Cheung 2009
Cheung 2009
Cheung 2009
To cite this article: Derek Cheung (2009) Developing a Scale to Measure Students’ Attitudes
toward Chemistry Lessons, International Journal of Science Education, 31:16, 2185-2203, DOI:
10.1080/09500690802189799
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or
arising out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
International Journal of Science Education
Vol. 31, No. 16, 1 November 2009, pp. 2185–2203
RESEARCH REPORT
Derek Cheung*
The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
[email protected]
00
Prof.
000002008
DerekCheung
International
10.1080/09500690802189799
TSED_A_319146.sgm
0950-0693
Original
Taylor
2008 and
& Article
Francis
(print)/1464-5289
Francis
Journal of Science
(online)
Education
Students’ attitudes toward chemistry lessons in school are important dependent variables in curri-
culum evaluation. Although a variety of instruments have been developed by researchers to evalu-
ate student attitudes, they are plagued with problems such as the lack of theoretical rationale and
of empirical evidence to support the construct validity of data. This paper describes a study of
students’ attitudes toward chemistry lessons in Hong Kong secondary schools. One of the scales in
the Test of Science-Related Attitudes developed by Fraser was modified to form an Attitude
Toward Chemistry Lessons Scale (ATCLS). The construction of the ATCLS was based on a
theoretical model with four dimensions: liking for chemistry theory lessons, liking for chemistry
laboratory work, evaluative beliefs about school chemistry, and behavioural tendencies to learn
chemistry. The arguments for inclusion of these four dimensions are presented. The final version
of ATCLS was administered to 954 students. The results of confirmatory factor analysis indicated
that there was a good fit between the hypothesised model and the observed data.
Introduction
There is considerable agreement among science educators on the importance of
students’ attitudes toward science lessons in school (Osborne, Simon, & Collins,
2003). However, there is much less agreement about how to measure them. In Hong
Kong, secondary school chemistry teachers are encouraged to collect their students’
attitudes toward chemistry lessons as part of the information for curriculum evalua-
tion. Unfortunately, an extensive review of the literature on attitude research indi-
cated that there are no instruments that can provide valid and reliable data on
students’ attitudes toward chemistry lessons.
*Department of Curriculum & Instruction, Ho Tim Building, The Chinese University of Hong
Kong, Shatin, Hong Kong. Email: [email protected]
Interest in Science, and Career Interest in Science. The TOSRA has been widely
used in science education research (e.g., Joyce & Farenga, 1999; Lang, Wong, &
Fraser, 2005; Smist, Archambault, & Owen, 1994; White & Richardson, 1993).
Student attitudes are important dependent variables in the evaluation of science
curricula (Bennett, Lubben, & Hogarth, 2007; Fraser, 1979; Gardner, 1975a).
According to Fraser (1977), selection of attitude scales for curriculum evaluation
should consider three criteria: educational importance, multidimensionality, and
economy of time for administration. However, the multidimensionality of the
TOSRA items has not been confirmed by empirical research. For example, Schibeci
and McGaw (1981) administered the TOSRA to 1,041 Australian students in
Grades 8–10. Confirmatory factor analysis of student responses to the 70 items indi-
cated that the student data did not fit the seven TOSRA scales. Khalili (1987)
surveyed 336 American students in Grades 11 and 12, and reported that the seven
TOSRA scales were highly reliable, but the results of the principal component factor
analysis did not support the distinctiveness of the seven scales. Smist et al. (1994)
administered the TOSRA to 572 American high school students. They tested the
multidimensionality of student data with exploratory factor analysis but failed to find
seven distinctive factors among the 70 items.
