, particularly for small deep structures. Invasive recordings (in patients with
implanted electrodes for neurological treatment) have been made of some structures, for
example, of the MBs and anterior thalamic nuclei [112,113]. These have identified cross-
species similarities in oscillatory mechanisms, but there are few studies to date, and these
only involve individuals with underlying pathology. Future improvements in human imaging
should address some of these outstanding issues; and combining techniques such as fMRI
and EEG could also be advantageous. Testing the hypotheses derived from animal work
in humans, coupled with detailed analysis of circuits and task phases, should elucidate
the processing implied by Figures 1-3. Critically, we think that analyses should assess
the role of the multiple parallel networks that we know exist in some form across a

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 10

wide range of species. As with task selection, a key to future progress will be the use
of appropriate comparative neural techniques [114] that allow for species-specific (often
cortical) expression while assessing species-general (often subcortical) processes.

The extended Papez circuits, including the MBs and/or the anterior thalamic nuclei
have been implicated in several neurological disorders that are associated with memory
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

impairments, e.g., Korsakoff syndrome, Alzheimer’s disease, Down syndrome, and hypoxic-
ischaemic encephalopathy [115,116]. However, there is also increasing evidence for a role
for the medial diencephalon in psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders [117-119],
where memory impairments are present but also emotional dysregulation. Given the role we
attribute to the supramammillary and mammillary areas in Papez circuit processing we think
it is time for a closer look into overlaps between memory and emotion across mnemonic,
neurodevelopmental, and psychiatric disorders.

Acknowledgements
This work was funded in whole, or in part, by the Wellcome Trust [Wellcome Trust Senior Research Fellowship
awarded to SDV; Grant number WT 212273/Z/18/Z]. For the purpose of open access, the author has applied a CC
BY public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission.

References
1. Kandel ER. The molecular biology of memory storage: A dialogue between genes and synapses.
Science. 2002; 294: 1030–1038.
2. Scarf D, et al. Inhibition, the final frontier: The impact of hippocampal lesions on behavioral
inhibition and spatial processing in pigeons. Behav Neurosci. 2014; 128: 42–47. [PubMed:
24512064]
3. Thome A, et al. Evidence for an evolutionarily conserved memory coding scheme in the mammalian
hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2017; doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3057-16.2017
4. Franklin N, Bower GH. Retrieving actions from goal hierarchies. Bull Psychonom Soc. 1988; 26:
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

15–18. DOI: 10.3758/BF03334847


5. Gray, JA. In: Porter, R; Knight, J, editors. The structure of the emotions and the limbic system;
Physiology, emotion and psychosomatic illness, ciba foundation symposium 8 (new series); 1972.
87–130.
6. Harley CW. A role for norepinephrine in arousal, emotion and learning?: Limbic modulation by
norepinephrine and kety hypothesis. Prog Neuropsychopharm Biol Psychiatr. 1987; 11: 419–458.
7. Vanderwolf CH, et al. Are emotion and motivation localized in the limbic system and nucleus
accumbens? Behav Brain Res. 1988; 27: 45–58. [PubMed: 3342115]
8. LeDoux JE. Emotion and the limbic system concept. Conc Neurosci. 1991; 2: 169–199.
9. Mobbs D, LeDoux JE. Editorial overview: Survival behaviors and circuits. Current Opinion in
Behavioral Sciences. 2018; 24: 168–171. DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.10.004
10. McNaughton N, Corr PJ. Survival circuits and risk assessment. Current Opinion in Behavioral
Sciences. 2018; 24: 14–20. DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.018
11. Canteras NS. Hypothalamic survival circuits related to social and predatory defenses and their
interactions with metabolic control, reproductive behaviors and memory systems. Current Opinion
in Behavioral Sciences. 2018; 24: 7–13. DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.017
12. Sternson SM. Hypothalamic survival circuits: Blueprints for purposive behaviors. Neuron. 2013;
77: 810–824. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.018 [PubMed: 23473313]
13. Josselyn SA, Tonegawa S. Memory engrams: Recalling the past and imagining the future. Science.
2020; 367 doi: 10.1126/science.aaw4325

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 11

14. Papez JW. A proposed mechanism of emotion. Archives of Neurological Psychiatry. 1937; 38:
725–743.
15. Ashwell KWS. Development of the dorsal and ventral thalamus in platypus (ornithorhynchus
anatinus) and short-beaked echidna (tachyglossus aculeatus). Brain Structure and Function. 2012;
217: 577–589. DOI: 10.1007/s00429-011-0364-3 [PubMed: 22113857]
16. Wolf A, Ryu S. Specification of posterior hypothalamic neurons requires coordinated activities of
fezf2, otp, sim1a and foxb1.2. Development. 2013; 140: 1762–1773. DOI: 10.1242/dev.085357
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

