Craft Selected Pages
Craft Selected Pages
Piano Tuning
BY
Daniel Levitan
SUPPLEMENTAL READINGS
Acknowledgments 234
INDEX 245
Introduction
The craft of piano tuning is a collaboration between the ear and the
hand: The ear monitors the changing pitch of a piano string as the
hand adjusts its tension.
A tuner’s ear can detect minute changes in the pitch of a piano
string because it has become sensitive to certain aural sensations
produced by two strings vibrating together, sensations that change a
great deal when the pitch of one of the strings is altered slightly. A
tuner’s hand can accurately and stably control the tension of a piano
string because it has become sensitive to the tactile sensations pro-
duced by the string and by the system through which its tension is
adjusted.
Tuners develop these sensitivities of the ear and hand through
practice: The ear imagines how a change in pitch might improve the
sound of a piano, the hand attempts to make that change, and then
the ear judges the results. By repeating this process over and over a
tuner gradually becomes more expert, as the ear becomes more sen-
sitive to the part of the sound of the string that is affected by pitch
change and the hand becomes more capable of controlling that
sound.
If we have an innate talent for piano tuning, we can become
skilled tuners purely through this kind of repetitive practice, with
very little conscious awareness of what our ear and hand are doing.
But in learning to tune, most people find it useful to develop some
analytical skills. Analysis can not only help us to develop sensitivity in
our ears and hands more quickly and to solve problems more effi-
ciently; it can also help us to examine the practices of other tuners, to
understand why certain techniques are effective, to make our tunings
more consistent and accurate, and to develop new techniques.
The aim of the two long essays that form the heart of this
book—Four Lectures on Basic Aural Piano Tuning and Fundamentals of
1
2 INTRODUCTION
9
10
11
12 the tuner’s ear
Sound wave 1
Sound wave 2
In phase
figure 1.1. When two sound waves of the same frequency combine in phase,
they produce a stronger wave.
Sound wave 1
Sound wave 2
Out of phase
figure 1.2. When two sound waves of the same frequency combine out of
phase, they produce a weaker wave.
Sound wave 1
Sound wave 2
figure 1.3. When two sound waves of slightly different frequencies combine,
they regularly strengthen and weaken each other.
14 THE TUNER ’ S EAR
cycles per second, or Hertz (Hz), and the other has a frequency of
439Hz, then they beat at the rate of 440 minus 439, or 1, beat per
second (bps).
Notice that the more closely matched two frequencies are, the
slower their beat rate. A wave whose frequency is 440Hz beats with
a wave whose frequency is 438Hz at a rate of 2bps, but at a rate of
only 0.5bps with a wave whose frequency is 439.5Hz.
Notice also that a beat rate gives no direct information about
pitch, since any two notes whose frequencies differ by the same
amount beat at the same rate. Counting beats, in other words, is only
useful for determining the pitch of a note if the pitch of the note with
which it is beating is already known.
Beats offer us a way of adjusting the relative pitches of two
notes with tremendous accuracy, much more than is possible
through the use of either perfect or relative pitch. In fact, listening
to beats allows piano tuners to achieve aurally a refinement and con-
sistency in tuning that closely matches, and at the highest levels sur-
passes, tunings achieved by the most sophisticated electronic devices.
Keep in mind, though, that the ability to hear and manipulate
beat rates is a technical, not a musical, skill. Most pianists do not
consciously isolate beat rates, but instead hear them as one compo-
nent of the overall tone quality of a piano. To please pianists, there-
fore, a piano tuner, whose ear has learned to isolate the beat rates of
intervals, must also be able to perceive piano sound as a musician
does—as an integrated whole.
strings half as long as the original, each one of which vibrates twice
as fast as the whole string. The string also divides into thirds, making
three even shorter strings that vibrate three times as fast as the whole
string; into fourths, making four strings that vibrate four times as
fast; and so on.
The effect of this spontaneous division of the string into
smaller segments is that when the string is played not just one but a
whole series of higher frequencies is produced, each higher fre-
quency being an integral multiple of the frequency of the whole
string. Each of these many frequencies produced by the string con-
tributes to its overall sound, and so collectively they are called par-
tial pitches, or partials for short.