Tapia and Marsh emphasised that ‘Attitude scales must withstand factor analysis,
tap important dimensions of attitudes, and require a minimum amount of time for
administration’ (2004, p. 17). Also, researchers such as Breckler (1984), Krosnick,
Judd, and Wittenbrink (2005), and Munby (1997) have emphasised the importance
of a confirmatory approach to testing the construct validity of attitudinal data, which
can be accomplished with confirmatory factor analysis (Byrne, 1998). A systematic
examination of the construct validity of attitudinal data is most critical because
construct validity subsumes content relevance, content representativeness, and crite-
rion-relatedness, and is the evidential basis of interpretation of data as well as the use
of data (Messick, 1989). Unfortunately, most science educators have used an
exploratory approach rather than a confirmatory approach to validation in their atti-
tude studies (see, e.g., Berg, 2005; Dalgety, Coll, & Jones, 2003; Kind, Jones, &
Barmby, 2007; Orion & Hofstein, 1991; Parkinson, Hendley, Tanner, & Stables,
1998; Salta & Tzougraki, 2004). In fact, the validity of many attitude instruments is
so notorious that science researchers do not trust the quantitative data generated by
these instruments (see, e.g., Henderleiter & Pringle, 1999). Therefore, a need to
look at this area of science education research is very pressing.
Students’ Attitudes toward Chemistry Lessons 2187
that were not indicative of attitude (Bennett et al., 2007). If an attitude scale
contains an ill-defined hodge-podge of different items, and student responses are
summarised as an average of these items, then we do not know what is being
measured. About 10 years ago, Ramsden already made the following recommenda-
tions for improving attitude research in science education:
Firstly, the different dimensions, or constructs, of attitude need to be carefully defined
and separated out, and secondly, there may be a need to consult appropriate psycholog-
ical theory on attitude development if an intention is to use the findings to inform teach-
ing or curriculum planning. (1998, p. 129)
the attitude object. The cognitive component is the beliefs that one holds about the
attitude object. The tri-component viewpoint of attitude was popular in the 1960s,
but its weaknesses have been identified by researchers. For example, research has
indicated that some individuals base their attitudes predominantly on their feelings,
whereas others base their attitudes mainly on beliefs (Huskinson & Haddock, 2004).
The second theoretical viewpoint about the nature of attitudes assumes that the
three components mentioned above are distinct, separate entities. The term ‘attitude’
is reserved for the affective component only. Cognition and behaviour are treated as
determinants rather than constituents of an attitude. This viewpoint has been advo-
Downloaded by [University of Colorado at Boulder Libraries] at 08:28 05 January 2015
cated by researchers such as Thurstone (1931), Bem (1970), and Fishbein and Aizen
(1975). They have conceptualised attitude as the amount of affect for or against an
object. However, some researchers (e.g., Oskamp & Schultz, 2005; Zanna & Rempel,
1988) doubt this simple theoretical conceptualisation of attitudes. Fishbein and
Aizen (1975) themselves also admitted that this viewpoint does not capture the full
complexity of the attitude concept.
The third theoretical viewpoint conceptualises attitudes as a latent variable that
can help to explain the relationship between certain observable stimulus events and
behaviours. Contemporary attitude researchers generally agree that attitudes can be
formed from cognitive, affective, and/or behavioural information about the attitude
objects and expressed through cognitive, affective, and/or behavioural responses
(Eagly & Chaiken, 2005; Fabrigar, MacDonald, & Wegener, 2005; Oskamp &
Schultz, 2005). They postulate that the stimulus events will trigger some latent
cognitive, affective, or behavioural processes within the individual, and an attitude is
a general evaluative summary of the information derived from these hidden
processes. As an internal state, attitude is not directly observable; its existence can
only be inferred from observable responses. Although the observable responses may
be shown as cognitive, affective, or behavioural responses, not all attitudes can
produce the three types of responses. In other words, the responses generated by an
attitude can be unidimensional or multidimensional. Affective responses may be
measured by collecting self-reports of feelings. Cognitive responses may be collected
through written or verbal statements of beliefs about the attitude object. Behavioural
responses include overt actions and self-report of behavioural intentions concerning
an attitude object. Thus, attitudes can have varied antecedents on the input side and
varied responses on the output side. According to Oskamp and Schultz (2005), the
latent process viewpoint is better than the tri-component viewpoint or the separate
entities viewpoint because it is more consistent with findings of contemporary atti-
tude research. In the present study, I used Oskamp and Schultz’s (2005) definition
of attitude (i.e., an attitude is a predisposition to respond in a favourable or unfavour-
able manner with respect to a given attitude object) and measured student attitudes
toward chemistry lessons based on the latent process viewpoint (see Figure 1).