[PubMed: 23533176]
17. Gerlai, RT. Behavioral and neural genetics of zebrafish. Gerlai, RT, editor. Academic Press; 2020.
205–220.
18. Saleem S, Kannan RR. Zebrafish: An emerging real-time model system to study alzheimer’s
disease and neurospecific drug discovery. Cell Death Discovery. 2018; 4: 45. doi: 10.1038/
s41420-018-0109-7
19. Folgueira M, et al. Experimental study of the connections of the preglomerular nuclei and corpus
mamillare in the rainbow trout, oncorhynchus mykiss. Brain Res Bull. 2005; 66: 361–364. DOI:
10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.03.001 [PubMed: 16144615]
20. Nauta WJ. Hypothalamic nuclei and their fiber connections. The hypothalamus. 1969. 136–209.
21. Buzsaki G, et al. Scaling brain size, keeping timing: Evolutionary preservation of brain rhythms.
Neuron. 2013; 80: 751–764. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.002 [PubMed: 24183025]
22. Buzsaki G, et al. Emergence of cognition from action. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol. 2014;
79: 41–50. DOI: 10.1101/sqb.2014.79.024679 [PubMed: 25752314]
23. Vann SD, Nelson AJ. The mammillary bodies and memory: More than a hippocampal relay. Prog
Brain Res. 2015; 219: 163–185. DOI: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.03.006 [PubMed: 26072239]
24. Le Gros Clark, WE. The hypothalamus : Morphological, functional, clinical and surgical aspects.
Oliver and Boyd; 1938.
25. Hofman MA, Swaab DF. The human hypothalamus: Comparative morphometry and photoperiodic
influences. Prog Brain Res. 1992; 93: 133–147. DOI: 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)64569-0 [PubMed:
1480746]
26. Saper CB, Lowell BB. The hypothalamus. Curr Biol. 2014; 24: R1111–R1116. DOI: 10.1016/
j.cub.2014.10.023 [PubMed: 25465326]
27. Burdakov D, Peleg-Raibstein D. The hypothalamus as a primary coordinator of memory updating.
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

Physiol Behav. 2020; 223 112988 doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112988 [PubMed: 32485184]


28. Pan, W-x; McNaughton, N. The supramammillary area: Its organization, functions and relationship
to the hippocampus. Prog Neurobiol. 2004; 74: 127–166. [PubMed: 15556285]
29. Pan WX, McNaughton N. The medial supramammillary nucleus, spatial learning and the frequency
of hippocampal theta activity. Brain Res. 1997; 764: 101–108. [PubMed: 9295198]
30. Dillingham CM, Vann SD. Why isn't the head direction system necessary for direction?
Lessons from the lateral mammillary nuclei. Front Neural Circuits. 2019; 13: 60. doi: 10.3389/
fncir.2019.00060 [PubMed: 31619970]
31. Dillingham CM, et al. How do mammillary body inputs contribute to anterior thalamic function?
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015; 54: 108–119. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.07.025 [PubMed:
25107491]
32. Heiss JE, et al. Parallel arousal pathways in the lateral hypothalamus. eNeuro. 2018; 5 doi:
10.1523/eneuro.0228-18.2018
33. Li Y, et al. Supramammillary nucleus synchronizes with dentate gyrus to regulate spatial memory
retrieval through glutamate release. eLife. 2020; 9 doi: 10.7554/eLife.53129
34. Chen S, et al. A hypothalamic novelty signal modulates hippocampal memory. Nature. 2020; 586:
270–274. DOI: 10.1038/s41586-020-2771-1 [PubMed: 32999460]
35. Dillingham CM, et al. Mammillothalamic disconnection alters hippocampocortical oscillatory
activity and microstructure: Implications for diencephalic amnesia. J Neurosci. 2019; 39: 6696–
6713. DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0827-19.2019 [PubMed: 31235646]

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 12

36. Billwiller F, et al. Gaba-glutamate supramammillary neurons control theta and gamma oscillations
in the dentate gyrus during paradoxical (rem) sleep. Brain Struct Funct. 2020; 225: 2643–2668.
DOI: 10.1007/s00429-020-02146-y [PubMed: 32970253]
37. Harland B, et al. The head direction cell system and behavior: The effects of lesions to the lateral
mammillary bodies on spatial memory in a novel landmark task and in the water maze. Behav
Neurosci. 2015; doi: 10.1037/bne0000106
38. Woodnorth MA, McNaughton N. Similar effects of medial supramammillary or systemic injection
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