While in theory this series of partial pitches is infinite, the par-
tial series of a real piano string does not go on indefinitely. The
higher partials of piano strings have progressively less volume and
sustain than the lower ones, and eventually become so short and weak
as to effectively cease to exist. In any case, the limit of frequencies
that most people can hear is around 20,000Hz, so of course any par-
tials above that frequency can’t be heard and therefore make no con-
tribution to the sound of a string. As a consequence, the treble strings
in a piano have many fewer audible partials than those in the bass. It
is quite easy to hear partials up to the 32nd in the low bass of a piano,
quite difficult to hear partials above the 2nd in the high treble.
STRING A4
2 3 4 5 6
1
VIRTUAL STRINGS
As we have seen, the 2nd partial of a vibrating string is produced by
its spontaneous division in half. Let’s now expand that idea in a way
that will help us later to conceptualize some of the more compli-
cated aspects of tuning theory.
The 2nd partial of the whole string can be thought of as the
virtual 1st partial of the virtual 1/2-strings formed by the division of
the whole string. Similarly, the 4th partial of the whole string can be
thought of as the virtual 2nd partial of the same virtual 1/2-strings,
20 THE TUNER ’ S EAR
virtual
1/2
strings
1 2 3 4
STRING A4
2 4 6 8
1
virtual
1/3
strings
1 2 3
STRING A4
3 6 9
1
since it is an octave above their virtual 1st partial; and the 6th partial
of the whole string can be thought of as the virtual 3rd partial of the
virtual 1/2-strings, since it is a perfect twelfth above their virtual 1st
partial. This alternate way of conceptualizing the partial series of A4
is shown in Figure 1.5.
To put it another way, we can think of the whole string not
only as spontaneously dividing in half; we can also think of the virtual
1/2-strings formed by this division as having a complete virtual partial
series of their own, exactly equivalent in their pitch relationships to the
partial series of the whole string, only an octave higher. This virtual par-
tial series is composed of all the partials of the whole string whose
numbers are multiples of 2.
Now let’s picture the whole string as being divided into three
INTERVALS AND BEATS 21
identical smaller strings, as shown in Figure 1.6. The virtual 1st par-
tial of these virtual 1/3-strings is the 3rd partial of the whole string.
The virtual 2nd partial of the virtual 1/3-strings is the 6th partial of
the whole string, an octave above the 3rd partial of the whole string.
The virtual 3rd partial of the virtual 1/3-strings is the 9th partial of
the whole string, a perfect twelfth above the 3rd partial of the whole
string.
These virtual 1/3-strings, in other words, can also be thought
of as having their own complete virtual partial series, exactly equiva-
lent in their pitch relationships to the partial series of the whole
string, only a perfect twelfth higher. This virtual partial series is
composed of all the partials of the whole string whose numbers are
multiples of 3.
Similarly, we can picture the whole string as being divided into
virtual 1/4-strings, with a virtual partial series composed of all the
partials of the whole string whose numbers are multiples of 4; into
virtual 1/5-strings, with a virtual partial series composed of all the
partials of the whole string whose numbers are multiples of 5; and
into virtual 1/6-strings, with a virtual partial series composed of all
the partials of the whole string that are multiples of 6. To make a
general statement, each partial of the whole string can also be thought of
as the 1st partial of a virtual string having a complete partial series com-
posed of all the partials of the whole string whose numbers are multiples of
the number of the partial.
strings and find the difference between them. We already know the
frequency of the first string: We set it to 440Hz. Now, in the same
way that we were able to derive the frequency of Gଈ4 from A4 by
dividing the frequency of A4 by the twelfth root of two, we can find
out what the frequency of the out-of-tune 1st partial of the second
string would be by dividing the frequency of A4 by one hundredth
that amount, or one cent. That would be the twelve-hundredth root
of two, which is around 1.00058.
Dividing 440Hz by 1.00058, we get 439.74Hz. This should be
the frequency of the 1st partial of the second string. The 1st partial
of the second string, then, should beat with the 1st partial of the
first string at the rate equal to their difference, about 0.25bps.