According to Gardner (1975a), attitudes in science education can be divided into
Figure 1. Latent process viewpoint
two broad categories: attitudes toward science (e.g., attitude toward social responsi-
bility in science), and scientific attitudes (e.g., open-mindedness). The first category
of attitudes must involve some attitude objects whereas the second category refers to
Students’ Attitudes toward Chemistry Lessons 2189
Affective
school processes lessons in
school Evaluative beliefs about
Behavioral school chemistry
processes
Behavioural tendencies to
learn chemistry
the qualities possessed by scientists. The present study belongs to the first category
of attitudes, and the attitude object is ‘chemistry lessons’ rather than such objects as
chemists. The term ‘lessons’ refers to theory classes and laboratory classes in
secondary school. I included a total of four dimensions in my theoretical model
(Figure 1) to serve as bases to guide the construction of the ATCLS items. The
rationale for inclusion of these dimensions is summarised below.
may infer that students have a positive attitude to chemistry theory lessons if they
like the lessons and agree that the lessons are interesting.
of the attitude construct when he surveyed high school students in Israel. Two of the
10 items (Table 1) in the Enjoyment of Science Lessons scale in the TOSRA
measure students’ evaluative beliefs about science lessons: ‘School should have more
science lessons each week’ and ‘Science lessons are a waste of time’.
Methodology
Collection of Information to develop the ATCLS
Two types of instruments have been commonly used in attitude research in
science education: semantic differential scales (e.g., Nyberg & Clarke, 1983; Reid &
Skryabina, 2002), and Likert scales (e.g., Dhindsa & Chung, 1999; Jenkins & Nelson,
2005; Menis, 1983; Pell & Jarvis, 2001). Schibeci (1982) suggested that high school
students’ general attitudes to science can be measured with the semantic differential
technique; but if more specific attitudes are to be measured, Likert scales are more
appropriate. Simpson and Oliver (1990) also found that Likert-type items had
produced the highest reliability when several formats were tested. Therefore, I decided
2192 D. Cheung
to keep the Likert-type format used by Fraser (1981) when I modified his Enjoyment
of Science Lessons scale.
In Hong Kong, secondary schooling consists of seven years (Secondary 1–7),
and academic year in secondary schools begins in September. Chemistry is offered
as a separate discipline to Secondary 4–7 students (aged about 16–19 years). To
begin the process of developing a scale to measure students’ attitudes toward
chemistry lessons implemented in secondary school, I started with semi-structured
interviews with 10 Secondary 4–7 students randomly selected from 10 chemistry
classes in two secondary schools in Hong Kong. The purpose of the interviews was
Downloaded by [University of Colorado at Boulder Libraries] at 08:28 05 January 2015
to solicit student input (Koballa & Glynn, 2007) so that an item pool was gener-
ated on the basis of views expressed by chemistry students. The interviews were
conducted in October 2005. The students were interviewed individually right after
school for about 15 minutes and asked to respond to the following three open-
ended questions:
(1) When compared with other subjects that you have studied in this school, do you
like chemistry lessons? Why?
(2) Do you believe that chemistry is an important subject in the school curriculum?
Why?
(3) Do you intend to learn more chemistry? Why?
Semi-structured interviews were used to allow me to ask students to clarify their
ideas and to pose follow-up questions. The interviews were conducted in Chinese
and audio-taped. The interview data were transcribed in full and were content-
analysed and categorised into the four dimensions conceptualised in the present
study.
In addition to interviews, an extensive review of the literature on students’ atti-
tudes toward school chemistry or science was also conducted to identify instruments
used in past studies. Items that had previously been used to measure students’ atti-
tudes and were consistent with at least one of the four dimensions in my theoretical
framework were selected for examination (e.g., Dhindsa & Chung, 1999; Fraser,
1981; Hofstein, Ben-Zvi, & Samuel, 1976). The information collected from the
literature review and student interviews was synthesised and used to construct items
for the four dimensions.
reduce the effects of acquiescence and other response biases. However, research has
indicated that negative items, written as reversals of positive items, may load on a
separate factor, forming a measurement artefact (Miller & Cleary, 1993; Pilotte &
Gable, 1990; Schmitt & Stults, 1985). Therefore, negatively worded items were not
included in the ATCLS.