of chlordiazepoxide on both theta frequency and fixed-interval responding. Cogn Affect Behav
Neurosci. 2002; 2: 76–83. DOI: 10.3758/cabn.2.1.76 [PubMed: 12452586]
39. Ogden, J, Corkin, S. Memory mechanisms: A tribute to g.V. Goddard. Abraham, WC, , et al.,
editors. Lawrence Erlbaum; 1991. 195–215.
40. Davidson TL, et al. Contributions of the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex to energy and
body weight regulation. Hippocampus. 2009; 19: 235–252. DOI: 10.1002/hipo.20499 [PubMed:
18831000]
41. Tracy AL, et al. The hippocampus and motivation revisited: Appetite and activity. Behav Brain
Res. 2001; 127: 13–23. [PubMed: 11718882]
42. Davidson T, et al. Memory inhibition and energy regulation. Physiol Behav. 2005; 86: 731–746.
[PubMed: 16263144]
43. Lathe R. Hormones and the hippocampus. J Endocrinol. 2001; 169: 205–231. DOI: 10.1677/
joe.0.1690205 [PubMed: 11312139]
44. Lathe R, Singadia S, Jordan C, Riedel G. The interoceptive hippocampus: Mouse brain endocrine
receptor expression highlights a dentate gyrus (DG)–cornu ammonis (CA) challenge–sufficiency
axis. PLoS ONE. 2020; 15 (1) e0227575 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0227575 [PubMed: 31940330]
45. Lathe R. The individuality of mice. Genes, Brain and Behav. 2004; 3: 317–327. DOI: 10.1111/
j.1601-183X.2004.00083.x
46. Van der Werf YD, et al. Deficits of memory, executive functioning and attention following
infarction in the thalamus; a study of 22 cases with localised lesions. Neuropsychologia. 2003; 41:
1330–1344. S0028393203000599 [pii] [PubMed: 12757906]
47. Oudman E, et al. Procedural learning and memory rehabilitation in korsakoff's syndrome - a
review of the literature. Neuropsychol Rev. 2015; 25: 134–148. DOI: 10.1007/s11065-015-9288-7
[PubMed: 26047664]
48. McNaughton N, Wickens J. Hebb, pandemonium and catastrophic hypermnesia: The hippocampus
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

as a suppressor of inappropriate associations. Cortex. 2003; 39: 1139–1163. [PubMed: 14584571]


49. Tsivilis D, et al. A disproportionate role for the fornix and mammillary bodies in recall versus
recognition memory. Nat Neurosci. 2008; 11: 834–842. nn.2149 [pii] doi: 10.1038/nn.2149
[PubMed: 18552840]
50. Dillingham CM, et al. Time to put the mammillothalamic pathway into context. Neurosci Biobehav
Rev. 2021; 121: 60–74. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.11.031 [PubMed: 33309908]
51. Gray, JA, McNaughton, N. The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of
the septo-hippocampal system. 2 ed. Vol. 33. Oxford University Press; 2000. Oxford psychology
series no. 33
52. Woodnorth MA, McNaughton N. Benzodiazepine receptors in the medial-posterior hypothalamus
mediate the reduction of hippocampal theta frequency by chlordiazepoxide. Brain Res. 2002; 954:
194–201. DOI: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)03267-5 [PubMed: 12414102]
53. Zezula J, et al. Benzodiazepine receptor sites in the human brain: Autoradiographic mapping.
Neuroscience. 1988; 25: 771–795. DOI: 10.1016/0306-4522(88)90036-x [PubMed: 2841624]
54. Sharp PE, Turner-Williams S. Movement-related correlates of single-cell activity in the medial
mammillary nucleus of the rat during a pellet-chasing task. J Neurophysiol. 2005; 94: 1920–1927.
DOI: 10.1152/jn.00194.2005 [PubMed: 15857969]
55. Sharp PE, Koester K. Lesions of the mammillary body region alter hippocampal movement signals
and theta frequency: Implications for path integration models. Hippocampus. 2008; 18: 862–878.
DOI: 10.1002/hipo.20474 [PubMed: 18702112]
56. Farrell JS, et al. Supramammillary regulation of locomotion and hippocampal activity. Science.
2021; 374: 1492–1496. DOI: 10.1126/science.abh4272 [PubMed: 34914519]

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 13

57. Russell NA, et al. Lesions of the vestibular system disrupt hippocampal theta rhythm in the rat. J
Neurophysiol. 2006; 96: 4–14. [PubMed: 16772515]
58. Russell NA, et al. Long-term effects of permanent vestibular lesions on hippocampal spatial firing.
J Neurosci. 2003; 23: 6490–6498. [PubMed: 12878690]
59. Smith PF. The vestibular system and cognition. Curr Opin Neurol. 2017; 30: 84–89. [PubMed:
27845944]
60. Albers C, et al. Theta-specific susceptibility in a model of adaptive synaptic plasticity. Front
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

Comput Neurosci. 2013; 7: 170. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00170 [PubMed: 24312047]


