The Formulation of A RBANS Effort Supplement
The Formulation of A RBANS Effort Supplement
The Formulation of A RBANS Effort Supplement
TheScholarsRepository@LLU: Digital
Archive of Research, Scholarship &
Creative Works
9-2019
Recommended Citation
Goldberg, Joshua Seth, "The Formulation of a RBANS Effort Supplement" (2019). Loma Linda University
Electronic Theses, Dissertations & Projects. 1883.
https://scholarsrepository.llu.edu/etd/1883
This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by TheScholarsRepository@LLU: Digital Archive of
Research, Scholarship & Creative Works. It has been accepted for inclusion in Loma Linda University Electronic
Theses, Dissertations & Projects by an authorized administrator of TheScholarsRepository@LLU: Digital Archive of
Research, Scholarship & Creative Works. For more information, please contact [email protected].
LOMA LINDA UNIVERSITY
School of Psychology
in conjunction with the
Faculty of Graduate Studies
____________________
by
____________________
____________________
September 2019
© 2019
Chai erson
lll
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I would like to express my deepest gratitude to Dr. Grace Lee for continuously
beyond. Your mentorship is truly unparalleled and it has been one of my life’s great joys
to have learned from you and to have worked with you so closely throughout my
I would also like to thank Dr. Travis Fogel for introducing me to the world of
how thankful I am for your guidance and your willingness to share this project’s vision
with me.
This project also would not have been possible without Dr. Kyrstle Barrera and
Dr. Kenny Boyd. Thank you both for the time and effort you dedicated to this study. I
most assuredly could not have completed this project without your advice and direction.
This project also would not have been possible without my family and friends.
Your support propelled me along my graduate career. I extend a special thank you to my
parents, Maureen and Harry, for their unconditional love, generosity, and encouragement.
And of course, I am eternally grateful for my fiancé, Sarah Barney; thank you for always
believing in me.
iv
CONTENT
Acknowledgements ............................................................................................................ iv
Chapter
1. Introduction ..............................................................................................................1
3. Methods..................................................................................................................17
Immediate Memory..............................................................................20
v
Delayed Memory .................................................................................22
Language ..............................................................................................23
Attention ..............................................................................................24
4. Results ....................................................................................................................29
5. Discussion ..............................................................................................................46
vi
References ..........................................................................................................................52
Appendices
vii
FIGURES
Figure Page
Chapter 4
viii
TABLES
Tables Page
ix
ABBREVIATIONS
L Lie
F Infrequency
Fb F Back
K Correction
F-K F Minus K
S Superlative Self-Presentation
Fp Infrequency/Psychopathology
Memory
Neuropsychological Status
x
MCI Mild Cognitive Impairment
AD Alzheimer’s Disease
EI Effort Index
ES Effort Scale
PD Parkinson’s Disease
LD Learning Disorder
MS Multiple Sclerosis
KR-20 Kuder-Richardson 20
xi
ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION
by
evaluation to ensure results of testing are valid indicators of an individual’s true level of
administering longer testing batteries. Two effort measures are embedded in the
elevated false positive rates for classifying individuals with memory impairment as those
putting forth poor effort. These embedded measures rely on cut-off scores on digit span
and memory subtests. In contrast, this RBANS Effort Supplement (RES) utilizes several
multiple methods of effort detection; subtests in this measure included list learning
and a story recognition component utilized for face validity of memory assessment. Fifty-
conjunction with 14 poor effort simulators; each participant was administered the
RBANS, the RES, and the Dot Counting Test (DCT). Results supported the RES’
xii
reliability at the individual decision-making level. Validity analyses demonstrated that
the RES exhibited strong convergent validity with established effort detection measures
and that individuals putting for poor effort scored significantly lower on the RES than
individuals who put forth adequate effort, as delineated by the established DCT cutoff
score of 17. In summary, the RES was shown to be a valid indicator of effort detection.
Clinical implications of the RES include reduction of time and costs involved in
neuropsychological assessment.
xiii
CHAPTER ONE
INTRODUCTION
useful tool in diagnosing prominent neurological disorders. Often, patients are referred
changes within cognitive functioning become more apparent to either the patient and/or
their surrounding community. As part of the assessment, patients are asked to put forth
their best effort throughout the administration of cognitive testing so that valid data may
There are three prominent psychological occurrences that may explain the
somatoform disorder. The conscious need for a patient to assume a sick role is defined as
1
utilizing alternative phrasing rather than malingering, as the rationale for improper effort
during testing may not be definitively identifiable. Thus, researchers within the
neuropsychological field suggest using phrasing such as the mobilization of effort and
test investment when referring to possible cases of malingering (Carone, Iverson, &
Bush, 2010). Regardless, the predominant focus of this study was effort detection within
neuropsychological testing.
in 29% of personal injury cases, 30% of disability cases, 19% of criminal cases, 38.5% of
personal injury cases, and 8% of general medical cases involving symptom exaggeration.
settings and as such, there is a necessity for valid measures of poor effort to distinguish
between individuals who have genuine impairments and those whose symptoms may be
integral part of both forensic and clinical settings (Martin, Schroeder, & Odland, 2015)
measures in forensic type assessments (Slick, Tan, Strauss, & Hultsch, 2004).
confidence in definitively diagnosing poor effort occurs through multiple effort measures
2
The assessment of suboptimal effort can be achieved through several different
Personality assessments often utilize subscales that identify when subjects are
reporting symptomatology that is rare even among those with confirmed psychiatric
illnesses. Specifically, the MMPI-2 has developed particular scales for individuals
good (S), and overreporting of psychopathological symptoms (Fp). The validity scales for
the MMPI-2 and other self-report measures that simultaneously measure feigned
tests (PVT’s; Larrabee, 2012). A primary focus of this study was to formulate an
screening process.
Often, neuropsychologists utilize clinical judgment when incorporating a PVT’s into their
3
testing batteries (Bigler, 2012). However, the use of standalone PVT’s in forensic
Standalone measures are often lengthier than comparative PVT’s, but such time is
considered medically necessary given the risk of suboptimal effort in these clinical
presents with subjective cognitive abilities usually associated with mood concerns.
targeted analysis of an individual’s mood concerns and their association with their current
cognitive symptoms. General personality inventories with validity scales are also
encouraged when time is available to more fully grasp an individual’s response bias
modalities and formats. Primarily, effort is analyzed through either standalone measures
(TOMM; Tombaugh, 1997) and the Dot Counting Test (DCT; Boone, Lu, & Herzberg,
2002) are tests specifically designed to measure effort that can be utilized independently
without incorporating information present in any other neuropsychological test within the
4
An embedded memory measure is an analysis of effort utilizing data collected
embedded measures include Reliable Digit Span from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Scale- Fourth edition (RDS; WAIS-IV; Wechsler, 2008) and the California Verbal
& Ober, 2000). Effort measures of this nature typically utilize memory recognition
paradigm. Forced choice measures appear to be difficult but are in fact, easy tasks.
and then later asked to select each of the target pictures or words from two choices.
Participants who perform below chance levels (i.e., below 50%) are identified as
individuals who may been putting forth suboptimal effort. However, commercially
available neuropsychological effort measures typically do not rely on comparing the total
effort measures typically examine poor effort as falling in a range of scores that would be
recognition, such as Logical Memory II Recognition from the Wechsler Memory Scale-
Fourth Edition, motor skills (The B Test; Boone, Lu, & Herzberg, 2002), and perceptual
detection to create a more comprehensive approach. The purpose of this study was to
5
incorporate various methods of effort methodology into one brief but comprehensive
process. To fully examine the framework of the measure, it is necessary to discuss the
Forced-Choice Recognition
are presented, after which the original targets are presented together with a foil and the
subject is asked to choose which of the two items was presented previously. Forced
choice recognition of memory malingering typically assesses how the examinee performs
according to chance level (Grote & Hook, 2007). If an examinee performs below chance
levels (i.e., less than 50% accuracy), it is thought that the examinee must knowingly be
choosing the wrong answer, as an individual with no previous exposure to the original
stimuli would still be expected to perform at chance levels. Research has indicated there
(Root, Robbins, Chang, & Van Gorp, 2006). Clinically referred patients in
neuropsychology clinics routinely performed at near perfect levels within the forced-
suboptimal effort.