The trial version of ATCLS was pilot tested on a convenience sample of 777
Secondary 4–7 chemistry students in December 2005, close to the middle of the
academic year in Hong Kong. The 20 items were randomly distributed in the
ATCLS. Students responded to the items on a seven-point rating scale with labels
Downloaded by [University of Colorado at Boulder Libraries] at 08:28 05 January 2015
strongly disagree, moderately disagree, slightly disagree, not sure, slightly agree, moderately
agree, and strongly agree. A study by Alwin and Krosnick (1991) found that fully
labelled seven-point attitude scales can generate the most reliable data. The goal of
the pilot study was to conduct an item analysis to eliminate any ambiguous or non-
discriminating items. Student responses to the 20 items were first coded on a scale of
one to seven. Using the SPSS program, the reliabilities of student responses to indi-
vidual items and to the four subscales were then examined on the basis of item–total
correlations and Cronbach alpha values, respectively. Additionally, five chemistry
students were invited to respond verbally to the items. They were asked to identify
any ambiguous items and underline those words lacking clarity. These students
found that the items were readable and unambiguous.
errors of measurement associated with all items were posited to be uncorrelated. The
confirmatory factor analysis was performed by the LISREL program using maximum
likelihood estimates derived from a covariance matrix based on listwise deletion for
missing data (Byrne, 1998). The ability of the hypothesised four-dimension model
to fit student data was judged by the values of overall model fit indices such as the
goodness of fit index and comparative fit index.
Student Interviews
The data from the student interviews indicated that traditional instruction—lots of
teacher talk and practice in solving sample problems at the board—was popular in
most of the Secondary 4–7 classes. Seven of the 10 students interviewed mentioned
that they did not like chemistry lessons. Much of the dissatisfaction arose because
they felt that their teachers had relied on rote-learning in chemistry classrooms. This
feeling was reinforced if their teachers seldom organised laboratory work to facilitate
them to learn chemistry. Few of the students had first-hand experience of conduct-
ing their own scientific investigations. They reported that their teachers drilled them
in preparation for public examinations. The following excerpts demonstrate what
made students dislike chemistry lessons.
I hate chemistry. We have four chemistry periods per week. But they are all boring stuff
… Chalk-and-talk … We have had few opportunities to carry out experiments in the
lab. (Student 1, male, Secondary 4, School A)
My chemistry teacher seldom asks questions in class. We are afraid to speak up because
my teacher may scold us for raising silly questions. Chemistry lessons are much more
boring than biology lessons. (Student 2, female, Secondary 4, School A)
I don’t like chemistry. My teacher just gives notes. She expects us to memorise every-
thing … I don’t understand why the mole concept is so important to chemists. We have
started to do past exam papers. (Student 6, male, Secondary 4, School B)
We perform laboratory work every week because we have school-based assessment of
practical skills as a component of the public exam. Practical marks are submitted to the
Exam Authority. But very often the chemistry experiments are not related to the topic
we are learning. (Student 4, male, Secondary 6, School A)
Proper use of teaching aids can help students to learn chemistry by making the
concepts more visual and memorable. However, only three of the 10 students talked
about how their chemistry teachers motivated them to learn using a variety of teach-
ing aids and learning activities. These three students also felt very positive about
scientific inquiry.
Chemistry is one of my favourite subjects. I like doing chemistry experiments. Last year,
we were asked to compare the effectiveness of three different brands of toothpaste—
Crest, Colgate, and Salz. It’s a group project. We were required to design the proce-
dures by ourselves. Sometimes, we didn’t know what steps to take next. It was a chal-
lenge because it was harder than following a given procedure. I think that’s a very good
Students’ Attitudes toward Chemistry Lessons 2195
opportunity for me to apply what I had learned acid-base chemistry. (Student 8, female,
Secondary 5, School B)
Sometimes, chemistry lessons were interesting. We sang an exciting song about
Mendeleev when we learned about the periodic table … I liked watching demonstration
experiments. My teacher added potassium to water. We all saw the beautiful lilac flame.