61. McNaughton N, et al. Restoring theta-like rhythmicity in rats restores initial learning in the morris
water maze. Hippocampus. 2006; 16: 1102–1110. [PubMed: 17068783]
62. Mizumori SJ, et al. Reversible inactivation of the medial septum differentially affects two forms of
learning in rats. Brain Res. 1990; 528: 12–20. [PubMed: 2245328]
63. Mizumori SJY, et al. Reversible inactivation of the lateral dorsal thalamus disrupts hippocampal
place representation and impairs spatial learning. Brain Res. 1994; 644: 168–174. [PubMed:
8032944]
64. Calton JL, et al. Hippocampal place cell instability after lesions of the head direction cell network.
J Neurosci. 2003; 23: 9719–9731. 23/30/9719 [pii] [PubMed: 14585999]
65. Sainsbury RS, Montoya CP. The relationship between type 2 theta and behavior. Physiol Behav.
1984; 33: 621–626. [PubMed: 6522481]
66. Hebb, DO. The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. Wiley-Interscience; 1949.
67. Miller R. Cortico-hippocampal interplay: Self-organizing phase-locked loops for indexing memory.
Psychobiology. 1989; 17: 115–128.
68. Miller, R. Cortico-hippocampal interplay and the representation of contexts in the brain. Springer-
Verlag; 1991.
69. Ruan M, et al. Bi-directional theta modulation between the septo-hippocampal system and the
mammillary area in free-moving rats. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2017; 11 Article 62 doi:
10.3389/fncir.2017.00062 [PubMed: 28955209]
70. Koster R, et al. Big-loop recurrence within the hippocampal system supports integration
of information across episodes. Neuron. 2018; 99: 1342–1354. e1346 doi: 10.1016/
j.neuron.2018.08.009 [PubMed: 30236285]
71. Kinnavane L, et al. Collateral projections innervate the mammillary bodies and retrosplenial
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

cortex: A new category of hippocampal cells. eNeuro. 2018; 5 doi: 10.1523/eneuro.0383-17.2018


72. Sif J, et al. Time-dependent sequential increases in [14c]2-deoxyglucose uptake in subcortical and
cortical structures during memory consolidation of an operant training in mice. Behav Neural Biol.
1991; 56: 43–61. DOI: 10.1016/0163-1047(91)90279-y [PubMed: 1867626]
73. Vann SD, Aggleton JP. Evidence of a spatial encoding deficit in rats with lesions of the
mammillary bodies or mammillothalamic tract. J Neurosci. 2003; 23: 3506–3514. 23/8/3506 [pii]
[PubMed: 12716960]
74. Caulo M, et al. Functional mri study of diencephalic amnesia in wernicke-korsakoff syndrome.
Brain. 2005; 128: 1584–1594. awh496 [pii] doi: 10.1093/brain/awh496 [PubMed: 15817513]
75. Gauthier M, et al. Late post-learning participation of entorhinal cortex in memory processes. Brain
Res. 1982; 233: 255–264. DOI: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)91201-x [PubMed: 7059810]
76. Milczarek MM, et al. Spatial memory engram in the mouse retrosplenial cortex. Curr Biol. 2018;
28: 1975–1980. e1976 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.002 [PubMed: 29887312]
77. Mitchell AS, et al. Dissociable roles for cortical and subcortical structures in memory retrieval and
acquisition. J Neurosci. 2008; 28: 8387–8396. DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1924-08.2008 [PubMed:
18716197]
78. LeDoux JE. Emotion, memory and the brain. Sci Am. 1994; June. 270: 50–59.
79. Marr D, Poggio T. Cooperative computation of stereo disparity. Science. 1976; 194: 283–287. DOI:
10.2307/1742217 [PubMed: 968482]
80. Marr, D. Vision: A computational approach. 1982.

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 14

81. Frizzati A, et al. Comparable reduction in zif268 levels and cytochrome oxidase activity in the
retrosplenial cortex following mammillothalamic tract lesions. Neuroscience. 2016; 330: 39–49.
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.05.030 [PubMed: 27233617]
82. Vann SD, Albasser MM. Hippocampal, retrosplenial, and prefrontal hypoactivity in a model of
diencephalic amnesia: Evidence towards an interdependent subcortical-cortical memory network.
Hippocampus. 2009; 19: 1090–1102. DOI: 10.1002/hipo.20574 [PubMed: 19280662]
83. Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL. “Replay” of hippocampal “memories” - response. Science. 1996;
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

274: 1216–1217. [PubMed: 8966590]


