PredationLect2 - Tagged
PredationLect2 - Tagged
Feeding rate = a N
Prey eaten /
unit time Feeding rate = attack rate * prey density
Prey density
3
Holling, 1959
Type 1 1. Consumption rate is a function of prey
Prey density alone, handling time is not
eaten / important
unit
time
Prey density
2. As prey density increases, handling
Type 2 time of the prey begins to limit
Prey consumption rate, so increases at a
eaten /
unit decreasing rate
time
Prey density
3. As prey density increases, predator
Type 3 searching efficiency increases; at a
Prey certain density handling time limits
eaten / consumption rate
unit
time Based on assumption that feeding rates are not dependent
Prey density on predator density 4
Functional response curves
Type 3 – sigmoidal
5
Type 1 Functional response curves
Prey
eaten / Predator feeding rate
unit time
Attack rate
Prey density
Handling time
Search time
Type 2
Prey
eaten /
How would an inducible defence change curve?
unit time
If multiple predators, how would curve change?
(expressed as prey eaten/time/predator)
Prey density
How would an increase in prey refuges change curve?
Type 3
Prey
eaten /
unit time
Prey density
6
Summary Points
• Functional response curves capture how prey consumption rates depend on an
interaction between prey density and predator behaviour.
• Predators tend to be selective (both positively and negatively), with some showing
ranked preferences (e.g., carnivores) and others showing balanced preferences.
Variety of reasons why a predator may switch from one prey type to another
• When handling time exceeds search time, expect specialist predation, when search
time exceeds handing time, expect generalist. When prey are rare and
environment is unproductive, expect generalist diet, when prey are abundant and
environment is productive, expect specialist.
Food preference
Ranked preference =most valuable of what is available
Balanced preferenceitems
= that provide a mixed and balanced diet
9
Balanced preference (mixed diet)
10
Balanced preference (mixed diet)
Ricklefs & Relyea (2014) Ecology: The Economy of Nature. 7th Edition. W H Freeman and Company. Pp 107 11
Ranked preferences (most valuable)
Predators (stickleback)
of different lengths
14
After Kacelnik A (1984) Central place foraging in starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). I. Patch residence time. J An Ecol 53: 283-299.
Ricklefs & Relyea (2014) Ecology: The Economy of Nature. 7th Edition. W H Freeman and Company. Pp 105
Optimize time spent
Find more food if you travel further
Central place foraging but . . .
………greater exposure to risk
……….…increased energy cost
…………………….increased time
15
After Kacelnik A (1984) Central place foraging in starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). I. Patch residence time. J An Ecol 53: 283-299.
Ricklefs & Relyea (2014) Ecology: The Economy of Nature. 7th Edition. W H Freeman and Company. Pp 105
Optimize time spent
To maximize food acquisition, need to balance travel time plus search time (time
spent finding the food once you arrive at destination)
16
After Kacelnik A (1984) Central place foraging in starlings (Stumus vulgaris). I. Patch residence time. J An Ecol 53: 283-299.
Ricklefs & Relyea (2014) Ecology: The Economy of Nature. 7th Edition. W H Freeman and Company. Pp 105
Optimize time spent
How does the distance you live from the
grocery store affect how much you buy on a
given visit?
Experimental set-up
tables set distances, tubes to deliver mealworms,
released at progressively longer intervals
Ricklefs & Relyea (2014) Ecology: The Economy of Nature. 7th Edition. W H Freeman and Company. Pp 105 17
Risk sensitive foraging
Foraging behaviour that is influenced by the
presence of predators
Gilliam & Fraser (1987) Ecology 68: 1856-1862. Taken from Ricklefs & Relyea pp. 106 18
Predator preferences
Preferences – can be fixed
or can switch
Snails offered two species of mussel Guppies offered fruit fly larvae and tubificids
Predator preferences
Fixed preferences
or switch Why switch?
Different prey occupy different
microhabitats, concentrate on most
profitable habitat
• Predators tend to be selective (both positively and negatively), with some showing
ranked preferences (e.g., carnivores) and others showing balanced preferences.
Variety of reasons why a predator may switch from one prey type to another
• When handling time exceeds search time, expect specialist predation, when search
time exceeds handing time, expect generalist. When prey are rare and environment
is unproductive, expect generalist diet, when prey are abundant and environment is
productive, expect specialist.
22
Predator preferences
Optimal foraging theory
Optimal diet: 2. switch prey
23
Predator preferences
Optimal foraging theory
Energy gained during feeding period fx Time searching + Time handling
Switching
An efficient forager should broaden its diet to include low value
prey when high value prey decrease in abundance
24
Predator preferences
If interested in theory, read Begon et al. 20006 chapter 9
Theory
. . . . A predator will encounter a variety of food when searching
. . . . Specialists will keep searching until find preferred (most profitable)
. . . . Generalist will pursue a large proportion of what they encounter,
regardless of profitability of the prey.
Predictions
• When handling times are short, relative to search time, makes
sense to be a generalist [Insectivorous birds]
• When handling times are long, relative to search time, makes
sense to be a specialist [Lions]
28
Summary Points
• Functional response curves capture how prey consumption rates depend on an
interaction between prey density and predator behaviour.
• Predators tend to be selective (both positively and negatively), with some showing
ranked preferences (e.g., carnivores) and others showing balanced preferences.
Variety of reasons why a predator may switch from one prey type to another
• When handling time exceeds search time, expect specialist predation, when search
time exceeds handing time, expect generalist. When prey are rare and
environment is unproductive, expect generalist diet, when prey are abundant and
environment is productive, expect specialist.
30
References
• Begon et al. (2006) Chapter 10. The population dynamics of predation. In, Ecology.
From Individuals to Ecosystems. Pp. 297-
• Connell (1975) Some mechanisms producing structure in natural communities: a
model and evidence from field experiments. In: Cody & Diamond (Eds). Ecology
and Evolution of Communities, pp. 460-490. Harvard Univ Press, Cambridge, MA
• Hudson et al. (1998) Prevention of population cycles by parasite removal. Science
282: 2256-2258.
• Menge et al. (1986) A test of the Menge-Sutherland model of community
organization in a tropical rocky intertidal food web. Oecologia 71: 75-89.
• Scheffer, M. 1999. Searching explanations of nature in the mirror world of math.
Conservation Ecology 3(2): 11. [online] URL:
http://www.consecol.org/vol3/iss2/art11/
• Sih et al. (1985) Predation, competition and prey communities. A review of field
experiments. Ann Rev Ecol Syst 16: 269-311.
• Stenseth et al. (1997) Regulation in snowshoe hare and Canadian lynx: asymmetric
food web configurations between hare and lynx. PNAS 94 (10): 5147-5152.