One forced-choice recognition test that is commonly utilized within the clinical
neuropsychological field is the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM). When given the
6
TOMM, subjects are presented a series of 50 pictures of objects. Immediately following
the initial presentation, the examinee is presented with each object along with a foil and
asked to pick the picture they saw previously. Subjects are corrected on incorrect
responses. Following the first trial, the subject is once again presented with the same
pictures, the subject is once again asked to pick the correct pictures from foils. After a
fifteen-minute delay, an optional retention trial can be administered where the subject is
once again administered the forced-choice paradigm between original images and foils,
but without being presented with the original stimuli. Results from TOMM research
studies have found that the test is considered relatively easy for individuals with
depression, chronic pain, and dementia. The TOMM is considered a good screener for
overall effort but is often criticized for being too easy and too long (Strauss, Sherman, &
Spreen, 2006). Like the majority of forced-choice measures, it is recommended that the
setting, the TOMM achieved high sensitivity of 90% when diagnosis of dementia was
ruled out. However, when accounting for dementia diagnoses, the TOMM misclassified
patients with dementia as putting forth suboptimal effort by over 70%, suggesting that the
measure may be overly sensitive for individuals with dementia (Teicher & Wagner,
2004). These contrasts findings suggesting that the TOMM, along with the CVLT-II
injury (Moore & Donders, 2004). Thus, this current study will aim to provide a globally
7
As alluded to previously, another forced-choice measure is an embedded measure
in the second edition of the California Verbal Learning Test. The CVLT-II is a verbal
memory test where subjects are presented with a list of 16 words and asked to recall them
over several trials at several different time points, both spontaneously and with category
cues (immediate free recall, immediate cued recall, short delay free recall, long delay free
recall, long delay cued recall, and yes/no recognition). Following the yes/no recognition
portion of the CVLT-II and a ten-minute delay thereafter, subjects can be given a forced-
similarly to the TOMM, in that it was suggested to be very sensitive (ranging from 81-
93%), and only moderately specific (32-60%). Furthermore, it was recommended that the
forced-choice component of the CVLT-II not be used for individuals suffering from frank
dementia (Root et al., 2006). Thus, caution should be taken when definitively diagnosing
poor effort as reflected by the CVLT-II forced-choice, especially in settings assessing for
cognitive impairment. This is inherently problematic considering that dementia cases are
mentioning that forced-choice measures should not be used in isolation to identify faulty
effort. Although researchers have stated that forced-choice measures are the most
individual’s effort as suboptimal. This study will attempt to create a globally specific and
8
sensitive effort measure utilizing both forced-choice and non-forced-choice paradigms to
Non-Forced-Choice Measures
there are some limitations that warrant utilization of additional measures. As discussed
(Boone & Lu, 2007). Additionally, some standalone forced-choice measures often require
lengthy durations to properly administer, and many can also be overly sensitive to
tasks after delayed recall (Axelrod, Fichtenberg, Millis, & Wertheimer, 2006). On the
2009) patients are presented with two stories that they are asked to recall immediately
and after a 20-30-minute delay. After the delay, patients are asked to recall the story from
memory and are then administered a series of yes/no questions designed to see if they can
recognize story details in this format. Literature has indicated that yes/no questions are
different from forced-choice recognition in that they present targets and foils one after
another, as opposed to forced-choice recognition measures that present targets and foils
9
Recognition is typically easier than spontaneous recall (McDougall, 1904;
Postman, 1963), as researchers have identified that recalling an item from memory
requires more memory storage than simply recognizing the item via prompt. Thus, many
patients (apart from those with severe dementia) who have difficulty recalling story
details during the delayed recall trials tend to perform better on the recognition trial,
when questions are posed in a yes/no format. In a study examining simulators acting as
test than both patients with mixed etiology and the healthy controls (Bouman, Hendriks,
Schmand, Kessels, & Aldenkamp, 2016). Such findings indicated that individuals who
Another prominent embedded measure is the Reliable Digit Span (RDS). RDS
was originally derived from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R;
Wechsler, 1981) Digit Span subtest, a measure of attention and working memory
(Greiffenstein, Baker, & Gola, 1994). Within this subtest, examinees are asked to repeat a
series of digits, initially forwards and then in backwards order until they provide incorrect
responses on both trials of any given length of digit sequence. RDS is calculated by
taking the sum of the length of the longest consecutive strings successfully repeated
forward and backward. RDS is utilized within neuropsychological effort testing because
it is based on the assumption that digit span appears to be a test on which brain-injured
patients may exhibit difficulty but in reality, it is relatively preserved among patients with
brain dysfunction including amnesia (Etherton, Bianchini, Greve, & Heinly, 2005).
10
Research has demonstrated RDS is moderately sensitive and specific to poor
effort in a forensic setting (Sensitivity = 63%, Specificity = 86%) and can distinguish
individuals who provide suboptimal effort from individuals with appropriate effort by
more than one pooled standard variation (Jasinski, Berry, Shandera, & Clark, 2011:
Larrabee & Berry, 2007). Thus, RDS seems to be an adequate measure of detecting poor
standalone non-forced-choice effort measure was an ideal choice for optimizing effort
forced-choice measures. A commonly used effort measure that is neither embedded nor
of the forced-choice variety is the Dot Counting Test (DCT; Boone, Lu, & Herzberg,
2002). On the DCT, patients are presented a series of cards with dots and are asked to
count the dots as quickly as possible without committing any errors. Cards one through
six contain dots disseminated randomly across the page, whereas cards 7-12 contain dots
that are organized in clusters. A composite score (E-score) is computed based on the
patient’s average time to complete cards 1-6, summed with the patient’s average time on
cards 7-12 and total number of errors. Patients who may attempt to feign impairments
often overestimate the difficulty of the DCT, and consequently take an inordinate amount
of time to complete each item and/or commit numerous counting errors (Strauss,
Sherman, & Spreen, 2006). The DCT has been shown to have moderate sensitivity (70%)
and high specificity (90%) within clinical settings and is highly correlated with simple
11
digit span (56% shared variance). However, like other effort measures, it is recommended
definitively confirm suspect effort (Heilbronner et al., 2009). Specifically, research has
indicated that failure on two effort measures likely suggests the presence of feigned
the likelihood of correctly identifying suspect effort, whereas chaining effort measures
with methodological overlap may inflate such probability (Grimes & Schulz, 2005). This
study similarly aimed to create an effort measure utilizing multiple methods of analyzing
The RBANS
Effort measures can often be integrated into initial consultations along with
patients as well as determine whether interpretations and future testing may be needed
after the initial consult. A commonly administered screening measure is the Repeatable
memory. One of the key utilities of the RBANS is that it is highly correlated with longer
12
neuropsychological assessments, such as the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale IV Full
Scale Intelligence Quotient (r = .75), but it only requires thirty minutes to complete
(Hartman, 2009). Research within the last decade has also revealed that among
commonly used dementia screening measures, total RBANS performance is one of the
better measures in predicting total brain volume (Paul et al., 2011). Despite the RBANS
with and without Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI; Duff, Hobson, Beglinger, &
O’Bryant, 2010), the RBANS does seem to be a valid diagnostic indicator of more
individuals with and without AD across all index scores (Duff, Clark, O’Bryant, Mold,
Schiffer, & Sutker, 2008). Specifically, Duff et al. analyzed areas under the curve (AUC)
performance on all RBANS indices within a 0 to 1 scale. High diagnostic accuracy was
language (AUC = 0.83), and attention (AUC = 0.81), and particularly high accuracy on
immediate memory (AUC = 0.96), delayed memory (AUC = 0.98), and total index score
(0.98).
However, the effort measures associated with the RBANS currently do not completely
13
cognitive screener has become more established, neuropsychological researchers have
attempted to develop embedded malingering measures within its framework. Two such
measures include the RBANS Effort Index (EI; Silverberg, Wertheimer, Fichtenberg,
2007) and the RBANS Effort Scale (ES; Novitski, Steele, Karantzoulis, & Randolph,
2012). The EI is calculated by combining the digit span subtest and list recognition scores
into weighted scores, based on the utility of digit span and recognition formats that have
been previously validated for symptom validity measurement. The ES utilizes the same
subtests as the EI but includes an additional adjustment based on free recall scores (ES =
List Recognition – (List Recall + Story Recall + Figure Recall) + Digit Span). Novitski et
al. (2012) formulated the ES in this manner in order to discriminate between memory
impairment and feigned impairment, as patients with true memory impairment are likely
to have extremely low free recall scores (close to zero) by the time recognition scores
begin to drop.
Despite the empirically validated research from which these measures were
constructed, research has demonstrated that their validity may be somewhat limited.
Research has illustrated that although the EI exhibits good specificity for simulated
malingerers with a false-positive rate of 19% or less at selected cutoffs, it has only
moderate sensitivity (66%), which risks the possibility of misdiagnosing malingerers with
the EI has been shown to have an elevated false-positive rate within populations of
individuals suffering from dementias (Novitski et al., 2012; Duff et al, 2011).
emphasis on subtracting free recall scores as an overall reflection of its focus on patients
14
with amnesia and has also reflected high false-positive rates as well (Crighton et al.,
2015). Thus, despite the presence of current embedded effort measures within the
RBANS, such measures have exhibited limitations in correctly categorizing good effort
and poor effort in dementia populations. There would appear to be a need for a more
In this study, a new measure, the RBANS Effort Supplement, was formulated and
assessed for reliability and validity to detect suboptimal effort through the sole usage of
the RBANS assessment. The formulation of the RES had several particular advantages. It
was designed to be a quick measure to administer, with the opportunity for cost-
efficiency in that a subsequent longer evaluation would not be needed if effort were
concerning how the aggregation of varying measures of effort provide a more definitive
finding of suboptimal effort (Larrabee, 2008). Thus, the primary aim of this study was to
establish the reliability and validity of the RBANS Effort Supplement (RES). It was
hypothesized that the RES would be specific and sensitive towards detecting suboptimal
population.
15
CHAPTER TWO
The primary aim of this study was to determine if the RBANS Effort Supplement
(RES) was a reliable and valid measure of effort. To measure the RES’ reliability, the
RES was assessed for internal consistency utilizing the Kuder-Richardson 20 method
(Kuder & Richardson, 1937). It was hypothesized that the RES would be internally
consistent. Following reliability analysis, the construct validity of the RES was examined.
Specifically, The RES was assessed for convergent validity utilizing partial correlations
controlling for age and years of education. It was hypothesized that the RES would
exhibit convergent validity with the RBANS Effort Index, RBANS Effort Scale, and the
Dot Counting Test. Further, we hypothesized that participants within the experimental
malingering sample would score significantly lower on the RES in comparison to clinical
groups.
An exploratory aim of this study was to examine the specificity and sensitivity of
the RES. Such analyses were conducted utilizing ROC curve analyses according to a RES
was hypothesized that the RES will be specific and sensitive in correctly classifying
16
CHAPTER THREE
METHODS
outpatient (CNO) and a comparative suboptimal effort group. The CNO group was
Loma Linda University Medical Center East Campus neuropsychology service. Our
suboptimal effort group was recruited from Loma Linda University and included 15
students from the graduate student population. All subjects fell within the age range of
20-89 and all spoke English fluently. One participant was excluded utilizing the outlier
labelling rule on the RES total score to help correct for the skewness of the data. As such
Participants involved in the CNO group were individuals who had been referred
for clinical neuropsychological services for various reasons, including mild cognitive
impairment, traumatic brain injury, stroke, epilepsy, ADHD, and varying mood disorders.
neuropsychological referral, they were asked to participate in the current study. Agreeing
participants completed the informed consent process and gave permission to use the
results of their clinical testing (i.e. RBANS, RES, Dot Counting Test) for the current
study. Participants then completed a brief additional structured interview asking for basic
17
Participants were administered the RBANS as part of their routine neuropsychological
Participants enrolled in the suboptimal effort group (SEG) were recruited from
Loma Linda University’s graduate population. Subjects were recruited from various
wide postings. Participants completed the informed consent process and a brief structured
(DenBoer & Hall, 2007), prompting them to approach the neuropsychological tests as if
they were trying to appear brain damaged in order to receive financial compensation in an
ongoing lawsuit:
You are about to take some cognitive tests that examine mental abilities such as attention, memory,
thinking and reasoning skills, and your ability to think quickly. While responding to the tests, please
pretend that you have experienced brain damage from a car accident involving a head-on collision.