84. Fujisawa S, Ouchi A. Replays of socially acquired information in the hippocampus. Neuron. 2022;
110: 744–745. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2022.02.005 [PubMed: 35240063]
85. Gillespie AK, et al. Hippocampal replay reflects specific past experiences rather than a plan
for subsequent choice. Neuron. 2021; 109: 3149–3163. e3146 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.07.029
[PubMed: 34450026]
86. Mou X, et al. Observational learning promotes hippocampal remote awake replay toward future
reward locations. Neuron. 2022; 110: 891–902. e897 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.12.005 [PubMed:
34965381]
87. Rule ME, O'Leary T. Self-healing codes: How stable neural populations can track continually
reconfiguring neural representations. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2022; 119 doi: 10.1073/
pnas.2106692119
88. Hattori R, Komiyama T. Context-dependent persistency as a coding mechanism for robust and
widely distributed value coding. Neuron. 2021; doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.11.001
89. Sumner RL, et al. The role of hebbian learning in human perception: A methodological and
theoretical review of the human visual long-term potentiation paradigm. Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
2020; 115: 220–237. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.013 [PubMed: 32562886]
90. Nadel L, Maurer AP. Recalling lashley and reconsolidating hebb. Hippocampus. 2020; 30: 776–
793. DOI: 10.1002/hipo.23027 [PubMed: 30216593]
91. Walker MP. A refined model of sleep and the time course of memory formation. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences. 2005; 28: 51–104. [PubMed: 16047457]
92. Inostroza M, Born J. Sleep for preserving and transforming episodic memory. Annu Rev Neurosci.
2013; 36: 79–102. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170429 [PubMed: 23642099]
93. Perry BAL, et al. Anterior thalamic nuclei lesions have a greater impact than mammillothalamic
tract lesions on the extended hippocampal system. Hippocampus. 2018; 28: 121–135. DOI:
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

10.1002/hipo.22815 [PubMed: 29150979]


94. Fuster JM. Cognitive networks (cognits) process and maintain working memory. Front Neur Circ.
2022; 15 doi: 10.3389/fncir.2021.790691
95. Adeyelu T, Ogundele OM. Anterior thalamic glutamatergic neurons modulate prefrontal cortical
firing and regularity. Neurosci Lett. 2022; 776 136565 doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2022.136565
[PubMed: 35278647]
96. Vann SD. Dismantling the papez circuit for memory in rats. eLife. 2013; 2 e00736 doi: 10.7554/
eLife.00736 [PubMed: 23805381]
97. Bassett JP, et al. Lesions of the tegmentomammillary circuit in the head direction system disrupt
the head direction signal in the anterior thalamus. J Neurosci. 2007; 27: 7564–7577. 27/28/7564
[pii] doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0268-07.2007 [PubMed: 17626218]
98. Küçükerden M, et al. Compromised mammillary body connectivity and psychotic symptoms in
mice with di- and mesencephalic ablation of st8sia2. Translational Psychiatry. 2022; 12: 51. doi:
10.1038/s41398-022-01816-1 [PubMed: 35115485]
99. Stein H. Why does the neocortex need the cerebellum for working memory? J Neurosci. 2021; 41:
6368–6370. DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0701-21.2021 [PubMed: 34321336]
100. Yu W, Krook-Magnuson E. Cognitive collaborations: Bidirectional functional connectivity
between the cerebellum and the hippocampus. Front Syst Neurosci. 2015; 9
101. Raymond JL. Research on the cerebellum yields rewards. Nature. 2020; 106: 202–203.
102. Zeidler Z, et al. Hippobellum: Acute cerebellar modulation alters hippocampal dynamics and
function. J Neurosci. 2020; 40: 6910–6926. DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0763-20.2020 [PubMed:
32769107]

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 15

103. Watson TC, et al. Anatomical and physiological foundations of cerebello-hippocampal


interaction. eLife. 2019; 8 e41896 doi: 10.7554/eLife.41896 [PubMed: 31205000]
104. Vinogradova OS. Hippocampus as comparator: Role of the two input and two output systems of
the hippocampus in selection and registration of information. Hippocampus. 2001; 11: 578–598.
DOI: 10.1002/hipo.1073 [PubMed: 11732710]
105. De Zeeuw CI, et al. Diversity and dynamism in the cerebellum. Nat Neurosci. 2021; 24: 160–167.
DOI: 10.1038/s41593-020-00754-9 [PubMed: 33288911]
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

106. Low AYT, et al. Reverse-translational identification of a cerebellar satiation network. Nature.
2021; 600: 269–273. DOI: 10.1038/s41586-021-04143-5 [PubMed: 34789878]
107. Raymond JL, Medina JF. Computational principles of supervised learning in the cerebellum.
Ann Rev Neurosci. 2018; 41: 233–253. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-080317-061948 [PubMed:
29986160]
108. Barton RA, Venditti C. Rapid evolution of the cerebellum in humans and other great apes. Curr
Biol. 2014; 24: 2440–2444. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.08.056 [PubMed: 25283776]
109. Brissenden JA, et al. Stimulus-specific visual working memory representations in
human cerebellar lobule viib/viiia. J Neurosci. 2021; 41: 1033–1045. DOI: 10.1523/
jneurosci.1253-20.2020 [PubMed: 33214320]
110. Garden DL, et al. Anterior thalamic lesions stop synaptic plasticity in retrosplenial cortex slices:
Expanding the pathology of diencephalic amnesia. Brain. 2009; 132: 1847–1857. awp090 [pii]
doi: 10.1093/brain/awp090 [PubMed: 19403787]
111. Barbizet J. Defect of memorizing of hippocampal-mammillary origin: A review. J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiat. 1963; 26: 127–135. DOI: 10.1136/jnnp.26.2.127 [PubMed: 13966551]
112. Sweeney-Reed CM, et al. Corticothalamic phase synchrony and cross-frequency coupling predict
human memory formation. eLife. 2014; 4 doi: 10.7554/eLife.05352
113. van Rijckevorsel K, et al. Deep eeg recordings of the mammillary body in epilepsy patients.
Epilepsia. 2005; 46: 781–785. DOI: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2005.45704.x [PubMed: 15857449]
114. McNaughton N. Brain maps of fear and anxiety. Nat Hum Behav. 2019; 3: 662–663. DOI:
10.1038/s41562-019-0621-7 [PubMed: 31110336]
115. Meys KME, et al. The mammillary bodies: A review of causes of injury in infants and children.
AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2022; doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A7463
116. Forno G, et al. Going round in circles-the papez circuit in alzheimer's disease. Eur J Neurosci.
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

