Teaching Student-Centered Mathematics
Teaching Student-Centered Mathematics
Teaching Student-Centered Mathematics
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted
in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, without either the
prior written permission of the publisher or a licence permitting restricted copying in the United Kingdom
issued by the Copyright Licensing Agency Ltd, Saffron House, 6–10 Kirby Street, London EC1N 8TS.
All trademarks used herein are the property of their respective owners. The use of any trademark
in this text does not vest in the author or publisher any trademark ownership rights in such
trademarks, nor does the use of such trademarks imply any affiliation with or endorsement of this
book by such owners.
Table of Contents
I
14. Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
John Van de Walle/Lou Ann H. Lovin/Karen H Karp/Jennifer M. Bay Williams 277
15. Building Measurement Concepts
John Van de Walle/Lou Ann H. Lovin/Karen H Karp/Jennifer M. Bay Williams 297
16. Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
John Van de Walle/Lou Ann H. Lovin/Karen H Karp/Jennifer M. Bay Williams 329
17. Helping Children Use Data
John Van de Walle/Lou Ann H. Lovin/Karen H Karp/Jennifer M. Bay Williams 365
Common Core State Standards - Standards for Mathematical Practice
John Van de Walle/Lou Ann H. Lovin/Karen H Karp/Jennifer M. Bay Williams 387
Common Core State Standards - Grades K–2 Critical Content Areas and Overviews
John Van de Walle/Lou Ann H. Lovin/Karen H Karp/Jennifer M. Bay Williams 391
A Guide to the Blackline Masters
John Van de Walle/Lou Ann H. Lovin/Karen H Karp/Jennifer M. Bay Williams 395
Index 403
II
Teaching Mathematics
for Understanding
From Chapter 1 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
1
Teaching Mathematics for Understanding
The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM, 2000) identifies the pro-
cess standards of problem solving, reasoning and proof, representation, communication, and
connections as ways to think about how children should engage in learning the content as
they develop both procedural fluency and conceptual understanding. Children engaged in
the process of problem solving build mathematical knowledge and understanding by grap-
pling with and solving genuine problems, as opposed to completing routine exercises. They
use reasoning and proof to make sense of mathematical tasks and concepts and to develop,
justify, and evaluate mathematical arguments and solutions. Children create and use repre-
sentations (e.g., diagrams, graphs, symbols, and manipulatives) to reason through problems.
They also engage in communication as they explain their ideas and reasoning verbally, in
writing, and through representations. Children develop and use connections between math-
ematical ideas as they learn new mathematical concepts and procedures. They also build con-
nections between mathematics and other disciplines by applying mathematics to real‐world
situations. By engaging in these processes, children learn mathematics by doing mathematics.
Consequently, the process standards should not be taught separately from but in conjunc-
tion with mathematics as ways of learning mathematics.
Adding It Up (National Research Council, 2001), an influential research review on how
children learn mathematics, identifies the following five strands of mathematical proficiency
as indicators that someone understands (and can do) mathematics.
• Conceptual understanding: Comprehension of
mathematical concepts, operations, and relations
Figure 1 • Procedural fluency: Skill in carrying out procedures
Interrelated and intertwined strands of mathematical flexibly, accurately, efficiently, and appropriately
proficiency.
• Strategic competence: Ability to formulate, repre-
sent, and solve mathematical problems
• Adaptive reasoning: Capacity for logical thought,
Conceptual understanding:
comprehension of mathematical reflection, explanation, and justification
concepts, operations, and
Strategic competence: relations • Productive disposition: Habitual inclination to see
ability to formulate, mathematics as sensible, useful, and worthwhile,
represent, and solve
mathematics problems coupled with a belief in diligence and one’s own effi-
Procedural fluency:
skill in carrying out cacy (Reprinted with permission from p. 116 of Add-
procedures flexibly, ing It Up: Helping Children Learn Mathematics, 2001,
accurately, efficiently, by the National Academy of Sciences, Courtesy of
and appropriately
the National Academies Press, Washington, D.C.)
Adaptive reasoning:
capacity for logical This report maintains that the strands of mathematical
Productive disposition:
thought, reflection, habitual inclination to
proficiency are interwoven and interdependent—that is,
explanation, and see mathematics as the development of one strand aids the development of
justification sensible, useful, and others (Figure 1).
worthwhile, coupled
with a belief in diligence Building on the NCTM process standards and the
and one’s own efficacy five strands of mathematical proficiency, the Common
Core State Standards (CCSSO, 2010) outline the fol-
lowing eight Standards for Mathematical Practice as
ways in which children can develop and demonstrate
a deep understanding of and capacity to do math-
ematics. Keep in mind that you, as a teacher, have a
Source: Reprinted with permission from Kilpatrick, J., Swafford, J., &
responsibility to help children develop these practices.
Findell, B. (Eds.), Adding It Up: Helping Children Learn Mathematics.
Copyright 2001 by the National Academy of Sciences. Courtesy of the Here we provide a brief discussion about each mathemat-
National Academies Press, Washington, D.C. ical practice.
2
Teaching Mathematics for Understanding
1. Make sense of problems and persevere in solving them. To make sense of problems,
children need to learn how to analyze the given information, parameters, and relationships
in a problem so that they can understand the situation and identify possible ways to solve
it. Encourage younger students to use concrete materials or bar diagrams to investigate and
solve the problem. Once children learn strategies for making sense of problems, encourage
them to remain committed to solving them. As they learn to monitor and assess their
progress and change course as needed, they will solve the problems they set out to solve!
2. Reason abstractly and quantitatively. This practice involves children reasoning
with quantities and their relationships in problem situations. You can support children’s
development of this practice by helping them create representations that correspond to
the meanings of the quantities and the units involved. When appropriate, children should
also learn to represent and manipulate the situation symbolically. Encourage children to
find connections between the abstract symbols and the representation that illustrates the
quantities and their relationships. For example, when children use drawings to show that
they made 5 bears from 3 red bears and 2 yellow bears, encourage them to connect their
representation to the number sentence 5 = 3 + 2. Ultimately, children should be able to
move flexibly between the symbols and other representations.
3. Construct viable arguments and critique the reasoning of others. This practice
emphasizes the importance of children using mathematical reasoning to justify their ideas
and solutions, including being able to recognize and use counterexamples. Encourage
children to examine each others’ arguments to determine whether they make sense
and to identify ways to clarify or improve the arguments. This practice emphasizes that
mathematics is based on reasoning and should be examined in a community—not carried
out in isolation.
4. Model with mathematics. This practice encourages children to use the mathematics
they know to solve problems in everyday life. For younger students this could mean writing
an addition or a subtraction equation to represent a given situation or using their number
sense to determine whether there are enough plates for all the children in their class. Be
sure to encourage children to determine whether their mathematical results make sense in
the context of the given situation.
5. Use appropriate tools strategically. Children should become familiar with a variety of
problem‐solving tools that can be used to solve a problem and they should learn to choose
which ones are most appropriate for a given situation. For example, second graders should
experience using the following tools for computation: pencil and paper, manipulatives,
calculator, hundreds chart, and a number line. Then in a situation when an estimate is
needed for the sum of 23 and 52, some second graders might consider paper and pencil,
manipulatives, and a calculator as tools that would slow down the process and would select a
hundreds chart to quickly move from 50 down two rows (20 spaces) to get to 70.
6. Attend to precision. In communicating ideas to
others, it is imperative that children learn to be explicit
about their reasoning. For example, they need to be clear
Research suggests that children, in particular girls, may tend
about the meanings of the operations and symbols they use,
to continue to use the same tools because they feel comfort-
to indicate the units involved in a problem, and to clearly
able with the tools and are afraid to take risks (Ambrose,
label the diagrams they provide in their explanations. As
2002). Look for children who tend to use the same tool or
children share their ideas, make this expectation clear and
strategy every time they work on tasks. Encourage all chil-
ask clarifying questions that help make the details of their
dren to take risks and try new tools and strategies.
reasoning more apparent. Teachers can further encourage
3
Teaching Mathematics for Understanding
Constructivism
At the heart of constructivism is the notion that learners are not blank slates but rather
creators (constructors) of their own learning. All people, all of the time, construct or give
meaning to things they think about or perceive. Whether you are listening passively to a
lecture or actively engaging in synthesizing findings in a project, your brain is applying prior
knowledge (existing schemas) to make sense of new information.
Constructing something in the physical world requires tools, materials, and effort. The
tools you use to build understanding are your existing ideas and knowledge. Your materials
might be things you see, hear, or touch, or they might be your own thoughts and ideas. The
effort required to construct knowledge and understanding is reflective thought.
Through reflective thought people connect existing ideas to new information and in
this way modify their existing schemas or background knowledge to incorporate new ideas.
Making these connections can happen in either of two ways—assimilation or accommodation.
4
Teaching Mathematics for Understanding
Assimilation occurs when a new concept “fits” with prior knowledge and the new Figure 2
information expands an existing mental network. Accommodation takes place when
How someone constructs
the new concept does not “fit” with the existing network, thus creating a cogni- a new idea.
tive conflict or state of confusion that causes what theorists call disequilibrium. As an
example, consider what happens when children start learning about numbers and
counting. They make sense of a number by counting a quantity of objects by ones.
With larger numbers, such as two‐digit numbers, they continue to use this approach
to give meaning to the number (assimilation). Eventually, counting large amounts of
objects becomes cumbersome and, at the same time, they are likely learning about
grouping in tens. Over time they begin to view two‐digit numbers differently—
as groups of tens and ones—and they no longer have to count to give a number
meaning (accommodation). It is through the struggle to resolve the disequilibrium
that the brain modifies or replaces the existing schema so that the new concept fits
and makes sense, resulting in a revision of thought and a deepening of the learner’s
understanding.
For an illustration of what it means to construct an idea, consider Figure 2. The
gray and white dots represent ideas, and the lines joining the ideas represent the
logical connections or relationships that develop between ideas. The white dot is an
emerging idea, one that is being constructed. Whatever existing ideas (gray dots) are used in
the construction are connected to the new idea (white dot) because those are the ideas that
give meaning to the new idea. The more existing ideas that are used to give meaning to the
new one, the more connections will be made.
Each child’s unique collection of ideas is connected in different ways. Some ideas are
well understood and well formed (i.e., connected), others less so as they emerge and build
connections. Children’s experiences help them develop connections and ideas about what-
ever they are learning.
Understanding exists along a continuum (Figure 3) from an instrumental understanding—
knowing something by rote or without meaning (Skemp, 1978)—to a relational understanding—
knowing what to do and why. Instrumental understanding, at the left end of the continuum,
shows that ideas (e.g., concepts and procedures) are learned, but in isolation (or nearly so)
to other ideas. Here you find ideas that have been memorized. Due to their isolation, poorly
understood ideas are easily forgotten and are unlikely to be useful for constructing new
ideas. At the right end of the continuum is relational understanding. Relational understand-
ing means that each new concept or procedure (white dot) is not only learned, but is also
connected to many existing ideas (gray dots), so there is a rich set of connections.
A primary goal of teaching for understanding is to help children develop a relational
understanding of mathematical ideas. Because relational understanding develops over time
and becomes more complex as a person makes more connections between ideas, teaching
for this kind of understanding takes time and must be a goal of daily instruction.
Figure 3
Continuum of understanding.
Instrumental Relational
Understanding Understanding
5
Teaching Mathematics for Understanding
Sociocultural Theory
Like constructivism, sociocultural theory not only positions the learner as actively engaged
in seeking meaning during the learning process, but it also suggests that the learner can
be assisted by working with others who are “more knowledgeable.” Sociocultural theory
proposes that learners have their own zone of proximal development, which is a range of
knowledge that may be out of reach for individuals to learn on their own but is accessible if
learners have the support of peers or more knowledgeable others (Vygotsky, 1978). For ex-
ample, when young children are learning to measure length, they do not necessarily recog-
nize the significance of placing measurement units end to end. As children measure objects,
they may leave gaps between units or overlap units. A more knowledgeable person (a peer or
teacher) can draw their attention to this critical idea in measurement.
The best learning for any given child will occur when the conversation of the classroom
is within his or her zone of proximal development. Targeting that zone helps teachers pro-
vide children with the right amount of challenge, while avoiding boredom on the one hand
and anxiety on the other when the challenge is beyond the child’s current capability. Conse-
quently, classroom discussions based on children’s own ideas and solutions to problems are
absolutely “foundational to children’s learning” (Wood & Turner‐Vorbeck, 2001, p. 186).
When Carla was at the zoo, she saw the monkeys eating bananas. She asked the zookeeper
how many bananas the monkeys usually ate in one day. The zookeeper said that yesterday
they ate 36 bananas but today they ate only 25 bananas. How many bananas did the mon-
keys eat in those two days?
6
Teaching Mathematics for Understanding
Some children use counters and count by ones. Some use the hundreds chart or base‐ten
models and others use mental strategies or an open number line. All are expected to use
words and numbers and, if they wish, drawings to show what they did and how they thought
about the problem. After about 20 minutes, the teacher begins a discussion by having chil-
dren share their ideas. As the children report, the teacher records their ideas on the board so
everyone can see them. Sometimes the teacher asks questions to help clarify ideas but makes
no evaluative comments. The teacher asks the children who are listening if they understand
or have any questions to ask the presenters. The following solution strategies are common
in classrooms where children are regularly asked to generate their own approaches.
Avery: I know that 25 and 25 is 50—like two quarters. And 35 is ten more so that is 60.
And then one more is 61.
Teacher: What do you mean when you say “35 is ten more”?
Avery: Well, I used 25 of the 36 and 25 and ten more is 35.
Sasha: I did 30 and 20 is 50 and then 6 + 5 more. Five and five is ten and so 6 + 5 is 11.
And then 50 and 11 is 61.
Juan: I counted on using the hundreds chart. I started at 36 and then I had to go 20
from there and so that was 46 and then 56. And then I went five more: 57, 58,
59, 60, 61.
Marie: I used an open number to help me. I started at 36 and went up 4 to 40. Then I
went up a jump of 20 and then one more to get to 61. (Figure 4)
Teacher: Where is the “25” in your strategy?
Marie: It’s above the jumps. 4 + 20 + 1 is the same as 25.
This vignette illustrates that when children are encouraged to solve a problem in their own
way (using their own particular set of gray dots or ideas), they are able to make sense of their
solution strategies and explain their reasoning. This is evidence of their development of
mathematical proficiency.
During the discussion periods in classes such as this one, ideas
continue to grow. The children may hear and immediately under- Figure 4
stand a clever strategy that they could have used but that did not A child uses an open number line to solve
occur to them. Others may begin to create new ideas to use that 36 + 25 by starting at 36 and then adding 4,
build from thinking about their classmates’ strategies over multiple 20, and 1. The child wrote the numbers on the
discussions. Some in the class may hear excellent ideas from their number line as the numbers move from 36 to 61.
peers that do not make sense to them. These children are simply not
4 20 1
ready or do not have the prerequisite concepts (gray dots) to under-
stand these new ideas. On subsequent days there will be similar op-
portunities for all children to grow at their own pace based on their
36 40 60 61
own understandings.
7
Teaching Mathematics for Understanding
8
Teaching Mathematics for Understanding
number relationships in their addition strategies (taking numbers apart and putting them
together differently). The children in the first classroom are being taught mathematics for
understanding—relational understanding—and are developing the kinds of mathematical
proficiency described earlier.
In the second classroom, the teacher provides one strategy for how to add—the stan-
dard algorithm. Although the standard algorithm is a valid strategy, the entire focus of the
lesson is on the steps and procedures that the teacher has outlined. The teacher solicits no
ideas from individual children about how to combine the numbers and instead is only able
to find out who has and who has not been able to follow directions.
When children have more choice in determining which strategies to use, as in the first
classroom, they can learn more content and make more connections. In addition, if teachers
do not seek out and value children’s ideas, children may come to believe that mathematics
is a body of rules and procedures that are learned by waiting for the teacher to tell them
what to do. This view of mathematics—and what is involved in learning it—is inconsistent
with mathematics as a discipline and with the learning theories described previously. There-
fore, it is a worthwhile goal to transform your classroom into a mathematical community of
learners who interact with each other and with the teacher as they share ideas and results,
compare and evaluate strategies, challenge results, determine the validity of answers, and
negotiate ideas. The rich interaction in such a classroom increases opportunities for pro-
ductive engagement and reflective thinking about relevant mathematical ideas, resulting in
children developing a relational understanding of mathematics.
A second difference between the two classrooms is the learning goals. Both teachers
might write “understand two‐digit addition” as the objective for the day. However, what is
captured in “understand” is very different in each setting. In the first classroom, the teacher’s
goals are for children to connect addition to what they already know and to see that two
numbers can be combined in many different ways. In the second classroom, understanding
is connected to being able to carry out the standard algorithm. The learning goals, and more
specifically, how the teacher interprets the meaning behind the learning goals, affect what
children learned.
These lessons also differ in terms of how accessible they are—and this, in turn, affects
who learns the mathematics. The first lesson is differentiated in that it meets children where
they are in their current understanding. When a task is presented as “solve this in your own
way,” it has multiple entry points, meaning it can be approached in a variety of ways. Con-
sequently, children with different prior knowledge or learning strategies can figure out a
way to solve the problem. This makes the task accessible to more learners. Then, as children
observe strategies that are more efficient than their own, they develop new and better ways
to solve the problem.
In the second classroom, everyone has to do the problem in the same way. Children do
not have the opportunity to apply their own ideas or to see that there are numerous ways
to solve the problem. This may deprive children who need to continue working on the de-
velopment of basic ideas of tens and ones as well as children who could easily find one or
more ways to do the problem if only they were asked to do so. The children in the second
classroom are also likely to use the same method to add all numbers instead of looking for
more efficient ways to add based on the relationships between numbers. For example, they
are likely to add 29 + 29 using the standard algorithm instead of thinking 30 + 30 and then
take away 2. Recall in the discussion of learning theory the importance of building on prior
knowledge and learning from others. Student‐generated strategies, multiple approaches,
and discussion about the problem in the first classroom represent the kinds of strategies that
enhance learning for a range of learners.
Children in both classrooms will eventually succeed at finding sums, but what they
learn about addition—and about doing mathematics—is quite different. Understanding
9
Teaching Mathematics for Understanding
and doing mathematics involves generating strategies for solving problems, applying those
approaches, seeing if they lead to solutions, and checking to see whether answers make
sense. These activities were all present in the first classroom but not in the second. Conse-
quently, children in the first classroom, in addition to successfully finding sums, will develop
richer mathematical understanding, become more flexible thinkers and better problem solv-
ers, remain more engaged in learning, and develop more positive attitudes toward learning
mathematics.
10
Teaching Mathematics for Understanding
1997; Hoffman, Breyfogle, & Dressler, 2009). In particular, notice who is doing the think-
ing, the talking, and the mathematics—the children.
• Children’s ideas are key. Mathematical ideas expressed
by children are important and have the potential to
contribute to everyone’s learning. Learning math-
ematics is about coming to understand the ideas of the Listen carefully to children as they talk about what they are
mathematical community. thinking and doing as they engage in a mathematical task.
If they respond in an unexpected way, try to avoid imposing
• Opportunities for children to talk about mathematics your ideas onto their ideas. Ask clarifying questions to try to
are common. Learning is enhanced when children are make sense of the sense your children are making!
engaged with others who are working on the same
ideas. Encouraging student‐to‐student dialogue can
help children think of themselves as capable of making sense of mathematics. Children
are also more likely to question each other’s ideas than the teacher’s ideas.
• Multiple approaches are encouraged. Children must recognize that there is often a
variety of methods that will lead to a solution. Respect for the ideas shared by others is
critical if real discussion is to take place.
• Mistakes are good opportunities for learning. Children must come to realize that er-
rors provide opportunities for growth as they are uncovered and explained. Trust must
be established with an understanding that it is okay to make mistakes. Without this
trust, many ideas will never be shared.
• Math makes sense. Children must come to understand that mathematics makes sense.
Teachers should resist always evaluating children’s answers. In fact, when teachers rou-
tinely respond with “Yes, that’s correct,” or “No, that’s wrong,” children will stop trying
to make sense of ideas in the classroom and discussion and learning will be curtailed.
To create a climate that encourages mathematics understanding, teachers must first
provide explicit instruction on the ground rules for classroom discussions. Second, teach-
ers may need to model the type of questioning and interaction that they expect from their
children. Direct instruction would be appropriate in such a situation. The crucial point in
teaching for understanding is to highlight and use children’s ideas to promote mathematical
proficiency.
Most people go into teaching because they want to help children learn. It is hard to
think of allowing—much less planning for—the children in your classroom to struggle. Not
showing them a solution when they are experiencing difficulty seems almost counterintui-
tive. If our goal is relational understanding, however, the struggle is part of the learning, and
teaching becomes less about the teacher and more about what the children are doing and
thinking.
Keep in mind that you too are a learner. Some ideas in this text may make more sense
to you than others. Others may even create dissonance for you. Embrace this feeling of
disequilibrium and unease as an opportunity to learn—to revise your perspectives on math-
ematics and on the teaching and learning of mathematics as you deepen your understanding
so that you can help your children deepen theirs.
11
Teaching Mathematics for Understanding
References
Ambrose, R. (2002). Are we overemphasizing should multiple solution methods be introduced?
manipulatives in the primary grades to the detriment Paper presented at the Annual Conference of
of girls? Teaching Children Mathematics, 9(1), 16–21. the American Educational Research Association,
Denver, CO.
Hoffman, B. L., Breyfogle, M. L., & Dressler, J. A.
(2009). The power of incorrect answers. Mathematics Skemp, R. (1978). Relational understanding and
Teaching in the Middle School, 15(4), 232–238. instrumental understanding. Arithmetic Teacher,
26(3), 9–15.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
(2000). Principles and standards for school Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind and society. Cambridge,
mathematics. Reston, VA: Author. MA: Harvard University Press.
National Research Council. (2001). Adding it up: Wiggins, G., & McTighe, J. (2005). Understanding
Helping children learn mathematics. J. Kilpatrick, by design (2nd ed.). Alexandria, VA: Association for
J. Swafford, & B. Findell (Eds.). Washington, DC: Supervision and Curriculum Development.
National Academies Press.
Wood, T., & Turner‐Vorbeck, T. (2001). Extending
Norton, A., & D’Ambrosio, B. S. (2008). ZPC and the conception of mathematics teaching. In T. Wood,
ZPD: Zones of teaching and learning. Journal for B. S. Nelson, & J. Warfield (Eds.), Beyond classical
Research in Mathematics Education, 39(3), 220–246. pedagogy: Teaching elementary school mathematics (pp.
185–208). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Rittle‐Johnson, B., Star, J. R., & Durkin, K. (2010,
April). Developing procedural flexibility: When
12
Teaching Mathematics
through Problem Solving
From Chapter 2 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
13
Teaching Mathematics
through Problem Solving
14
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
problem solving might be described as “upside down” from the traditional approach of
teaching for problem solving because the problem is presented at the beginning of a les-
son and skills and ideas emerge from working with the problem. An example of teaching
through problem solving might have children explore the following situation before they
are taught the basic facts related to five (e.g., 0 + 5; 1 + 4; 2 + 3, and so on).
Tatyana’s mother is decorating a cake for Tatyana’s fifth birthday but she only has green and
blue candles. If she wants to use exactly 5 candles on the cake, how many green and blue
candles could she use?
The teacher would explain to the class that there is more than one correct solution to
this problem and that they are to find as many different solutions as they can. As children
work on the problem, they may use green and blue counters, they may choose to draw the
candles, or they may simply use numbers to capture their ideas.
Through this context and exploration, children could grapple with the commutative prop-
erty of addition as they compare combinations such as 2 (green) + 3 (blue) and 3 (green)
+ 2 (blue). This problem also generates opportunities for children to investigate 0 as they
consider whether they can have 5 green candles and 0 blue candles or vice versa.