As we have seen, for a piano tuner this is a rather slow beat, dif-
ficult to hear accurately. However, the 2nd partials of the two strings
should beat twice as fast. Here’s why: Although the 2nd partials are
also one cent apart, their frequencies are double the frequencies of
the 1st partials, and therefore so is their beat rate. The 2nd partial of
the first string should be twice the frequency of its 1st partial—twice
440, or 880, Hertz—and the 2nd partial of the second string should
be twice the frequency of its 1st partial—twice 439.74, or 879.49Hz.
These two 2nd partials, therefore, should beat at double the rate of
the 1st partials, about 0.5bps, which is much easier to hear.
There is a crucial distinction to be made here between cents
and beats. Cents represent a fraction of a semitone: One cent is
always one-hundredth of a semitone, whether we are in the middle
of the piano, the bass, or the treble. The number of beats per second
represented by one cent of difference, however, changes depending
on the frequencies of the notes involved. The higher notes are in
the piano, the higher their frequencies, and therefore the more beats
a cent of difference represents.
Beat rates double when an interval goes up an octave, and halve
when an interval drops an octave. As we have just seen, at 440Hz, one
cent is about a quarter of a beat; while at 880Hz, a cent is about half
a beat. At 1760Hz, an octave higher, a cent and a beat are roughly
equivalent.
To put it another way, one beat per second at 440Hz, a region of
INTERVALS AND BEATS 23
the purpose of creating novel tone colors. In the piano, unisons are
tuned among three strings that are as identical as possible for the
purpose of creating greater volume and richness.
The three strings of a piano unison inevitably have minute dif-
ferences in tone color. These differences come from a variety of
sources, including the density of the hammer felt striking each
string, the precise moment the hammer contacts each string, and
the position of each string on the bridge. In addition, the vibrations
of the three strings influence each other through the bridge. The
large number of higher partials, the long sustain time, and the lack
of vibrato in piano strings mean that these slight variations in tone
will all be audible in the sound of the unison.
The sound of a single piano string is pure and lifeless com-
pared with the sound of a three-stringed unison, which, being much
more complex, helps to give the illusion of life to piano tone. It is
often said that the most important interval in piano tuning is the
unison. This is not just because unisons that have gone out of tune
will be noticed by pianists long before other intervals that have
drifted much more. It is also because the tuning of its unisons deter-
mines to such a great degree the tone quality of a piano.
STRING A4
2 3 4 5 6
1
virtual
1/2
strings
1 2 3 4 5 6
STRING A3
2 4 6 8 10 12
1
octave
partial of A4. If there are no beats between these two partials, we say
that the octave is pure at the 2:1 level. Similarly, the 4th partial of A3
has the same pitch as the 2nd partial of A4. If we eliminate beats
between these two partials, we say that the octave is pure at the 4:2
level. The 6th partial of A3 has the same pitch as the 3rd partial of
A4. If we there are no beats between these two partials, we say that
the octave is pure at the 6:3 level.
Pairs of partial like these, whose pitches are close enough to
each other to have the same pitch name, are called coincident partials.
In the unison, all partials are coincident.
In the octave, every partial in the upper note is coincident with
a partial twice its number in the lower note. All these pairs of coin-
cident partials can be expressed as ratios, such as 4:2, 6:3, 8:4, and so
on. If we remove the common factors from these ratios, they all
reduce down to the ratio of 2:1. This ratio, whose numbers have no
common factors, is the fundamental ratio of the octave.
The fundamental ratio of the octave, 2:1, indicates not only
that the 2:1 level is its lowest level of coincident partials, but also
that an octave is tuned as a unison between the virtual 1/2-strings of
its lower note and the whole string of its upper note.
26 THE TUNER ’ S EAR
figure 1.8. The coincident partials of the P5 A3-E4 and the P12 A3-E5.
STRING A5
2 3
1
virtual
1/3
strings
1 2 3
STRING D4
3 6 9
1
virtual
1/4
strings
1 2 3
STRING A3
4 8 12
1
P4
double octave
1.9. The reason these two intervals are roughly equivalent in their
number of audible coincident partials should now be clear: The vir-
tual 1/4-strings of the lower note form unisons with the virtual
1/3-strings of the upper note of the P4 and the whole string that is
the upper note of the double octave.