2021; 54: 7668–7687. DOI: 10.1111/ejn.15494 [PubMed: 34656073]


117. Küçükerden M, et al. Compromised mammillary body connectivity and psychotic symptoms in
mice with di- and mesencephalic ablation of st8sia2. Transl Psychiatr. 2022; 12: 51. doi: 10.1038/
s41398-022-01816-1
118. Roy DS, et al. Anterior thalamic dysfunction underlies cognitive deficits in a subset
of neuropsychiatric disease models. Neuron. 2021; 109: 2590–2603. e2513 doi: 10.1016/
j.neuron.2021.06.005 [PubMed: 34197733]
119. Bernstein HG, et al. Strongly reduced number of parvalbumin-immunoreactive projection neurons
in the mammillary bodies in schizophrenia: Further evidence for limbic neuropathology. Ann N
Y Acad Sci. 2007; 1096: 120–127. DOI: 10.1196/annals.1397.077 [PubMed: 17405923]
120. Joyce MKP, et al. Pathways for memory, cognition and emotional context: Hippocampal,
subgenual area 25, and amygdalar axons show unique interactions in the primate thalamic
reuniens nucleus. J Neurosci. 2022; 42: 1068–1089. DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1724-21.2021
[PubMed: 34903572]
121. Squire LR, Zola-Morgan S. The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science. 1991; 253:
1380–1386. [PubMed: 1896849]
122. Kitanishi T, et al. Robust information routing by dorsal subiculum neurons. Sci Adv. 2021; 7
eabf1913 doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abf1913 [PubMed: 33692111]
123. Heilbronner SR, Hayden BY. Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: A bottom-up view. Annu
Rev Neurosci. 2016; 39: 149–170. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-070815-013952 [PubMed:
27090954]
124. Vogt, B. Cingulate neurobiology and disease. Oxford University Press; 2009.

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 16

125. Naber PA, et al. Perirhinal cortex input to the hippocampus in the rat: Evidence for paralle
pathways, both direct and indirect. A combined physiological and anatomical study. Eur J
Neurosci. 1999; 11: 4119–4133. [PubMed: 10583500]
126. Bassant MH, Poindessous-Jazat F. Ventral tegmental nucleus of gudden: A pontine hippocampal
theta generator? Hippocampus. 2001; 11: 809–813. DOI: 10.1002/hipo.1096 [PubMed:
11811675]
127. Kocsis B, et al. Theta synchronization in the limbic system: The role of gudden's tegmental
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

nuclei. Eur J Neurosci. 2001; 13: 381–388. [PubMed: 11168543]


128. Harley CW, et al. Hypothalamic afferents to the dorsal dentate gyrus contain acetylcholinesterase.
Brain Res. 1983; 270: 335–339. DOI: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90609-1 [PubMed: 6883102]
129. de Lima MA, et al. A role for the anteromedial thalamic nucleus in the acquisition of contextual
fear memory to predatory threats. Brain Struct Funct. 2017; 222: 113–129. DOI: 10.1007/
s00429-016-1204-2 [PubMed: 26951288]
130. MacKay, DG. The organization of perception and action: A theory for language and other
cognitive skills. Springer-Verlag; 1987.
131. Fuster, JM. Prog Brain Res. Uylings, HBM, , et al., editors. Elsevier Science publishers B. V.
(Biomed division); 1990. 313–324.
132. Goldman-Rakic PS. Working memory and the mind. Sci Am. 1992; 267: 73–79.
133. Fuster, JM. Memory in the cerebral cortex. MIT Press; 1995.
134. Berry SD, Hoffmann LC. Hippocampal theta-dependent eyeblink classical conditioning:
Coordination of a distributed learning system. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2011; 95: 185–189.
[PubMed: 21129492]
135. Berger TW, et al. Lesions of the retrosplenial cortex produce deficits in reversal learning
of the rabbit nictitating membrane response: Implications for potential interactions between
hippocampal and cerebellar brain systems. Behav Neurosci. 1986; 100: 802–809. [PubMed:
3814339]
136. Cacciola A, et al. Constrained spherical deconvolution tractography reveals cerebello-mammillary
connections in humans. Cerebellum. 2017; 16: 483–495. DOI: 10.1007/s12311-016-0830-9
[PubMed: 27774574]
137. Bohne P, et al. A new projection from the deep cerebellar nuclei to the hippocampus via
the ventrolateral and laterodorsal thalamus in mice. Front Neur Circ. 2019; 13 doi: 10.3389/
fncir.2019.00051
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