Teaching through problem solving requires a paradigm shift, which means that teachers
are doing more than just tweaking a few things about their teaching; they are changing their
philosophy of how they think children learn best and how they can best help them learn.
At first glance, it may seem that the teacher’s role is less demanding because the children
are doing the mathematics, but the teacher’s role is actually more demanding in such class-
rooms. Here are some of the important teacher responsibilities:
• Select high-quality tasks that allow children to learn the content by figuring out their
own strategies and solutions.
• Ask high-quality questions that allow children to verify and relate their strategies.
• Listen to children’s responses and examine their work, determining in the moment how
to extend and formalize their thinking through targeted feedback.
There is no doubt that teaching mathematics through problem solving can be challeng-
ing, but the results are worth the effort! It promises to be a better approach if our ultimate
goal is deep (relational) understanding because teaching through problem solving accom-
plishes these goals:
• Focuses children’s attention on ideas and sense making. When solving problems, children
are necessarily reflecting on the concepts inherent in the problems. Emerging concepts
are more likely to be integrated with existing ones, thereby improving understanding.
• Emphasizes mathematical processes and practices. Children who are solving problems
will engage in all five of the processes of doing mathematics—problem solving, reason-
ing, communication, connections, and representation (NCTM, 2000), as well as the
eight mathematical practices outlined in the Common Core State Standards, resulting in
mathematics that is more accessible, more interesting, and more meaningful. Note that
15
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
the first Standard for Mathematical Practice is “Make sense of problems and persevere
in solving them” (CCSSO, 2010).
• Develops children’s confidence and identities. Every time teachers pose a problem-
based task and expect a solution, they implicitly say to children, “I believe you can do
this.” When children are engaged in problem solving and discourse in which the cor-
rectness of the solution lies in the justification of the process, they begin to see them-
selves as capable of doing mathematics and that mathematics makes sense.
• Provides a context to help children build meaning for the concept. Using a context
facilitates mathematical understanding, especially when the context is grounded in an
experience familiar to children and when the context uses purposeful constraints that
potentially highlight the significant mathematical ideas (Fosnot & Dolk, 2001).
• Allows entry and exit points for a wide range of children. Good problem-based tasks
have multiple paths to the solution, so each child can make sense of and solve the task by
using his or her own ideas. Furthermore, children expand their ideas and grow in their
understanding as they hear, critique, and reflect on the solution strategies of others.
• Allows for extensions and elaborations. Extensions and “what if” questions can moti-
vate advanced learners or quick finishers, resulting in increased learning and enthusiasm
for doing mathematics.
• Engages children so that there are fewer discipline problems. Many discipline issues in
a classroom are the result of children becoming bored, not understanding the teacher
directions, or simply finding little relevance in the task. Most children like to be chal-
lenged and enjoy being permitted to solve problems in ways that make sense to them,
giving them less reason to act out or cause trouble.
• Provides formative assessment data. As children discuss ideas, draw diagrams, or use
manipulatives, defend their solutions and evaluate those of others, and write reports or
explanations, they provide the teacher with a steady stream of valuable information that
can be used to inform subsequent instruction.
• Is a lot of fun! Children enjoy the creative process of problem solving and sharing how
they figured something out. After seeing the surprising and inventive ways that children
think and how engaged children become in mathematics, very few teachers stop using a
teaching-through-problem-solving approach.
Features of a Problem
Problems that can serve as effective tasks or activities for children to solve have common
features. Use the following points as a guide to assess whether a task or an activity has the
potential to be a genuine problem.
• The problem should engage children where they are in their current understanding.
Children should have the appropriate ideas to begin engaging with the problem and to
solve the problem, and yet still find it challenging and interesting.
16
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
• The problematic or engaging aspect of the problem must be a result of the mathemat-
ics that the children are to learn. In solving the problem or doing the activity, children
should be concerned primarily with making sense of and developing their understand-
ing of the mathematics involved. Any context or external constraints used should not
overshadow the mathematics to be learned.
• The problem must require justifications and explanations for answers and methods.
In a high-quality problem, neither the process nor the answer is straightforward, so
justification is central to the task. Children should understand that the responsibility
for determining whether answers are correct and why they are correct rests on their
mathematical reasoning, not on the teacher telling them that they are correct.
Examples of Problems
Problems can be used to develop both concepts and procedures, as well as the connection
between concepts and procedures. In the following examples, the first two problems focus
on concepts and the third problem focuses on a procedure.
CONCEPT: Equality
3 + 6 = 1 + ____
Find a number for the blank so that the equation is true. Is there more than one number
that will make the equation true? Why or why not? Can you find more than one way to find
a number for the blank so that the equation is true?
Note that a task in the form of a story problem does not automatically make the task a
problem. A story problem can be “routine” if children read it and know right away that it is
a subtraction problem and subtract to answer it. Conversely, an equation with no words, as
in the second example above, is not necessarily routine and can actually be a rich problem
to investigate.
The third example, although focused on a procedure, is a problem because children must
figure out how they are going to approach the task (assuming they have not been taught the
standard algorithm at this point). Children are also challenged to find more than one way to
solve the problem. Implicit is the challenge to determine how the two solution strategies are
different. The third example is important because it illustrates that virtually all mathematics—
concepts and procedures—can be taught through problem solving.
17
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
children. In selecting such a task, consider the level of cognitive demand, the potential of the
task to have multiple entry and exit points, and the relevancy of the task to children.
Source: Adapted with permission from Stein, M., Smith, M., Henningsen, E., & Silver, E. (2009). Implementing Standards-Based Mathematics
Instruction: A Case for Professional Development, copyright 2009 by the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. All rights reserved.
18
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
second task requires them to use number sense to generate three reasonable numbers that
will result in a given sum. As a consequence of working on this second task, children have
potential opportunities to think about and use number relationships while they work on
their computational skills for adding.
TASK 1:
(The teacher places snacks on a table.) Do we have enough snacks for everyone in the class?
TASK 2:
(The teacher gives each child a sheet of paper with a picture of the snack item copied in
rows a certain number of times.) Do we have enough snacks for everyone in the class?
Source: Adapted with permission from Young Mathematicians at Work: Constructing Number Sense, Ad-
dition, and Subtraction by Catherine Twomey Fostnot and Maarten Dolk. Copyright © 2001 by Catherine
Twomey Fosnot and Maarten Dolk. Published by Heinemann, Portsmouth, NH. All rights reserved.
19
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
If the snacks are readily available, as in the first task, the children will most likely pass
them out to see if there are enough snacks and will miss any opportunity to think deeply
about the situation. The second task offers more opportunity for children to engage with
the task in a variety of ways, which also offers the teacher more information about each
child’s level of understanding. For example, how children organize their count of the pic-
tured snacks is informative. Do they start at the top and count across the rows? Or do they
haphazardly count and miss or double-count? Do they count by ones? Do they count from
one or do they count on from a recognized amount? Once they know how many snacks are
on the sheet, can they automatically state whether there are enough snacks for everyone? Or
do they need to represent each child in the class, say, with a counter, and match a counter
with a pictured snack? Clearly, the second task offers many more opportunities for all chil-
dren to engage in the task in a variety of ways.
20
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
developing more sophisticated strategies that honor where the children currently are in
their understanding (Fosnot & Dolk, 2001).
Classroom Discussions
The value of student talk throughout a mathematics lesson cannot be overemphasized. As
children describe and evaluate solutions to tasks, share approaches, and make conjectures,
learning will occur in ways that are otherwise unlikely to take place. As they listen to other
children’s ideas, they come to see the varied approaches in how problems can be solved and
see mathematics as something that they can do. Questions such as those that ask children
if they would do it differently next time, which strategy made sense to them (and why), and
what caused problems for them (and how they overcame them) are critical in developing
mathematically proficient children. Orchestrating discourse after children have worked on
problems is particularly important because it is this type of discussion that helps children
connect the problem to more general or formal mathematics and make connections to other
ideas.
Implementing effective discourse in the classroom can be challenging. Finding ways to
encourage children to share their ideas and to engage with others about their ideas is es-
sential to productive discussions. Consider the following research-based recommendations
that can be useful in a whole-class setting, in small groups, and in peer-to-peer discussions
(Chapin, O’Connor, & Anderson, 2009; Rasmussen, Yackel, & King, 2003; Stephan & Whit-
enack, 2003; Wood, Williams, & McNeal, 2006; Yackel & Cobb, 1996).
• Clarify children’s ideas in a variety of ways. You can restate children’s ideas as ques-
tions in order to verify what they did as well as what they meant to confirm what you’ve
heard or observed. You can also apply precise language and make significant ideas more
apparent. Paying attention to children’s ideas sends the message that their ideas are valued
and, therefore, is a key step to encouraging participation of individual children. In addition,
modeling how to ask clarifying questions demonstrates to children that it is okay to be un-
sure and that asking questions is appropriate. It is important to keep in mind that although
you may understand a child’s ideas and reasoning, there may be children in the class who do
not. Therefore, look for opportunities to ask clarifying questions even if you do not need
clarification. You can also ask children to restate someone else’s ideas in their own words in
order to ensure that ideas are stated in a variety of ways and to encourage children to listen
to one another. This strategy of clarification is important for English language learners
(ELLs) because it reinforces language and enhances comprehension.
• Emphasize reasoning. Ask follow-up questions whether the answer is right or wrong
to place an emphasis on the reasoning process. Your role is to understand children’s thinking
(not to lead them to the correct answer and move on). Therefore, follow up with probes to
learn more about their answer and their reasoning. Sometimes you will find that what you
21
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
assumed they were thinking is not correct. Also, if you only follow up on wrong answers,
children quickly figure this out and get nervous when you ask them to explain their thinking.
In addition, move children to more conceptually based explanations when appropriate. For
example, if a child says that he knows 5 + 3 is the same as 3 + 5, ask him (or another child)
to explain why this makes sense. Also ask children what they think of the idea proposed by
another child, or ask if they see a connection between two classmates’ ideas or between a
classmate’s idea and a concept previously discussed.
• Encourage student–student dialogue. You want children to think of themselves as ca-
pable of making sense of mathematics so that they do not always rely on the teacher to ver-
ify the correctness of their ideas. Encouraging student-to-student dialogue can help build
this sense of self. Children are also more likely to question one another’s ideas than the
teacher’s ideas. When children have different solutions, ask them to discuss one another’s
solutions. Or ask someone to rephrase another child’s ideas or to add something further to
someone else’s ideas. Provide opportunities that allow children to share their ideas in small
groups or with a peer. This will ensure that all children are able to participate in sharing
because not all children will be able to share during every whole-class discussion. Before
a whole-class discussion, children can practice their explanations with a peer, which is one
way to support ELLs and other children with special needs during mathematical discus-
sions. Figure 2 offers examples of teacher prompts that can support classroom discussions.
Be sure to explain to children that after they hear a question or a prompt they will have
time to think so that silence in the classroom does not feel uncomfortable. For example,
you can say, “This question is important. Let’s take some time to think about it.” There will
be times when no one responds to your question or prompt. If the situation gets awkward,
make sure children understand the question or prompt, then ask them to talk with a partner
and try the discussion again.
Emphasize Reasoning “Why does it make sense to start with that particular
number?”
“Explain how you know that your answer is
correct.”
“Can you give an example?”
“Do you see a connection between Julio’s idea and
Rhonda’s idea?”
“What if …?”
“Do you agree or disagree with Johanna? Why?”
22
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
23
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
Figure 3
Mathematical understanding can be demonstrated through these different representations
of mathematical ideas. Translations between each can help children develop new concepts
and demonstrate a richer understanding.
Give a context
(real-life
example)
Explain
Create a
meaning in
graph
words
Ways to
demonstrate
mathematical Illustrate with
Display data understanding physical tools
in a table
(manipulatives)
24
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
This will help you gather information about children’s understanding of the idea and
also their understanding of the representations that have been used in the classroom. It
can also be helpful to other children in the classroom who may be struggling with the
idea or the explanation being offered.
• In creating tasks and when facilitating classroom discussions, focus on making con-
nections among the different representations used (and make sure each is understood).
Helping children make these connections is very important to their learning.
Manipulatives
Let’s turn to one kind of representation that is commonly used to support children’s learn-
ing of mathematics: manipulatives or concrete objects. Used wisely, they can be a positive
factor in children’s learning, but just using manipulatives—particularly in a rote manner—
does not ensure children will understand. It is important to consider how manipulatives can
help, or fail to help, children construct mathematical knowledge.
First of all, manipulatives alone have no inherent meaning. A person has to impose
meaning onto them. The manipulative is not the concept. Figure 4 shows three blocks
commonly used to represent ones, tens, and hundreds. If a child is able to identify the
rod as the “ten” piece and the large square block as the “hundred” piece, does this mean
he has constructed the concepts of ten and hundred? No, all you know for sure is that he
has learned the names typically assigned to the manipulatives. The mathematical concept
of a ten is that a ten is the same as ten ones. This concept is the relationship between the
rod and the small cube. This relationship called “ten” must be created by children in their
own minds and imposed on the manipulative or the model used to represent the concept.
For a child who does not yet understand the relationship, the model does not illustrate the
concept for that individual. Through discussions that explicitly focus on the mathematical
concepts over time, the connections between manipula-
tives and related concepts are developed.
It is incorrect to say that a manipulative or object “illustrates”
Second, the most widespread misuse of manipulatives
or shows a concept. Manipulatives can help children visualize
occurs when teachers tell children, “Do exactly as I do.”
the relationships and talk about them, but what they see are
There is a natural temptation to get out the materials and
the manipulatives, not concepts.
show children exactly how to use them. Children mimic the
teacher’s directions, and it may even look as if they under-
stand, but they could be just following what they see.
A rote procedure with a manipulative is still just that, a Figure 4
rote procedure. Objects and names of objects are not the same as
A third and related misuse of manipulatives occurs mathematical ideas and relationships between objects.
when teachers always tell children which manipulative
to use for a given problem. Children need opportuni- Names Models Relationships
ties to choose their own representations to use when
reasoning through a problem (Mathematical Practice Ten “ones”
“One”
5: Use appropriate tools strategically) and when com- is the same as
municating their ideas to others. “Ten” one “ten”
25
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
Figure 5 children are showing 12 with their own drawings, you can
observe whether they understand that each half must be
A kindergartner shows her thinking about
ways to make 5. the same size. Second, manipulatives can sometimes restrict
how children can model a problem, whereas a drawing al-
How many ways can five people be on two stories of a house? lows children to use any strategy they want. Plus, because
children enter school with a limited ability to express their
ideas in writing, a drawing may be the most appropriate way
for them to express their ideas. Figure 5 shows an example
of one kindergartner’s solution for ways to make 5. Children
should eventually be encouraged to connect their drawings
to symbols, but they should not be forced to do so too soon.
Some children will take longer than others to make this
connection and that difference should be honored. Look
for opportunities to use children’s representations during
classroom discussions to help them make sense of the more
abstract mathematical symbols. Furthermore, as children
use different representations to solve a problem, have them compare and contrast the vari-
ous ways.
Representations generated and manipulated through technology can also support
children as they reason about and communicate their mathematical ideas. In particular,
virtual manipulatives often mirror the mental actions you want children to learn better
than physical manipulatives do (Clements & Sarama, 2009). For example, with comput-
erized base-ten blocks, children can break the blocks into ones using a hammer-like tool
(decomposition) or they can glue or lasso ones together to form tens (composition) (see,
for example, www.ejad.best.vwh.net/java/b10blocks/b10blocks.html). These actions can
be done more quickly using a computer than using physical base-ten blocks, leaving
more time for exploration. Plus, virtual manipulatives can help children link manipula-
tives to symbols. For example, at some websites a number representing the computerized
base-ten blocks is displayed and changes as the base-ten blocks change, so children can
see the results of the actions they take on the virtual manipulatives (e.g., go to http://
nlvm.usu.edu/en/nav/category_g_2_t_1.html and select “Base Blocks,” “Base Blocks Ad-
dition,” or “Base Blocks Subtraction”). This dynamic link between these two representa-
tions helps children make sense of their activity as well as the numbers. An added bonus
with technology is that sometimes the language displayed on the computer program can
be changed for ELLs.
But don’t forget about using real objects. Young children are better able to relate to a
context when they work with real objects. Consider the example of the classroom in which
children were ordering seeds to plant in their school garden. Part of the power of using
contexts is to be able to use real objects for children to manipulate, organize, and count in
ways that make sense to them.
26
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
Table 1 Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving Lends Itself to a Three-Phase Structure for Lessons
Before
In the Before phase of the lesson you are preparing children to work on the problem. As you
plan for the Before part of the lesson, analyze the problem you will give to children in order
to anticipate children’s approaches and possible misinterpretations or misconceptions (Wal-
lace, 2007). This can inform the questions you ask in the Before phase of the lesson to clarify
children’s understanding of the problem (i.e., knowing what it means rather than how they
will solve it).
27
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
During
In the During phase of the lesson children explore the problem (alone, with partners, or in
small groups). This is one of two opportunities you will get in the lesson to find out what
the children know, how they think, and how they are approaching the task you have given
them (the other is in the discussion period of the After phase). You want to convey a genuine
interest in what the children are doing and thinking. This is not the time to evaluate or to
tell children how to solve the problem. When asking whether a result or method is correct,
ask children, “How can you decide?” or “Why do you think that might be right?” or “How
can we tell if that makes sense?” Use this time in the During phase to identify different rep-
resentations and strategies children used, interesting solutions, and any misconceptions that
arise that you will highlight and address during the After phase of the lesson.
After
In the After phase of the lesson your children will work as a community of learners, discuss-
ing, justifying, and challenging various solutions to the problem that they have just worked
on. It is critical to plan for and save ample time for this part of the lesson. Twenty minutes
is not at all unreasonable for a good class discussion and sharing of ideas. It is not neces-
sary to wait for every child to finish. Here is where much of the learning will occur as chil-
dren reflect individually and collectively on the ideas they have explored. This is the time
to reinforce precise terminology, definitions, or symbols. After children have shared their
ideas, formalize the main ideas of the lesson, highlighting connections between strategies or
different mathematical ideas.
28
Teaching Mathematics through Problem Solving
References
Breyfogle, M., & Williams, L. (2008/2009). Lesh, R. A., Cramer, K., Doerr, H., Post, T., &
Designing and implementing worthwhile tasks. Zawojewski, J. (2003). Model development sequences.
Teaching Children Mathematics, 15(5), 276–280. In R. A. Lesh & H. Doerr (Eds.), Beyond constructivism:
A models and modeling perspective on mathematics
Carter, S. (2008). Disequilibrium and questioning
teaching, learning, and problem solving (pp. 35–58).
in the primary classroom: Establishing routines
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
that help students learn. Teaching Children
Mathematics, 15(3), 134–137. Pesek, D., & Kirshner, D. (2002). Interference of
instrumental instruction in subsequent relational
CCSSO (Council of Chief State School Officers).
learning. In J. Sowder & B. P. Schappelle (Eds.),
(2010). Common core state standards. Retrieved from
Lessons learned from research (pp. 101–107).
http://corestandards.org
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of
Chapin, S., O’Connor, C., & Anderson, N. (2009). Mathematics.
Classroom discussions: Using math talk to help students
Philipp, R., & Vincent, C. (2003). Reflecting on
learn (2nd ed.). Sausalito, CA: Math Solutions.
learning fractions without understanding. OnMath,
Clement, L. (2004). A model for understanding, 2(7).
using, and connecting representations. Teaching
Rasmussen, C., Yackel, E., & King, K. (2003).
Children Mathematics, 11(2), 97–102.
Social and sociomathematical norms in the
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2009). Learning and mathematics classroom. In H. L. Schoen & R. I.
teaching early math: The learning trajectories approach. Charles (Eds.), Teaching mathematics through problem
New York, NY: Routledge. solving: Grades 6–12 (pp. 143–154). Reston, VA:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Cooper, H. (2007). The battle over homework:
Common ground for administrators, teachers, and parents Smith, M., Hughes, E., Engle, R., & Stein, M.
(3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press. (2009). Orchestrating discussions. Mathematics
Teaching in the Middle School, 14(9), 548–556.
Cramer, K., Wyberg, T., & Leavitt, S. (2008). The
role of representations in fraction addition and Smith, M. S., & Stein, M. K. (1998). Selecting and
subtraction. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle creating mathematical tasks: From research to
School, 13(8), 490–496. practice. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School,
3, 344–350.
Fosnot, C. (2007). The sleepover. Portsmouth, NH:
Heinemann. Stephan, M., & Whitenack, J. (2003). Establishing
classroom social and sociomathematical norms for
Fosnot, C. T., & Dolk, M. (2001). Young mathematicians
problem solving. In F. K. Lester, Jr., & R. I. Charles
at work: Constructing number sense, addition, and
(Eds.), Teaching mathematics through problem solving:
subtraction. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Grades pre‐K–6 (pp. 149–162). Reston, VA: National
Fosnot, C. T., & Jacob, B. (2007). Young Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
mathematicians at work: Constructing algebra.
Wallace, A. (2007). Anticipating student responses
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
to improve problem solving. Mathematics Teaching
Hiebert, J., Carpenter, T. P., Fennema, E., Fuson, in the Middle School, 12(9), 504–511.
K., Wearne, D., Murray, H., Olivier, A., & Human,
Wood, T., Williams, G., & McNeal, B. (2006).
P. (1997). Making sense: Teaching and learning
Children’s mathematical thinking in different
mathematics with understanding. Portsmouth, NH:
classroom cultures. Journal for Research in
Heinemann.
Mathematics Education, 37(3), 222–255.
Jacobs, V. R., & Ambrose, R. C. (2008). Making
Yackel, E., & Cobb, P. (1996). Sociomathematical
the most of story problems. Teaching Children
norms, argumentation, and autonomy in mathematics.
Mathematics, 15(5), 260–266.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27(4),
Kilic, H., Cross, D. I., Ersoz, F. A., Mewborn, D. 458–477.
S., Swanagan, D., & Kim, J. (2010). Techniques
for small‐group discourse. Teaching Children
Mathematics, 16(6), 350–357.
29
This page intentionally left blank
Differentiating Instruction
From Chapter 4 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
31
Differentiating Instruction
All first graders do not learn the same thing in the same way and at
[Differentiation] moves the same rate. In fact, every classroom at every grade level contains
us away from seeing a range of children with varying abilities and backgrounds. Perhaps
the most important work of teachers today is to be able to plan (and
and teaching students as teach) lessons that support and challenge all children to learn impor-
tant mathematics.
a unit toward reflect-
ing on and responding Differentiation and Teaching
to them as individuals. Mathematics through Problem Solving
Sousa and Tomlinson Teachers have for some time embraced the notion that children vary in
(2011, p. 9) reading ability, but the idea that children can and do vary in mathemati-
cal development may be new. Mathematics education research reveals
a great deal of evidence demonstrating that children can vary in their
understanding of specific mathematical ideas. Attending to these differ-
ences in children’s mathematical development is key to differentiating
mathematics instruction for your children.
Interestingly, the problem‐based approach to teaching is the best
way to teach mathematics while attending to the range of children in
your classroom. In a traditional, highly directed lesson, it is often as-
sumed that all children will understand and use the same approach and
the same ideas as determined by the teacher. Children not ready to un-
derstand the ideas presented by the teacher must focus their attention
on following rules or directions without developing a conceptual or re-
lational understanding (Skemp, 1978). This, of course, leads to endless
difficulties and can leave children with misunderstandings or in need
of significant remediation. In contrast, in a problem‐based classroom,
children are expected to approach problems in a variety of ways that
make sense to them, bringing to each problem the skills and ideas that
they own. So, with a problem‐based approach to teaching mathematics,
differentiation is already built in to some degree.
32
Differentiating Instruction
How many dogs are there? How many dogs are there?
(a) (b)
To illustrate, let’s consider a first‐grade classroom in which the teacher provided the
children with a picture of six dogs as shown in Figure 1(a). She asked the children to deter-
mine how many dogs are in the picture and to be ready to explain how they know. Following
are some of the children’s explanations:
Carmen: I counted them and got 6. (Points to each dog and counts by ones.)