My teacher also showed us a video. I forgot the name of that metal but my teacher told
us that it is more reactive than potassium. The video showed that the vigorous reaction
broke the trough. It’s really wonderful. (Student 7, female, Secondary 4, School B)
A few weeks ago, we completed a project in organic chemistry. We had to determine
Downloaded by [University of Colorado at Boulder Libraries] at 08:28 05 January 2015
the amount of ethanol in a bottle of red wine. I had fun designing the experiment. And,
I learned that there is often more than one way to solve a chemistry problem. (Student
10, male, Secondary 7, School B)
One of the tensions about the school chemistry curriculum is the extent to which
its contents should be relevant to student interests and the modern world. Five of
the 10 students interviewed believed that chemistry is an important subject in the
school curriculum if the contents are related to their daily lives. In other words, the
learning of chemistry should be personally meaningful to them. Two students also
thought that chemistry lessons are important if they want to pursue science-related
degree programmes at university.
Chemistry is useful if the topics are connected with our daily life. I remember that my
teacher explained why the pH of Coca-Cola is about 3 last year. I didn’t know that
Coca-Cola contains an acid called phosphoric acid which can reduce the amount
of calcium ions in our blood. But Sprite and 7-Up do not contain phosphoric acid. Now
I don’t drink Coca-Cola. (Student 3, female, Secondary 5, School A)
I think some chemistry concepts are useful for people to solve everyday problems. For
example, my teacher told us that in the 1960s, thousands of babies were born without
arms and legs in Europe. It’s due to a drug called Thalidomide. Now pharmacists
understand that Thalidomide is a chiral drug. One of the enantiomers has serious side-
effects. So, we should understand chemistry because it affects human lives. (Student
5, female, Secondary 7, School A)
I would like to study medicine when I leave school. Chemistry is one of the most impor-
tant subjects for me to study in school. I hope I’d get a grade A in the public exam.
(Student 9, male, Secondary 6, School B)
I would like to be a scientist or science teacher. I’ll choose chemistry as one of my elec-
tives at university. I think working in a chemistry lab would be an interesting way to
earn a living, particularly working in a forensic lab to help with police investigations.
I already told my teacher that I would like to do a project in chemistry after the school
exam. (Student 5, female, Secondary 7, School A)
In Hong Kong, I’m afraid that it’s difficult to get a job with a chemistry degree. We
don’t have large pharmaceutical companies in Hong Kong. We also don’t have any oil
refinery. (Student 10, male, Secondary 7, School B)
I’m not going to select chemistry in Secondary 6, because I want to obtain a BBA
Downloaded by [University of Colorado at Boulder Libraries] at 08:28 05 January 2015
Overall, the present findings generally confirm those of earlier studies in showing
that chemistry lessons are not particularly attractive to students across all grade
levels. For example, in a qualitative study involving 144 16-year-old students in the
UK, Osborne and Collins were surprised to find that ‘the subject that attracted the
most vehement expression for its lack of relevance and appeal was chemistry’ (2001,
pp. 448–449). Rop (1999) also criticised the chemical education in the USA. He
argued that the school chemistry is not good enough because it does not help
students understand what happens in daily things and fails to stimulate their desire
to learn. He pointed out that educational researchers are ‘good at saying that
subjects such as chemistry are important in themselves and important components
of a liberal education’ but they are not very good at ‘forming or articulating honest
reasons or incentives for learning difficult things such as real chemistry—those
reasons which will convince students who are understandably sceptical of the use
value of what they learn’ (Rop, 1999, p. 233).
The interview data were categorised on the basis of the four dimensions. Owing to
limitation of space I cannot describe the findings in detail here, but Table 2 demon-
strates how the interview data were categorised. After synthesising the information
collected from student interviews and literature review, the item pool consisted of
32 items—and 20 items were selected to form the trial version of the ATCLS.
1. Liking for chemistry theory Chemistry lessons are much more boring than biology
lessons lessons.
Chemistry is one of my favourite subjects.
Chemistry lessons are interesting.
2. Liking for chemistry laboratory I like doing chemistry experiments.
work I had fun designing chemistry experiments.
3. Evaluative beliefs about school Some chemistry concepts are useful for people to solve
Downloaded by [University of Colorado at Boulder Libraries] at 08:28 05 January 2015
only three items were retained on each dimension in the final version of the ATSCS.