You hit your head against the windshield and were knocked out for 15 minutes. Afterwards, you felt
‘‘dazed’’ so you were hospitalized overnight for observation. Because the driver of the other car is at
fault, you have decided to go to court to get money from the person responsible. During the next few
months following the accident, the negative effects from your head injury disappear. Your lawsuit has
not been settled yet, and your lawyer has told you that you may get more money if you look like you
are still suffering from brain damage. As you pretend to be this car accident victim, try to respond to
each test as a patient who is trying to appear brain damaged in order to get money from the lawsuit.
Thus, your performance on the tests should convince the examiner as well as the people involved in
deciding the outcome of your lawsuit that you are still suffering from brain damage.
Approval for the study was obtained from the Loma Linda University Human
Subjects Committee Institutional Review Board, and written informed consent was
acquired from all participants upon enrollment. It should be noted that Loma Linda
University associated legal counsel stated that the RBANS Effort Supplement was
considered legally permissible as long as primary investigators did not attempt to earn a
profit from the measure itself. The RES was only utilized for the purposes of this study.
18
Instruments
the participants were involved in previous litigation and three participants had engaged in
previous neuropsychological testing (one participant in 2016, another in 1985, and the
Boone, Lu, and Herzberg’s Dot Counting Test (2002) is a measure of symptom
validity and malingering. Participants are presented with a series of twelve dotted cards
and are asked to count the number dots as quickly as possible and relay to the examiner
the number of dots that they counted. On cards one through six, the dots on the cards are
disseminated in no organizational fashion. In cards seven through twelve, the dots on the
cards are grouped in such a way that it is easier to count the number of dots quickly. An
E-score is tabulated according to the participant’s response times and number of errors on
the test itself (lower E-scores reflect fewer errors and faster response times). Research has
identified that the DCT is an adequate measure of suspect effort, with moderate
encouraged that the DCT be utilized in conjunction with other measures when assessing
for symptom validity (Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2008). Previous research has
19
suggested a general cut-off score of >17 for classification of suboptimal effort (Boone et
al., 2002).
The RBANS
cognitive status of individuals suffering from neurological diseases or head trauma. One
of the core advantages to using the RBANS is its brevity. The RBANS takes
visuospatial ability, language, and attention) and twelve subtests (list learning, story
memory, figure copy, line orientation, digit span, symbol digit coding, picture naming,
semantic fluency, list recall, list recognition, story recall, and figure recall). All index
scores are comprised of two subtests except for the delayed memory domain, which
consists of four subtests. The RBANS total score provides an overall outcome statistic for
attention, and delayed memory can be calculated. All subtests are given a subtest raw
score. Raw scores of subtests within each domain are added and converted to an age-
corrected index score. Index scores can also be converted to percentile scores, according
20
Immediate Memory
recall a small amount of information directly after it has been presented. The immediate
List Learning
List Learning consists of a list of 10 unrelated words, read for immediate recall
over four trials, with a maximum score of 40. Words used in the List Learning task are
considered moderate-high imagery words with relatively low age of acquisition. The high
imagery levels and low age of acquisition of these words is considered helpful in
Story Memory
This subtest is comprised of a story with 12 itemized details; the story is read for
immediate recall over two trials, for a total maximum score of 24.
Visuospatial Ability
recreate spatial relations. Notably, this domain assesses participants’ ability to estimate
distance and depth and navigate the surrounding environment. The subtests used to
21
Figure Copy.
The Figure Copy subtest prompts participants to draw an exact copy of a complex
figure comprised of geometric shapes. The subtest itself is considered very similar yet
less complex to the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (Meyers & Meyers, 1995). The
Line Orientation
beginning at a common point of origin and fanning out across 180 degrees, which serves
as the reference figure. Each item consists of two target lines that are shown beneath the
reference figure. Subjects must correctly identify which two lines in the reference match
the two target lines. Line orientation consists of 10 items, each comprised of two target
Delayed Memory
information for an extended length of time. These subtests are presented to the
Free recall of the words from the List Learning subtest (max = 10).
22
List Learning Recognition
Yes/No recognition of the words from the List Learning subtest, with 10 foils
(max = 20).
Free recall of the story from the Story Memory subtest (max=12).
Free recall of the figure from the Figure Copy subtest (max = 20).
Language
verbally name and retrieve previously learned semantic information. Two subtests are
Picture Naming
Semantic Fluency
Participants are allotted one minute to provide as many examples from a semantic
23
Attention
The attention domain prompts the participant to manipulate previously presented material
(visual and oral) that has been stored within the individual’s short-term memory. This
Digit Span
Subjects are asked to repeat a series of numbers, with stimulus items increasing in
length from 2 digits to 9 digits. The items are presented in order of length (shortest to
longest), and the test itself is discontinued when the participant fails all trials within a
given string length. It should be noted that there is no digit span backwards on the
RBANS.
Coding
the Coding subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Subjects are asked to fill in
digits matching with corresponding shapes on a coding key as fast as they can. After
practice items are completed, participants have 90 seconds to complete as many items as
possible.
24
Total Scale
The Total Scale is the overall outcome statistic for an individual’s overall
neuropsychological functioning as comprised by the sum of all the index scores of the
and Language).
Naming, Figure Copy, and Coding. It should be noted that the RES has never been
utilized in previous research. The RES was constructed utilizing the stimuli in RBANS
form A, with all non-target stimuli for verbal and nonverbal information derived from
from the story that was read to them twice previously in the RBANS Story Memory
subtest (max = 12). This subtest was not included in the final RES Total score and was
the List Learning subtest. For each item, participants were prompted with two words, one
25
word from the original list and one novel word, and subsequently asked to select the word
the Picture Naming subtest. For each item, participants were prompted with two pictures,
one that was presented during the Picture Naming task and one that was not and asked to
select the picture they had seen previously. It should be noted that the non-target pictures
subtest. On each item, participants were prompted with two figures, one that was a
component of the original figure presented during the Figure Copy task and one that was
not and asked to select the component they had seen previously. It should be noted that
figures that were presented that were not components of the original complete figure
were figure components from alternate forms of the RBANS (max = 12).
Coding Task
Participants were administered a task involving the 9 symbols from the Coding
subtest. Participants were asked to select 9 coding symbols from a larger set, which they
thought matched those they had seen during the previous administration of the RBANS
Coding subtest. Participants were also asked to recall where each symbol was located in
26
the original key; this location task was not included in the final RES Total Score and was
meant to serve as a ruse that the measure appeared to be more difficult than it actually
was. It should be noted that symbols that were presented that were not components of the
original complete figure were symbols used in alternate forms of the RBANS (max = 9).
The Total RES score was computed by adding all total scores except for RES
The RBANS Effort Scale (Novitski et al., 2012) is an existing embedded measure
in the RBANS, which is calculated by subtracting delayed free recall scores from
recognition and then adding the score from the RBANS digit span subtest. The measure
against a mild traumatic brain injury group who had failed a second measure of effort. ES
scores less than 12 are considered suspicious for poor effort. However, a limitation of the
ES is that it yields significantly negative scores when individuals perform at a high level
on measures of delayed free recall and has been cautioned to only be utilized in
another embedded effort measure in the RBANS. Primary investigators for the EI
27
converted raw scores into a common metric based on their relative infrequency in a
derivation sample with true cognitive impairment and then summed these weighted
scores to arrive at an index score. More infrequent scores on digit span and list
recognition were assigned higher weighted values. The EI is then calculated by using
weighted scores on RBANS raw scores of digit span and list recognition and computed
by adding the sum of these weighted scores. Thus, a higher EI score indicates worse
effort. The measure was validated on a clinical neurological disorders population and
compared against a mild traumatic brain injury group in conjunction with three
“suboptimal” groups. EI scores greater than 3 are considered suspicious for suboptimal
effort.
28
CHAPTER FOUR
RESULTS
The demographic characteristics of participants in the CNO and SEG are shown
in Table 1. In sum, 73 participants were included in analyses for this study. The CNO
contrast, the SEG included 14 participants (36.7% male) with an average age of
SD = 1.16)). Of note, the SEG was significantly younger and had more years of education
The distributions of outcome measures (e.g. RES, Dot Counting Test, RBANS
Effort Scale and RBANS Effort Index) were examined. The RES was found to be
used; the RES was then normally distributed. We found that the Dot Counting Test
(DCT) and RBANS Effort Index (EI) were positively skewed. We then performed
distributions for both outcome measures. The RBANS Effort Scale (ES) was normally
29
Table 1. Demographic Statistics for Experimental Groups
30
Independent Variables of Interest
31
Table 2. Descriptive Statistics for Experimental Groups on RBANS Indices
Immediate Visuospatial Language Attention Delayed Total Scale
Clinical Groups (Total) 77.46 (15.34) 88.92 (16.59) 92.81 (13.29) 89.44 (16.83) 82.63 (20.87) 82.25 (14.94)
MCI 75.00 (15.04) 91.35 (18.39) 91.55 (12.55) 97.20 (14.70) 81.70 (20.79) 83.55 (14.20)
Somatoform 78.14 (13.40) 89.14 (21.24) 94.29 (9.36) 75.57 (17.03) 80.00 (20.73) 78.86 (16.64)
Normal 88.33 (20.39) 90.17 (13.57) 101.50 (9.48) 97.33 (22.12) 91.83 (19.29) 91.33 (24.11)
ADHD 80.83 (6.31) 91.50 (13.53) 94.83 (16.33) 84.17 (8.84) 89.67 (10.69) 84.33 (8.94)
TBI 82.40 (14.54) 90.80 (14.69) 89.80 (11.67) 88.00 (19.90) 82.00 (34.76) 83.20 (21.42)
MND 59.00 (6.93) 67.50 (5.80) 77.75 (16.46) 79.00 (10.68) 58.75 (14.64) 61.00 (8.60)
32
PD 68.67 (6.35) 88.67 (13.50) 93.33 (7.09) 95.00 (6.25) 93.33 (8.08) 83.67 (5.51)
Mood 87.67 (28.10) 97.67 (12.50) 107.67 (10.97) 88.33 (22.19) 84.67 (21.36) 91.67 (24.11)
LD 77.00 (5.66) 97.00 (7.07) 100.00 (11.31) 73.00 (12.73) 77.00 (5.66) 78.00 (16.97)
MS 65.00 (-) 64.00 (-) 99.00 (-) 91.00 (-) 65.00 (-) 75.00 (-)
Epilepsy 78.00 (-) 72.00 (-) 74.00 (-) 72.00 (-) 78.00 (-) 72.00 (-)
Executive dysfunction 94.00 (-) 92.00 (-) 71.00 (-) 100.00 (-) 94.00 (-) 87.00 (-)
Actor Group 65.93 (15.32) 65.07 (11.17) 70.00 (27.47) 62.00 (20.36) 61.43 (20.20) 59.50 (15.47)
Notes. Scores are standard scores (M = 100, SD = 15). Abbreviations: MCI (Mild Cognitive Impairment), ADHD (Attention-Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder), TBI (Traumatic Brain Injury), MND (Major Neurocognitive Disorder), PD (Parkinson’s Disease), LD
(Learning Disorder), MS (Multiple Sclerosis).