Sam: I counted them, too. But I counted by twos. (Puts two fingers over two dogs
at a time and says, “Two, four, six.”)
Edwin: I saw a pattern. I recognized the five, like on a die. I knew one more is six.
Nora: I also saw a pattern. But I saw a group of 3 and 3—that is 6. (See Figure 1(b).)
Some children are still counting by ones while others have begun skip counting or even
recognizing the number of objects without counting. If the teacher had expected all children
to count the dogs by ones, then many of the children may have been using less-efficient
methods than they would have independently used. Also, the cognitive demand of the task
would have been lowered! If the teacher had expected all the children to recognize the num-
ber of objects without counting, then some children may have been confused because they
still need to count objects by ones to determine how many. Instead, the teacher allowed the
children to use their own ideas to determine how many dogs are in the picture. This expec-
tation and the recognition that different children will approach and solve the same problem
in various ways honors children’s varying mathematical development and sets the stage for
differentiated mathematics instruction. In addition, by listening to how different children
approach the task, the teacher has acquired important information that can be used to plan
subsequent instruction that meets a variety of children’s needs.
33
Differentiating Instruction
child in the classroom. Rather, it requires emphasizing three basic ideas (Sousa & Tomlin-
son, 2011):
• Planning lessons around meaningful content, grounded in authenticity
• Recognizing each child’s readiness, interest, and approach to learning
• Connecting content and learners by modifying content, process, product, and the learn-
ing environment
34
Differentiating Instruction
n other
after school, on the weekends, and during the summer; what school subjects they find most
interesting and why) and information about pets, siblings, and extracurricular activities.
Increase your children’s motivation and engagement by using their interests to provide
contexts for the mathematics they are learning. Learning profile surveys or questionnaires
also help children think about what helps them learn and what does not, such as preferring
to work in pairs versus alone, being able to work with background noise, and needing to
process ideas verbally (Figure 2). For younger children, use icons or images on the survey
that they can circle to indicate their choice or conduct a quick informal interview using a
checklist. Teacher observation can also provide valuable insights. By recording children’s
information on index cards, you can quickly refresh your memory by looking through the
cards as you plan lessons. You can also sort the cards to help you create groups based on
interests or learning profiles.
35
Differentiating Instruction
represent data and answer questions based on the data. However, some children may have
a smaller set of data to deal with, or they may be asked to answer given questions about
the data, while others, who are ready for more sophisticated content, are asked to gener-
ate their own questions about the data. An example of a breadth adaptation for the same
objective is to allow children a choice in terms of the kind of data with which to work. For
example, based on their interests, children might choose to work with data pertaining to
sports, books, science, or pets. By working with data from various contexts, children not only
learn something about those contexts but also can begin to see the broader applications of
organizing, representing, and interpreting data.
36
Differentiating Instruction
• Set up learning centers or a tiered lesson (a lesson that offers learners different path-
ways to reach a specific learning goal).
Parallel Tasks
Parallel tasks are two or three tasks that focus on the same big idea but offer different levels
of difficulty. The tasks should be created so that all children can meaningfully participate in
a follow‐up discussion with the whole class. You can assign tasks to children based on their
readiness, or children can choose which task to work on. If they choose a task that is too
difficult, they can always move to another task. Consider how the following parallel tasks
emphasize the big idea of subtraction, but at different levels of difficulty.
TASK 1:
There are 38 second graders on the playground; 22 of them come in for lunch. How many
children are left on the playground?
37
Differentiating Instruction
TASK 2:
There are 108 second graders in our school; 29 of them leave on a field trip. How many sec-
ond graders are left in the school?
Both tasks provide opportunities for children to work with subtraction, but the numbers in
the second task increase the level of difficulty regardless of the strategy a child uses. First,
the numbers in the second task, 29 and 108, are further apart than the numbers in the first
task, 22 and 38. In the second task, if children use a counting‐up strategy to determine the
difference, they will need to move across multiple decades while keeping track of the count.
Plus, crossing over the 100 mark can also be difficult for children. Even if children use the
typical procedure for multidigit subtraction, the second task requires regrouping whereas
the first task does not.
You can facilitate a whole-class discussion by asking questions that are relevant to both
tasks. For example, with respect to the previous two tasks, you could ask the following ques-
tions of the whole class:
• How did you determine how many children were left?
• Some of you indicated that you added to find your answer. Why does adding make
sense?
• Suppose one more child left. How would that change your answer?
• Suppose there had been one more child to begin with. How would that change your
answer?
Although children work on different tasks, because the tasks are focused on the same big
idea, these questions allow them to extend their thinking as they hear others’ strategies and
ideas.
For many problems involving computation, you can simply insert multiple sets of num-
bers to vary the difficulty. In the following problem, children are permitted to select the
first, second, or third number in each set of brackets. Giving a choice increases motivation
and helps children become more self‐directed learners (Bray, 2009; Gilbert & Musu, 2008).
The following parallel tasks for kindergarten focus on the big idea of cardinality or counting
to tell the number of objects (see Figure 3).
TASK 1:
How many circles are there? (Circles are in a scattered configuration.)
TASK 2:
How many stars are there? (Stars are arranged in a rectangular array.)
38
Differentiating Instruction
With the first task, the teacher provides a task for children who are ready Figure 3
to find ways to organize their count. The parallel task still offers an oppor-
Parallel tasks for telling how many
tunity for children to count but provides a built‐in structure that can help objects.
children keep track of their count.
In thinking about how to create parallel tasks, once you have identi- Task 1: How many circles are there?
fied the big idea you wish to focus on, consider how children might dif-
fer in reasoning about that idea. The size of the numbers involved, the
operations children can use, and the type of measurement with which
children are most familiar are just a few differences to consider. Start
with a task from your textbook and then modify it to make it suitable for
a different developmental level. The original task and the modified task Task 2: How many stars are there?
will serve as the parallel tasks offered simultaneously to your children. If
you number the parallel tasks and allow children to choose the task they
will work on, sometimes let the first task be the more difficult task. This
randomness will ensure that children consider both options before they
choose their task.
Open Questions
A question is open when it can be solved in a variety of ways or when it can have different an-
swers. Following are two examples of open questions. Both questions can have different answers
and can also be solved in a variety of ways.
• I measured an object in the classroom and found that it was 8 inches long. What could
the object be?
• The sum of three numbers is 25. What could the three numbers be?
Open questions have a high level of cognitive demand, because children must use more
than recall or do more than merely follow steps in a procedure. As such, there are ample
opportunities for them to approach the problems at their own level, which means open
questions automatically accommodate for student readiness. Consequently, when given an
open question, most children can find something appropriate to contribute, which helps
to increase their confidence in doing mathematics and can inform you of their level of
understanding.
A variety of strategies you can use to create open questions (Small, 2009; Sullivan &
Lilburn, 2002) include the following:
• Give the answer and ask for the problem.
• Replace a number in a given problem with a blank or a question mark.
• Offer two situations or examples and ask for similarities and differences.
• Create a question in which children have to make choices.
The two preceding examples illustrate the first strategy of giving an answer and asking for
the problem. The following are examples that show how to use the other strategies to con-
vert standard questions to open questions.
39
Differentiating Instruction
Facilitating follow‐up discussions is also important when you use open questions. While
children work on an open question, walk around and observe the variety of strategies and
answers children are finding. During this time, plan which children you will ask to share
their ideas during the follow‐up discussion to ensure that multiple strategies and answers
are examined. During the discussion, look for opportunities to help children make connec-
tions between different ideas that are shared. For example, in the preceding task in which
children are finding two numbers that are 10 apart from each other, suppose some children
identified 45 and 55 as their two numbers. One child might explain that to find the two
numbers she used the hundreds chart, started at 45, and then moved down a row to 55. An-
other child might say that he started at 45 and counted 10 more using his fingers. Ask the
class how the strategy of counting on your fingers could be shown on the hundreds chart
or how moving down one row on the hundreds chart is the same as counting 10 on your
fingers. Asking questions that help children build connections can support those who need
additional help and can also challenge children to extend their understanding.
Learning Centers
Sometimes a mathematical concept or topic can be explored by having children work on
different tasks at various classroom locations called learning centers. Children can work on
concepts or topics in learning centers as an initial introduction, as a midway exploration, or
as a follow‐up task that provides practice or allows extension. Because you can decide which
children will be assigned to which centers, you can differentiate the content at each center.
For example, each center can use a different representation of the concept, require children
to use a different approach to solve a problem, or vary in terms of the difficulty of the task
(e.g., different centers can use different numbers that change the level of difficulty).
A good task for a learning center is one that can be repeated multiple times during one
visit. This allows children to remain engaged until you are ready for them to transition to an-
other center or activity. For example, at one center children might play a “game” in which
they take turns covering part of a known number of counters while the other child names
the amount in the covered part. Technology‐enhanced tasks on the computer or interac-
tive whiteboard that can be repeated can provide the focus of a center, but these tasks must
be carefully selected. Among other aspects, you will want to choose technology‐based tasks
that require children to engage in reflective thought. For example, “Hiding Ladybug” in the
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics online resources (www.nctm.org/standards
/content.aspx?id=25009) offers children opportunities to plan and check a path for a ladybug to
take so it can hide under a leaf. They can quickly check to see whether their plan works, make
changes based on the results, and check the revised plan. As they engage in this interactive
40
Differentiating Instruction
environment, they are enhancing their understanding of location and movement in space. Once
they have successfully created a path, they can move the leaf and start the challenge again.
You may want children to work at centers in small groups or individually. Therefore, for
a given topic, you might prepare four to eight different activities (you can also use the same
activity at two different learning centers). However, be sure to keep the centers focused on
the same topic or concept so that you can help children build connections across the cen-
ters. Using centers that focus on a variety of topics will more likely result in a disconnected
learning experience for children.
To ensure greater student success at the centers, review with your whole class any in-
structions you have provided at each center on cards. For younger children or children who
have difficulty reading, provide audio‐recorded directions at each center. If necessary, model
or teach again any necessary skills. After children have had time to work in several centers,
follow up with individual or class discussions to ensure that children are learning the essen-
tial ideas and connections that the centers are meant to elicit.
Tiered Lessons
In a tiered lesson, you set the same learning goals for all children, but different pathways
are provided to reach those learning goals, thereby creating the various tiers. First, you need
to decide which category you wish to tier: content, process, or product. If you are new to
preparing tiered lessons, tier only one category until you become more comfortable with
the process. Once you decide which category to tier, determine the challenge of each of
the defined tiers based on student readiness levels, interests, and learning profiles (Kingore,
2006; Murray & Jorgensen, 2007; Tomlinson, 1999). Murray and Jorgensen (2007) sug-
gest starting by creating three tiers to make the process more manageable: a regular tier or
lesson, an extension tier that provides extra challenge, and a scaffolding tier that provides
more background or support. Once you have this framework, you can design as many tiers
as needed to meet your children’s needs. All tiered experiences should have the following
characteristics (Sousa & Tomlinson, 2011):
• Address the same learning goals
• Require children to use reasoning
• Be equally interesting to children
We have already considered some ways to tier the content Figure 4 Problem‐solving cue sheet.
by using parallel tasks and open questions. However, varying
the degree of challenge is not just about the content. You Ways to help
5+7=
me think about
can also tier lessons using any of the following four aspects the problem
(Kingore, 2006):
Counting chips
• Degree of assistance. If some children need additional
support, you can partner children, provide examples,
Ten-frame
help them brainstorm ideas, or provide a cue sheet (Fig-
ure 4).
• Structure of the task. Some children, such as children
with disabilities, benefit from highly structured tasks. Finger counting
41
Differentiating Instruction
• Complexity of the process. As you think about your learners, ask yourself these ques-
tions: How quickly should I pace this lesson? How many instructions should I give at
one time? How many higher‐level thinking questions are included as part of the task(s)?
Consider how the following original task is modified in the adapted task to change the level
of challenge:
ORIGINAL TASK:
Elliot had 9 toy cars. Sasha came over to play and brought 8 cars. How many cars do Elliot
and Sasha have together? Explain how you know.
The teacher has distributed cubes to children to model the problem and paper and pencil to
illustrate and record how they solved the problem. He asks them to model the problem and
be ready to explain their solution.
ADAPTED TASK:
Elliott had some toy cars. Sasha came over to play and brought her cars. How many cars do
Elliott and Sasha have together? Explain how you know.
The teacher asks children what is happening in this problem, how they might solve the prob-
lem, and what tools might help them solve the problem. Then the teacher distributes task
cards that tell how many cars Elliot and Sasha have. The teacher has varied the difficulty of
the numbers, giving children who are struggling sums that are less than 10 and the more
advanced children sums greater than 20.
Card 1 (easier)
Card 2 (middle)
42
Differentiating Instruction
Card 3 (advanced)
In each case, children must use words, pictures, models, or numbers to show how they fig-
ured out the solution. Various tools are provided (connecting cubes, counters, number line,
and hundreds chart) for children’s use.
You would preassess your children to determine the best ways to use these task cards. One
option is to give children only one card, based on their current academic readiness (e.g., easy
cards to those who have not yet mastered addition of single‐digit numbers). A second option
is to give out cards 1 and 2 based on readiness, then use card 2 as an extension for those who
successfully complete card 1, and card 3 as an extension for those who successfully complete
card 2. In each of these cases, you will need to record at the end of the lesson which children
were able to model and explain the various levels of the problems so that the next lesson can
be planned appropriately. Notice that this tiered lesson addresses both the complexity of the
task (difficulty of different cards) and the process (instructions are broken down by starting
with the no‐numbers scenario).
The following example illustrates how to tier a lesson based on structure. Notice that the
different tasks vary in how open‐ended the work is, yet all tasks focus on the same learning
goal of analyzing and comparing two‐dimensional shapes.
Children are given a collection of two‐dimensional shapes (e.g., squares, a variety of triangles,
nonsquare rectangles, hexagons). Some children may be given collections that have fewer
shapes and fewer varieties of shapes in each category. The tasks are distributed to different
groups based on their learning needs and prior knowledge of two‐dimensional shapes.
• Group A: Explore the set of shapes. For each kind of shape, what do you notice about
the shape? Make a list of the ideas that you think are true for each kind of shape. [open‐
ended]
43
Differentiating Instruction
• Group B: Explore the set of shapes. For each kind of shape, what do you notice about
the number of sides and the number of corners (vertices)? [slightly structured]
• Group C: First, sort the shapes into like shapes. Count the number of sides and the num-
ber of corners (vertices) of each shape. Use the following table to record your findings.
What do you notice about the number of sides and the number of corners (vertices) for
each kind of shape? [most structured]
Triangles
Squares
Rectangles
Hexagons
Flexible Grouping
Allowing children to collaborate on tasks supports and challenges their thinking and in-
creases their opportunities to communicate about mathematics and build understanding. In
addition, many children feel that working in groups improves their confidence, engagement,
and understanding (Nebesniak & Heaton, 2010). Even children who prefer to work alone
need to learn the life skill of collaboration and should be provided opportunities to work
with others.
Determining how to place children in groups is an important decision. Avoid continu-
ally grouping by ability. This kind of grouping, although well‐intentioned, perpetuates low
levels of learning and actually increases the gap between more and less dependent children.
Instead, consider using flexible grouping, in which the size and makeup of small groups vary
in a purposeful and strategic manner (Murray & Jorgensen, 2007). When coupled with the
use of differentiation strategies, flexible grouping gives all children the chance to work suc-
cessfully in groups.
Flexible groups can vary based on children’s readiness, interests, language proficiency,
and learning profiles, as well as the nature of the tasks. For example, sometimes children can
44
Differentiating Instruction
work with a partner because the nature of the task best suits two people working together.
At other times, flexible groups might be created with four children because their assigned
task has enough components or roles to warrant a larger team. Note that it can be effective
to periodically place struggling learners with more capable children who are likely to be
helpful. However, constantly pairing struggling learners with more capable children is not
helpful for either group. The idea behind flexible grouping is that groups can and do easily
change in response to all children’s readiness, interests, and learning profiles and the nature
of the task they will be doing.
Regardless of how you group your children, the first key to successful grouping is indi-
vidual accountability. While the group is working together on a product, individuals must
be able to explain the content, the process, and the product. Second, and equally important,
is building a sense of shared responsibility within a group. At the start of the year, it is im-
portant to engage children in team‐building activities and to set expectations that all group
members will participate in the assigned group task(s) and that all group members will be
responsible for ensuring that the entire group understands the concept.
Reinforcing individual accountability and shared responsibility may create a shift in
your role as the teacher. When a member of a small group asks you a question, pose the
question to the whole group to find out what the other members think. Children will soon
learn that they must use teammates as their first resource and seek teacher help only when
the whole group needs help. Also, when you are observing groups, rather than asking a child
what she is doing, ask another child in the group to explain what the first child is doing.
Having all children participate in the oral report to the whole class also builds individual
accountability. Letting children know that you may call on any member to explain what
the group did is a good way to ensure that all group members understand what they did.
Additionally, having children individually write and record their strategies and solutions is
important. Using these techniques will increase the effectiveness of grouping, which in turn
will help children learn mathematical concepts more successfully.
References
Bray, W. S. (2009). The power of choice. Teaching Kingore, B. (2006, Winter). Tiered instruction:
Children Mathematics, 16(3), 178–184. Beginning the process. Teaching for High Potential, 5–6.
Cassone, J. D. (2009). Differentiating mathematics Murray, M., & Jorgensen, J. (2007). The differentiated
by using task difficulty. In D. Y. White & J. S. Spitzer math classroom: A guide for teachers, K–8. Portsmouth,
(Eds.), Mathematics for every student: Responding NH: Heinemann.
to diversity, grades Pre‐K–5 (pp. 89–98). Reston, VA:
Nebesniak, A. L., & Heaton, R. M. (2010). Student
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
confidence and student involvement. Mathematics
CCSSO (Council of Chief State School Officers). Teaching in the Middle School, 16(2), 97–103.
(2010). Common core state standards. Retrieved from
Skemp, R. (1978). Relational understanding and
http://corestandards.org
instrumental understanding. Arithmetic Teacher,
Gilbert, M. C., & Musu, L. E. (2008). Using 26(3), 9–15.
TARGETTS to create learning environments that
Small, M. (2009). Good questions: Great ways to
support mathematical understanding and adaptive
differentiate mathematics instruction. Reston, VA:
motivation. Teaching Children Mathematics, 15(3),
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
138–143.
45
Differentiating Instruction
Sousa, D., & Tomlinson, C. (2011). Differentiation Tomlinson, C. (1999). Mapping a route towards
and the brain: How neuroscience supports the learner‐ differentiated instruction. Educational Leadership,
friendly classroom. Bloomington, IN: Solution Tree 57(1), 12–16.
Press.
Tomlinson, C. (2003). Fulfilling the promise of
Storeygard, J. (2010). My kids can: Making math the differentiated classroom: Strategies and tools for
accessible to all learners, K–5. Portsmouth, NH: responsive teaching. Alexandria. VA: Association of
Heinemann. Supervision and Curriculum Development.
Sullivan, P., & Lilburn, P. (2002). Good questions for
math teaching: Why ask them and what to ask, K–6.
Sausalito, CA: Math Solutions.
46
Assessing for Learning
I realize how valuable a Assessment That Supports Instruction
well‐designed, research‐ In a problem‐based approach, teachers often ask, “How do I assess?”
based probe can be in The assessment principle in Principles and Standards for School Mathe-
matics (NCTM, 2000) stresses two main ideas: (1) Assessment should
finding evidence of student enhance children’s learning, and (2) assessment is a valuable tool for
making instructional decisions.
understanding. Also how Assessment is not separate from instruction and in fact should in-
clude the critical mathematical practices (CCSSO, 2010) and processes
this awareness of children’s (NCTM, 2000) that occur in the course of effective problem‐based in-
structional approaches. The typical approach of an end‐of‐chapter test
thinking helped me decide of skills may have value but rarely reveals the type of data that can fine‐
what they (students) ac- tune instruction so that it is tailored to improving the performance of in-
dividual children. In fact, Daro, Mosher, and Corcoran (2011) state that
tually knew versus what “the starting point is the mathematics and thinking the student brings
to the lesson, not the deficit of mathematics they do not bring” (p. 48).
I thought they knew. Stiggins (2009) goes further to suggest that children should be informed
partners in understanding their progress in learning and how to enhance
A teacher from the Vermont their growth. They should begin to use their own assessment results to
Mathematics Partnership* move forward as learners as they see that “success is always within reach”
(p. 420). Using carefully selected assessment tasks allows you to integrate
assessment into instruction and make it part of the learning process.
Assessments usually fall into one of two major categories: summa-
tive or formative. A summative assessment is a cumulative evaluation
that may generate a single score, such as an end‐of‐unit test or a stan-
dardized test that is used in your state or school district. Although the
scores are important for schools and teachers, used individually they
often do not help shape teaching decisions on particular topics or iden-
tify misunderstandings that may hinder children’s future growth.
A formative assessment is used to determine the point‐in‐time status
of children’s understanding, to preassess, or to attempt to identify chil-
dren’s naïve understandings or misconceptions so that the information
From Chapter 3 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
47
Assessing for Learning
is interpreted and used to provide feedback and make decisions about the next instructional
steps (Wiliam, 2010). Wiliam (2010) goes on to note three key processes in formative assess-
ment: “1) Establishing where the learners are in their learning, 2) establishing where they are
going and 3) working out how to get there” (p. 45). As Wiliam (2010) states, “To be formative,
assessment must include a recipe for future action” (slide 41).
For example, a formative assessment to see whether first graders can find a missing
addend could be the following word problem: “If Lindy has 6 shells in her collection, how
many more does she need to get 13?” The teacher observes one child taking out connecting
cubes, counting out 6 and then adding more until she reaches 13. Then she goes back and
takes out 6 and counts the remaining cubes, stating “seven.” Another child places her hands
on the table with fingers stretched out and, if observed carefully, shows signs that she is
“counting on” from 6 by pressing 7 fingers down one at a time until she reaches 13. A differ-
ent child just calls out 7 almost immediately. When asked how he arrived at that answer, he
says that 6 + 6 = 12, so 6 + 7 = 13. The information gathered from observing these children
reveals very different “paths” for next steps. This teacher is at the first step in Wiliam’s three
key processes, noting where children are in their learning. Moving into the second step, the
teacher notes that one child should move to more challenging tasks while two children need
to move closer to the standard of using addition and subtraction within 20 to solve a variety
of word problems through more targeted instruction (CCSSO, 2010).
If summative assessment could be described as a digital snapshot, formative assess-
ment is like streaming video. One is a picture of what a child knows captured in a single
moment of time and the other is a moving picture demonstrating the child’s active think-
ing and reasoning. In the following pages, various approaches are presented including
Piaget’s three broad categories of formative assessments: observations, interviews, and tasks
(Piaget, 1976).
Observations
All teachers learn useful bits of information about their children every day. When the three‐
phase lesson format (Before, During, and After phases) is followed, the flow of evidence about
children’s performance increases dramatically, especially in the During and After portions
of lessons. If you have a systematic plan for gathering this information while observing and
listening to children, at least two very valuable results occur. First, information that may have
gone unnoticed is suddenly visible and important. Second, observation data gathered system-
atically can be combined with other data and used in planning lessons, providing feedback to
children, conducting parent conferences, and determining grades.
Depending on the information you are trying to gather, several days to two weeks may
be required to complete a single observation of how every child in a class is progressing on a
standard. Shorter periods of observation will focus on a particular cluster of concepts or skills
or on particular children. Over longer periods, you can note growth in mathematical processes
or practices, such as the development of problem solving, representation, or reasoning. To use
observation effectively, you should take seriously the following maxim: Only try to collect data
on a reasonable number of children in a single class period.