138. Hoffmann LC, et al. Harnessing the power of theta: Natural manipulations of cognitive
performance during hippocampal theta-contingent eyeblink conditioning. Front Syst Neurosci.
2015; 9 doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00050
139. Dietrichs E, Haines DE. Interconnections between hypothalamus and cerebellum. Anat Embryol.
1989; 179: 207–220. DOI: 10.1007/BF00326585
140. Cheng DT, et al. Neural substrates underlying human delay and trace eyeblink conditioning.
PNAS. 2008; 195: 8108–8113.

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 17

Box 1

Connections of SuM and MB – two hierarchies of iterative loops


Cortex-subcortex connections are complicated, involving sets of hierarchically organised
parallel closed loops. The higher functional levels of the hippocampus proper (area CA1
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

and the subiculum/SUB) send direct input only to evolutionarily old parts of the frontal
lobe (i.e., prefrontal allocortex). In contrast, the Papez circuit allows CA3 to control
more evolutionarily recent frontal isocortex [14] via the MB ➔ anterior thalamic nuclei
(ATN)3 ➔ frontal cortex path. This ATN projection also allows the MB wide influence
on temporal lobe memory systems [121] including the entorhinal cortex, retrosplenial
cortex (RSp), and SUB (completing the Papez circuit). The MB are much more than
a ‘relay’ – integrating hippocampal outflow with independent mnemonic inputs from
Gudden’s tegmental nuclei in the midbrain.

The nuclei of the MB are connected by an onion-like series of nested loops to parallel
levels of the hippocampal formation (Figure 1). “The different levels are unlikely to exist
simply so as to pass unaltered information on to one another; each must allow some
particular transformation or integration of its inputs. We propose that the hippocampus
is a system of logical gates which allows different types of information to progress to
different points of the circuit and hence to produce (or in many cases not produce)
outputs from different levels of the system subject to different conditions” [51, p221].
Importantly, the intra-hippocampal connections are all in the same single direction. So,
the hippocampal formation is rectifying (forcing information to pass in only one direction
in the loops), as are the connections from SuM to MB.

Important features of the hippocampal ‘onion’ are: a) initial, partially-processed, output


is from area CA3, ‘relayed’ by the lateral septum, to SuM; while, b) the main final
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

highly-processed output is from SUB/RSp direct to MB. The similarity of the parallel
loops suggests similar computational function, but the specific connections suggest that
SuM mediates reflexive [78] and SUB/RSp mediate sophisticated processing. That is,
SuM controls subcortical processing, using partially-processed (CA3) information and
with no neocortical efference copy. Fully processed information from SUB/RSp goes
directly to the higher (goal processing) areas of prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate;
with an efference copy (often via collaterals [71] and with similar cell-firing fields [122])
going directly to MB as part of “slow and sophisticated” processing.

The Papez circuit is also onion-like. Lateral, medio-lateral, and medial areas of
MB primarily project to anterodorsal, anteroventral, and anteromedial thalamus,
respectively. Prefrontal cortex and perirhinal/parahippocampal cortex complete the circuit
in entorhinal cortex. All three thalamic areas make return projections to entorhinal cortex,
SUB, and RSp.

3Loops through, e.g. nucleus reuniens are also important [120] but beyond the scope of this article

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 18

Outstanding Questions
There are many hippocampus⬄cortex and hippocampus⬄cerebellum loops, with
widely varying architectures, and important mammillary connections independent of
hippocampus. How do the architecturally different loops through, e.g., nucleus reuniens,
interact with the anterior thalamic Papez-architecture circuits described in the current
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

article? How do the circuits through the neocortex compare to those through the
cerebellum?

Other types of circuit exist for other purposes. Papez-architecture circuits are not required
for learning and memory in terms of simple stimuli and simple motor responses (e.g.,
eyeblink or freezing) but become involved when interference from pre-emptive (trace
conditioning) or competing (water maze, conditioning to ‘context’) responses must
be suppressed. What qualitatively and/or quantitatively determines the threshold for
requiring Papez-architecture processing? Is this linked to the need for inhibition? Does
this vary across individuals?