Table 3. Descriptive Statistics for Experimental Groups on Effort Outcome Measures
RES ES EI DCT
Clinical Groups (Total) 39.59 (2.29) 7.69 (10.15) 0.95 (1.46) 11.76 (4.10)
Executive dysfunction 41.00 (-) -6.00 (-) 0.00 (-) 12.00 (-)
Actor Group 33.14 (8.05) 5.43 (5.40) 4.79 (4.84) 19.79 (7.57)
Note. Abbreviations: MCI (Mild Cognitive Impairment), ADHD (Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder), TBI (Traumatic Brain
Injury), MND (major neurocognitive disorder), PD (Parkinson’s Disease), LD (Learning Disorder), MS (Multiple Sclerosis)
RES Reliability Analyses
To analyze the primary aim of assessing the internal consistency of the RES, the
Kuder-Richardson Formula 20 (KR-20; Kuder & Richardson, 1937) was utilized. The
KR-20 is recommended over the split half method of internal consistency reliability
because the split-half method artificially reduces a test’s reliability by its division of the
analysis into two parts. Additionally, the KR-20 is recommended for a test that is
dichotomously scored such as the RES (Cortina, 1993). Our internal consistency analysis
revealed that the 41-item RES with picture naming, figure copy, coding, and word list
analyses for individual subtests were as follows: RES picture naming α = 0.81, RES
figure copy α = 0.72, RES coding α = 0.65, RES word list α = 0.81. As such, no
decision-making. Considering the low reliability level of the RES coding, the RES’
reliability was assessed once again after extracting the coding subtest, which
RES Validity
To determine convergent validity, partial correlations were used between the RES
total score to assess for associations with existing effort measures such as the DCT, ES,
and EI controlling for age and years of education. Analyses revealed that the RES was
negatively associated with the EI (r = - 0.83, p <.01) and the DCT (r = -0.52, p <.01). As
34
such, higher scores on the RES were associated with lower scores on the EI and DCT. It
Additionally, partial correlations were utilized for all individual RES subtests to
examine their associations with the DCT, ES, and EI, again controlling for age and years
of education. RES picture naming was negatively associated with the EI (r = -0.86, p <
.01) and the DCT (r = -0.53, p < .01) but was not significant associated with the ES, p
>.05. RES figure copying was negatively associated with the ES (r = -0.28, p < .01), the
EI (r = -0.73, p < .01), and the DCT (r = -0.56, p < .01). The RES word list was
negatively associated with the EI (r = -0.85, p < .01) and the DCT (r = -0.52, p < .01) but
was not significantly associated with the ES, p > .05. RES coding was significantly
associated with the ES (r = -0.35, p < .01) and the EI (r = -0.42, p < .01) but was not
35
Table 4. Partial Correlations among RES and Effort Indices
ES - - - - - - -0.15 0.13
EI - - - - - - - .51**
DCT - - - - - - - -
and weakest associations with existing effort detection measures in this study, an
additional exploratory analysis was included. After eliminating coding from the RES,
the RES was more significantly associated with the EI (r = -.86, p <.01) and the DCT
was utilized to examine how the RES could accurately differentiate between participants
groups espousing adequate and suboptimal effort, see Table 5. Because of the possibility
that members of the CNO would also provide suboptimal effort on neuropsychological
testing, it was decided to recategorize the groups according to the more established DCT
E-score. Previous research has suggested a general cut-off score of >17 for classification
of suboptimal effort (Boone et al., 2002), which was used for our reclassification of
variables. As such, we re-classified our data into two groups (good and poor effort
according to DCT E score) and compared the two groups on their RES performance.
Following this reclassification, 17 participants were left in the suboptimal effort group
and 56 participants in the adequate effort group. Using the log-based transformation for
the RES to conform with the univariate assumption of normality, the ANCOVA was
significant [F (1,69) = 14.87, p < .01, r2 = .19]. As such, individuals engaging in adequate
effort (M = 39.41, SD = 3.01) scored significantly higher on the RES than individuals
who engaged in suboptimal effort (M = 34.88, SD = 7.30), p <.01 which suggests that the
full RES was a valid indicator of effort detection on neuropsychological testing, see
Table 6. Similarly, when RES Coding was extracted from the full RES analyses, the
37
adequate effort group continued to perform significantly better than the suboptimal effort
group [F (1,69) = 16.48, p < .01] with an equivalent effect size (r2 = .19).
subtests. The adequate effort group performed significantly better than the suboptimal
effort group on RES Picture Naming [F (1,69) = 38.99, p < .01, r2 = .39], RES Figure
Copy , [F (1,69) = 23.15, p < .01, r2 = .25], RES List Learning, [F (1,69) = 21.81, p < .01,
r2 = .26], and RES Coding , [F (1,69) = 8.30, p < .01, r2 = .17], see Table 6. Analyses
indicated that RES Picture Naming demonstrated the largest effect among individual
subtests (r2 = .39), whereas coding demonstrated the smallest effect (r2 = .17).
impairment or dementia did not perform significantly differently on the RES than other
38
Table 5. Descriptive Statistics of RES Performance by Raw Subtest and Total Score
RES Picture Naming RES Coding RES Figure RES List RES Total Score
Clinical Groups 9.92 (0.28) 8.44 (1.21) 11.48 (0.86) 9.76 (0.73) 39.59 (2.29)
MCI 9.90 (0.31) 8.35 (1.39) 11.25 (1.07) 9.70 (0.57) 39.20 (2.82)
Somatoform 9.71 (0.49) 8.86 (0.38) 11.71 (0.49) 9.00 (1.73) 39.29 (2.63)
Normal 10.00 (0.00) 8.67 (0.82) 11.83 (0.41) 10.00 (0.00) 40.50 (0.84)
ADHD 10.00(0.00) 9.00 (0.00) 11.83 (0.41) 10.00 (0.00) 40.83 (0.41)
TBI 10.00 (0.00) 8.00 (1.73) 11.20 (1.10) 10.00 (0.00) 39.20 (2.68)
MND 9.75 (0.50) 7.00 (2.16) 10.50 (1.00) 9.75 (0.50) 37.00 (2.16)
PD 10.00 (0.00) 9.00 (0.00) 12.00 (0.00) 10.00 (0.00) 41.00 (0.00)
39
Mood 10.00 (0.00) 8.33 (1.15) 11.67 (0.58) 10.00 (0.00) 40.00 (1.73)
LD 10.00 (0.00) 8.50 (0.71) 12.00 (0.00) 10.00 (0.00) 40.50 (0.71)
MS 10.00 (-) 9.00 (-) 11.00 (-) 10.00 (-) 40.00 (-)
Epilepsy 10.00 (-) 8.00 (-) 12.00 (-) 10.00 (-) 40.00 (-)
Executive dysfunction 10.00 (-) 9.00 (-) 12.00 (-) 10.00 (-) 41.00 (-)
Actor Group 7.96 (2.34) 7.93 (1.27) 9.42 (2.41) 7.86 (2.57) 33.14 (8.05)
Note. Abbreviations: MCI (Mild Cognitive Impairment), ADHD (Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder), TBI (Traumatic Brain
Injury), MND (major neurocognitive disorder), PD (Parkinson’s Disease), LD (Learning Disorder), MS (Multiple Sclerosis)
Table 6. RES Descriptive Statistics for Effort Groups
Suboptimal Effort 8.53 (2.18) 8.12 (0.93) 9.88 (11.45) 8.35 (2.52) 34.88 (7.30)
40
Exploratory Analyses
specificity of the RES with and without the coding subtest. When examining the full
RES, our analyses revealed a cutoff score of 39.50 was associated with moderate
sensitivity (sensitivity = 0.73) with moderate specificity (specificity = 0.59), see Figure 1.
When excluding the coding subtest, a cut-off of 30.50 (out of a total of 32 points) was
In comparison to the RES, the EI also had moderate sensitivity (sensitivity = 0.65)
and moderate specificity (specificity = 0.68) at a cut-off at 0.5, see Figure 3. The ES had
41
Figure 1. ROC curve analyzing RES sensitivity and specificity.
42
Figure 2. ROC curve analyzing RES sensitivity and specificity.
43
Figure 3. ROC Curve analyzing EI sensitivity and specificity.
44
Figure 4. ROC curve analyzing ES sensitivity and specificity.
45
CHAPTER FIVE
DISCUSSION
supplement to the RBANS. Data was collected for this study from September 2018 until
April of 2019. This study analyzed data from 59 clinical neuropsychology outpatients
from Loma Linda University Medical Center’s Clinical Neuropsychology Clinic and 14
experimental suboptimal effort actors from Loma Linda University’s graduate student
population.
The purpose of this study was to build upon existing measures of effort detection
within the initial screening process. Researchers have developed embedded effort
detection measures in the RBANS, namely the RBANS Effort Scale (2012) and the
RBANS Effort Index (2007), which estimate effort through analysis of recall and digit
span scores. Both measures have been found to be sensitive but limited in specificity
when classifying clinical patients from individuals exhibiting suboptimal effort. As such,
this study centered around the validation of a new supplement, which incorporated
measure.