Further research is planned to increase the number of items per subscale. As shown
in Table 3, all of the 12 item–total correlations were moderately positive, giving
support for the reliability of student data.
Item–total
Subscale and item correlation
1 .83
2 .74
4 .91
6 .84
Downloaded by [University of Colorado at Boulder Libraries] at 08:28 05 January 2015
7 .68
10 .88
3 .67
8 .75
11 .77
5 .71
9 .82
12 .64
Note. All factor loadings not shown in the table were set to zero. Effective sample size = 930.
Results of the confirmatory factor analysis are presented in Table 4. 24 out of the
954 students had missing data and were deleted by the listwise method. The stan-
dardised factor loadings were all most reasonable and statistically significant (t
values varied between 17.75 and 31.64). There were 48 possible factor loadings
(i.e., 12 items × 4 factors), but 36 of them were fixed at zero. Each of the 12 items
was retained in exactly the same subscale to which it had been assigned when the
ATCLS was developed. Fit indices generated by the LISREL program showed that
the model fitted the data well (e.g., χ2(48, N = 930) = 288.72, goodness of fit index
= .95, adjusted goodness of fit index = .92, normed fit index = .95, comparative fit
index = .96, root mean square error of approximation = .073). Because the chi-
square statistic is sensitive to sample size, I based evaluation of goodness-of-fit on
considerations of multiple measures of fit and beyond the statistical significance of
the chi-square. For example, the goodness of fit index has a maximum of 1.0 that
implies perfect fit, and a value close to .95 indicates a good model fit (Byrne, 1998).
Hence, the final version of the ATCLS not only managed to produce reliable data,
but also valid information about the multidimensionality of data.
Social psychologists have found that consistency may vary across the three types of
attitudinal responses (i.e., affective, cognitive, and behavioural), ranging from highly
consistent to highly inconsistent (Eagly & Chaiken, 1998). Table 5 presents the
correlations generated by the confirmatory factor analysis of student data collected
by the final version of the ATCLS. All the correlations were positive and consider-
able, indicating that student responses to the four subscales were fairly consistent,
and thus no attitudinal ambivalence (Fabrigar et al., 2005) occurred. The correla-
tion between the scores on the first and fourth dimensions was the highest (.81).
This indicates that those students who liked chemistry theory lessons tended to
Students’ Attitudes toward Chemistry Lessons 2199
Subscale 1 2 3 4
have a mental state of readiness to study more chemistry, and vice versa. This finding
is consistent with previous studies. For example, Crawley and Koballa (1992)
measured Hispanic-American students’ attitudes toward signing-up to take a chem-
istry course in school using four, seven-point, bipolar, evaluative, adjective pairs.
They found that students who intended to sign-up to take chemistry held favourable
attitudes toward chemistry enrolment. As can be seen in Table 5, the correlation
between the scores on the first and second dimensions was the smallest (.56). Such a
relatively small correlation was not unexpected because the teaching effectiveness of
chemistry teachers in a classroom may be different from that in a laboratory session.
A student may like chemistry laboratory work but hates theory lessons. Thus, this
finding provided support for constructing two subscales to separate students’ attitu-
dinal responses to laboratory work from those to theory lessons.
Clearly, the above findings indicate that the scores on the four subscales of the
ATCLS should not be combined to form a single total because comparisons of the
attitudes of subgroups of students may show larger differences on certain dimen-
sions than on others. When the subscale scores were subjected to two-way multivari-
ate analysis of variance, the interaction effect between grade level and gender on
students’ attitudes toward chemistry lessons was statistically significant. The interac-
tion effect was attributable to scores on the theory lessons subscale and laboratory
work subscale. Male students in Secondary 4 and Secondary 5 liked chemistry
theory lessons more than their female counterparts. However, male students’ liking
for chemistry laboratory work declined when they progressed from Secondary 4 to
Secondary 7; no such a significant decline in attitude toward chemistry laboratory
work was found in females. Readers may refer to Cheung (in press) for details.