Disruption of MB/SuM disrupts a number of electrophysiological signatures across


hippocampus and cortex. We know that behavioural impairments can be ameliorated
with artificial stimulation that reverses this disruption – but how general is this? Would
theta-frequency stimulation be sufficient to reverse all impairments due to Papez-circuit
dysfunction or would the stimulation need to be more nuanced, e.g., taking into account
gamma rhythmicity and theta-gamma coupling?

Memory construction happens across different time frames: during the event,
immediately following the event and during subsequent periods of sleep. MB/SuM
circuits have been implicated in all stages. Is essentially the same iterative integrative
computation carried out during these different stages or does it vary according to state of
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

the constructed engram?

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 19
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

Figure 1.
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

Overview of long and short loop connections from the hippocampus via mammillary area
to the frontal cortex and back present in mammals ranging from rodents to primates
[28,51,71,123-128]. The mammillary bodies (MB) and supramammillary area (SuM) have
aligned medial, mediolateral, and lateral parts. MB targets prefrontal and anterior cingulate
cortex, which target the hippocampal formation, completing the Papez circuit. Tonic
arousing reticular input to medial (p = parvicellular [28]) SuM is converted to phasic theta
rhythmicity (θ), passed to mediolateral (g = grandicellular) SuM, then diagonal band of
Broca (DBB)/medial septum (MS) complex then hippocampal formation. Lateral (s = shell)
SuM project to entorhinal cortex (EC). The fimbria (fi), fornix (fx), and internal capsule
(ic) return hippocampal formation output to SuM/MB in onion-like, nested loops. EC,
dentate gyrus (DG), CA3, CA1, subiculum (SUB), and retrosplenial cortex (RSp) connect
unidirectionally. Successive loops are similar, but outside loops have greater delays and
more highly processed information. There is a similar ‘onion’ with mammillothalamic tract
(mt) output from MB and output from AMT/AVT/ADT to infralimbic (IFL), prelimbic
(PRL) and anterior cingulate (ACC) cortex. Dorsal and ventral prefrontal (PRFd, PRFv)
then perirhinal (Peri) and parahippocampal (Para) cortex complete the Papez circuit in
EC. We have not included, e.g., the AMT-CA1 connection [129], to keep the fundamental
architecture of the loop circuits clear. Abbreviations: ADT, AMT, AVT = anterior thalamus,

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 20

dorsal, medial, ventral, respectively. ML, MML, MMM = mamillary nucleus, lateral, medial
pars lateralis, medial pars medialis, respectively.
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 21
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

Figure 2.
Perception and action are intertwined in a cycle in mammals. They share their neural
circuitry [130,131]. The will to act must start with a goal, which is usually marked by an
external percept. The percept, itself, may be fleeting, but then prefrontal cortex uses iterative
loops [132] to hold information in posterior cortex in the form of active memory [133]. The
figure illustrates these general principles with a simple example based on a delayed response
working-memory task in monkeys [132]. A target position is briefly indicated on a screen
and registered by the retina (top left) which passes information to visual cortex, which
in turn activates prefrontal cortex. During a delay interval, activity from prefrontal cortex

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 22

refreshes visual cortex, keeping the stimulus location in active memory. When the end of
the delay interval is signalled, this location is read out to circuits controlling eye movement
and the monkey then looks at the position where the target was before the delay. Note that,
unlike trace conditioning tasks, delay tasks do not depend on hippocampal circuitry. Figure
adapted from [51] with permission.
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.


McNaughton and Vann Page 23
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

Figure 3.
The role of the hippocampus (HPC) in eyeblink conditioning in mammals, based on
[134-138]. HPC cells show firing patterns – triggered by the conditional (CS) but not
unconditional (US) stimulus – that arise in training, progress during conditioning, and often
model the conditioned eyeblink response (CR). Note that this combination of stimulus
control with response-related firing implies that the hippocampal circuit is processing
Europe PMC Funders Author Manuscripts

complex goal information rather than simple stimuli or actions. Hippocampal lesions
do not affect simple, or delayed, or discriminative (CS+/CS-) conditioning. However
hippocampal lesions affect both trace conditioning and discrimination reversal learning.
Trace conditioning is mediated via output from delay-line activity from prefrontal cortex
to lateral pontine nuclei (LPN) that inhibits activation of the eyeblink by the CS+ (in this
case there is no CS-). Reversal is mediated via output from the retrosplenial cortex (RSp)
that inhibits activation of the eyeblink by the CS- (which was the CS+ until reversal was
started). Hippocampal theta-related output from HPC via the supramammillary nucleus,
medial mammillary nucleus (MMM), and anterior thalamus (ATN) via pontine nuclei [139],
impacts rate of learning [138]. Note that, in humans, “comparable delay and trace activation
was measured in the cerebellum, whereas greater hippocampal activity was detected during
trace compared with delay conditioning” [140] and there is good evidence for involvement
of such cerebellar circuits in working memory generally [99].

Trends Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2022 July 01.

You might also like