The primary hypothesis of this study was that the RES would be a reliable and
valid measure of effort detection. KR-20 analyses revealed that our hypothesis was
confirmed from a reliability standpoint. However, none of the individual subtests alone
demonstrated the lowest alpha level and after extracting it from the total RES, the RES
had an equivalent alpha level. Validity analyses confirmed our hypothesis that the RES
46
would demonstrate convergent validity with existing measures of effort detection
including the EI and DCT. It should be noted that the RES was not significantly
correlated with ES; this may be emblematic of the primary caveat of the ES in that
individuals who excel on free recall on the RBANS have significantly negative scores on
associations with the EI and the DCT, with RES Picture Naming having the strongest
correlation among subtests with existing effort measures. RES Coding had the weakest
correlation with existing effort measures and after extracting it from the total RES score,
the RES’ associations with the EI and the DCT slightly improved. The RES also
demonstrated construct validity; participants who had been classified into a suboptimal
effort group according to DCT E-score performed significantly worse than their
counterparts in the similarly classified in the adequate effort group. All individual
subtests reflected similar group differences, with RES Picture Naming again
demonstrating the strongest effect and RES Coding demonstrating the weakest effect.
When extracting RES Coding from the RES Total score, the effect size was equivalent.
Notably, the no significant differences were detected in the Total RES and the
RES without Coding scores were between individuals with a memory disorder and other
clinical participants. Participants presenting with memory impairment are not expected
to perform significantly worse than individuals without memory impairment on the RES,
as the RES is not a memory measure. These results demonstrate the RES’ strength as an
effort detection measure, despite its face validity as memory measure. Given these
results, the RES appears to be a true measure of effort, and not a measure of memory
function.
47
Exploratory analyses indicated that the Total RES was moderately sensitive and
specific at a cutoff of 39.50; when coding was extracted, the measure was slightly more
sensitive and slightly less specific. It should be noted that the RES demonstrated greater
Clinical Implications
There are many exciting clinical implications from this study. Given the RES’
observed reliability and validity, our study demonstrates its utility in the initial
in one supplement may provide clinicians with a more well-rounded analysis and
also a measure that can be completed in approximately 10-15 minutes with the
reduction while also saving significant time. Additionally, the RES may provide
all clinical contexts. Effort detection options are widely available for neuropsychologists
to utilize with most referral questions. Effort detection also validates the nature of
48
rules out the possibility of feigned impairment for personal gain and essentially provides
Limitations
This study is not without limitations. Primarily, a control group would have
provided a baseline comparison to both the clinical and suboptimal effort groups.
Additionally, the study would have benefitted from a larger sample size in general with
experimental groups differed significantly in terms of sample size, which may have
contributed to the skewness of the original raw data. Additionally, sampling in itself may
effort group were highly educated, averaging over 16 years of education, and were
testing.
This study leads to several questions regarding future research. It may be useful to
consider including a digits backward component to the RES; this may allow for the
computation of reliable digits similar to the WAIS-IV and would add yet another
49
component of effort detection to the supplement. Additionally, it is recommended that the
RES be analyzed for reliability and validity in other clinical settings as well. The RES
would certainly benefit significantly from replication in other settings and among a wide
Conclusion
In summary, this study analyzed the reliability and the validity of a novel measure
of effort and motivation, the RBANS effort supplement. This study found that the RES
was a reliable measure of effort detection. Additionally, the RES exhibited convergent
validity with an established embedded effort detection measure from the RBANS (the
RBANS Effort Index) and the DCT, which is another well-established independent effort
detection measure. The RES demonstrated construct validity in that participants who
were classified in the suboptimal effort group according to their performance on the DCT
performed significantly worse on the RES than did individuals who had been classified
into the adequate effort group. A ROC curve analysis was performed and demonstrated
that the RES exhibited moderate sensitivity and specificity at a cut-off score of 39.50.
Clinical implications of this study include the potential for screening for effort from a
may significantly reduce costs and save a significant amount of time. Key limitations
include a lack of a control group, small sample size, and lack of greater representation
from common outpatient referral sources. Future research directions include replication
identified the RES as a useful measure in detecting effort, but further research is
50
undoubtedly necessary to fully understand the extent of its utility in a clinical
neuropsychological setting.
51
REFERENCES
Axelrod, B. N., Fichtenberg, N. L., Millis, S. R., & Wertheimer, J. C. (2006). Detecting
Incomplete Effort with Digit Span from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—
Third Edition. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20(3), 513-523.
Bayley, P. J., Wixted, J. T., Hopkins, R. O., & Squire, L. R. (2008). Yes/No Recognition,
Forced-choice Recognition, and the Human Hippocampus. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 20(3), 505–512. http://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20038
Bianchini, K. J., Mathias, C. W., & Greve, K. W. (2001). Symptom Validity Testing: A
Critical Review. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,15(1), 19-45.
doi:10.1076/clin.15.1.19.1907
Boone, K. B., Lu, P. and Herzberg, D. S. (2002). The b Test Manual, Los Angeles,
CA: Western Psychological Services.
Boone, K., Lu, P., & Herzberg, D. S. (2002). The Dot Counting Test. Los Angeles:
Western Psychological Services.
Boone, K. B., Lu, P., Back, C., King, C., Lee, A., Philpott, L., ... & Warner-Chacon, K.
(2002). Sensitivity and specificity of the Rey Dot Counting Test in patients with
suspect effort and various clinical samples. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 17(7), 625-642.
Bouman, Z., Hendriks, M. P., Schmand, B. A., Kessels, R. P., & Aldenkamp, A. P.
(2016). Indicators of suboptimal performance embedded in the Wechsler Memory
Scale–Fourth Edition (WMS–IV). Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology,38(4), 455-466. doi:10.1080/13803395.2015.1123226
52
Butcher, J. N., Dahlstrom, W. G., Graham, J. R., Tellegen, A, & Kaemmer, B. (1989).
The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 (MMPI-2): Manual for
administration and scoring. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
Carone, D. A., Iverson, G. L., & Bush, S. S. (2010). A Model to Approaching and
Providing Feedback to Patients Regarding Invalid Test Performance in Clinical
Neuropsychological Evaluations. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,24(5), 759-778.
doi:10.1080/13854041003712951
Crighton, A. H., Wygant, D. B., Holt, K. R., & Granacher, R. P. (2015). Embedded Effort
Scales in the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological
Status: Do They Detect Neurocognitive Malingering? Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology,30(3), 181-185. doi:10.1093/arclin/acv002
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. (2000). California Verbal Learning
Test—2nd edition: Manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
Duff, K., Hobson, V.L, Beglinger, L.J, & O’Bryant, S. E. (2010). Diagnostic Accuracy of
the RBANS in mild cognitive impairment: limitations on assessing milder
impairments. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 25(5), 429-441.
Duff, K., Humphreys Clark, J. D., O’Bryant, S. E., Mold, J. W., Schiffer, R. B., Sutker,
P.B. (2008). Utility of the RBANS in detecting cognitive impairment associated
with Alzheimer’s disease: Sensitivity, specificity, and positive and negative
predictive powers. Archives of Classical Neuropsychology 23, 603-612.
Duff, K., Spering, C. C., O’Bryant, S. E., Beglinger, L. J., Moser, D. J., Bayless, J. D., . .
. Scott, J. G. (2011). The RBANS Effort Index: Base Rates in Geriatric
Samples. Applied Neuropsychology,18(1), 11-17.
doi:10.1080/09084282.2010.523354
Etheron, J.L., Bianchini, K.J., Greve, K.W., & Heinly, M.T. (2005). Sensitivity and
specificity of reliable digit span in malingered pain-related disability. Assessment,
12 (2), 130-136.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical
power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences.
Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. doi:10.3758/bf03193146
53
Frederick, R. I., & Speed, F. M. (2007). On the interpretation of below-chance
responding in forced-choice tests. Assessment, 14, 3-11.
Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., & Gola, T. (1994). Validation of malingered amnesia
measures with a large clinical sample. Psychological Assessment,6(3), 218-224.
doi:10.1037//1040-3590.6.3.218
Grimes, D., A., & Schulz, K. F. (2005). Refining clinical diagnosis with likelihood ratios.
The Lancet, 365, 1500-1505.
Hanley, J. A., & McNeil, B. J. (1982). The meaning and use of the area under a Receiver
Operating Characteristic (ROC) curve. Radiology, 143, 29–36.
Heilbronner, R., Sweet, J., Morgan, J., Larrabee, G., Millis, S., & Participants, C. (2009).
American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology Consensus Conference
Statement on the Neuropsychological Assessment of Effort, Response Bias, and
Malingering. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,23(7), 1093-1129.
doi:10.1080/13854040903155063
Jasinski, L. J., Berry, D. T., Shandera, A. L., & Clark, J. A. (2011). Use of the Wechsler
Adult Intelligence Scale Digit Span subtest for malingering detection: A meta-
analytic review. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,33(3),
300-314. doi:10.1080/13803395.2010.516743
Kuder, G. F., & Richardson, M. W. (1937). The theory of the estimation of test
reliability. Psychometrika, 2(3), 151-160.
54
Larrabee, G. J. (2008). Aggregation across multiple indicators improves the detection of
malingering: relationship to likelihood ratios. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22
(4), 666-679. DOI: 10.1080/13854040701494987
Larrabee, G. J., & Berry, D. T. (2007). Diagnostic Classification Statistics and Diagnostic
Validity of Malingered Assessment. In G. J. Larrabee (Ed.), Assessment of
Malingered Neuropsychological Deficits (pp. 14-27). New York, New York:
Oxford University Press.
Martin, P.K, Schroeder, R. W., & Odland, A.P. (2015). Neuropsychologists validity
testing beliefs and practices: a survey of North American professionals. The
Clinical Neuropsychologist, (29) 6, 741-776. doi:
10.1080/13854046.2015.1087597
Meyers, J. E., & Meyers, K.R. (1995). Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial.
Odessa, Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Mittenberg, W., Patton, C., Canyock, E. M., & Condit, D. C. (2002). Base Rates of
Malingering and Symptom Exaggeration. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology (Neuropsychology, Development and Cognition: Section
A),24(8), 1094-1102. doi:10.1076/jcen.24.8.1094.8379
Nelson, N.W., Boone, K., Dueck, A., Wagener, L., Lu, P., & Grills, C. (2003).