Conclusions
The development of positive attitudes regarding chemistry as a school subject is one
of the major responsibilities of chemistry teachers. In the present study, the 10-item
Enjoyment of Science Lessons scale in Fraser’s (1981) TOSRA was modified to
form a 12-item ATCLS. According to Fraser (1977), selection of attitude scales for
curriculum evaluation should consider three criteria: educational importance, multi-
dimensionality, and economy of time for administration. The ATCLS has met all
these criteria. The study described in the present paper differs from previous work in
that the development of the ATCLS started with a theoretical framework with four
2200 D. Cheung
References
Adesoji, F. A., & Raimi, S. M. (2004). Effects of enhanced laboratory instructional technique on
senior secondary students’ attitude toward chemistry in Oyo Township, Oyo State, Nigeria.
Journal of Science Education and Technology, 13(3), 377–385.
Alwin, D. F., & Krosnick, J. A. (1991). The reliability of survey attitude measurement: The
influence of question and respondent attributes. Sociological Methods and Research, 20(1),
139–181.
Bem, D. J. (1970). Beliefs, attitudes, and human affairs. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Bennett, J., Lubben, F., & Hogarth, S. (2007). Bringing science to life: A synthesis of the research
evidence on the effects of context-based and STS approaches to science teaching. Science
Education, 91(3), 347–370.
Bennett, J., Rollnick, M., Green, G., & White, M. (2001). The development and use of an instru-
ment to assess students’ attitude to the study of chemistry. International Journal of Science
Education, 23(8), 833–845.
Berg, C. A. R. (2005). Factors related to observed attitude change toward learning chemistry
among university students. Chemistry Education Research and Practice, 6(1), 1–18.
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.
Breckler, S. J. (1984). Empirical validation of affect, behavior, and cognition as distinct compo-
nents of attitude. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(6), 1191–1205.
Byrne, B. M. (1998). Structural equation modeling with LISREL, PRELIS, and SIMPLIS: Basic
concepts, applications, and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cheung, D. (in press). Students’ attitudes toward chemistry lessons: The interaction effect
between grade level and gender. Research in Science Education.
Crawley, F. E., & Koballa, T. R., Jr. (1992). Hispanic-American students’ attitudes toward enroll-
ing in high school chemistry: A study of planned behavior and belief-based change. Hispanic
Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 14(4), 469–486.
Students’ Attitudes toward Chemistry Lessons 2201
Dalgety, J., Coll, R. K., & Jones, A. (2003). Development of chemistry attitudes and experience
questionnaire (CAEQ). Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 40(7), 649–668.
Dhindsa, H. S., & Chung, G. (1999). Motivation, anxiety, enjoyment and values associated with
chemistry learning among form 5 Bruneian students. Paper presented at the MERA-ERA Joint
Conference, Malacca, Malaysia.
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace
Jovanovich College Publishers.
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1998). Attitude structure and function. In D.T. Gilbert, S.T. Fiske,
& G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., vol. 1, pp. 269–322). Boston:
McGraw-Hill.
Downloaded by [University of Colorado at Boulder Libraries] at 08:28 05 January 2015
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (2005). Attitude research in the 21st century: The current state of
knowledge. In D. Albarracin, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes
(pp. 743–767). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Fabrigar, L. R., MacDonald, T. K., & Wegener, D. T. (2005). The structure of attitudes. In D.
Albarracin, B. T. Johnson, & M.P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. 79–124).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Fishbein, M., & Aizen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory
and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Fraser, B. J. (1977). Selection and validation of attitude scales for curriculum evaluation. Science
Education, 61(3), 317–329.
Fraser, B. J. (1978). Development of a test of science-related attitudes. Science Education, 62(4),
509–515.
Fraser, B. J. (1979). Evaluation of a science-based curriculum. In H. J. Walberg (Ed.), Educational
environments and effects: Evaluation, policy, and productivity (pp. 218–234). Berkeley, CA:
McCutchan.
Fraser, B. J. (1981). Test of science-related attitudes. Camberwell, Victoria, Australia: Australian
Council for Educational Research.
Gardner, P. L. (1975a). Attitudes to science: A review. Studies in Science Education, 2, 1–41.
Gardner, P. L. (1975b). Attitude measurement: a critique of some recent research. Educational
Research, 17(2), 101–109.