The four intervals with the next greatest number of audible
coincident partials, all shown in Figure 1.10, are the M3, with a fun-
damental ratio of 5:4; the M6, with a fundamental ratio of 5:3; the
M10, with a fundamental ratio of 5:2; and the M17, with a funda-
mental ratio of 5:1. The virtual 1/5-strings of the lower note form
unisons with the virtual 1/4-strings of the upper note of the M3, the
virtual 1/3-strings of the upper note of the M6, the virtual
1/2-strings of the upper note of the M10, and the whole string that
is the upper note of the M17.
Finally, the intervals of basic aural tuning with the least num-
ber of audible coincident partials are the m3, with a fundamental
ratio of 6:5, and the P19, with a fundamental ratio of 6:1.
To draw a general conclusion, the lower the first number of the
fundamental ratio of an interval, the greater is its number of audible
coincident partials.
Although the m3 has, along with the P19, the least number of
audible coincident partials of all the intervals of basic aural piano
tuning, it sounds much rougher to the ear than the P19. The reason
is that the ear hears in a m3 the beating of its coincident partials not
only at the 6:5 level, but also at the 7:6 level—in other words, the
beating of the unison formed by the virtual 1/7-strings of the lower
note and the virtual 1/6-strings of the upper note. The 7:6 m3 has a
very rapid beat in equal temperament and contributes a good deal of
its characteristic roughness to the sound of the m3.
STRING C#6
2 3
1
virtual
1/2
strings
1 2 3
STRING C#5
2 4 6
1
virtual
1/3
strings
1 2 3
STRING F#4
3 6 9
1
virtual
1/4
strings
1 2 3
STRING C#4
4 8 12
1
virtual
1/5
strings
1 2 3
STRING A3
5 10 15
1
M3
M6
M10
M17
153
figure 1. The system through which the tension of the speaking length is
adjusted and stabilized.
154
The Speaking Length
When we tune a piano string, we adjust the tension of one small seg-
ment, called the speaking length. The speaking length lies between
the two termination points of the string—a bridge pin and an upper
termination such as an agraffe or capo bar. The vibrations of the
speaking length are transmitted through the bridge to the sound-
board, producing the sound we listen to when we tune.
But we don’t adjust the tension of the speaking length directly.
Instead, we adjust it by manipulating the tension of the front string,
the portion of the string that lies on the near side of the upper
termination.
We manipulate the tension of the front string by manipulating
the tuning pin to which it is attached.
We manipulate the tuning pin with a tuning hammer.
And finally, we manipulate the tuning hammer with our hand.
The process of stably adjusting the tension of the speaking
length by manipulating the front string, tuning pin, and tuning
hammer is generally referred to as setting the pin. It may seem like an
unnecessarily complex and roundabout way to tune a string, but we
should not be too quick to condemn it. It has evolved into its cur-
rent form over the span of several centuries, and it remains the uni-
versal method for tuning piano strings, numerous patents of
alternative systems notwithstanding. Using this system, a skilled
tuner can quickly, reliably, and accurately make a change in the pitch
of A4 of two tenths of a cent, equivalent to one part in 10,000.
Clearly, this system is well suited to its purpose.
155
The Front String
The front string has two functions: to change the tension of the
speaking length, and to hold the speaking length at pitch.
The two hallmarks of expert piano tuning, accuracy and stabil-
ity, depend on our ability to control the front string as it performs
these two functions. Accurate tuning requires accurate manipulation
of the speaking length with the front string, and stable tuning
requires a finely developed sense of what tension of the front string
will stably hold the speaking length at pitch.
Lacking the skill to adjust the speaking length with the front
string, a novice tuner often finds it difficult to tune accurately, and
may, for example, move a tuning pin and its attached front string a
fair amount without making any discernible change in the pitch of
the speaking length, only to find that a very slight additional motion
suddenly changes its pitch a great deal. This is because changes in
the tension of the front string only produce changes in the tension
of the speaking length once they are great enough to overcome the
static friction of the upper termination point of the string. Once this
static friction is overcome, the friction at the upper termination
changes to kinetic, or rolling, friction, which for typical piano
strings and upper terminations is much lower than static friction.