Relationships between eight measures of suspect effort. The Clinical
Neuropsychologist, 17 (2), 263-272.
55
Novitski, J., Steele, S., Karantzoulis, S., & Randolph, C. (2012). The Repeatable Battery
for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale. Archives of
Clinical Neuropsychology,27, 190-195.
Paul, R., Lane, E. M., Tate, D. F., Heaps, J., Romo, D. M., Akbudak, E., … Conturo, T.
E. (2011). Neuroimaging signatures and cognitive correlates of the montreal
cognitive assessment screen in a nonclinical elderly sample. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology : The Official Journal of the National Academy of
Neuropsychologists, 26(5), 454–60. doi:10.1093/arclin/acr017
Postman, L. (1963). One trial learning. In C. F. Cofer & B. S. Musgrave (Eds.), Verbal
behavior and Learning (pp. 295-332). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Root, J. C., Robbins, R. N., Chang, L., & Gorp, W. G. (2006). Detection of inadequate
effort on the California Verbal Learning Test-Second edition: Forced choice
recognition and critical item analysis. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society,12(05). doi:10.1017/s1355617706060838
Silverberg, N. D., Wertheimer, J. C., & Fichtenberg, N. L. (2007). An effort index for the
Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status
(RBANS). The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21(5), 841-854.
Slick, D.J., Tan, J.E., Strauss, E.H., & Hultsch, D.F. (2004). Detecting malingering: a
survey of experts’ practices. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 465-473.
Teicher, G., & Wagner, M. T. (2004). The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM):
Normative data from cognitively intact, cognitively impaired, and elderly patients
with dementia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 455–464.
Wechsler, D. (2009). Advanced clinical solutions for the WAIS-IV and WMS-IV. San
Antonio, TX: Pearson.
56
Wechsler, D. (2009). Wechsler Memory Scale–Fourth Edition. Pearson; San Antonio,
TX: Pearson.
57
APPENDIX A
58
APPENDIX B
59
60
APPENDIX C
JOURNAL
Author Note
CA, 92354
Email: [email protected]
61
Abstract
evaluation to ensure results of testing are valid indicators of an individual’s true level of
administering longer testing batteries. Two effort measures are embedded in the
elevated false positive rates for classifying individuals with memory impairment as those
putting forth poor effort. These embedded measures rely on cut-off scores on digit span
and memory subtests. In contrast, this RBANS Effort Supplement (RES) utilizes several
multiple methods of effort detection; subtests in this measure included list learning
and a story recognition component utilized for face validity of memory assessment. Fifty-
conjunction with 14 poor effort simulators; each participant was administered the
RBANS, the RES, and the Dot Counting Test (DCT). Results supported the RES’
the RES exhibited strong convergent validity with established effort detection measures
and that individuals putting for poor effort scored significantly lower on the RES than
individuals who put forth adequate effort, as delineated by the established DCT cutoff
score of 17. In summary, the RES was shown to be a valid indicator of effort detection.
62
Clinical implications of the RES include reduction of time and costs involved in
neuropsychological assessment
63
The Formulation of a RBANS Effort Supplement
As part of the neuropsychological assessment, patients are asked to put forth their
best effort throughout the administration of cognitive testing so that valid data may be
in 29% of personal injury cases, 30% of disability cases, 19% of criminal cases, 38.5% of
personal injury cases, and 8% of general medical cases involving symptom exaggeration.
confidence in definitively diagnosing poor effort occurs through multiple effort measures
Forced choice measures appear to be difficult but are in fact, easy tasks. Typically, in
forced-choice, an individual is asked to encode a series of pictures or words and then later
asked to select each of the target pictures or words from two choices. Participants who
perform below chance levels (i.e., below 50%) are identified as individuals who may
Effort measures can often be integrated into initial consultations along with
64
patients as well as determine whether interpretations and future testing may be needed
after the initial consult. A commonly administered screening measure is the Repeatable
Research within the last decade has also revealed that among commonly used dementia
screening measures, total RBANS performance is one of the better measures in predicting
total brain volume (Paul et al., 2011). The RBANS’ relevance to dementia screening in
detect feigned impairment. Two such embedded measures include the RBANS Effort
Index (EI; Silverberg, Wertheimer, Fichtenberg, 2007) and the RBANS Effort Scale (ES;
Despite the empirically validated research from which these measures were
constructed, research has demonstrated that their validity may be somewhat limited.
Research has illustrated that although the EI exhibits good specificity for simulated
malingerers with a false-positive rate of 19% or less at selected cutoffs, it has only
moderate sensitivity (66%), which risks the possibility of misdiagnosing malingerers with
subtracting free recall scores as an overall reflection of its focus on patients with amnesia
and has also reflected high false-positive rates as well (Crighton et al., 2015).
In this study, the RBANS Effort Supplement was formulated and assessed for reliability
and validity to detect suboptimal effort through the sole usage of the RBANS assessment.
It was designed to be a quick measure to administer, with the opportunity for cost-
efficiency in that a subsequent longer evaluation would not be needed if effort were
65
found to be a significant issue. It also included different methods/formats of malingering
(Larrabee, 2008). Thus, the primary aim of this study was to establish the reliability and
66
Aims and Hypotheses
The primary aim of this study was to determine if the RBANS Effort Supplement
(RES) was a reliable and valid measure of effort. To measure the RES’ reliability, the
RES was assessed for internal consistency utilizing the Kuder-Richardson 20 method
(Kuder & Richardson, 1937). It was hypothesized that the RES would be internally
consistent. Following reliability analysis, the construct validity of the RES was examined.
Specifically, The RES was assessed for convergent validity utilizing partial correlations
controlling for age and years of education. It was hypothesized that the RES would
exhibit convergent validity with the RBANS Effort Index, RBANS Effort Scale, and the
Dot Counting Test. Further, we hypothesized that participants within the experimental
malingering sample would score significantly lower on the RES in comparison to clinical
groups.
An exploratory aim of this study was to examine the specificity and sensitivity of
the RES. Such analyses were conducted utilizing ROC curve analyses according to a RES
was hypothesized that the RES will be specific and sensitive in correctly classifying
67
Methods
outpatient (CNO) and a comparative suboptimal effort group. The CNO group was
Loma Linda University Medical Center East Campus neuropsychology service. Our
suboptimal effort group was recruited from Loma Linda University and included 15
students from the graduate student population. All subjects fell within the age range of
20-89 and all spoke English fluently. One participant was excluded utilizing the outlier
labelling rule on the RES total score to help correct for the skewness of the data. As such
Participants involved in the CNO group were individuals who had been referred
for clinical neuropsychological services for various reasons, including mild cognitive
impairment, traumatic brain injury, stroke, epilepsy, ADHD, and varying mood disorders.
neuropsychological referral, they were asked to participate in the current study. Agreeing
participants completed the informed consent process and gave permission to use the
results of their clinical testing (i.e. RBANS, RES, Dot Counting Test) for the current
study. Participants then completed a brief additional structured interview asking for basic
68
Participants were administered the RBANS as part of their routine neuropsychological
Participants enrolled in the suboptimal effort group (SEG) were recruited from
Loma Linda University’s graduate population. Subjects were recruited from various
wide postings. Participants completed the informed consent process and a brief structured
(DenBoer & Hall, 2007), prompting them to approach the neuropsychological tests as if
they were trying to appear brain damaged in order to receive financial compensation in an
ongoing lawsuit:
You are about to take some cognitive tests that examine mental abilities such as attention, memory,
thinking and reasoning skills, and your ability to think quickly. While responding to the tests, please
pretend that you have experienced brain damage from a car accident involving a head-on collision.
You hit your head against the windshield and were knocked out for 15 minutes. Afterwards, you felt
‘‘dazed’’ so you were hospitalized overnight for observation. Because the driver of the other car is at
fault, you have decided to go to court to get money from the person responsible. During the next few
months following the accident, the negative effects from your head injury disappear. Your lawsuit has
not been settled yet, and your lawyer has told you that you may get more money if you look like you
are still suffering from brain damage. As you pretend to be this car accident victim, try to respond to
each test as a patient who is trying to appear brain damaged in order to get money from the lawsuit.
Thus, your performance on the tests should convince the examiner as well as the people involved in
deciding the outcome of your lawsuit that you are still suffering from brain damage.
Approval for the study was obtained from the Loma Linda University Human
Subjects Committee Institutional Review Board, and written informed consent was
acquired from all participants upon enrollment. It should be noted that Loma Linda
University associated legal counsel stated that the RBANS Effort Supplement was
considered legally permissible as long as primary investigators did not attempt to earn a
profit from the measure itself. The RES was only utilized for the purposes of this study.
Instruments
69
Prior to the examination of participants, examiners interviewed participants using
the participants were involved in previous litigation and three participants had engaged in
previous neuropsychological testing (one participant in 2016, another in 1985, and the
Boone, Lu, and Herzberg’s Dot Counting Test (2002) is a measure of symptom
validity and malingering. Participants are presented with a series of twelve dotted cards
and are asked to count the number dots as quickly as possible and relay to the examiner
the number of dots that they counted. On cards one through six, the dots on the cards are
disseminated in no organizational fashion. In cards seven through twelve, the dots on the
cards are grouped in such a way that it is easier to count the number of dots quickly. An
E-score is tabulated according to the participant’s response times and number of errors on
the test itself (lower E-scores reflect fewer errors and faster response times). Research has
identified that the DCT is an adequate measure of suspect effort, with moderate
encouraged that the DCT be utilized in conjunction with other measures when assessing
for symptom validity (Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2008). Previous research has
suggested a general cut-off score of >17 for classification of suboptimal effort (Boone et
al., 2002).