Anecdotal Notes
One system for recording observations is to write short notes either during or immediately
after a lesson in a brief narrative. One possibility is to have a card for each child taped to a
clipboard (see Figure 1). Another option is to write anecdotal notes on an electronic tablet
and store them in a spreadsheet. In either case, focus your observations on approximately
five children a day. The children selected may be members of one or two cooperative groups
or a group previously identified as needing additional support.
48
Assessing for Learning
Checklists
To cut down on writing and to help focus your attention, a checklist duplicated for each
child with several specific processes or content objectives can be devised (see Figure 2). As
you build your checklist, include a place for comments. These comments should focus on
big ideas and conceptual understanding rather than small skills. For example, you will prob-
ably find the comment “is beginning to see how addition facts can be related—such as using
6 + 6 to think about 6 + 16” more helpful than “knows the easy addition facts but not harder
addition problems.”
Another format involves listing all children in a class on a single page (see Figure 3).
Across the top of the page are specific abilities or common misconceptions to look for
(based on learning progressions or trajectories). Pluses and minuses, checks, or codes can be
entered in the grid. A full‐class checklist is more likely to be used for long‐term objectives.
Topics that might be appropriate for this format include mathematical practices and such
skill areas as basic addition and subtraction fact fluency. Dating entries or noting specific
observed performance is also helpful.
Figure 1 Figure 2
Preprinted cards for observation notes can be taped A focused checklist and rubric can be printed for
to a clipboard or folder for quick access. each child.
NAME: Sharon V.
ABOVE AND
COMMENTS
ON TARGET
BEYOND
PLACE VALUE
Connie Rico Skip counts by
tens
Uses appropriate
tools
49
Assessing for Learning
Figure 3
A full‐class observation checklist can be used for long‐term objectives or for several days to cover a short‐term
objective.
George
Beginning to look for
Pam ways to make ten
Maria Use a counting on strategy
3-24-2013 from the highest addend
Questioning
Observations do not have to be silent. Probing into children’s thinking through the use of
questions can provide better data and more insights to inform instruction. As you circulate
around the classroom, your use of questions is one of the most important ways to forma-
tively assess and evaluate children’s understanding in each lesson phase. Keep the following
questions in mind (or on a clipboard, index card, or bookmark) as you move about the class-
room to prompt and probe into children’s thinking:
• What can you tell me about [today’s topic]?
• How can you put the problem in your own words?
• Tell me how your picture fits with the story?
• Was there something in this problem that reminded you of another problem we’ve
done?
• How did you decide what to do?
• How did you decide whether your answer was right?
• Did you try something that didn’t work? How did you figure out that it was not going
to work?
Getting the children used to responding to these questions (as well as getting accustomed to
asking questions about their thinking and the thinking of others) will help prepare them for
the more intensive questioning used in diagnostic interviews.
Diagnostic Interviews
A diagnostic interview uses what we know about children’s cognition to design an assess-
ment (Huff & Goodman, 2007). The interview is usually a one‐on‐one investigation of a
child’s thinking about a particular concept or the processes that are being used to solve
problems. The interview usually lasts from three to ten minutes. The challenge of diag-
nostic interviews is to remember that they are assessment opportunities and not teaching
50
Assessing for Learning
opportunities. It is hard to listen when children are making errors and not respond immedi-
ately. Instead, the interviews are used to listen and probe to discover strengths as well as gaps
in children’s understanding, which will lead to more targeted instruction.
Tasks should be aligned to recent work or your attempts to pinpoint underlying foun-
dational gaps in understanding. Primary diagnostic interviews might include tasks such as
counting a group of objects and writing down the number on paper, or asking children to
solve a missing addend problem such as 4 + n = 12. As the child solves the task, the teacher
is able to watch and listen.
Diagnostic interviews have the potential to provide information that you simply cannot get
in any other way. Think of interviews as a formative assessment tool to be used for only a few
selected children at a time—not for every child in the class. You can briefly interview a single
child while the rest of the class is working on a task. Some teachers work with the child at an
interactive whiteboard and record the whole conversation and written work.
The most obvious reason to consider conducting an interview is that you need more in-
formation concerning a particular child and how he or she is constructing concepts or using
a procedure. In fact, these dialogues can be considered intense error analysis. Remediation
will be more successful if you can pinpoint why a child is having difficulty before you try to
fix the problem.
A second reason for conducting an interview is to gather information to plan your next
instructional steps or to assess the effectiveness of your instruction. In an examination of hun-
dreds of research studies, Hattie (2009) found the feedback that teachers received from children
on what they knew and did not know was critical in improving children’s performance. That is
precisely what diagnostic interviews are designed to do!
For example, are you sure that your children have a good understanding of place value,
or are they just doing the exercises according to rote procedures? Let’s look at an actual
classroom situation.
A teacher was working with a child who was displaying difficulty with calculating
subtraction problems. To get the child to reveal where her thinking was in terms of
what she understood and where some gaps might be, the teacher planned a diag- Figure 4
nostic interview. Using an adaptation of a task (Philipp, Cabral, & Schappell, 2012), A child’s work on a
she showed the child a problem (see Figure 4) and asked the child to talk about diagnostic interview
her thinking as she answered. The child said, “Four from zero is four and five from task.
eight is three, so my answer is 34.” Although base‐ten materials were on the table,
they went untouched. The teacher resisted the temptation to immediately correct
the child, as is necessary in these interviews, and instead probed further by asking
the child if she could show the same problem using the base‐ten materials. Showing
fluency with the values of base‐ten materials, the child took out the correct amounts
and placed them on the table. She used the 54 that she took out as a reference and
touched the other materials (in the group of 80) to show how many she would be
taking away. This time she got 26 as the answer. The confusion was evident but
when asked which answer was correct, she pointed back to her original calculation.
She even redid the algorithm and repeated her mistaken “four from zero is four.”
The child quietly pondered and then pointed again to the answer of 34.
Although this interview revealed that the child had a good grasp of the value of the
base‐ten materials, it also revealed that the child was not seeing the need to regroup and
may not fully understand place‐value concepts. The child also referred to the numbers in
the tens column as “8” and “5” rather than 80 and 50. Notice that the teacher linked the
assessment to the classroom instruction through the use of concrete materials. This connec-
tion provided a way for the child to think about the numbers rather than just the individual
digits. Additionally, the cognitive dissonance caused by the difference in the two numerical
outcomes, one responding to the procedure alone and the other with concrete materials,
51
Assessing for Learning
enabled the possibility for more connected ideas to emerge. Then planning for future in-
struction based on actual evidence could begin.
There is no one right way to plan or structure a diagnostic interview. In fact, flexibility
is a key ingredient. You should, however, have an overall plan that includes an easier task and
a more challenging task in case you have misjudged your starting point. Also, did you notice
that the teacher in the vignette had instructional materials ready for the child to use? Be
sure you have materials available that match those that the child has used during instruction
and that will provide insights into what the child understands.
Begin by asking the child to complete the first task you’ve planned. When the opening
task has been completed, ask the child to explain what was done. “How would you explain
this to a kindergartner (or your younger sister)?” “What does this (point to something on
the paper) stand for?” “Tell me why you did this that way.” You may want to ask, “Can you
show me what you are thinking with the materials?” If a child gets two different answers, as
in the preceding scenario, ask “Why do you think you got two different answers? Which one
do you think is correct? If you tried to do this problem again, which approach would you try
first?” In each case it is important to explore if the child can connect actions using models to
what he or she wrote or explained earlier.
Consider the following suggestions as you implement your diagnostic interview:
• Avoid revealing whether the answer is right or wrong. Often your facial expressions,
tone of voice, or body language can give children clues that the answer they gave is cor-
rect or incorrect. Instead, use a response such as “Can you tell me more?” or “I think
I know what you are thinking.” If a child asks whether the answer is right, you can say
“That’s fine” or “I see what you are doing.”
• Avoid asking leading questions. Comments such as “Are you sure about that?” or “Wait.
Is that what you mean?” may indicate to children that they have made a mistake and
cause them to reconsider their answer. This can hinder your ability to discover what
they know and understand.
• Wait silently for the child to give an answer. Give ample time to allow the child to
think and respond. Only then should you move to rephrase the question or probe for a
better understanding of the child’s thoughts. After the child gives a response (whether it
is accurate or not), wait again! This second wait time is even more important because it
encourages the child to elaborate on his or her initial thought and provide more infor-
mation. Waiting can also provide you with more time to think about the direction you
want the interview to take.
• Remember that you should not interject clues or teach. The temptation to do this is some-
times overwhelming. Watch and listen. Your goal is to use the interview not to teach but to
find out where the child is in terms of conceptual understanding and procedural fluency.
• Let children share their thinking freely without interruption. Encourage children to
use their own words and ways of recording. Interjecting questions or correcting lan-
guage can be distracting to the flow of children’s thinking and explanations.
• Ask children to demonstrate their understanding in multiple ways. For example, ask
“Can you show me that with the materials? Can you draw a picture to help think about
this problem? Can you write a word problem to go with that equation?” or “Can you
explain what you just did?”
The benefits of the diagnostic interview become evident as you plan instruction that capital-
izes on children’s strengths while recognizing possible weaknesses and confusion. Also, un-
like large‐scale testing, you can always ask another question to find out more when children
take an incorrect or unexpected path. These insights are invaluable in moving children to
mathematical proficiency.
52
Assessing for Learning
Tasks
The category of tasks refers to written products and can include performance‐based tasks,
writing (e.g., journal entries, children’s self‐assessments), and tests. Good assessment tasks
for either instructional or formative assessment purposes should permit every child in the
class, regardless of mathematical ability, to demonstrate his or her knowledge, skill, or
understanding.
Problem‐Based Tasks
When problem‐based tasks are used for assessment and evaluation, the intent is to find what
children do know rather than only identifying what they do not know (e.g., he can’t count
and state how many he has—cardinality). The result is a broad description of the ideas and
skills that children possess—for example, “Adam can identify triangles given a selection of
three‐sided figures, but he is challenged to select squares as rectangles when asked to find
rectangles in a large group of four‐sided figures.”
Problem‐based tasks have several critical components that make them good tasks for
assessment, such as the following:
• Focus on a central mathematics concept or skill aligned to valued learning targets
• Stimulate the connection of content a child knows to new content
• Allow for multiple solution methods or approaches using a variety of tools
• Offer opportunities along the way for children to correct themselves
• Confront common misconceptions
• Encourage children to use reasoning and explain their thinking
• Create opportunities for observing children’s use of mathematical processes and practices
• Generate data for instructional decision making as you “listen” to your children’s thinking
Notice that the following examples of performance‐based tasks are not elaborate, yet
when followed by a discussion, each task could engage children for a large part of the les-
son. What mathematical ideas and practices are required to successfully respond to each
of these tasks? Will the task help you determine how well children understand these ideas?
Leila has 6 gumdrops, Darlene has 2, and Melissa has 4. They want to share them equally.
How will they do it? Draw a picture to help explain your answer.
At second grade, the numbers in the “Shares” task should be larger. What additional
concepts would be involved if the task were about sharing cookies and the total number of
cookies, 34, was given?
When you add 7 + 7, you get 14. When you make the first number 1 more and the second
number 1 less, you get the same answer. Does this work any time the numbers are the same?
Does it work when the addends are not the same?
53
Assessing for Learning
Six bowls of cereal are placed at two different tables. Draw a picture to show a way that six
bowls might be placed at two tables. Can you find more than one way? How many ways do
you think there are?
Translation Tasks
One important option is referred to as a translation task. Using the seven representations for
concepts, children are asked to use more than one representation (e.g., words, tools, and
numbers) to demonstrate understanding of a single problem. As children move between
these representations, there is a better chance they will form the concept correctly and inte-
grate it into a rich web of ideas.
So what is a good way of structuring a translation task? Using an adaptation of a tem-
plate for assessing concept mastery from Frayer, Fredrick, and Klauseier (1969) (Figure 5),
children can be given a computational equation and be asked to
• Tell a story to match an equation.
• Illustrate the equation with materials or drawings.
• Draw a picture and write an equation to represent a story.
In particular, children’s ability to communicate how they solved a problem is critical for
responding to open‐response questions on state assessments (Parker & Breyfogle, 2011).
Translation tasks can be used for whole-class lessons or for individual or small-group
diagnosis. For example, second‐grade children may be given an equation such as 36 + 49 = ?
(see Figure 5). Their task could be to draw a model, say, of base‐ten materials, in the “Manip-
ulatives/Illustration” area (younger children could just show a manipulative), describe a real‐
world situation in which that addition is used in the “Real‐World Story or Word Problem”
area, and explain to another person in writing (or scripted or audio recorded by the teacher
for younger children) how they solved the addition in the fourth area marked “Explanation.”
Think about using translation tasks when you want to find out more about children’s
thinking. If children represent ideas in various forms and can explain why these representa-
tions are similar or different, you can use this valuable information to recognize miscon-
ceptions they may have and then identify the type of activity you can provide children to
36 + 49 =
Manipulatives/Illustration Explanation
54
Assessing for Learning
advance their learning. Here are two other options for tasks that can be used with the trans-
lation template to assess children’s understanding. Remember that a translation task may
start off in any quadrant of the template and the children proceed to fill in the other three
sections. Consider the following two “starters.”
• Start with this word problem in the section labeled “Real‐World Story or Word Problem”:
Jack was at the pet store. A group of puppies came over and sat on Jack’s lap. Two of the
puppies jumped off. Now Jack has three puppies on his lap. How many puppies did Jack
have on his lap in the beginning?
Then children create the equation that corresponds with the problem, make a matching
drawing, and explain how they came to their answer in the last of the four sections.
• In the “Manipulatives/Illustration” section show two groups of coins in two circles. In
one circle show an illustration of 2 quarters and 2 pennies (you can use a coin stamp) and in
the other circle show 3 dimes, 1 nickel, and 3 pennies with the question, “How much money
do you have?” Then children should write a corresponding equation to show the combining
of both groups of coins, write a word problem, and in the last section explain to a friend how
to approach this problem.
In some instances, the real value of a task is in what it can reveal about children’s un-
derstanding, which will come primarily through discussion in the After phase of your lesson.
It is important that you help children develop the habit of adding justifications to their an-
swers and listening to and evaluating the explanations of others. Importantly, as illustrated
here, do not always start with the same section—sometimes children can translate one way
but not in reverse.
55
Assessing for Learning
Task‐Specific Rubrics
Task‐specific rubrics include specific statements, also
known as indicators, that describe what performance
looks like at each level of the rubric. In so doing, they
establish criteria for acceptable performance. Initially, when you create a task‐specific ru-
bric, it may be difficult to predict what children’s performances at different levels will or
should look like. One important part of setting performance levels is predicting children’s
common misconceptions or the expected thinking or approaches to the same or similar
problems.
56
Assessing for Learning
Figure 7
With a four‐point rubric, performances are first sorted into two categories. Then each performance is considered
again and assigned to a point on the scale.
4 3 2 1
Excellent: Full Proficient: Substantial Marginal: Partial Unsatisfactory: Little
Accomplishment Accomplishment Accomplishment Accomplishment
Strategy and execution meet Could work to full accomplish- Part of the task is accom- The task is attempted
the content, processes, and ment with minimal feedback. plished, but there is lack of and some mathematical
qualitative demands of the Errors are minor, so teacher evidence of understanding effort is made. There
task. Communication is is confident that understand- or evidence of not under- may be fragments of
judged by effectiveness, not ing is adequate to accomplish standing. Direct input or accomplishment but little
length. May have minor the objective. further teaching is required. or no success.
errors.
Prior to teaching the lesson, write out indicators of “proficient” or “on target” perfor-
mances. This excellent self‐check will ensure that the task is likely to accomplish the purpose
for which you selected it in the first place. Think about how children are likely to approach
the activity. If you find yourself writing performance in-
dicators in terms of the number of correct responses, you
are most likely looking at drill or practice exercises and not
performance‐based tasks for which a rubric is appropriate.
Determining performance indicators is always a subjec- When you return papers, review the indicators with the chil-
tive process based on your professional judgment. Here is dren, including examples of correct answers and success-
one possible set of indicators for the “Shares” task: ful responses. This will help children understand how they
may have done better. Often it is useful to show work from
Level 3: Determines the correct answer or uses an ap-
classmates (anonymously) or from a prior class. Let children
proach that would yield a correct answer if not for minor
decide on the scores for these children. Children need to see
errors. The picture drawn or the explanation does not
models of what a level 4 performance looks like.
fully explain the combining and sharing process.
57
Assessing for Learning
Above and Beyond Level 2: Uses only two numbers in addition instead
Clear understanding. Communi- of three and, therefore, fails to come up with the
cates concept in multiple repre- Sally correct amount to share or adds correctly but does
sentations. Shows evidence of
using idea without prompting. not carry out the division. The child shows some
Latania
evidence of knowledge of addition but explana-
Can partition rectangles and Greg Zal tions and drawings are not properly aligned with the
circles into two, four, and eight
equal shares. Explains that situation.
partitioning the same wholes into
more shares makes smaller shares.
Level 1: Shows some effort but little or no under-
standing of addition or how to make equal shares.
On Target Unexpected methods and solutions happen. Don’t limit your
Understands or is developing Lavant Tanisha
well. Uses designated models.
children by expecting them to demonstrate their understand-
Julie Lee ing only as you thought or hoped they would when there is
Can partition regions into evidence that they are accomplishing your objectives in dif-
George J.B.
equal shares and describes
as “halves” and “fourths.” May ferent ways. Such occurrences can help you revise or refine
Maria John H.
need prompt to compare your rubric for future use.
halves and fourths.
58
Assessing for Learning
language of the rubric can be used informally with your children. “Maggie, the rubric states
that to get a 4 you need to solve the problem using two different methods and explain your
thinking. Is that what you did?”
The rubric scale can also be used in recording observations of student performance. If
you describe the task across the top of a class checklist and list the children’s names down
the left side, then it is useful to record a 1, 2, 3, or 4 next to each name. You may want to
leave space for writing detailed comments for some children so that they can be grouped for
follow‐up instruction according to common misunderstandings.
59
Assessing for Learning
References
CCSSO (Council of Chief State School Officers). assessment for education: Theory and applications (pp.
(2010). Common core state standards. Retrieved from 19–60). New York, NY: Cambridge.
http://corestandards.org
Parker, R., & Breyfogle, L. (2011). Learning
Daro, P., Mosher, F., & Corcoran, T. (2011). to write about mathematics. Teaching Children
Learning trajectories in mathematics: A foundation Mathematics, 18(2), 90–99.
for standards, curriculum assessment and instruction.
Philipp, R., Cabral, C., & Schappell, B. (2012).
Philadelphia, PA: Consortium for Policy Research
IMAP integrating mathematics and pedagogy.
in Education.
New York, NY: Pearson.
Frayer, D. A., Fredrick, W. C., & Klausmeier, H. J.
(April, 1969). A schema for testing the level of concept Piaget, J. (1976). The child’s conception of the world.
mastery (Working Paper No. 16). University of Totowa, NJ: Littlefield, Adams.
Wisconsin Center for Educational Research. Stiggins, R. (2009). Assessment for learning in
Hattie, J. (2009). Visible learning: A synthesis of over upper elementary grades. Phi Delta Kappan, 90(6),
800 meta‐analyses relating to achievement. New York, 419–421.
NY: Routledge. Wiliam, D. (2010). Practical techniques for formative
Huff, K., & Goodman, D. P. (2007). The demand assessment. Presentation given in Boras, Sweden,
for cognitive diagnostic assessment. In J. P. September 2010. Retrieved June 11, 2011, from
Leighton & M. J. Gierl (Eds.), Cognitive diagnostic www.slideshare.net/BLoPP/dylan‐wiliam‐bors‐2010
60
Planning, Teaching, and
Assessing Culturally and
Linguistically Diverse
Children
From Chapter 5 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
61
Planning, Teaching, and
Assessing Culturally and
Linguistically Diverse Children
One of the aims of schools
should be to produce citizens
who treat one another Culturally and Linguistically
Diverse Children
with respect, who value
We are lucky to be in a country in which people from all over the
the contributions of those world bring us rich diversity in cultural practices and languages.
Children’s native languages are an important part of their cultural
with whom they interact heritage, and students also think, communicate, and learn in their
irrespective of race, class, or native languages. Since 1980, the number of school‐aged children
who speak a language other than English at home has risen from
gender, and who act with 4.7 million to 11.2 million (National Center for Education Statistics,
2011).
a sense of social justice. Jo Boaler’s quote captures the essence of the equity principle
from Principles and Standards for School Mathematics: “Excellence in
Boaler (2006, p. 74) mathematics education requires equity—high expectations and strong
support for all students” (National Council of Teachers of Mathemat-
ics, 2000, p. 12). Teaching for equity is much more than providing
children with an equal opportunity to learn mathematics. Attention
to language and culture, two interrelated and critical considerations,
is important in planning, teaching, and assessing children from di-
verse backgrounds. Children who are given instructional tasks that
are well supported and thought provoking—rather than low‐level
tasks with short‐term gains—can reach higher levels of mathematics
proficiency.
2
62
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
Funds of Knowledge
Children from different countries, regions, or experiences, including those who speak dif-
ferent languages, are often viewed as challenges to a teacher or school. Rather, children’s
varied languages and backgrounds should be seen as a resource in teaching (Gutiérrez,
2009). Valuing a person’s cultural background is more than a belief statement; it is a
set of intentional actions that communicates to the child, “I want to know about you, I
want you to see mathematics as part of your life, and I expect that you can do high‐level
mathematics.” In getting to know these children, we access their funds of knowledge (the
essential knowledge or information children use to survive and thrive) (Gonzáles, Moll,
& Amanti, 2005). Instead of teaching English language learners (ELLs) from a deficit
model (lack of knowledge and experience), we can connect their experiences from home
and family to that of the mathematics classroom. The more we enhance learning for
all children, regardless of their places of birth, the more enriched the opportunities for
learning become.
Mathematics as a Language
Mathematics is commonly referred to as a “universal language,” but this is not entirely
true. Conceptual knowledge (e.g., what division is) is universal. Procedures (e.g., how you
add) and symbols are culturally determined and are not universal. Treating mathematics as
universally the same can lead to inequities in the classroom. For example, the subtraction
process varies from country to country in how it is notated and the language that is used
to describe the process. Table 1 illustrates three ways to subtract, along with the countries
that teach that process (Perkins & Flores, 2002; Secada, 1983). Commas and periods are
sometimes used in reverse, for example, 1,400 meaning 1.400 as written in the United
States. Numerals are sometimes written differently. For example, in many countries the
numeral 1 is written more like a 7 with a sharper angle (1), and the 7 has a horizontal line
through it to distinguish it from the one (7 ) (Secada, 1983). If these and other differences
are not recognized, children from other cultures may not understand the symbols and
processes being used in their class and, therefore, not be able to participate and learn.
How we do mathematics is also influenced by culture. For example, mental math-
ematics is highly valued in many countries, whereas in the United States recording every
step is valued. Look again at the three strategies shared in Table 1, but imagine no “tick
marks” recorded and no explanation provided. Could you follow what the first child did?
Each represents different, yet equally efficient and effective, strategies for solving subtrac-
tion problems. The critical equity question, though, is not just whether you can follow an
alternative approach, but how you will respond when you encounter children using such
an approach.
• Will you require children to show their steps (disregarding the way they learned it)?
• Will you ask children to elaborate on how they did it?
• Will you have these children show other children their way of thinking?
The latter two responses communicate to children that you are interested in their way of
knowing mathematics and that there are many ways in which different people and different
cultures approach mathematics. Supporting invented strategies for algorithms is an impor-
tant way to show that you value children as individuals and a good way to gain insights into
useful and interesting culturally influenced strategies.
63
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
Missing Addend Approach (used in France, Vietnam, Thailand, Laos, Mexico, Latin America,
and other places)
Equal Addition Subtraction (used in Latin America and the United States, less common)
52 (Done mentally) I can’t take 7 from 2, but I can add 3 to the 7 to get 0 (0
–17 can be taken away from any number). If I add 3 to the bottom number, I
must also add it to the top number.