This lower kinetic friction allows the tension of the front string to
pass through to the speaking length more easily, resulting in a sud-
den large change in the pitch of the speaking length.
A novice tuner also frequently finds it difficult to tune stably,
and may, for example, put the speaking length exactly at target pitch
only to find that a few forceful blows on the key throw it wildly out
of tune, even though the tuning pin has not been touched again.
To hold the speaking length stably at pitch, the tension of the
front string must be slightly higher than the tension of the speaking
length. When the speaking length is struck forcefully, a shock wave
156
THE FRONT STRING 157
runs down the string to the upper termination and the tension of
the speaking length increases slightly. The tension of the front
string must be high enough that when the shock wave reaches the
upper termination, the momentary release of static friction there
doesn’t allow the increased tension of the speaking length to pull
some of the front string across the upper termination, thereby flat-
tening the pitch of the speaking length.
On the other hand, the tension of the front string must not be
too much higher than that of the speaking length, or it may pull
the speaking length across the upper termination, sharpening its
pitch. This can happen either spontaneously or as the result of a
forceful blow.
Although in theory there is one ideal tension for any given
front string that will hold a particular speaking length most securely
at pitch, as a practical matter there is a range of tensions for the
front string that hold the speaking length securely. The hand skill of
piano tuning consists to a large extent of determining what that
range is, and then of putting the speaking length at pitch with the
front string in that range.
Of the two hallmarks of good tuning, accuracy and stability,
stability is probably the more important. From a practical stand-
point, it is preferable to leave a string stable but slightly out of tune
than to leave it perfectly in tune but unstable.
The Tuning Pin
The motions of the tuning pin—The axes of a strung tuning pin
158
THE TUNING PIN 159
pin bushings, the tilting axes are higher than they are in a block
without bushings.
Tilting a tuning pin moves it only a limited amount. As the pin
tilts, it flexes almost exclusively in the exposed portion above the pin
block; the wood of the pin block, compressed by the pin, flexes as
well. The more we tilt a pin, the more elastic forces in both it and the
pin block resist further tilting. This means that most of the motion we
produce when we tilt a pin occurs when we first apply force.
Within the ordinary range of forces used in piano tuning, a
tuning pin can be considered to be perfectly elastic: Whether we
flex it by twisting or by tilting, as soon as we release it the pin
immediately springs back to its original, unflexed state.
The same is true of the wood of the pin block: Upon release of
the ordinary tilting forces of tuning, it immediately springs back to
its unflexed state. However, we must take some care in tilting the
pin, since the wood of the pin block can be forced more easily than
the pin past its elastic limit, and be permanently crushed.
Because of its elasticity, the tuning pin is not ordinarily a
source of instability in tuning. The portion of the tuning pin above
the pin block springs into a stable position as soon as we release it.
Nor, I believe, is the portion of the tuning pin gripped by the
pin block unstable. Experiments I have conducted at my workbench
seem to indicate that when a pin is turned and then released, about
three-fourths of the twist in its gripped portion releases. The
remaining twist appears to be quite stable, releasing only when the
pin is moved again.
Two properties of the tuning pin make it a useful way for us to
manipulate the front string.
First, since we can turn a tuning pin to any one of an infinite
number of rotational positions in the pin block, we can set the front
string to any tension we like. For any given tension of the speaking
length, each particular position of the tuning pin directly corre-
sponds to one particular tension of the front string. We take full
advantage of this correspondence when we tune, because in practice
the way that we set the tension of the front string is by setting the
rotational position of the tuning pin.
160 THE TUNER ’ S HAND
Second, since we have two ways of flexing the tuning pin with-
out turning it in the block—twisting and tilting—we have two ways
of temporarily changing the tension of the front string without
repositioning the tuning pin. This increases enormously the facility
with which we can set pins.
In a piano with tuning pins of average tightness, twisting and
tilting usually both produce roughly the same amount of change in
the tension of the front string.
horizontal, and runs along the line of the string. The tuning pin tilts
from side to side around this axis. Tilting the pin around this axis
has the least effect on the tension of the front string.