70
Randolph’s RBANS (1998) is a neuropsychological assessment used to test the
cognitive status of individuals suffering from neurological diseases or head trauma. One
of the core advantages to using the RBANS is its brevity. The RBANS takes
visuospatial ability, language, and attention) and twelve subtests (list learning, story
memory, figure copy, line orientation, digit span, symbol digit coding, picture naming,
semantic fluency, list recall, list recognition, story recall, and figure recall). All index
scores are comprised of two subtests except for the delayed memory domain, which
consists of four subtests. The RBANS total score provides an overall outcome statistic for
attention, and delayed memory can be calculated. All subtests are given a subtest raw
score. Raw scores of subtests within each domain are added and converted to an age-
corrected index score. Index scores can also be converted to percentile scores, according
ability to remember and recall a small amount of information directly after it has been
List Learning: List Learning consists of a list of 10 unrelated words, read for immediate
recall over four trials, with a maximum score of 40. Words used in the List Learning task
are considered moderate-high imagery words with relatively low age of acquisition. The
71
high imagery levels and low age of acquisition of these words is considered helpful in
Story Memory: This subtest is comprised of a story with 12 itemized details; the story is
read for immediate recall over two trials, for a total maximum score of 24.
comprehend, and recreate spatial relations. Notably, this domain assesses participants’
ability to estimate distance and depth and navigate the surrounding environment. The
Figure Copy: The Figure Copy subtest prompts participants to draw an exact copy of a
complex figure comprised of geometric shapes. The subtest itself is considered very
similar yet less complex to the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (Meyers & Meyers,
lines, beginning at a common point of origin and fanning out across 180 degrees, which
serves as the reference figure. Each item consists of two target lines that are shown
beneath the reference figure. Subjects must correctly identify which two lines in the
reference match the two target lines. Line orientation consists of 10 items, each
participants to recall information for an extended length of time. These subtests are
72
List Learning free recall: Free recall of the words from the List Learning subtest (max =
10).
List Learning Recognition: Yes/No recognition of the words from the List Learning
Story Memory Free Recall: Free recall of the story from the Story Memory subtest
(max=12).
Figure Free Recall: Free recall of the figure from the Figure Copy subtest (max = 20).
skills to verbally name and retrieve previously learned semantic information. Two
Semantic Fluency: Participants are allotted one minute to provide as many examples from
The attention domain prompts the participant to manipulate previously presented material
(visual and oral) that has been stored within the individual’s short-term memory. This
Digit Span: Subjects are asked to repeat a series of numbers, with stimulus items
increasing in length from 2 digits to 9 digits. The items are presented in order of length
(shortest to longest), and the test itself is discontinued when the participant fails all trials
73
within a given string length. It should be noted that there is no digit span backwards on
the RBANS.
to the Coding subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Subjects are asked to fill
in digits matching with corresponding shapes on a coding key as fast as they can. After
practice items are completed, participants have 90 seconds to complete as many items as
possible.
Total Scale. The Total Scale is the overall outcome statistic for an individual’s
overall neuropsychological functioning as comprised by the sum of all the index scores of
Naming, Figure Copy, and Coding. It should be noted that the RES has never been
utilized in previous research. The RES was constructed utilizing the stimuli in RBANS
form A, with all non-target stimuli for verbal and nonverbal information derived from
format regarding details from the story that was read to them twice previously in the
RBANS Story Memory subtest (max = 12). This subtest was not included in the final
RES Total score and was meant to serve as face valid indicator of memory performance.
74
List Learning Forced Choice: Participants were administered a forced-choice task
involving the 10 words from the List Learning subtest. For each item, participants were
prompted with two words, one word from the original list and one novel word, and
subsequently asked to select the word that appeared on the original list (max =10).
involving the 10 objects from the Picture Naming subtest. For each item, participants
were prompted with two pictures, one that was presented during the Picture Naming task
and one that was not and asked to select the picture they had seen previously. It should be
noted that the non-target pictures were pictures from alternate forms of the RBANS. (max
=10)
involving the Figure Copy subtest. On each item, participants were prompted with two
figures, one that was a component of the original figure presented during the Figure Copy
task and one that was not and asked to select the component they had seen previously. It
should be noted that figures that were presented that were not components of the original
complete figure were figure components from alternate forms of the RBANS (max = 12).
Coding Task: Participants were administered a task involving the 9 symbols from the
Coding subtest. Participants were asked to select 9 coding symbols from a larger set,
which they thought matched those they had seen during the previous administration of
the RBANS Coding subtest. Participants were also asked to recall where each symbol
was located in the original key; this location task was not included in the final RES Total
Score and was meant to serve as a ruse that the measure appeared to be more difficult
than it actually was. It should be noted that symbols that were presented that were not
75
components of the original complete figure were symbols used in alternate forms of the
The Total RES score was computed by adding all total scores except for RES
The RBANS Effort Scale (Novitski et al., 2012) is an existing embedded measure
in the RBANS, which is calculated by subtracting delayed free recall scores from
recognition and then adding the score from the RBANS digit span subtest. The measure
against a mild traumatic brain injury group who had failed a second measure of effort. ES
scores less than 12 are considered suspicious for poor effort. However, a limitation of the
ES is that it yields significantly negative scores when individuals perform at a high level
on measures of delayed free recall and has been cautioned to only be utilized in
another embedded effort measure in the RBANS. Primary investigators for the EI
converted raw scores into a common metric based on their relative infrequency in a
derivation sample with true cognitive impairment and then summed these weighted
scores to arrive at an index score. More infrequent scores on digit span and list
recognition were assigned higher weighted values. The EI is then calculated by using
weighted scores on RBANS raw scores of digit span and list recognition and computed
by adding the sum of these weighted scores. Thus, a higher EI score indicates worse
76
effort. The measure was validated on a clinical neurological disorders population and
compared against a mild traumatic brain injury group in conjunction with three
“suboptimal” groups. EI scores greater than 3 are considered suspicious for suboptimal
effort.
77
Results
The demographic characteristics of participants in the CNO and SEG are shown
in Table 1. In sum, 73 participants were included in analyses for this study. The CNO
contrast, the SEG included 14 participants (36.7% male) with an average age of
SD = 1.16)). Of note, the SEG was significantly younger and had more years of education
The distributions of outcome measures (e.g. RES, Dot Counting Test, RBANS
Effort Scale and RBANS Effort Index) were examined. The RES was found to be
used; the RES was then normally distributed. We found that the Dot Counting Test
(DCT) and RBANS Effort Index (EI) were positively skewed. We then performed
distributions for both outcome measures. The RBANS Effort Scale (ES) was normally
78
Descriptive statistics calculated for all experimental groups on RBANS indices
are shown in Table 2 (See Appendix A). Additionally, descriptive statistics on relevant
To analyze the primary aim of assessing the internal consistency of the RES, the
Kuder-Richardson Formula 20 (KR-20; Kuder & Richardson, 1937) was utilized. The
KR-20 is recommended over the split half method of internal consistency reliability
because the split-half method artificially reduces a test’s reliability by its division of the
analysis into two parts. Additionally, the KR-20 is recommended for a test that is
dichotomously scored such as the RES (Cortina, 1993). Our internal consistency analysis
revealed that the 41-item RES with picture naming, figure copy, coding, and word list
analyses for individual subtests were as follows: RES picture naming α = 0.81, RES
figure copy α = 0.72, RES coding α = 0.65, RES word list α = 0.81. As such, no
decision-making. Considering the low reliability level of the RES coding, the RES’
reliability was assessed once again after extracting the coding subtest, which
RES Validity
To determine convergent validity, partial correlations were used between the RES
total score to assess for associations with existing effort measures such as the DCT, ES,
and EI controlling for age and years of education. Analyses revealed that the RES was
79
negatively associated with the EI (r = - 0.83, p <.01) and the DCT (r = -0.52, p <.01). As
such, higher scores on the RES were associated with lower scores on the EI and DCT. It
Additionally, partial correlations were utilized for all individual RES subtests to
examine their associations with the DCT, ES, and EI, again controlling for age and years
of education. RES picture naming was negatively associated with the EI (r = -0.86, p <
.01) and the DCT (r = -0.53, p < .01) but was not significant associated with the ES, p
>.05. RES figure copying was negatively associated with the ES (r = -0.28, p < .01), the
EI (r = -0.73, p < .01), and the DCT (r = -0.56, p < .01). The RES word list was
negatively associated with the EI (r = -0.85, p < .01) and the DCT (r = -0.52, p < .01) but
was not significantly associated with the ES, p > .05. RES coding was significantly
associated with the ES (r = -0.35, p < .01) and the EI (r = -0.42, p < .01) but was not
Because the RES coding subtest demonstrated the weakest reliability (α = 0.65)
and weakest associations with existing effort detection measures in this study, an
additional exploratory analysis was included. After eliminating coding from the RES,
the RES was more significantly associated with the EI (r = -.86, p <.01) and the DCT
was utilized to examine how the RES could accurately differentiate between participants
groups espousing adequate and suboptimal effort, see Table 5. Because of the possibility
that members of the CNO would also provide suboptimal effort on neuropsychological
testing, it was decided to recategorize the groups according to the more established DCT
80
E-score. Previous research has suggested a general cut-off score of >17 for classification
of suboptimal effort (Boone et al., 2002), which was used for our reclassification of
variables. As such, we re-classified our data into two groups (good and poor effort
according to DCT E score) and compared the two groups on their RES performance.
Following this reclassification, 17 participants were left in the suboptimal effort group
and 56 participants in the adequate effort group. Using the log-based transformation for
the RES to conform with the univariate assumption of normality, the ANCOVA was
significant [F (1,69) = 14.87, p < .01, r2 = .19]. As such, individuals engaging in adequate
effort (M = 39.41, SD = 3.01) scored significantly higher on the RES than individuals
who engaged in suboptimal effort (M = 34.88, SD = 7.30), p <.01 which suggests that the
full RES was a valid indicator of effort detection on neuropsychological testing, see
Table 6. Similarly, when RES Coding was extracted from the full RES analyses, the
adequate effort group continued to perform significantly better than the suboptimal effort
group [F (1,69) = 16.48, p < .01] with an equivalent effect size (r2 = .19).
subtests. The adequate effort group performed significantly better than the suboptimal
effort group on RES Picture Naming [F (1,69) = 38.99, p < .01, r2 = .39], RES Figure
Copy , [F (1,69) = 23.15, p < .01, r2 = .25], RES List Learning, [F (1,69) = 21.81, p < .01,
r2 = .26], and RES Coding , [F (1,69) = 8.30, p < .01, r2 = .17], see Table 6. Analyses
indicated that RES Picture Naming demonstrated the largest effect among individual
subtests (r2 = .39), whereas coding demonstrated the smallest effect (r2 = .17).