5
Note: This is notated for the reader but is often done mentally in actual
52 practice.
–11 0 2 from 5 is 3 (tens). The answer is 35.
35
Negative Numbers to Subtract (used in Eastern Europe, Russia, Latvia, Ukraine, and other
places)
52 7 from 2 is –5.
–17
52 1 from 5 is 4.
–17
–5
5 12 40 minus 5 is 35.
–1 7
4 –5
3 5
64
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
Communicate high Does the content include a balance of procedures and concepts?
expectations. Are children expected to engage in problem solving and generate
their own approaches to problems?
Are connections made between mathematics topics?
Make content relevant. In what ways is the content related to familiar aspects of
children’s lives?
In what ways is prior knowledge elicited or reviewed so that all
children can participate in the lesson?
To what extent are children asked to make connections between
school mathematics and mathematics in their own lives?
How are children’s interests (events, issues, literature, or pop
culture) used to build interest and mathematical meaning?
Communicate the value In what ways are children invited to include their own experiences
of children’s identities. within a lesson?
Are story problems generated from children and teachers? Do
stories reflect the real experiences of children?
Are individual children’s approaches presented and showcased
so that children see their ideas as important to the teacher and
their peers?
Are alternative algorithms shared as a point of excitement and
pride (as appropriate)?
Are multiple modes to demonstrate knowledge (e.g., visuals,
explanations, models) valued?
Model shared power. Are children (rather than just the teacher) justifying the
correctness of solutions?
Are children invited to (expected to) engage in whole‐class
discussions in which children share ideas and respond to each
other’s ideas?
In what ways are roles assigned so that all children feel that they
contribute to and learn from other members of the class?
Are children given a choice in how they solve a problem? In how
they demonstrate knowledge of the concept?
65
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
and stay focused on those big ideas. For example, a critical area in grades 1 and 2 is addition,
which includes moving between stories and equations. The stories can be carefully selected
to use contexts that are familiar to ELLs and that lend themselves to using visuals (such as
cars). Rather than having a new context for every story, the stories can focus on the same
theme (and connect to the English that children are learning in their ESL instruction, if
possible). This provides a context for the mathematics without adding unnecessary linguistic
demands. The teacher can incorporate opportunities for children to share their approaches
to adding and illustrate (with the visuals) how they thought about it. In this way, ELLs are
able to learn the important content and engage in classroom discourse.
Mathematical Connections. Helping children see that mathematical ideas are inter-
related will fill in or deepen their understanding of and connections to previously taught con-
tent. For example, consider the following first‐grade problem:
Edwin has some trains. He gives 2 to Marta. Edwin now has 6 trains. How many did Edwin
have before he gave some away?
You may recognize that this task connects addition and subtraction, and that the initial value
(how many trains Edwin had before he gave any away) is the unknown amount. Although the
mathematics is already presented in a conceptual and meaningful manner, it is important to con-
nect addition and subtraction, as well as to connect the symbols to the situation. For example,
one child might use a think-addition approach: “I know that he has 6 now and plus the 2 from
Marta means he had 8.” Another child might think: “I thought ‘what minus 2 is 6 and I know
that is 8.” The symbols for each child’s thinking are 6 + 2 = ? and ? - 2 = 6, respectively.
Having children connect the symbols back to their thinking and to the story helps build strong
mathematical connections and understanding of addition and subtraction.
Context Connections. Making content relevant is also about contexts. If the “trains”
context is familiar, it can ground children’s thinking so they can focus on reasoning about
the mathematical relationships. Using problems that connect children to developmentally
appropriate social or peer connections is one way to contextualize learning. Another is to
make connections to historical or cultural contexts. Seeing mathematics from various cul-
tures provides opportunities for children to “put faces” on mathematical contributions. For
example, you can introduce the Mayan place‐value system as a way to think about how we
write numerals (grade K) and to think about our place‐value (base‐ten) system (grade 2).
You can also have children create freedom quilts, which tell stories about the Underground
Railroad (Neumann, 2005), or other geometric art patterns
can be used to develop standards content such as partition-
ing circles or rectangles into halves and fourths (grade 1) or
Teachers can adapt the way children consider each other’s into halves, fourths, and thirds (grade 2).
ideas so that all members of the class feel comfortable
participating in a discussion by asking specific prompts such Incorporate Children’s Identities
as, “Which strategy would you use if the numbers were more
Incorporating children’s identities in the mathematics
difficult? Why? Talk to your partner and then we will share as
they do overlaps with the previous category, but it mer-
a whole class.”
its its own discussion. Children should see themselves in
66
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
mathematics and see that mathematics is a part of their culture. The classroom environment
should incorporate children’s cultural practices. For example, children from some countries
may not feel comfortable challenging an approach used by other children in a classroom
discussion.
Both researchers and teachers have found that telling stories about their own lives, or
asking children to tell stories, makes the mathematics relevant to children and can raise
student achievement (Turner, Celedón‐Pattichis, Marshall, & Tennison, 2009). Table 3 pro-
vides ideas for making mathematics relevant to a child’s home and community.
The following teacher’s story illustrates one way to incorporate family history and
culture by reading The Hundred Penny Box (Mathis, 1986). In Mathis’s story, a 100‐year‐
old woman remembers an important event in every year of her life as she turns over each
of her 100 pennies. Each penny is more than a piece of money; it is a “memory trigger”
for her life.
Taking a cue from the book, I asked each child to collect one penny from each year
they were alive starting from the year of their birth and not missing a year. Children
were encouraged to bring in additional pennies their classmates might need. Then
the children consulted with family members to create a penny time line of impor-
tant events in their lives. Using information gathered at home, they started with the
year they were born, listing their birthday and then recording first steps, accidents,
vacations, pets, births of siblings, and so on.
Children in grades K–2 can prepare a time line of their key events too, determining when
between 0 (i.e., the day they were born) and their current age a memorable event happened.
The number line is an important model to use in counting, adding, and subtracting, and
this context helps children better understand the number line, which is abstract and more
challenging than using set models (e.g., counters). For example, you can ask children “How
Grocery store or marketplace Cost of an item of which they bought more than one
(repeated addition or multiplication)
Cost of an item that came with a quantity (e.g., dozen
eggs) (division)
Better buy of two different‐sized items (division)
Shapes of different containers (geometry)
Different types/brands of foods they select, such as what
kind of bread (data)
67
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
many years between [these two events]?” and “How many years ago was [this event]?” These
questions focus on subtraction as difference, rather than as take‐away, an important and
underemphasized subtraction situation.
68
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
Table 4 Reflective Questions for Planning and Teaching Mathematics Lessons for ELLs
Mathematics Content
Process Considerations Language Considerations
1. Determine the • What mathematical concepts • What language objectives might I add (e.g., include
mathematics. (aligned to grade-level reading, writing, speaking, and listening)?
standards) am I teaching? • What visuals or words will I use to communicate
• What child-friendly learning the content and language objectives?
objectives will I post?
• How does this mathematics
concept connect to other
concepts children have learned?
2. Consider children’s • How can I connect the content to be • What context or models might I select that are a good
needs. taught to content that children have match to children’s social/cultural backgrounds and previ-
learned? Or how will I fill in the gaps ously learned vocabulary?
if children don’t have prerequisite
content needed for the lesson?
3. Select, design, or • What task can I use that • What context might I use that is meaningful
adapt a task. addresses the content identified to children’s cultures and backgrounds?
in item 1 and the needs of my • What language pitfalls does the task have? Which of these
children identified in item 2? will I eliminate and which of these need explicit attention?
• How might I adapt a task so that • Which words or phrases, even if familiar to children, take on
it has multiple entry and exit new meaning in a mathematics context (e.g., homonyms,
points (i.e., is challenging and homophones, and words such as mean, similar, find)?
accessible to a range of children)?
1. Introduce the task • How will I introduce the task in a • How can I connect the task to children’s
(the Before phase). way that elicits prior mathematics experiences and to familiar contexts?
knowledge needed for the task? • What key vocabulary do I want to introduce so that
• Is a similar task needed to build the words will be used throughout the lesson?
background related to the (Post key vocabulary in a prominent location.)
content (or would such a preview • What visuals and real objects can I use that
take away from the purpose bring meaning to the selected task?
or challenge of the task)? • How can I present the task in visual,
written, and oral formats?
• How will I be sure that children understand
what they are to do in the During phase?
2. Work on the task • What hints or assists might I • Have I grouped children for both
(the During phase). give as children work to help academic and language support?
them focus without taking • Have I encouraged children to draw pictures,
away from their thinking? make diagrams, and/or use manipulatives?
• What extensions or challenges • Have I used strategies to reduce the linguistic demands
will I offer for children who without hindering the problem solving (e.g., using a
successfully solve the task? graphic organizer, sentence starters such as “I solved the
• What questions will I pose to problem by . . . ,” recording tables, and concept maps)?
push the mathematics identified
in the learning goals?
(continued )
69
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
Table 4 (Continued )
Mathematics Content
Process Considerations Language Considerations
3. Debrief and discuss • How will children report • What ways can I maximize language use in
the task and the their findings? nonthreatening ways (e.g., think‐pair‐share)?
mathematics (the • How will I format the • How can I encourage and reinforce different
After phase). discussion of the task? formats (multiple exit points) for demonstrating
• What questions will I pose to understanding of the lesson content?
push the mathematics identified • How might I provide advance notice, language
in the learning goals? support, or rehearsal to ELLs so that they will
be comfortable speaking to their peers?
• Am I using appropriate “wait time”?
Formative Assessment
Throughout lesson • What questions will I ask during • What words will I use in my questions to be
and unit the lesson or what will I look sure the questions are understood? How might
for in the children’s work as I use a translator to assist in assessing?
evidence of learning the objectives • If a child is not succeeding, how might I diagnose whether
(During and After phases)? the problem is with language, content, or both?
• What follow‐up might I provide to • What accommodations can I provide to be sure
children who are not demonstrating I am accessing what the children know?
understanding of the mathematics?
70
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
expectations are explicitly included, children will know that they will be responsible for
reaching certain language goals alongside mathematical goals and will be more likely to
attempt to learn those skills or words. Here are two examples of dual objectives:
1. Children will determine the defining attributes of a triangle (mathematics).
2. Children will describe in writing and orally characteristics that are true for any
triangle (e.g., three angles) and which characteristics do not define a triangle
(e.g., color, size) (language and mathematics).
MacKenna and Sydney pick red and yellow apples from the apple orchard. Sydney picks 5
apples to take home. How many of each kind of apple might she take?
In order for children to engage in this task, the contextual terms pick, apples, and
orchard must be understood. The phrase how many of each must be understood, as it may
not be clear that this means to have some apples of each color so that the total is five
apples. In first and second grades, children could be asked to list all the combinations
in a table.
Lesson Considerations
Support for academic language use is a significant part of lesson considerations. In addition,
facilitating discourse that provides access to ELLs is critical. This includes efforts to ensure
71
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
ELLs understand and have the background for engaging in the focus task(s) and the need to
put structures in place for participation throughout the lesson.
Build Background
Similar to building on prior knowledge, building background also takes into consideration
native language and culture, as well as content (Echevarria, Vogt, & Short, 2008). If possible,
use appropriate visuals and context to help children understand the problem you want them
to solve. This nonthreatening, engaging activity helps children make connections between
what they have learned and what they need to learn.
Some aspects of English and mathematics are particularly challenging to ELLs (Whit-
eford, 2009/2010). For example, teen numbers sound a lot like their decade number—if you
say sixteen and sixty out loud, you can hear how similar they are. Emphasizing the n sound
helps ELLs hear the difference. For example, in a lesson on the teen numbers and place
value, a teacher may have children partner and have one say the place‐value words and the
partner say the actual teen name (emphasizing the n):
Partner A: Ten and four
Partner B: Fourteen
And the teacher can help children see the connection between ten and teen.
Remember, too, that the U.S. measurement system may be unfamiliar to ELLs. When
encountering content that may be unfamiliar or difficult for ELLs, devote additional time to
build background so that children can engage in the mathematical tasks without also having
to navigate language and background knowledge.
NOT MODIFIED:
You have a worksheet in front of you that I just gave out. For every story problem, I want you to
draw a picture and write an equation. You will be working with your partner, but each of you
needs to record your answers on your own paper. If you get stuck on a problem, raise your hand.
MODIFIED:
Please look at your paper. (Holds paper and points to the first story, which now has a picture
next to it). Let’s read together. (Everyone reads.) What is this story about? (Waits) What
words tell us what the story is about (points to story itself)? (Notes context‐related words
on board, as well as words that indicate the action in the problem, so they can be used for
reference.) Talk to your partner about how to find the answer. (Points to mouth and then to
a pair of children as she says this.) Write your answers. (Makes a writing motion over paper.)
Notice that four things have been done differently in the teacher talk: Sentences have been
shortened, confusing words have been removed, attention is drawn to making meaning
of the first example, and related gestures and motions have been added to the oral direc-
tives. Also notice the visuals that were added and the wait time the teacher gives. It is very
72
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
important to provide extra time after posing a question or giving instructions to allow ELLs
time to translate, make sense of the request, and then participate.
Another way to provide comprehensible input is to use a variety of tools to help chil-
dren visualize and understand what is verbalized. In the preceding example, the teacher
models the instructions and adds pictures to the stories. Real objects, manipulatives, and
drawings that fit the context can be used to bring meaning to the problem and are important
in helping the child think through and solve the problem. Effective tools include manipula-
tives, real objects, pictures, visuals, multimedia, demonstrations, and children’s books (Eche-
varria, Vogt, & Short, 2008). Children should be expected to include various representations
of their understandings, such as drawing and writing to explain what they have done. Doing
so helps to develop children’s understanding and language, and also gives the teacher a bet-
ter idea of what they do and do not understand.
There are strategies you can use in classroom discourse that help ELLs understand and
participate. As described in the preceding quote, the use of gestures and visuals is critical to
learning English and mathematics. For example, revoicing is a research‐based strategy that
helps ELLs hear an idea more than once and hear it restated with the appropriate language
applied to concepts. But, because ELLs cannot always explain their ideas fully, don’t rush to
call on someone else; instead, patiently press for details. Pressing for details is not done just
so the teacher can decide whether the idea makes sense; it also allows other children to make
sense of the idea (Maldonado, Turner, Dominguez, & Empson, 2009). Because practicing
language is important for ELLs, offering opportunities for children to practice phrases or
words through pair‐share or choral response also is effec-
tive. Finally, children from other countries often solve or il-
lustrate problems differently, so allowing time for them to Making the strategies of ELLs public and connecting these
explain or show their way can involve students in discourse strategies to others is interesting and supports the learning
while introducing a novel approach to other children. of all children, while building the confidence of ELLs.
73
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
There are a number of strategies in the vignette that provide support for ELLs: rec-
ognizing the potential language support for measure and later for foot, as well as lack of
familiarity with measuring, which was supported by having an opportunity to measure prior
to hearing the story (building background). Ms. Cruz has children saying in full sentences
whose thumb is longer and shorter. She employs the think‐pair‐share technique, concrete
models (the thumb cut‐out), the story, and the children’s own measures to build meaning
and scaffold to measuring with a foot. Most importantly, Ms. Cruz did not diminish the chal-
lenge of the task with these strategies. If she had altered the task, for example, not expecting
the ELLs to explain why their measures turned out differently or what they predicted would
happen in the story, they would not be learning what they needed to learn. Conversely, if she
74
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
had simply asked children to begin measuring in feet, she might have kept her expectations
high but failed to provide the support that would enable her children to succeed.
Assessment Considerations
Throughout the discussion of strategies for supporting ELLs are opportunities for assess-
ment. Formative assessment is embedded in instruction and informs instructional decisions.
For example, the use of visuals and gestures is important in helping ELLs to
• Understand the instructions and mathematical ideas (comprehensible input)
• Participate in the lesson (small groups or discussion)
• Communicate their own understanding (formative assessment)
The use of native language is also important for assessment. Research shows that ELLs
perform better when a test is given in their native language (Robinson, 2010). If a teacher
wants to understand what a child knows about mathematics, then the child should be able to
communicate that understanding in a way that is best for the child, even if the teacher may
need a translation.
Several strategies can assist teachers in using formative assessments with ELLs, includ-
ing tasks with multiple entry and exit points, diagnostic interviews, tasks that limit linguistic
load, accommodations, and self‐assessments.
If you have 2 dimes and 3 pennies, how many cents do you have?
There are numerous reasons children might have struggled with this problem, includ-
ing a lack of understanding the mathematical concept of place value or the values of the coins.
Or it could be due to vocabulary, such as not knowing what the word cents means. It also
75
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
could be because of the sentence structure. Diagnostic interviews have found that the word
if, and the implied “If . . . then” sentence structure common to mathematics but not in other
reading, can prevent children from comprehending what the sentence is asking (a challenge
for native English speakers as well) (Fernandez, Anhalt, & Civil, 2009). The fact that there
are many possible reasons why a child might not be able to solve a task, some related to lan-
guage and others to mathematics, is a strong argument for using diagnostic interviews. If we
misdiagnose the reason for a child’s struggles, our interventions will be misguided.
Diagnostic interviews also can be used prior to instruction in order to assess the math-
ematical and language needs of children. Hearing an English language learner’s interpre-
tation of a problem and seeing how the child approaches the problem provide valuable
insights that you can incorporate into your planning and teaching. For example, a child
might say “Forty‐seven from one hundred eighty” rather than “One hundred eighty minus
forty‐seven,” which indicates the way the child talks about subtraction in his or her home.
Using and connecting both ways of talking about subtraction will strengthen everyone’s
understanding.
Jacob has 9 comic books. For his birthday, his grandpa gives him some more for his collec-
tion. If he has 13 now, how many did he get for this birthday?
Jacob has 9 comic books. Jacob’s grandpa gave Jacob some more comic books. If he has 13
comic books now, how many comic books did Jacob get from his grandpa?
Notice that it is not only reducing unnecessary language (e.g., birthday), but also adding
specific referents that make the meaning of the story more clear. Of course, this particular
problem could be adapted further by using illustrations or actual comic books.
Another way to reduce the linguistic load is to pick a context and stay with it for an en-
tire lesson or series of lessons. This allows children to focus their thinking on the mathemat-
ics without getting bogged down in the various contexts that might be on an assessment. For
example, children need opportunities to interpret story problems in which different parts
are missing (the starting part, the part being added, and the whole). In this example, the part
being added is what was missing. A collection of stories can be created with different parts
missing (and different values used). Using the comic book context but changing the problem
type reduces the linguistic load while keeping the mathematical challenge high.
Provide Accommodations
In assessing, providing accommodations refers to strategies for making sure that the as-
sessment itself is accessible to children. This might mean being able to hear the question
(children often can understand spoken English better than written English), shortening the
assessment, or extending the time (Kersaint, Thompson, & Petkova, 2009). In addition, you
can provide sentence starters so that the child knows what type of response you want. For
example, “My equation fits the story because. . . .”
76
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
Incorporate Self‐Assessment
It may take time to help children learn what it means to self‐assess, but creating a list of con-
tent from a unit and asking children to rate how well they know it can be one way to gather
information on what children know. Similarly, after a lesson or problem, children can rate
or describe how hard they thought the lesson or problem was (and why). This is valuable
not only for you in the formative assessment process but also for children as they learn to
self‐monitor and look for ways to measure their own improvement.
References
Averill, R., Anderson, D., Easton, H., Te Maro, P., households and classrooms. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Smith, D., & Hynds, A. (2009). Culturally responsive Erlbaum.
teaching of mathematics: Three models from linked
Gutiérrez, R. (2009). Embracing the inherent
studies. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
tensions in teaching mathematics from an equity
40(2), 157–186.
stance. Democracy and Education, 18(3), 9–16.
Boaler, J. (2006). Promoting respectful learning.
Haas, E., & Gort, M. (2009). Demanding more:
Educational Leadership, 63(5), 74–78.
Legal standards and best practices for English
Celedón‐Pattichis, S. (2009). What does that mean? language learners. Bilingual Research Journal, 32,
Drawing on Latino and Latina students’ language 115–135.
and culture to make mathematical meaning. In M. W.
Janzen, J. (2008). Teaching English language
Ellis (Ed.), Responding to diversity: Grades 6–8
learners in the content areas. Review of Educational
(pp. 59–74). Reston, VA: National Council of
Research, 78(4), 1010–1038.
Teachers of Mathematics.
Kersaint, G., Thompson, D. R., & Petkova, M.
Celedón‐Pattichis, S., & Ramirez, N. G. (2012).
(2009). Teaching mathematics to English language
Beyond good teaching: Advancing mathematics education
learners. New York, NY: Routledge.
for ELLs. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers
of Mathematics. Khisty, L. L. (1997). Making mathematics accessible
to Latino students: Rethinking instructional
Cummins, J. (1994). Primary language instruction
practice. In M. Kenney & J. Trentacosta (Eds.),
and the education of language minority students.
Multicultural and gender equity in the mathematics
In C. F. Leyba (Ed.), Schooling and language minority
classroom: The gift of diversity (pp. 92–101). Reston,
students: A theoretical framework (pp. 3–46). Los
VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Angeles: California State University, National
Evaluation, Dissemination and Assessment Center. Maldonado, L. A., Turner, E. E., Dominguez,
H., & Empson, S. B. (2009). English language
Echevarria, J., Vogt, M. E., & Short, D. (2008).
learning from, and contributing to, mathematical
Making content comprehensible for English learners:
discussions. In D. Y. White & J. S. Spitzer (Eds.),
The SIOP model (3rd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn &
Responding to diversity: Grades pre‐K–5 (pp. 7–22).
Bacon.
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of
Fernandez, A., Anhalt, C., & Civil, M. (2009). Mathematics.
Mathematical interviews to assess Latino students.
Mathis, S. B. (1986). The hundred penny box. New York,
Teaching Children Mathematics, 16(3), 162–169.
NY: Puffin Books.
Garrison, L. (1997). Making the NCTM’s
Moschkovich, J. (2009). NCTM research brief: Using
Standards work for emergent English speakers.
two languages when learning mathematics: How can
Teaching Children Mathematics, 4(3), 132–138.
research help us understand mathematics learners who
Gómez, C. L. (2010). Teaching with cognates. use two languages? Reston, VA: National Council of
Teaching Children Mathematics, 16(8), 470–474. Teachers of Mathematics.
González, N., Moll, L. C., & Amanti, C. (Eds.). National Center for Education Statistics (NCES).
(2005). Funds of knowledge: Theorizing practices in (2011). The condition of education 2011 (NCES
77
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Children
78
Planning, Teaching,
and Assessing Children
with Exceptionalities
From Chapter 6 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
79
Planning, Teaching,
and Assessing Children
with Exceptionalities
Most low achievers in mathematics are probably “instructionally
disabled,” not cognitively or “learning disabled.”
Baroody (2011, p. 31)
Talented [mathematics] students need teachers who can move beyond the traditional “teacher role”
of a dispenser of information to that of a role model who is passionate about learning, able to
translate that passion into action, aggressively curious, and comfortable with this change of role.
Greenes, Teuscher, and Regis (2010, p. 80)*
Instructional Principles
for Diverse Learners
The NCTM Principles and Standards for School Mathematics states, “All
students, regardless of their personal characteristics, backgrounds, or
physical challenges must have opportunities to study—and support to
learn—mathematics” (NCTM, 2000, p. 12). Within the same docu-
ment, the equity principle states, “Excellence in mathematics education
requires equity—high expectations and strong support for all students”
(NCTM, 2000, p. 12). We know that teaching for equity is much more
than providing children with an equal opportunity to learn mathematics;
instead, it attempts to attain equal outcomes for all children by being
sensitive to individual differences.
*Reprinted with permission from Preparing Teachers for Mathematically Talented
Middle School Students, p. 80. Copyright 2010 by the National Council of Teachers of
Mathematics. All rights reserved.