Hassan, A. M. A., & Shrigley, R. L. (1984). Designing a Likert scale to measure chemistry atti-
tudes. School Science and Mathematics, 84(8), 659–669.
Henderleiter, J., & Pringle, D. L. (1999). Effects of context-based laboratory experiments on atti-
tudes of analytical chemistry students. Journal of Chemical Education, 76(1), 100–106.
Hofstein, A., Ben-Zvi, R., & Samuel, D. (1976). The measurement of the interest in, and attitudes
to, laboratory work amongst Israeli high school chemistry students. Science Education, 60(3),
401–411.
Hofstein, A., Ben-Zvi, R., Samuel, D., & Tamir, P. (1977). Attitudes of Israeli high-school students
toward chemistry and physics: A comparative study. Science Education, 61(2), 259–268.
Huskinson, T. L. H., & Haddock, G. (2004). Individual differences in attitude structure: Variance
in the chronic reliance on affective and cognitive information. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 40, 82–90.
Jenkins, E. W. (2006). The student voice and school science education. Studies in Science Education,
42, 49–88.
Jenkins, E. W., & Nelson, N. W. (2005). Important but not for me: Students’ attitudes towards
secondary school science in England. Research in Science and Technological Education, 23(1),
41–57.
Joyce, B. A., & Farenga, S. J. (1999). Informal science experience, attitudes, future interest in
science, and gender of high-ability students: An exploratory study. School Science and
Mathematics, 99(8), 431–437.
Khalili, K. Y. (1987). A crosscultural validation of a test of science related attitudes. Journal of
Research in Science Teaching, 24(2), 127–136.
2202 D. Cheung
Kind, P., Jones, K., & Barmby, P. (2007). Developing attitudes towards science measures.
International Journal of Science Education, 29(7), 871–893.
Koballa, T. R., & Glynn, S. M. (2007). Attitudunal and motivational constructs in science
learning. In S. K. Abell & N. G. Lederman (Eds.), Handbook of research on science education
(pp. 75–102). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Krosnick, J. A., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2005). The measurement of attitudes. In D.
Albarracin, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. 21–76).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Lang, Q. C., Wong, A. F. L., & Fraser, B. J. (2005). Student perceptions of chemistry laboratory
learning environments, student–teacher interactions and attitudes in secondary school gifted
Downloaded by [University of Colorado at Boulder Libraries] at 08:28 05 January 2015
Rop, C. J. (1999). Student perspectives on success in high school chemistry. Journal of Research in
Science Teaching, 36(2), 221–237.
Salta, K., & Tzougraki, C. (2004). Attitudes toward chemistry among 11th grade students in high
schools in Greece. Science Education, 88, 535–547.
Schibeci, R. A. (1982). Measuring student attitudes: Semantic differential or Likert instruments?
Science Education, 66(4), 565–570.
Schibeci, R. A., & McGaw, B. (1981). Empirical validation of the conceptual structure of a test of
science-related attitudes. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 41(4), 1195–1201.
Schmitt, N., & Stults, D. M. (1985). Factors defined by negatively keyed items: The result of care-
less respondents? Applied Psychological Measurement, 9(4), 367–373.
Downloaded by [University of Colorado at Boulder Libraries] at 08:28 05 January 2015
Simpson, R. D., & Oliver, J. S. (1990). A summary of major influences on attitude toward and
achievement in science among adolescent students. Science Education, 74(1), 1–18.
Smist, J. M., Archambault, F. X., & Owen, S. V. (1994). Gender differences in attitude toward science.
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association,
New Orleans, LA.
Tapia, M., & Marsh, G. E. (2004). An instrument to measure mathematics attitudes. Academic
Exchange Quarterly, 8(2), 16–21.
Thurstone, L. L. (1931). The measurement of social attitudes. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 26, 249–269.
White, J. A. R., & Richardson, G.D. (1993). Comparison of science attitudes among middle and junior
high school students. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Mid South Educational
Research Association, New Orleans, LA.
Zanna, M. P., & Rempel, J. K. (1988). Attitudes: A new look at an old concept. In D. Bar-Tal &
A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), The social psychology of knowledge (pp. 315–334). New York:
Cambridge University Press.