81
Additional analyses indicated that individuals diagnosed with mild cognitive
impairment or dementia did not perform significantly differently on the RES than other
Exploratory Analyses
specificity of the RES with and without the coding subtest. When examining the full
RES, our analyses revealed a cutoff score of 39.50 was associated with moderate
sensitivity (sensitivity = 0.73) with moderate specificity (specificity = 0.59), see Figure 1.
When excluding the coding subtest, a cut-off of 30.50 (out of a total of 32 points) was
In comparison to the RES, the EI also had moderate sensitivity (sensitivity = 0.65)
and moderate specificity (specificity = 0.68) at a cut-off at 0.5, see Figure 3. The ES had
82
Discussion
supplement to the RBANS. Data was collected for this study from September 2018 until
April of 2019. This study analyzed data from 59 clinical neuropsychology outpatients
from Loma Linda University Medical Center’s Clinical Neuropsychology Clinic and 14
experimental suboptimal effort actors from Loma Linda University’s graduate student
population.
The purpose of this study was to build upon existing measures of effort detection
within the initial screening process. Researchers have developed embedded effort
detection measures in the RBANS, namely the RBANS Effort Scale (2012) and the
RBANS Effort Index (2007), which estimate effort through analysis of recall and digit
span scores. Both measures have been found to be sensitive but limited in specificity
when classifying clinical patients from individuals exhibiting suboptimal effort. As such,
this study centered around the validation of a new supplement, which incorporated
measure.
The primary hypothesis of this study was that the RES would be a reliable and
valid measure of effort detection. KR-20 analyses revealed that our hypothesis was
confirmed from a reliability standpoint. However, none of the individual subtests alone
demonstrated the lowest alpha level and after extracting it from the total RES, the RES
had an equivalent alpha level. Validity analyses confirmed our hypothesis that the RES
83
including the EI and DCT. It should be noted that the RES was not significantly
correlated with ES; this may be emblematic of the primary caveat of the ES in that
individuals who excel on free recall on the RBANS have significantly negative scores on
associations with the EI and the DCT, with RES Picture Naming having the strongest
correlation among subtests with existing effort measures. RES Coding had the weakest
correlation with existing effort measures and after extracting it from the total RES score,
the RES’ associations with the EI and the DCT slightly improved. The RES also
demonstrated construct validity; participants who had been classified into a suboptimal
effort group according to DCT E-score performed significantly worse than their
counterparts in the similarly classified in the adequate effort group. All individual
subtests reflected similar group differences, with RES Picture Naming again
demonstrating the strongest effect and RES Coding demonstrating the weakest effect.
When extracting RES Coding from the RES Total score, the effect size was equivalent.
Notably, the no significant differences were detected in the Total RES and the
RES without Coding scores were between individuals with a memory disorder and other
clinical participants. Participants presenting with memory impairment are not expected
to perform significantly worse than individuals without memory impairment on the RES,
as the RES is not a memory measure. These results demonstrate the RES’ strength as an
effort detection measure, despite its face validity as memory measure. Given these
results, the RES appears to be a true measure of effort, and not a measure of memory
function.
84
Exploratory analyses indicated that the Total RES was moderately sensitive and
specific at a cutoff of 39.50; when coding was extracted, the measure was slightly more
sensitive and slightly less specific. It should be noted that the RES demonstrated greater
Clinical Implications
There are many exciting clinical implications from this study. Given the RES’
observed reliability and validity, our study demonstrates its utility in the initial
in one supplement may provide clinicians with a more well-rounded analysis and
also a measure that can be completed in approximately 10-15 minutes with the
reduction while also saving significant time. Additionally, the RES may provide
all clinical contexts. Effort detection options are widely available for neuropsychologists
to utilize with most referral questions. Effort detection also validates the nature of
85
rules out the possibility of feigned impairment for personal gain and essentially provides
Limitations
This study is not without limitations. Primarily, a control group would have
provided a baseline comparison to both the clinical and suboptimal effort groups.
Additionally, the study would have benefitted from a larger sample size in general with
experimental groups differed significantly in terms of sample size, which may have
contributed to the skewness of the original raw data. Additionally, sampling in itself may
effort group were highly educated, averaging over 16 years of education, and were
testing.
This study leads to several questions regarding future research. It may be useful to
consider including a digits backward component to the RES; this may allow for the
computation of reliable digits similar to the WAIS-IV and would add yet another
RES be analyzed for reliability and validity in other clinical settings as well. The RES
86
would certainly benefit significantly from replication in other settings and among a wide
Conclusion
In summary, this study analyzed the reliability and the validity of a novel measure
of effort and motivation, the RBANS effort supplement. This study found that the RES
was a reliable measure of effort detection. Additionally, the RES exhibited convergent
validity with an established embedded effort detection measure from the RBANS (the
RBANS Effort Index) and the DCT, which is another well-established independent effort
detection measure. The RES demonstrated construct validity in that participants who
were classified in the suboptimal effort group according to their performance on the DCT
performed significantly worse on the RES than did individuals who had been classified
into the adequate effort group. A ROC curve analysis was performed and demonstrated
that the RES exhibited moderate sensitivity and specificity at a cut-off score of 39.50.
Clinical implications of this study include the potential for screening for effort from a
may significantly reduce costs and save a significant amount of time. Key limitations
include a lack of a control group, small sample size, and lack of greater representation
from common outpatient referral sources. Future research directions include replication
identified the RES as a useful measure in detecting effort, but further research is
neuropsychological setting.
87
Additional Information
Funding
88
References
Axelrod, B. N., Fichtenberg, N. L., Millis, S. R., & Wertheimer, J. C. (2006). Detecting
Incomplete Effort with Digit Span from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—
Bayley, P. J., Wixted, J. T., Hopkins, R. O., & Squire, L. R. (2008). Yes/No Recognition,
Bianchini, K. J., Mathias, C. W., & Greve, K. W. (2001). Symptom Validity Testing: A
doi:10.1076/clin.15.1.19.1907
640.
Boone, K. B., Lu, P. and Herzberg, D. S. (2002). The b Test Manual, Los Angeles,
Boone, K., Lu, P., & Herzberg, D. S. (2002). The Dot Counting Test. Los Angeles:
89
Boone, K. B., & Lu, P. H. (2007). Non-Forced-Effort Measures. In G. J. Larrabee
Boone, K. B., Lu, P., Back, C., King, C., Lee, A., Philpott, L., ... & Warner-Chacon, K.
(2002). Sensitivity and specificity of the Rey Dot Counting Test in patients with
Bouman, Z., Hendriks, M. P., Schmand, B. A., Kessels, R. P., & Aldenkamp, A. P.
Butcher, J. N., Dahlstrom, W. G., Graham, J. R., Tellegen, A, & Kaemmer, B. (1989).
Carone, D. A., Iverson, G. L., & Bush, S. S. (2010). A Model to Approaching and
doi:10.1080/13854041003712951
9010.78.1.98
Crighton, A. H., Wygant, D. B., Holt, K. R., & Granacher, R. P. (2015). Embedded Effort
90
Status: Do They Detect Neurocognitive Malingering? Archives of Clinical
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. (2000). California Verbal Learning
Duff, K., Hobson, V.L, Beglinger, L.J, & O’Bryant, S. E. (2010). Diagnostic Accuracy of
Duff, K., Humphreys Clark, J. D., O’Bryant, S. E., Mold, J. W., Schiffer, R. B., Sutker,
Duff, K., Spering, C. C., O’Bryant, S. E., Beglinger, L. J., Moser, D. J., Bayless, J. D., . .
doi:10.1080/09084282.2010.523354
Etheron, J.L., Bianchini, K.J., Greve, K.W., & Heinly, M.T. (2005). Sensitivity and
12 (2), 130-136.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical
power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences.
91
Frederick, R. I., & Speed, F. M. (2007). On the interpretation of below-chance
Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., & Gola, T. (1994). Validation of malingered amnesia
doi:10.1037//1040-3590.6.3.218
Grimes, D., A., & Schulz, K. F. (2005). Refining clinical diagnosis with likelihood ratios.
Hanley, J. A., & McNeil, B. J. (1982). The meaning and use of the area under a Receiver
doi:10.1080/09084280802644466
Heilbronner, R., Sweet, J., Morgan, J., Larrabee, G., Millis, S., & Participants, C. (2009).
doi:10.1080/13854040903155063
Jasinski, L. J., Berry, D. T., Shandera, A. L., & Clark, J. A. (2011). Use of the Wechsler
Adult Intelligence Scale Digit Span subtest for malingering detection: A meta-
92
analytic review. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,33(3),
300-314. doi:10.1080/13803395.2010.516743
Kuder, G. F., & Richardson, M. W. (1937). The theory of the estimation of test
630.
Larrabee, G. J., & Berry, D. T. (2007). Diagnostic Classification Statistics and Diagnostic
93
Larrabee, G.J. (2003). Detection of malingering using atypical performance patterns on
425.
Martin, P.K, Schroeder, R. W., & Odland, A.P. (2015). Neuropsychologists validity
10.1080/13854046.2015.1087597
Meyers, J. E., & Meyers, K.R. (1995). Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial.
Mittenberg, W., Patton, C., Canyock, E. M., & Condit, D. C. (2002). Base Rates of
doi:10.1080/02699050410001672350
Nelson, N.W., Boone, K., Dueck, A., Wagener, L., Lu, P., & Grills, C. (2003).
94
Novitski, J., Steele, S., Karantzoulis, S., & Randolph, C. (2012). The Repeatable Battery
Paul, R., Lane, E. M., Tate, D. F., Heaps, J., Romo, D. M., Akbudak, E., … Conturo, T.
Postman, L. (1963). One trial learning. In C. F. Cofer & B. S. Musgrave (Eds.), Verbal
Root, J. C., Robbins, R. N., Chang, L., & Gorp, W. G. (2006). Detection of inadequate
Silverberg, N. D., Wertheimer, J. C., & Fichtenberg, N. L. (2007). An effort index for the
Slick, D.J., Tan, J.E., Strauss, E.H., & Hultsch, D.F. (2004). Detecting malingering: a
95
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M., Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological
Teicher, G., & Wagner, M. T. (2004). The Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM):
Normative data from cognitively intact, cognitively impaired, and elderly patients
Wechsler, D. (2009). Advanced clinical solutions for the WAIS-IV and WMS-IV. San
TX: Pearson.
TX: Pearson.
96