80
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
Many achievement gaps are actually instructional gaps or expectation gaps. It is not helpful
when teachers establish low expectations for children, as when they say, “I just cannot put
this class into groups to work; they are too unruly” or “My children with disabilities can’t
solve word problems—they don’t have the reading skills.” Operating under the belief that
some children cannot “do mathematics” ensures that they don’t have ample opportunities to
prove otherwise. Instead, we suggest you consider Storeygard’s (2010) mantra for teachers
that proclaims “My kids can!”
As can be gleaned from the opening quotations, there is a spectrum of learners who
need to be considered if we intend to have equity in our instruction. Figuring out how you
will maintain equal outcomes (high expectations) while providing for individual differences
(strong support) can be challenging. Equipping yourself with an ever-growing collection of
instructional strategies for a variety of children is critical. A strategy that works for one child
may be completely ineffective with another, even a child who has the same exceptionality.
Addressing the needs of all means providing access and opportunity for
• Children who are identified as struggling or having a disability
• Children who are mathematically gifted
• Children who are unmotivated or need to build resilience
You may think, “I do not need to read the section on mathematically gifted children because
they will be pulled out for math enrichment.” Children who are mathematically talented
need to be challenged in the daily core instruction, not just when they are participating in a
program for gifted children.
The goal of equity is to offer all children access to important mathematics during
their regular classroom instruction. Yet inequities exist, even if unintentionally. For ex-
ample, if teachers do not build in opportunities for student‐to‐student interaction in a
lesson, they may not be addressing the needs of girls, who are often social learners, or
English language learners, who need opportunities to speak, listen, and write in small‐
group situations. It takes more than just wanting to be fair or equitable; it takes knowing
the strategies that accommodate each type of learner and making every effort to incor-
porate those strategies into your teaching. “Equity does not mean that every student
should receive identical instruction; instead, it demands that reasonable and appropriate
accommodations be made as needed to promote access and attainment for all children”
(NCTM, 2000, p. 12).
Across the wonderful and myriad diversities of your children, all of them learn math-
ematics in essentially the same way (Fuson, 2003). The authors of Adding It Up (National
Research Council, 2001) conclude that all children are best served when you give attention
to the following three principles:
1. Learning with understanding is based on connecting and organizing knowledge
around big conceptual ideas.
2. Learning builds on what children already know.
3. Instruction in school should take advantage of children’s informal knowledge of
mathematics.
These principles, also reflected in the tenets of constructivist theory, apply to all learners and,
therefore, are essential in making decisions about how you can adapt instruction to meet an
individual learner’s needs through accommodations and modifications. An accommodation is a
response to the needs of the environment or the learner; it does not alter the task. For example,
you might write down directions for a child instead of just presenting them orally. A modifica-
tion changes the task, making it more accessible to the child. For example, if kindergartners are
asked to try to use simple shapes to form larger shapes, you might show the outlines of the two
81
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
smaller shapes for the children to find and place directly on the outline, then also give them
the outline of the larger shape they will form. Subsequent shapes can be attempted without the
modification. When modifications result in an easier or less demanding task, expectations are
lowered. Modifications should be made in a way that leads back to the original task, providing
scaffolding or support for learners who may need it. In the sections in this chapter, we share
research‐based strategies that reflect these principles while providing appropriate accommoda-
tions and modifications for the wide range of children in your classroom.
Response to Intervention
RtI is a multitiered student support system that is frequently
80–90%
Tier 1 (all students) represented in a three‐tier triangular format. As you move
up the tiers, the number of children involved decreases, the
teacher–child ratio decreases, and the level of intervention
increases. As you might guess, there is a variety of RtI models
in use as states and districts structure their unique approaches
to meet local needs. Each tier in the triangle represents a level
of intervention with corresponding monitoring of results and
outcomes, as shown in Figure 1. The foundational and largest
Common Features across Tiers portion of the triangle (tier 1) represents the core instruction
• Research-Based Practices: Prevention begins with for all children based on a high‐quality mathematics
practices based on students’ best chances for success. curriculum and instructional practices (i.e., manipulatives,
• Data-Driven: All decisions are based on clear objectives conceptual emphasis, etc.), and progress‐monitoring
and formative data collection.
assessments. For example, if using a graphic organizer in tier
• Instructional: Prevention and intervention involve 1 core instruction, the following high‐quality practices would
effective instruction, prompts, cues, practice, and
environmental arrangements. be expected in the three phases of the lesson—Before, During,
• Context-Specific: All strategies and measures are and After:
selected to fit individual schools, classrooms, or students.
• Before. States purpose, introduces new vocabulary,
clarifies concepts from the prior knowledge in a visual
Source: Based on Scott, T., & Lane, H. (2001). Multi‐Tiered Interven-
tions in Academic and Social Contexts. Unpublished manuscript, organizer, and defines tasks of group members if groups
University of Florida, Gainesville. are being used
82
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
• During. Displays directions in a chart, poster, or list; provides a set of guiding questions
in a chart with blank spaces for responses
• After. Facilitates a discussion to highlight or make more explicit the significant concepts
or skills and then presents a summary and list of important concepts as they relate to one
another
Tier 2 represents children who did not reach the level of achievement expected during tier
1 instructional activities. Children in tier 2 should receive supplemental targeted instruction
(interventions) outside the core mathematics lessons that uses more explicit strategies with
systematic teaching of critical skills and concepts, more intensive and frequent instructional
opportunities, and more supportive and precise prompts (Torgesen, 2002). The position state-
ment of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics on interventions (NCTM, 2011a)
states, “Although we do not specifically state the precise interventions, we endorse the use of
increasingly intensive and effective instructional interventions for children who struggle with
mathematics.” Interventions are “reserved for disorders that prove resistant to lower levels of
prevention and require more heroic action to preclude serious complications” (Fuchs & Fuchs,
2001, p. 86).
Identifying and using these supplemental interventions is a flexible process that can be
adapted or tailored based on how children respond. If further assessments, such as diagnostic
interviews, reveal favorable progress, the children are weaned from the extra intervention ses-
sions. However, if difficulties and struggles remain, the interventions can be adjusted in inten-
sity, and in rare cases, children are referred to the next tier of support. Tier 3 is for children who
need more intensive periods of instruction, sometimes with one‐on‐one attention, which may
include comprehensive mathematics instruction or a referral for special education evaluation
or special education services. Instructional strategies for the three tiers are outlined in Table 1.
Progress Monitoring
A key to the multitiered system of support is monitoring children’s progress. One way to
collect evidence of children’s knowledge of concepts is through the use of diagnostic in-
terviews. Another approach is to assess children’s growth toward fluency in basic facts, an
area that is a well‐documented barrier for children with learning disabilities (Mazzocco,
Devlin, & McKenney, 2008). Combining instruction with short daily assessments to moni-
tor children’s knowledge of number combinations that were already taught proved that
the children with disabilities were better not only at remembering but also at generalizing
to other facts (Woodward, 2006). The collection of information gathered from these as-
sessments reveals whether children are making the progress expected or if more intensive
instructional approaches need to be put into place.
83
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
3. How can I provide additional support in these areas of weakness so that children with
learning disabilities can focus on the conceptual task in the activity? (Karp & Howell,
2004, p. 119).
Each phase of the lesson evokes specific planning considerations for children with dis-
abilities. Some strategies apply throughout a lesson. The following discussion is not exhaus-
tive, but it should provide you with specific suggestions for offering support to children
throughout the lesson while you maintain the challenge.
Tiers Interventions
Tier 2 A highly qualified regular classroom teacher with possible collaboration from a highly qualified special education
teacher
• Works with children (commonly in small groups) in supplemental sessions outside of the core instruction
• Conducts individual diagnostic interviews to target strengths and weaknesses
• Collaborates with special education, gifted, or English language learner (ELL) teachers
• Creates lessons that emphasize the big ideas (focal points) or themes
• Incorporates the CSA (concrete/semi-concrete/abstract) model
• Shares thinking in a think‐aloud to show children how to make problem‐solving decisions
• Incorporates explicit systematic strategy instruction (summarizes key points and reviews key vocabulary
or concepts before the lesson)
• Models specific behaviors and strategies, such as how to handle measuring materials
• Uses mnemonics or steps written on cards or posters to help children follow problem‐solving steps
• Uses peer‐assisted learning in which another child can provide help to a child in need
• Supplies families with additional support materials to use at home
• Encourages children’s use of self‐regulation and self‐instructional strategies, such as revising notes,
writing summaries, and identifying main ideas
• Teaches test‐taking strategies (e.g., allows children to use a highlighter on tests to emphasize
important information)
• Slices back (Fuchs & Fuchs, 2001) to material from a previous grade level to ramp back up
84
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
85
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
• Use methods for organizing written work. Provide tools and templates so that children
can focus on the mathematics rather than on the creation of a table or chart. Also use
graphic organizers, picture‐based models, and paper with columns or grids.
• Provide examples and nonexamples. To define triangles, give examples of triangles as
well as shapes that are not triangles. Help children focus on the characteristics that
differentiate the examples from the nonexamples.
4. Consider alternative assessments
• Propose alternative products. Provide options for how to demonstrate understanding
(e.g., a verbal response that is written by someone else, voice recorded, or modeled
with a manipulative). Use voice recognition software or word prediction software
that can generate a whole menu of word choices when children type a few letters.
• Encourage self‐monitoring and self‐assessment. Children with learning disabilities often
are not good at self‐reflection. Asking them to review an assignment or assessment to
explain what was difficult and what they think they got right can help them be more
independent and take greater responsibility for their learning.
• Consider feedback charts. Help children monitor their growth by charting progress
over time.
5. Emphasize practice and summary
• Help children bring ideas together. Create study guides that summarize the key
mathematics concepts and support students as they review key concepts. Older
children can begin to develop class study guides to transition into creating their own
study guides by identifying, summarizing, and coordinating the big ideas.
• Provide extra practice. Use carefully selected problems (not a large number) and allow
the use of familiar physical models.
Not all of these strategies will apply to every lesson and to every child with special
needs, but as you are thinking about a particular lesson and certain children in your class,
you will find that many of these will apply and will allow your children to engage in the task
and accomplish the learning goals of the lesson.
Implementing Interventions
NCTM (2007) has gathered a set of effective, research‐based approaches for teaching chil-
dren with difficulties in mathematics (such as children needing interventions in tier 2 or tier
3 of a support system such as RtI), highlighting the use of several key strategies that are also
suggested by Gersten, Beckmann, Clarke, Foegen, Marsh, Star, and Witzel (2009). These
strategies include systematic and explicit instruction, think‐alouds, concrete and visual rep-
resentations of problems, peer‐assisted learning activities, and formative assessment data
provided to children and teachers. These interventions, proven to be effective for children
with disabilities, may represent principles quite different from those at tier 1. The strate-
gies described here are interventions for use with the small subset of children for whom the
initial core instruction was ineffective.
86
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
teacher‐led explanations of concepts and strategies, including the critical connection build-
ing and meaning making that help learners relate new knowledge with concepts they know.
Let’s look at a classroom teacher who is using explicit instruction:
As you enter Mr. Logan’s classroom, you see a small group of children seated at
a table listening to the teacher’s detailed explanation and watching his demon-
stration of addition concepts. The children are using manipulatives, as prescribed
by Mr. Logan, and moving through carefully selected tasks. He tells the children
to take out three cubes and asks them then to take out five more cubes. He asks
the children, “Now how many do you have?” Mr. Logan asks, “Is add a word you
know?” Then, to make sure they don’t overcount, he asks them to talk about their
reasoning process with the question, “What are some things you need to keep in
mind as you put the two groups together?” Mr. Logan writes their responses on the
adjacent board along with 3 + 5 = 8. Then he asks them to talk about how many
more they have than when they started with the three cubes and records their re-
sponses. The children take turns answering these questions out loud. During the
lesson, Mr. Logan frequently stops the group, interjects points of clarification, and
directly highlights critical components of the task. For example, he asks, “Can you
count from three by adding the five cubes on?” and “Would it be better to count
on from the five? Why?” Vocabulary words, such as combine and equals, are written
on the “math word wall” nearby and the definitions of these terms are reviewed and
reinforced throughout the lesson. At the completion of the lesson, the children are
given several similar examples of the kind of combinations discussed in the lesson
as independent practice.
A number of aspects of explicit instruction can be seen in Mr. Logan’s approach to teaching
addition concepts. He employs a teacher‐directed teaching format, carefully describes the
use of manipulatives, and incorporates a model‐prompt‐practice sequence. This sequence
starts with verbal instructions and careful demonstrations with concrete models, followed
by prompting, questioning, and then independent practice. The children are deriving math-
ematical knowledge from the teacher’s oral, written, and visual clues.
As children with disabilities solve problems, explicit strategy instruction can help guide
them in carrying out tasks. First ask the children to read and restate the problem; draw a
picture; develop a plan by linking this problem to previous problems; write the problem
in a mathematical sentence; break the problem into smaller pieces; carry out operations;
and check answers with a calculator, hundreds chart, or other appropriate tools. These
self‐instructive prompts, or self‐questions, structure the entire learning process from begin-
ning to end. Unlike during more inquiry‐based instruction, the teacher models these steps
and explains the components with terminology that is easily understood by children with
disabilities—children who did not discover them independently through initial tier 1 or tier
2 activities. Yet, consistent with what we know about how all children learn, children are still
developing an understanding of the meaning of addition and are engaged in problem solv-
ing (not just in skill development).
Concrete models can support explicit strategy instruction. For example, when you dem-
onstrate the properties of two‐dimensional shapes, you might say, “Watch me. Now make a
rectangle with these four toothpicks that looks just like mine.” In contrast, a teacher with a
more inquiry‐oriented approach might say, “Using toothpicks, how can you show me a rect-
angle?” Although initially more structured, the use of concrete models will provide access to
more abstract concepts and can eventually lead to generalizations.
There are a number of possible advantages to using explicit strategy instruction for
children with disabilities. This approach helps you make more explicit for these children the
covert thinking strategies that others use in mathematical problem solving at a slower pace.
87
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
Although these students hear other children’s thinking strategies in the After phase of each
lesson, they frequently cannot keep up with the rapid pace of the sharing. Without extra
time to reprocess the conversation, children with disabilities may not have access to these
strategies. More explicit approaches are also less dependent on the children’s ability to draw
ideas from past experience or to operate in a self‐directed manner.
Explicit strategy instruction can also have distinct disadvantages for children with dis-
abilities. Some aspects of this approach rely on memorizing, which can be one of their
weakest skills. Taking a known weakness and building a learning strategy around it is not
productive. There is also the concern that highly teacher‐controlled approaches promote
long‐term dependency on teacher assistance. This is of particular concern for children with
disabilities because many are described as passive learners.
Children learn what they have the opportunity to practice. Children who are never
given opportunities to engage in self‐directed learning (based on the assumption that this
is not an area of strength) will be deprived of the opportunity to develop skills in this area.
In fact, the best explicit instruction is scaffolded, meaning it moves from a highly struc-
tured, single‐strategy approach toward multiple models, including examples and nonexam-
ples. It also includes immediate error correction followed by the fading of prompts to help
children move to independence. Explicit instruction, to be effective, must include making
mathematical relationships explicit (so that children, rather than only learning how to do
that day’s mathematics, make connections to other mathematical ideas). Because making
connections is a major component in how children learn, it must be central to instructional
strategies for children with disabilities.
Peer‐Assisted Learning
Children with special needs also benefit from other children’s modeling and support (Fuchs,
Fuchs, Yazdian, & Powell, 2002). The basic notion is that children learn best when they are
placed in the role of an apprentice working with a more skilled peer or “expert.” Although
the peer‐assisted learning approach shares some of the characteristics of the explicit strategy
instruction model, it is different because knowledge is presented on an “as‐needed” basis as
opposed to a predetermined sequence. The children can be paired with older children or
88
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
peers who have a more sophisticated understanding of a concept. At other times, tutors and
tutees can reverse roles during the tasks. Having children with disabilities “teach” others is
an important part of the learning process, so giving children with special needs a chance to
explain concepts to another child is valuable.
Think‐Alouds
When you use a think‐aloud as an instructional strategy, you demonstrate the steps to ac-
complish a task while verbalizing the thinking process and reasoning that accompany the
steps. Remember, don’t start where your thinking is—assess and start where the child’s
thinking is. The child follows this instruction by imitating your process of “talking through”
a solution on a different but parallel task. This is similar to the model in which “expert”
learners share strategies with “novice” learners.
Consider a problem in which first graders are given the task of determining which child
is taller by lying on the floor. Rather than merely demonstrating, for example, how to use a
ruler to measure the children, the think‐aloud strategy would involve talking through the
steps and identifying the reasons for each step while measuring each child with connecting
cubes and comparing the difference between the children’s heights. As you place a mark
on the floor to indicate where the cubes start, you might state, “I used this line to mark off
where Rebecca’s foot ends. How should I use this line as I measure Rebecca? I know I have
to add on cubes, but how do I know when to stop?” “How can I use both lines of cubes to
figure out if Rebecca is taller than Emma?” All of this dialogue occurs prior to placing the
ruler for a second measurement. Often teachers share alternatives about how else they could
have carried out the task. When you use this metacognitive strategy, try to talk about and
model possible approaches (and the reasons behind these approaches) in an effort to make
your invisible thinking processes visible to the children.
Although you can choose any of these strategies as needed, your goal is always to work
toward a high level of student responsibility for learning. Movement to higher levels of content
understanding can be likened to the need to move to a higher level on a hill. For some, formal
stair steps with support along the way are necessary (explicit strategy instruction); for others
ramps with encouragement at the top of the hill will work (peer‐assisted learning). Others can
find a path up the hill on their own with some guidance from visual representations (CSA ap-
proach). All people can relate to the need to have different support during different times of
their lives or under different circumstances, and it is no different for children with special needs
(see Table 2). Yet these children must eventually learn to create a path to new learning on their
own, as that is what will be required in the real world after formal education ends. Leaving chil-
dren knowing only how to climb steps with support and face hills with constant assistance and
encouragement from others will not help them attain their life goals.
89
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
Child has trouble • Can’t interpret a number line • Use larger versions of the
forming mental • Has difficulty identifying a representation (e.g., number
representations triangle when the top of line) so that children can move
of mathematical the triangle points down to or interact with the model
concepts • Explicitly teach the representation—
for example, exactly how to
draw a triangle while identifying
the critical properties
Child has difficulty • Has difficulty with basic • Explicitly teach multiple ways of
accessing numerical facts; for example, doesn’t representing a number showing the
meanings from recognize that 3 + 5 is the variations at exactly the same time
symbols (issues with same as 5 + 3, or that 5 + 1 • Use a number balance to support
number sense) is the same as the next understanding of the equal sign
counting number after 5 • Use multiple representations
• Does not understand the for a single problem to show
meaning of the equal sign how it would be carried out in a
• Can’t interpret whether variety of ways (base‐ten blocks,
an answer is reasonable illustrations, and numbers) rather
than using multiple problems
Child misapplies rules • Applies rules such as “Always • Always give examples as well as
or overgeneralizes subtract the smaller from the counterexamples to show how
larger” too literally, resulting and when “rules” should be used
in errors such as 25 - 9 = 24 and when they should not
• Tie all rules into conceptual
understanding; don’t
emphasize memorizing rote
procedures or practices
90
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
91
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
Number and operations • Count out a variety of items for general classroom activities.
• Create a list of supplies that need to be ordered for the
classroom or a particular event and calculate cost.
• Calculate the number of calories in a given meal.
• Compare the cost of two meals on menus from local
restaurants.
Data analysis and probability • Survey children on favorite games and use the top five
as choices for the class. Make a bar graph to represent
and compare the results.
• Tally the number of children ordering school lunch.
• Examine the outside temperatures for the past week and
estimate the temperatures for the next two days.
Acceleration
Acceleration recognizes that your children may already understand the mathematics con-
tent that you plan to teach. Some teachers use “curriculum compacting” (Reis & Renzulli,
2005) to give a short overview of the content and assess the children’s ability to respond to
mathematics tasks that would demonstrate their proficiency. Another option is to reduce the
amount of time these children spend on aspects of the topic or moving to more advanced
content at the next grade level or beyond. Allowing children to increase the pace of their
own learning can give them access to curriculum different from their grade level while
demanding more independent study. However, moving children to higher mathematics (by
moving them up a grade, for example) will not succeed in engaging them as learners if the
instruction is still at a slow pace. Research reveals that when gifted children are accelerated
through the curriculum, they become more likely to explore STEM (science, technology,
engineering, and mathematics) fields (Sadler & Tai, 2007).
Enrichment
Enrichment activities go beyond the topic of study to content that is not specifically a
part of your grade‐level curriculum but is an extension of the original mathematical tasks.
For example, while studying place value, mathematically gifted children can stretch their
92
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
knowledge to study other bases, such as base five. This provides an extended view of how
our base‐ten numeration system fits within the broader system. Other times the format of
enrichment can involve studying the same topic as the rest of the class while differing on the
means and outcomes of the work. Examples include group investigations, solving real prob-
lems in the community, writing letters to outside audiences, and identifying applications of
the mathematics learned.
Sophistication
Another strategy is to increase the sophistication of a topic by raising the level of complexity
or pursuing greater depth of content possibly outside of the regular curriculum or by con-
necting mathematics to other subject areas. Frequently gifted children explore topics similar
to those of their classmates but focus on higher‐level thinking and more complex or abstract
ideas. This can mean exploring a larger set of ideas in which a mathematics topic exists. For
example, while studying a unit on place value, mathematically gifted children can deepen
their knowledge to study other numeration systems such as Roman, Mayan, Egyptian, and
Babylonian. This study provides a multicultural view of how our numeration system fits
within the historical number systems (Mack, 2011).
Novelty
Novelty introduces completely different material from the regular curriculum and frequently
occurs in after‐school clubs, out‐of‐class projects, or collaborative school experiences. In
collaborative experiences, children from a variety of grades and classes may volunteer for
special mathematics projects with a classroom teacher, principal, or resource teacher taking
the lead. The novelty category includes having children explore topics that are within their
developmental grasp but outside the curriculum. For example, children may explore how to
build structures with toothpicks and gumdrops or Styrofoam peanuts and then use the char-
acteristics of the successful constructions to decide which designs are stable and which are
not, sorting the structures using an “in or out” reasoning approach. They may also explore
a large‐scale investigation of the amount of food thrown away at lunchtime or the amount
of water flushed in the school’s bathroom. Another aspect of the novelty approach provides
different options for children in culminating performances of their understanding, such as
demonstrating their knowledge through inventions, experiments, simulations, dramatiza-
tions, visual displays, and oral presentations.
Strategies to Avoid
There are a number of ineffective approaches for gifted children that find their way into
classrooms. Following are five common ones:
1. Assigning more of the same work. This is the least appropriate way to respond to
mathematically gifted children and the most likely to result in children eventually
hiding their abilities. This approach is described by Persis Herold as “all scales
and no music” (quoted in Tobias, 1995, p. 168).
2. Giving free time to early finishers. Although children may find this rewarding, it does
not maximize their intellectual growth and can lead to hurrying to finish a task.
3. Assigning gifted children to help struggling learners. Routinely assigning gifted children to
teach other children who have not achieved what the gifted students have mastered does
not stimulate their intellectual growth and can place them in a socially uncomfortable
and undesirable situation. Consistently using this approach puts mathematically
talented children in a constant position of tutoring rather than allowing them to
create deeper and more complex levels of understanding.
4. Providing gifted pull‐out opportunities. Unfortunately, generalized gifted programs are
often unrelated to the regular mathematics curriculum (Assouline & Lupkowski‐Shoplik,
93
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
2011). Although it can benefit students, add‐on experiences are not enough.
Gifted students need adaptations to the instruction in their mathematics classroom.
Learners with a high level of ability can’t get one‐stop shopping in a gifted program
that focuses on all academic subjects; they need individual attention to develop
depth and more complex understanding of mathematics.
5. Offering independent enrichment on the computer. This practice often does not engage
children with mathematics in a way that will enhance conceptual understanding
and support their ability to justify their thinking to others. However, there are some
excellent enrichment opportunities on the Internet that provide these kinds of
experiences.
Sheffield (1999) writes that gifted children should be introduced to the “joys and frustra-
tions of thinking deeply about a wide range of original, open‐ended, or complex problems
that encourage them to respond creatively in ways that are original, fluent, flexible and
elegant” (p. 46). Accommodations, modifications, and interventions for mathematically
gifted children must strive for this goal.
References
Assouline, S. G., & Lupkowski‐Shoplik, A. (2011). companion to Principles and Standards in School
Developing math talent: A comprehensive guide to Mathematics (pp. 68–94). Reston, VA: National
math education for gifted students in elementary Council of Teachers of Mathematics..
and middle school (2nd ed.). Waco, TX: Prufrock
Gagnon, J., & Maccini, P. (2001). Preparing
Press.
students with disabilities for algebra. Teaching
Baroody, A. J. (2011). Learning: A framework. In Exceptional Children, 34(1), 8–15.
F. Fennell (Ed.), Achieving fluency in special education
Gallagher, J., & Gallagher, S. (1994). Teaching the
and mathematics (pp. 15–58). Reston, VA: National
gifted child. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Gavin, M. K., & Sheffield, L. J. (2010). Using
Bruner, J. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction.
curriculum to develop mathematical promise in
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
the middle grades. In M. Saul, S. Assouline, & L. J.
CCSSO (Council of Chief State School Officers). Sheffield (Eds.), The peak in the middle: Developing
(2010). Common core state standards. Retrieved from mathematically gifted students in the middle grades
http://corestandards.org (pp. 51–76). Reston, VA: National Council of
Teachers of Mathematics, National Association
CCSSO (Council of Chief State School Officers).
of Gifted Children, and National Middle School
(2011). Application of Common Core State Standards
Association.
for English language learners. Retrieved from www
.corestandards.org/assets/application‐for‐english‐ Gersten, R., Beckmann, S., Clarke, B., Foegen, A.,
learners.pdf Marsh, L., Star, J. R., & Witzel, B. (2009). Assisting
students struggling with mathematics: Response to
Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (2001). Principles for
Intervention (RtI) for elementary and middle schools
the prevention and intervention of mathematics
(NCEE 2009‐4060). Washington, DC: National
difficulties. Learning Disabilities Research and
Center for Education Evaluation and Regional
Practice, 16(2), 85–95.
Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S.
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Yazdian, L., & Powell, S. R. Department of Education. Retrieved from http://ies
(2002). Enhancing first‐grade children’s mathematics .ed.gov/ncee/wwc/publications/practiceguides
development with peer‐assisted learning strategies.
Greenes, C., Teuscher, D., & Regis, T. (2010).
School Psychology Review, 31(4), 569–583.
Preparing teachers for mathematically talented
Fuson, K. C. (2003). Developing mathematical middle school students. In M. Saul, S. Assouline, &
power in whole number operations. In J. Kilpatrick, L.J. Sheffield (Eds.), The peak in the middle: Developing
W. G. Martin, & D. Schifter (Eds.), A research mathematically gifted students in the middle grades (pp.
94
Planning, Teaching, and Assessing Children with Exceptionalities
77–91). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers Saul, M., Assouline, S., & Sheffield, L. J. (Eds.)
of Mathematics, National Association of Gifted (2010). The peak in the middle: Developing
Children, and National Middle School Association. mathematically gifted students in the middle
grades. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers
Heddens, J. (1964). Today’s mathematics: A guide to
of Mathematics, National Association of Gifted
concepts and methods in elementary school mathematics.
Children, and National Middle School Association.
Chicago, IL: Science Research Associates.
Scott, T., & Lane, H. (2001). Multi‐tiered
Karp, K., & Howell, P. (2004). Building
interventions in academic and social contexts.
responsibility for learning in students with special
Unpublished manuscript, University of Florida,
needs. Teaching Children Mathematics, 11(3), 118–126.
Gainesville.
Mack, N. K. (2011). Enriching number knowledge.
Sheffield, L. J. (Ed.). (1999). Developing
Teaching Children Mathematics, 18(2), 101–109.
mathematically promising students. Reston, VA:
Mazzocco, M. M. M., Devlin, K. T., & McKenney, S. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
J. (2008). Is it a fact? Timed arithmetic performance
Tobias, S. (1995). Overcoming math anxiety. New York,
of children with mathematical learning disabilities
NY: W. W. Norton & Company.
(MLD) varies as a function of how MLD is defined.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 318–344. Torgesen, J. K. (2002). The prevention of reading
difficulties. Journal of School Psychology, 40(1), 7–26.
National Research Council. (2001). Adding it up:
Helping children learn mathematics. J. Kilpatrick, J. VanTassel‐Baska, J., & Brown, E. F. (2007). Toward
Swafford, & B. Findell (Eds.). Washington, DC: best practice: An analysis of the efficacy of curriculum
National Academies Press. models in gifted education. Gifted Child Quarterly,
51(4), 342–358.
Ravenna, G. (2008). Factors influencing gifted students’
preferences for models of teaching. Dissertation, Witzel, B. S. (2005). Using CRA to teach algebra
University of Southern California. to students with math difficulties in inclusive
settings. Learning Disabilities—A Contemporary
Reis, S., & Renzulli, J. S. (2005). Curriculum
Journal, 3(2), 49–60.
compacting: An easy start to differentiating for high
potential students. Waco, TX: Prufrock Press. Woodward, J. (2006). Developing automaticity in
multiplication facts: Integrating strategy instruction
Renzulli, J. S., Gubbins, E. J., McMillen, K. S.,
with timed practice drills. Learning Disability
Eckert, R. D., & Little, C. A. (Eds.). (2009). Systems &
Quarterly, 29(4), 269–89.
models for developing programs for the gifted & talented
(2nd ed.). Mansfield Center, CT: Creative Learning Ysseldyke, J. (2002). Response to “Learning
Press, Inc. disabilities: Historical perspectives.” In R. Bradley,
L. Danielson, & D. Hallahan (Eds.), Identification of
Rotigel, J., & Fellow, S. (2005). Mathematically
learning disabilities: Research to practice (pp. 89–98).
gifted students: How can we meet their needs?
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gifted Child Today, 27(4), 46–65.
Sadler, P., & Tai, R. (2007). The two pillars
supporting college science. Science, 317(5837),
457–458.
95
This page intentionally left blank
Collaborating with Families,
Community, and Principals
From Chapter 7 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
97
Collaborating with Families, Community, and Principals
98
Collaborating with Families, Community, and Principals
Pedagogy • Why isn’t the teacher teaching? (And what is the point of reinventing
the wheel?)
• Are children doing their own work when they are in groups? Is my child
having to do the work of children who don’t understand the work?
• Why is there so much reading and writing in math class?
• Why is my child struggling more than before?
Source: Adapted from Bay‐Williams, J. M., & Meyer, M. R. (2003). What Parents Want to Know about
Standards‐Based Mathematics Curricula. Principal Leadership, 3(7), 54–60. Copyright 2003, National
Association of Secondary School Principals. For more information on NASSP products and services to
promote excellence in middle-level and high school leadership, visit www.nassp.org.
When choosing mathematical tasks to use with parents, be sure the tasks focus on
content that really matters to them and relates to what they already know is a part of the
elementary curriculum (e.g., addition and subtraction, including basic facts, as well as
place value are good ideas in grades K–2). There are tasks throughout this text that are
ideal for a math night. Figure 2 contrasts two examples of first‐grade problems for learn-
ing about combinations (sums) that make 6—one that is a teaching‐by‐telling (traditional)
experience (solve the problems as modeled by the teacher using counters) and one that is
designed for a teaching‐through‐problem‐solving experience (explore and find different
combinations).
The contrasting addition problems are perfect for discussing with parents what it means
to do mathematics because they (1) address critical areas in the Common Core State Standards
and focal points in the Curriculum Focal Points, (2) involve using manipulatives (color tiles or
counters), (3) connect the mathematical ideas of partitioning and addition, and (4) (in the
latter example) have multiple solution strategies.
99
Collaborating with Families, Community, and Principals
Problem Set 1: Find the answers to these equations. Use counters or draw pictures to show your work.
1 + 5 = ________ 0 + 6 = __________
3 + 3 = __________ 2 + 4 = __________
Problem Set 2: The parking lot has only blue and red cars. There are 6 cars parked. How many blue
and how many red cars might be in the parking lot? Use counters to find as many ways as you can.
Draw a picture and write an equation for each answer.
Extensions: Can you find all of the ways to have 6 red and blue cars in the parking lot? How many
ways can you find to have a total of 5 cars? How many ways can you find to have a total of 7 cars?
Do you see a pattern?
The potential each of these problems has to support and challenge children in making
sense of mathematics should be made explicit during a discussion with parents. After giving
parents time to do both tasks and discuss solution strategies (as you would with children),
connect the learning experience to their questions and concerns. Ask participants to consider
the learning opportunities in the two contrasting tasks. Ask questions such as the following:
• What skills are being developed in each problem?
• Which problem gives more opportunity to make connections between mathematics and
the real world?
• Which task would your child be more motivated to solve? Why?
Help parents identify the depth of the mathematics in the teaching-through-problem-
solving task. In grades K–2 children are building important foundations of number and
operations through algebraic reasoning—looking for patterns, reasoning, and generalizing.
Help parents see these aspects in the car combination problem. Share the CCSSO and or
the NCTM standards (in parent‐friendly language), and focus on the goal of having chil-
dren becoming mathematically proficient as described in those standards. Ask “Where do
you see these proficiencies being supported in the two tasks we did?”
Another good choice for family math nights is basic fact strategies, as addition and
subtraction facts are a central part of the curriculum in
Provide copies of the appropriate Common Core State grades 1–2, and developing reasoning strategies is essential
Standards Introduction and Overview pages (the first two to learning the facts well. (Parents may only know flash
pages for each grade) and allow parents time to think about cards or other memorization activities.)
each “Critical Area.” Address any or all of the questions in Table 1 that apply
to your setting. One way to do this is to have parents write
their questions on note cards and collect them so you can identify common questions and decide
the order in which to discuss each question. The sections that follow provide possible responses
to questions that parents (or community members) commonly ask.
Pedagogy
When parents ask questions that point to their belief that mathematics is best learned
through direct instruction (just as they learned it), remind them of the experiences they had
in doing mathematics with the car combination task. Point to the difference between being
shown how to do something (e.g., “This is how you add; now practice this”) and developing
an understanding of something (e.g., “How many ways can you partition 6? How do you
know if you have found all the ways?”). You can help parents identify the skills and concepts
100
Collaborating with Families, Community, and Principals
that are developed through these two experiences. Ask, “In what ways are children learning
about addition? About subtraction? How do the different ways support eventual mastery of
the basic facts?” Point out that skills are still important, and children benefit by generating
their own procedures. Explicitly promote the fact that a developmental approach to learn-
ing mathematics provides the means for children to (1) use prior knowledge, (2) make con-
nections, (3) use alternative strategies and reasoning, (4) apply mathematical ideas to new
situations, and (5) develop positive dispositions about being able to do mathematics. Share
example tasks such as these:
9+6 6 + 28 57 + 26
Ask parents to partition 6 to solve these problems mentally. Connect to the meaning of
“procedural fluency” and the benefit in having children generate their own procedures, and
connect those procedures to a strong understanding of number and operations.
Cooperative Groups
Parents may also wonder about the frequency of their children working in cooperative
groups because it may differ from their own mathematics learning experiences. Help par-
ents see the role of others in their learning as they solved the problems and as they heard
solutions from those who were working at other tables. Connect that experience to the value
of cooperative learning. You can do this in a variety of ways:
1. Include a feature in your parent newsletter. Early in the year you can feature cooperative
learning, addressing its importance across content areas. In mathematics this can include
the following benefits: hearing different strategies, building meaning, designing solution
strategies, and justifying approaches—all essential to building a strong understanding of
mathematics and important life skills.
2. Send home letters introducing mathematics units of study. If you are about to teach a unit
on adding and subtracting two‐digit numbers, a letter can help parents know the important
aspects of the content. This is a great time to mention that children will work in groups so
that they can see different ways to add or subtract two numbers.
3. Do a cooperative learning mathematics activity at a family math night or back‐to‐school event.
Use a task that involves assigning roles to different members of the group and that won’t
take long to solve. Have parents work with two to three others to solve the task.
4. Invite parents to assist in a mathematics group assignment. Seeing firsthand the dialogue and
thinking that happens in cooperative groups can go a long way in illustrating how valuable
cooperative groups can be!
Parents may worry that children working in groups are
simply copying from others and not learning. Share strate-
Being proactive about communicating the benefits of
gies you use to build in individual accountability and shared
cooperative learning, as well as how you build in individual
responsibility. For example, teachers may ask each child to
accountability and shared responsibility, will go a long way
record explanations in his or her notebook. At other times,
toward converting parent concerns into parent support.
you may assign specific roles to each member of the group.
101
Collaborating with Families, Community, and Principals
Use of Technology
Parents may be avid users of technology yet still have concerns about their child using
calculators and computers in grades pre‐K–2, when they haven’t yet mastered their basic
facts for addition and subtraction. Even though research overwhelmingly finds that children
using calculators achieve at least as much as those not using calculators, the calculator is
widely blamed for children’s lack of reasoning and sense making. Reassure parents that chil-
dren will learn the basic facts and procedures and the calculator can support that learning.
Consider sharing or doing any of the following activities:
1. Type in 5 + 5 = . Continue to push the = key (most calculators continue to add 5 every time
you push =). What patterns do you notice as you count by fives?
2. Broken Key. Suppose the following keys do not work on your calculator: 5 and 8. Figure
out another way to add or subtract these numbers using the calculator:
6+5 15 - 6 35 + 28
102
Collaborating with Families, Community, and Principals
Source: CCSSO (Council of Chief State School Officers). (2010). Common Core State Standards, p. 6. Retrieved
from http://corestandards.org. The Common Core State Standards are © Copyright 2010. National Governors
Association Center for Best Practices and Council of Chief State School Officers. All rights reserved.
Share the first Standard for Mathematical Practice (see Figure 4) and ask the parents
what they notice. Focus on the importance of perseverance. This is true in mathematics and
in life. Reassure parents that some tasks take longer because of the nature of the task, not
because their child lacks understanding. Mathematics is not nearly as much about speed
and memorization as it is about being able to grapple with a novel problem, try various ap-
proaches from a variety of options, and finally reach an accurate answer.
Mathematics Content
A common concern of parents is that their children are not learning their basic facts and
standard algorithms. You must address (at least) two points related to these issues. First, the
skills that parents are looking for (e.g., U.S. standard algorithm for subtraction) are still
there—they just look different because they are presented in a way based on understanding
rather than memorization. Standard algorithms are still taught, but they are taught along
with invented strategies that build on children’s number sense and reasoning. Let parents
experience that both invented and standard algorithms are important in being mathemati-
cally proficient by inviting them to solve the following problems.
Ask for volunteers to share the ways that they thought about the problems. For the
subtraction problem, for example, the following might be shared:
• 300 take away 288 is 12, then add the 9 back on to get 21.
• 288 up to 300 is 12 and up 9 more is 21.
• 309 to 300 is 9, then down to 290 is 10 more (19), and then to 288 is 2 more (21).
These invented strategies, over numerous problems, reinforce place‐value concepts and
the relationship between addition and subtraction. Noticing that these values are both near
103
Collaborating with Families, Community, and Principals
300 helps the problem solver to select a strategy. This bird’s‐eye view of the problem is im-
portant in doing mathematics rather than always doing the same thing no matter what the
numbers. This is very evident in the third example, which can be solved without computa-
tion if the relationship between the numbers is noticed first.
Second, what is “basic” in the twenty‐first century is much more than computation.
Many topics in the elementary curriculum were not a part of the curriculum a generation
ago (e.g., connections of algebra to the operations). Looking together through the essential
concepts in the Common Core State Standards or the NCTM Curriculum Focal Points helps
parents see that the curriculum is not just an idea generated at their child’s school but the
national consensus on what students need to learn as well.
Student Achievement
At the heart of parents’ interest in school mathematics is wanting their child to be
successful, not only in the current classroom but also at the next level of school and
later on in high‐stakes assessments like the ACT or SAT for college entrance. If your
state has implemented the Common Core State Standards, you can share that the stan-
dards are for K–12 and are designed to prepare students for college and future careers.
The Common Core State Standards website (http://corestandards.org) has an increasing
number of resources for parents to help them ensure their child is college and career
ready.
Another approach to inform parents about student achievement is to share research on
the ineffectiveness of the traditional U.S. approach to mathematics. The Trends in Interna-
tional Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS), an international study conducted regularly
and including many countries, continues to find that U.S. students achieve at an average
level in fourth grade and then score lower in mathematics than international students from
that point on. Discuss the implications of unpreparedness for students who want to seek
higher‐paying jobs on what is now an international playing field.
Parents may be more interested in how your specific school is doing in preparing chil-
dren for the future. Share evidence from your school of mathematics success, including
stories about an individual child (no name given) or the success of a particular classroom,
like the following one received by a principal:
I was worried at the start of the year because my son didn’t have a lot of confidence
in math and he was coming home with problems that he was supposed to figure out
his own way to solve. I wondered why the teacher hadn’t shown him how to add
or subtract. But now I can really see his number sense—he has all kinds of ways he
adds numbers and can do it in his head. And he is really good at solving and writing
his own story problems! As an aside, I am also learning a lot—I didn’t learn this way,
but I am finding the homework problems are really interesting as we figure them
out. I am just curious if this is something he will get to do again in second grade, or
if this is just the way first‐grade teachers introduce the ideas.
Such communications help parents see that there is a transition period and that in the end
a standards‐based approach helps engage children and build their understanding over time.
104
Collaborating with Families, Community, and Principals
connected to better performance (Else‐Quest, Hyde, & Hejmadi, 2008). Therefore, parents
who exhibit positive interest, humor, and pride in their child’s homework support their
child’s mathematics learning.
You may have heard parents say, “I am not good at math” or “I don’t like solving math prob-
lems.” Parents may feel this way and, given the research just described, it is particularly important
to redirect parents to portray mathematics in a positive light. For example, “Even though math
can be hard, stick with it and you will figure it out.” Teaching parents how to help their children
has also been found to make a difference in supporting student achievement (Cooper, 2007).
How do you effectively encourage children and their families to support mathematics
learning at home? Here we break down the many possible ways into four categories: parents’
participation, homework support, resources for parents, and beyond‐homework experiences.
Parents’ Participation
If parents can witness firsthand your questioning and the many ways that problems can
be solved, they will have a vision of how they can support learning at home. For example,
they may notice that you encourage children to select their own strategy and explain how
they know it works. They will also pick up on the language that you are using and will
be able to reinforce that language at home. You can even provide a note‐taking template
that includes categories such as the following: What is the big idea of the lesson? What il-
lustrations or tools are being used to help children understand? What are some questions
the teacher is asking that I could also ask? What does the teacher do when a child is stuck?
Homework Support
Homework can be a positive experience for children, families, and the teacher. Take the fol-
lowing recommendations into consideration when thinking about the homework that you
will assign to your children.
1. Mimic the three‐phase lesson model. Complete a brief version of the Before phase of a
lesson to be sure the children understand the homework task before they go home. At home,
children complete the During phase. When they return with the work completed, apply
the sharing techniques of the After phase of the homework. Children can even practice the
After phase with their family if this is encouraged through communications to parents or
guardians. Some form of written work must be required so that children are held responsible
for the task and are prepared for the class discussion.
2. Use a distributed‐content approach. Homework can address content that has been taught
earlier in the year as practice, that day’s content as reinforcement, or upcoming content as
groundwork. Interestingly, research has found that distributed homework (homework that
combines all three components) is more effective in supporting student learning (Cooper,
2007). The exception is children with learning disabilities, who perform better when
homework focuses on reinforcement of skills and current class lessons.
3. Promote an “ask‐before‐tell” approach with parents. Parents may not know how best to
support their child when he or she is stuck or has gotten a wrong answer. One important
thing you can do is to ask parents to implement an “ask‐before‐tell” approach (Kliman,
1999). This means that before parents explain something they should ask their child to
explain how he or she did it. The child may self‐correct (a life skill) and, if not, at least
the parents can use what they heard from their child to
provide targeted assistance.
Providing specific guidance to families makes a big differ-
4. Provide good questioning prompts for parents. Providing
ence in what they do (and do not do) to help their children
guiding questions for parents or guardians supports a
learn mathematics and to be confident in doing mathematics.
problem‐based approach to instruction as they help their
105
Collaborating with Families, Community, and Principals
Figure 6
Sample letter home to parents regarding ways to infuse mathematics into their
interactions with their child.
Adults constantly use estimation and computation in doing everyday tasks. If you get
parents to talk about these instances with their children, imagine how much it can help chil-
dren learn about mathematics and its importance as a life skill.
108
Collaborating with Families, Community, and Principals
the mathematics program. And what they need to know is qualitatively different from what
a classroom teacher needs to know.
Since the launch of the Common Core State Standards, school administrators, parents,
and community members are more aware than ever about mathematics standards. If your
state has not adopted the Common Core State Standards, there are still state‐level standards
that are the focus of mathematics goals and assessments. So, while it may seem that the need
to communicate with administrators is something to simply check off your list, it must be a
top priority.
Even though principals are hearing more about mathematics standards, higher stan-
dards, and the need to ensure that all children are successful, it does not mean they un-
derstand what standards‐based mathematics curriculum is in terms of the content or the
related CCSS Standards for Mathematical Practice or NCTM Process Standards. The
principal is likely to get bombarded with broad or specific questions from parents: “Is
New Math back? Why isn’t the teacher teaching the procedure for adding and subtract-
ing? What are the Standards for Mathematical Practice?” When principals are asked these
questions, they need a convincing response that is both accurate and addresses the heart of
the parents’ concerns (that their child is going to get a good, sound, research‐based math-
ematics experience).
Meyer and Arbaugh (2008) suggest professional development specifically for principals.
While their focus is on the adoption of standards‐based textbooks, the plan they outline
applies to all principals who are seeking to be knowledgeable and effective advocates for
implementing new standards or mathematics curricula. The following ideas are adapted
from their suggested professional development to focus on one‐on‐one conversations.
1. Contrast old and new curriculum. As a first step, it is important to know what is new and
different in the mathematics program. One way to start is to provide a set of materials that
represents typical Common Core State Standards–aligned tasks alongside of what has been
the previous curriculum. Point out the noticeable similarities and differences or the key
features of the curriculum. (Note: It is important to focus on both similarities and
differences—not everything is getting replaced and this is an important message.)
2. Discuss how parents and children will respond. Anticipate what will be noticed by parents (or
their children). Which changes might be welcomed? Which changes might be worrisome?
How will the welcome aspects be promoted and the worrisome aspects be explained?
3. Experience the curriculum. Invite the principal to visit your classroom or other classrooms
where the Standards for Mathematical Practice or the NCTM Process Standards are being
infused. Ask the principal to join a group of children and listen to their discussion of how
they are solving a problem. Or organize a lesson in which the children actually present their
solutions to the principal in the After phase. For example, in a kindergarten class, ask children
to take pictures (or draw pictures) representing the two‐dimensional shapes in the school.
If possible, ask the principal to solve one of the problems children are doing and share
his or her strategy with the class. This firsthand experience can provide the principal with
a wonderful story to share with parents and insights that won’t be gained from reviewing
standards documents.
4. Discuss emerging issues. Plan a regular time to meet with the principal to discuss what he
or she has heard from families about the mathematics program. Discuss what you might do
to respond to questions (some of their anticipated issues may already have been described
in the preceding section on parents’ concerns).
109
Collaborating with Families, Community, and Principals
Finally, keep your principal apprised of successes and breakthroughs. These stories pro-
vide the principal with evidence to share when being pressed by parents or community
members. Principals are very often your strongest advocate and are in the position to serve
as buffers between school mathematics and the community.
Initially, the statements may not seem harmful, but they can set off alarms for the stake-
holder. Consider these reactions, and then review the shifted language in the third column,
which communicates a stronger (and less potentially disconcerting) message. Along these
lines, it is very important to convey to parents an excitement for and pride in your mathe-
matics program. Being tentative, reserved, vague, or silent on the mathematics program can
“The [mathematics program] still addresses “Why are they bringing skills up? They “The skills in the [mathematics program] are
skills, but it also includes concepts.” must be taking those away. My child/ expanding from what we once learned and
U.S. kids have to know basics. How can now include . . .”
I put a stop to this?”
“It is important for children to learn from “The teacher is not teaching? My child “In our classroom, we learn from one
one another, so I will be more in the role does better when things are explained another. I give carefully selected tasks for
of facilitator.” clearly. When I come see you teach, children to discuss and then we talk about
what am I looking for if you are just them together so that everyone has a chance
letting the kids learn on their own?” to learn the mathematics we are doing, and
that approach gives me the chance to work
one‐on‐one as needed.”
“This year we are doing a whole new “My worst nightmare—an experiment “We are doing some new things in order to
mathematics program that the state has of something new during the years my make sure your child is well prepared for . . .
adopted.” child is in elementary school. This will [or that our program is the best available]. You
cause problems for the rest of his life.” might have noticed that last year we [added
writing as a component to our mathematics
program]. This year, here are the big things we
hope to accomplish. . . .”
110
Collaborating with Families, Community, and Principals
only raise concerns in the community. Help parents and administrators to understand that
the mathematics program children are experiencing aligns with best practices in education,
represents what they need to know in today’s world, and prepares them for mathematics at
the next level as well as the mathematics they need for everyday life.
References
Aspiazu, G. G., Bauer, S. C., & Spillett, M. D. Henderson, A. T., Mapp, K. L., Jordan, C., Orozco, E.,
(1998). Improving the academic performance of Averett, A., Donnelly, D., Buttram, J., Wood, L.,
Hispanic youth: A community education model. Fowler, M., & Myers, M. (2002). A new wave of evidence:
Bilingual Research Journal, 22(2), 1–20. The impact of school, family, and community connections
on student achievement. Austin, TX: Southwest
Bay, J. M., Reys, B. J., & Reys, R. E. (1999). The
Education Development Laboratory.
top 10 elements that must be in place to implement
standards‐based mathematics curricula. Phi Delta Hiebert, J., & Grouws, D. A. (2007). The effects
Kappan, 80(7), 503–506. of classroom mathematics teaching on students’
learning. In F. K. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of
Bay‐Williams, J. (2010). Influences on student
research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 371–
outcomes: Teachers’ classroom practices. In
404). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
D. Lambdin (Ed.), Teaching and learning
mathematics: Translating research to the classroom (pp. Kliman, M. (1999). Beyond helping with
31–36). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers homework: Parents and children doing mathematics
of Mathematics. at home. Teaching Children Mathematics, 6(3),
140–146.
Bay‐Williams, J. M., & Meyer, M. R. (2003). What
parents want to know about standards‐based Meyer, M., & Arbaugh, F. (2008). Professional
mathematics curricula. Principal Leadership, 3(7), development for administrators: What they need
54–60. to know to support curriculum adoption and
implementation. In M. Meyer, C. Langrall,
CCSSO (Council of Chief State School Officers).
F. Arbaugh, D. Webb, & M. Hoover (Eds.), A decade
(2010). Common core state standards. Retrieved from
of middle school mathematics curriculum implementation:
http://corestandards.org
Lessons learned from the Show‐Me Project (pp. 201–
Civil, M., & Menendez, J. M. (2010). NCTM research 210). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
brief: Involving Latino and Latina parents in their
Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, J. C. (2008).
children’s mathematics education. Retrieved from www
Parent involvement in homework: A research
.nctm.org/uploadedFiles/Research_News_and_
synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 78(4),
Advocacy/Research/Clips_and_Briefs/Research_
1039–1101.
brief_17‐civil.pdf
Rodríguez‐Brown, F. V. (2010). Latino families:
Civil, M., & Planas, N. (2010). Latino/a immigrant
Culture and schooling. In E. G. Murillo, Jr., S. A.
parents’ voices in mathematics education. In E. L.
Villenas, R. T. Galván, J. S. Muñoz, C. Martínez, &
Grigorenko & R. Takanishi (Eds.), Immigration,
M. Machado‐Casas (Eds.), Handbook of Latinos and
diversity, and education (pp. 130–150). New York,
education: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 350–360).
NY: Routledge.
New York, NY: Routledge.
Else‐Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., & Hejmadi, A. (2008).
Seeley, C. L. (2009). Faster isn’t smarter: Messages
Mother and child emotions during mathematics
about math, teaching, and learning in the 21st century.
homework. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 10,
Sausalito, CA: Math Solutions.
5–35.
111
This page intentionally left blank
Developing Early Number
Concepts and Number Sense
From Chapter 8 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
113
Developing Early Number
Concepts and Number Sense
BigIDEAS To many adults number may seem like a simple idea. But
number is actually a complex and multifaceted concept.
1 Counting tells how many things are in a set. A complete and rich understanding of number involves
When counting a set of objects, the last word in the many different ideas, relationships, and skills. Although
counting sequence names the quantity for that set. children may come to school with many ideas about
2 Numbers are related to each other through number, it takes time and lots of experiences for them to
a variety of number relationships. The number 7, develop a full understanding that will enhance all of the
for example, is three more than 4, two less than 9, number‐related concepts they will encounter in future
composed of 3 and 4 as well as 2 and 5, is three away years.
from 10, and can be quickly recognized in several The emphasis that number and number sense receive
patterned arrangements of dots. These ideas further in the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics’
extend to an understanding of 17, 57, and 370. Curriculum Focal Points (NCTM, 2006) and the Common
3 Number concepts are intimately tied to the Core State Standards (CCSSO, 2010) speaks to their im-
world around us. Application of number relationships to portance in the early childhood curriculum. This chapter
the real world marks the beginning of making sense of looks at the development of number ideas for numbers up
the world in a mathematical manner. to about 20. These foundational ideas can all be extended
4 Having number sense means that you can think to larger numbers, operations, basic facts, and computation.
about and use numbers and their relationships in many
different ways.
2
114
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
with these experiences, there is variability in how different children learn to count and de-
velop number relationships. Much of this variability results from differences in opportuni-
ties to count and work with numbers and their relationships (National Research Council
Committee, 2009).
Early Counting
No matter what prior experiences children have had before coming to school, we need to
strive to help all children develop the following four interrelated aspects of early numerical
knowledge (Clements & Sarama, 2009):
1. Number sequence. The names and the ordered list of number words
2. One‐to‐one correspondence. Counting objects by saying number words in
a one‐to‐one correspondence with the objects
3. Cardinality. Understanding that the last number word said when counting
tells how many objects have been counted
4. Subitizing. Quickly recognizing and naming how many objects are in a small group
without counting. Young children can recognize and name quantities of objects
that are less than four without counting (Clements & Sarama, 2007).
Children must construct these ideas through a variety of experiences and activities.
They cannot be forced. As children work on each of these aspects of early counting, their
understanding about counting is continually refined. For example, children will learn how
to count (matching counting words with objects) before they understand that the last count
word indicates the amount of the set or the cardinality of the set. A teacher can at the same
time use subitizing to emphasize the notion of cardinality and to help emphasize the notion
that counting tells “how many.”
Although learning the number sequence may be considered a rote procedure, effort should
be given to help children build the number sequence in a meaningful way, especially numbers Standards for
in the teens. One recommendation is to connect English words with number words translated Mathematical Practice
from other languages that are more explicit about the structure of the numbers. For example,
7 Look for and make
map 11 to “ten and one,” 12 to “ten and two,” and so on. You can use a vertical number line ◀
use of structure
to help children visualize how numbers change as they begin writing two‐digit numbers (see
Figure 1). A vertical number line is also more intuitive than a typical horizontal number line
because it is consistent with the notion that as numbers become larger they go up the number
line as opposed to moving to the right.
Although the forward number sequence is relatively
familiar to most young children, mastering the backward
When learning the number sequence, some children do not
number sequence or counting back can be difficult. Pro-
realize that each number word represents a separate num-
vide children frequent opportunities to practice both the
ber, so they may touch two objects as they say two‐syllable
forward and backward number sequences through short
counting words such as “se‐ven” or “thir‐teen.”
and engaging activities such as the following.
115
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
nglish
e Lerane
visualize patterns in counting exercise. For example, line up five children and five chairs in front of the
rE
fo rs
our written numbers. class. As the whole class counts from 1 to 5, the children sit down one at a time. When the
target number, 5, is reached, it is repeated; the child who sat on 5 now stands, and the count
25
24 goes back to 1. As the count goes back, the children stand up one at a time, and so on, “1, 2,
23 3, 4, 5, 5, 4, 3, 2, 1, 1, 2, . . . .” Children find exercises such as this both fun and challenging.
22 Any rhythmic movement (clapping, turning around, doing jumping jacks) can be used as the
21 count goes up and back. You can modify the activity by varying the range of numbers. For
20 example, use 15 to 20 if the class is working on the teen numbers and use something like 55
19
to 65 if the class is ready to move to larger numbers (both of these ranges fit with kindergar-
18
17 ten expectations in CCSS). To modify this activity for English language learners, give each child
16 who is in front of the class one card from a set of cards with the target numerals (e.g., 1 to 5,
15 15 to 20, or 55 to 65) and the corresponding number
Numerals
14 word. These cards will provide a visual to help children
repeat
13
connect the written numeral and number word to the
12 1
11 2 number being said.
3 Another option involves having children stand in a
10 one
9 two circle and count around the circle to a target number.
three
8 One child starts the count at number 1; the next child
7 says the next number in the sequence, and so on, until
6
a child says the target number. That child sits down and
5
4 the next child starts the count again at number 1. The activity continues until one child is left
3 standing. You can vary the activity by using shorter or longer sequences, by starting the count
2 at a number other than 1, or by having the children count backward. Challenge children by
1 asking them to predict who will sit down next or who will say a particular number.
0
To prepare for this activity, stretch a clothesline across a space in the classroom at a level
where children can reach it. You can stretch it across a bulletin board or to the side of the
room. Prepare a set of numeral cards that represents the sequence of numbers you want
the children to work with, say, 1 through 15 for kindergarten. (You could also start at 0.)
There should be one numeral on each card. Shuffle the cards so that they are in a random
order and place the cards face down in a pile. Ask a child to take the top card from the
pile and use a clothespin to place the card on the clothesline. Have a second child take
the next card off the pile and place that card on the clothesline in what the child thinks
is the appropriate position. As children place their numeral cards, ask questions such as
“Is your number before or after . . .” and “Does your number go on the left or the right
of . . .?” Continue placing the cards until all the cards are placed on the clothesline in a
sequence. Once all the cards are placed, have the children read the sequence forward
as well as backward. If any number is out of sequence, see if the children can determine
how to correct the arrangement of cards. You can modify this activity by using shorter or
longer sequences and by using number sequences that start at different numbers.
The last two activities are designed only to help children become fluent with the number
words in both forward and reverse order and to begin counting with numbers other than 1.
116
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
To develop their understanding of counting, engage children in almost any game or activity
in which they have to purposefully count objects and make comparisons between counts.
The following is a simple suggestion.
Create a game board with four “towers.” Each tower is a column of twelve 1‐inch
squares with a star at the top. Children take turns rolling a die and collecting the
indicated number of counters. They then place these counters on one of the towers. The
object is to fill all of the towers with counters. As an option, require that the towers be
filled exactly so that a roll of 5 cannot be used to fill four empty spaces. To modify this
activity for a child with disabilities or for any child who is struggling with counting, use a
die with only two or three dots on each side. Using a larger‐sized die also makes it easier
for the child to count the dots. You can increase the number choices on the die when you
have evidence that the child is counting accurately. A good accommodation for gifted
children is to have them use a die with larger numbers and a different game board to
allow for the larger numbers.
This game provides opportunities for you to talk with children about number and assess
their thinking. Watch how the children count the dots on the die. Ask the children, “How
do you know you have the right number of counters?” and “How many counters did you put
in the tower? How many more do you need to fill the tower?” Fill the Towers
Place wooden cubes numbered 1 to 10 in a box. In pairs, children pull out a cube and then
collect that number of objects from a designated area in the classroom. The objects could be
books, pencils, cubes, crayons, counting bears, and so on. They place their numbered cube
and the collected objects in a container. Have pairs go around and check the other children’s
collections to make sure they have the correct number of objects. You can vary this activity
by changing the range of possible numbers to either a smaller range (2–5) or a larger range
(5–20) or by having each pair collect the same number of objects. Initially you may want chil-
dren to collect the same kind of object, but eventually have them collect a mix of objects to
reinforce the idea you can count different kinds of objects in a collection. For example, they
can collect two crayons, one cube, and one pencil for the number 4.
Regular classroom activities, such as counting how many napkins or snacks are needed at
snack time, how many materials are needed for an activity, how many children plan to eat the
school lunch, or even simply taking attendance, are additional opportunities for children to
engage in purposeful counting and to learn more about number and for teachers to listen to
children’s ideas. But make sure these activities involve more than the children simply following
the teacher’s count. Look for ways to make these situations into real problems.
117
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
Four—five, six, . . .
The following activity addresses the same concept in a more
problem‐based manner.
This game for two children requires a deck of cards (numbers 1 to 7), a die, a paper
cup, and counters. The first player turns over the top number card and places the
indicated number of counters in the cup. The card is placed next to the cup as a reminder
of how many are inside. The second player rolls the die and places that many counters next
to the cup. (See Figure 3.) Together they decide how many counters in all. A record
sheet with columns for “In the Cup,” “On the Side,” and “In All” will support children’s
118
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
organization. Increase the highest number in the card deck when children have
mastered the smaller numbers. For children with disabilities, keep the number of Figure 3
counters in the cup constant (say, 3) and have them count on from that number How many in all? How do
until they are fluent with that number. children count to tell the
total? Dump the counters?
Count up from 1 without
Observe how children determine the total amounts in the preceding activity.
dumping the counters?
Children who are not yet counting on may want to empty the counters from the cup Count on?
or will count up from 1 without emptying out the counters. Be sure to permit these
strategies. As children continue to play, they will eventually use counting on as that
strategy becomes meaningful and useful.
Initially when young children begin comparing sets, they may be easily confused by
perceptual cues such as the length of the row of counters or the spacing of counters in one set
119
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
Helping children relate numerals to number words and to quantities is important. Consider using a comput-
erized version of the game Concentration that offers options to limit the numbers between 1 and 6 or be-
tween 1 and 10 (http://illuminations.nctm.org/ActivityDetail.aspx?ID=73). Children can play individually
or with another player. You can also reduce the level of difficulty by selecting the option to have all the
numerals, numbers, and quantities visible so that the child can focus on moving back and forth between the
representations without having the added difficulty of remembering the locations of specific cards.
120
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
1 through 10
One More / Two More / One Less / Two Less
Once children have acquired a concept of cardinality and
can meaningfully use their counting skills, little more can
1 MORE 2 MORE
be gained from the kinds of counting activities described so
far. But too often teachers move directly from the beginning
ideas of counting to addition and subtraction, leaving chil- 6 7 9
dren with a very limited collection of ideas about number to
1 LESS 2 LESS
bring to these new topics. The result is often that children
continue to count by ones to solve simple story problems and
have difficulty mastering basic facts. Emphasizing number
Anchors to 5 and 10
relationships is key to helping children fully develop number
sense. Figure 6 illustrates the four different types of relation-
ships that children can and should develop with numbers:
• Spatial relationships. Children can learn to recognize
sets of objects in patterned arrangements and tell how
many without counting. For most numbers, there are Five and three more Two away
several common patterns. Patterns can also be made from ten
121
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
122
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
The next activity displays images quickly so that children do not have time to count
the dots one by one. Consequently, they are challenged to find another way to determine
how many. Dot plates that have three to five dots are useful in helping children work on
subitizing. Dot plates that have more than five dots are useful
in helping children work on advanced subitizing or decom-
Use quick images to encourage children to move beyond
position where they quickly see and use small groups within
counting by ones.
the larger amount to quickly determine how many.
Hold up a dot plate for only 1 to 3 seconds and say, “How many dots do you see? What
did the pattern look like?” Include easy patterns first and then add more dots as chil-
dren’s confidence builds. Initially you may need to show a plate a second time so that
children can get a second look. Children like to see how quickly they can recognize and
say how many dots. Children can also flash dot plates to each other as a workstation
activity.
The instant recognition activities with the plates are exciting and can be done in 5
minutes at any time of day or between lessons. There is value in using them at any primary
grade level and at any time of year.
In addition to dot plates, a good set of materials is a set of dot‐pattern dominoes. Make
a set of dominoes out of cardstock and put a dot pattern on each end. The size of the domi-
noes can be about 2 inches by 4 inches. The same patterns can appear on lots of dominoes
with different pairs of patterns making up each one. Let the children play dominoes in the
regular way, matching up the ends. As a speed activity, spread out all of the dominoes and see
how fast the children play all of the dominoes or play until no more can be played. Regular
dominoes could also be used, but there are not as many patterns.
123
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
The following activities are named for one of the more-than/less-than relationships,
but each activity can be done for any of these relationships.
Use the dot‐pattern dominoes or a standard set to play “one‐less‐than” dominoes. Play
in the usual way, but instead of matching ends, a new domino can be added if it has an
end that is one less than the end on the board. A similar game can be played for two less,
one more, or two more.
Provide children with about six dot cards. For each card, their task is to display a set of
counters that is two more than the set shown on the card. Similarly, spread out eight to
ten dot cards and, for each card, find a card that is two less than it. (Omit the 1 and 2
cards for two less than, and so on.)
In activities in which children find a set or make a set, they can select the appropriate
numeral card (a small card with a number written on it) that identifies the quantity in the
set. They can also be encouraged to take turns reading a number sentence to their partner.
If, for example, a set has been made that is two more than a set of four, the child can read
this by saying the number sentence “Two more than four is six.”
BLM The knowledge of 8 as “5 and 3 more” and as “2 away from 10” can play a role in how
a child thinks about these examples. Later, similar relationships can be used in the develop-
ment of mental computation skills on larger numbers such as 68 + 7.
Figure 8 The most common models to help children anchor numbers to 5 and 10 are five‐
Ten‐frames. frames and ten‐frames. The ten‐frame is a 2 * 5 array in which counters or dots are placed
to illustrate numbers (see Figure 8). Ten‐frames can be simply
drawn on a full sheet of cardstock (or use Blackline Master 1).
Nothing fancy is required, and each child can have one. The ten‐
frame is now a popular representation found in standard text-
books for children.
For children in pre‐kindergarten or kindergarten who have
not yet explored a ten‐frame, it is a good idea to begin with a
five‐frame. This row of five sections can also be drawn on a sheet
of cardstock (or use Blackline Master 2). Provide children with
about 10 counters that will fit in the five‐frame sections and con-
duct the following activity.
124
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
Explain that only one counter is permitted in each section of the five‐frame. No
other counters are allowed on the five‐frame mat. Have the children show 3 on
their five‐frame. “What can you tell us about 3 from looking at your mat?” After
hearing from several children, try other numbers from 0 to 5. Children initially Standards for
Mathematical Practice
may place their counters on the five‐frame in any manner. What they observe will
differ a great deal from child to child. For example, with four counters, a child 3 Construct
with two on each end may say, “It has a space in the middle” or “It’s two and ◀ viable arguments
and critique the
two.” There are no wrong answers. Focus attention on how many more counters
reasoning of others
are needed to make 5 or how far away from 5 a number is. Next try numbers
between 5 and 10. The rule of one counter per section still holds. As shown in
Figure 9, numbers greater than 5 are shown with a full five‐frame and additional
counters on the mat but not in the frame. In discussion, focus attention on these
Figure 9
larger numbers as 5 and some more: “Eight is five and three more.” A five‐frame focuses on the 5
anchor. Counters are placed one
to a section, and children tell how
Notice that the five‐frame really focuses on the relationship to 5 as an anchor for they see their number in the frame.
numbers but does not anchor numbers to 10. After five‐frames have been used for a
week or so, introduce ten‐frames. You may want to play a ten‐frame version of a “Five‐
Frame Tell‐About” but soon introduce the following rule for showing numbers on the
ten‐frame: Fill the top row first, starting on the left, the same way you read. When the
top row is full, counters can be placed in the bottom row, also from the left. This rule
will help children see the significance of a full row in the ten‐frame—in particular, if
the row is full, they do not need to count because it will always be 5. This observation
needs to come from the children, not the teacher. So look for opportunities to draw
children’s attention to this characteristic of the ten‐frame. Filling the ten‐frame in this
fashion also provides a visualization of numbers as 5 and some more, as can be seen in
Figure 8. Make sure to spend time asking questions such as “What are you looking at
in the ten‐frame to help you find how many?” and “How does knowing you have a full
row help you determine how many?”
For a while, many children will count every counter on their ten‐frame. When making a
new number, some children will remove all the counters from the ten‐frame and begin from
a blank frame. Others will soon learn to adjust numbers by adding on or taking off only what
is required, often capitalizing on a row of five without counting. Do not pressure children to
use one approach or another. With continued practice, all children will grow. How they are
using the ten‐frame provides insight into children’s current number concept development.
You can find virtual five‐ and ten‐frames at http://illuminations.nctm.org/Activity
Detail.aspx?ID=74 and http://illuminations.nctm.org/activitydetail.aspx?id=75, respectively.
These versions can be used not only with the whole class or small groups but also with
individual children because these activities feature a computerized voice that asks the chil-
dren questions. These versions allow children to build target numbers and include an option
that asks children to determine how many counters are displayed as well as how many empty
spaces are in a given frame.
First, have all children show the same number on their ten‐frame. Then call out random num-
bers between 0 and 10. After each number, the children change their ten‐frames to show
the new number. Children can also do this activity independently by using a prepared list of
about 15 random numbers. One child plays “teacher” while the rest use the ten‐frames.
125
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
“Number Medley” is much more of a problem‐solving situation than it first appears. How
do you decide how to change your ten‐frame to make the new number? Some children will
clear off the entire frame and start over with each new number. Others will have learned what
each number looks like. To add another dimension, have the children tell, before changing their
ten‐frames, how many more counters need to be added (“plus”) or removed (“minus”). They
then call out plus or minus whatever amount is appropriate. If, for example, the frames showed
six, and the teacher called out “Four,” the children would respond “Minus two!” and then
change their ten‐frames accordingly. A discussion of how they know what to do is valuable.
Ten‐frame flash cards are an important variation of ten‐frames. Make cards from cardstock
about the size of a small index card with a ten‐frame on each and dots drawn in the frames. A set
BLM of 20 cards consists of a 0 card, a 10 card, and two each of the numbers 1 to 9. The cards allow
for simple practice activities to reinforce the 5 and 10 anchors (see Blackline Masters 17 and 18).
Flash ten‐frame cards to the class or group and see how quickly the children can tell how
many dots are shown. This fast‐paced activity takes only a few minutes, can be done at
any time, and is a lot of fun.
Part–Part–Whole Relationships
126
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
Any child who has learned how to count meaningfully can count out eight objects as
you just did. What is significant about the experience is what it did not cause you to think
about. Nothing in counting a set of eight objects will cause a child to focus on the fact that
it could be made of two parts. For example, separate the counters you just set out into two
piles and reflect on the combination. It might be 2 and 6 or 7 and 1 or 4 and 4. Make a
change in your two piles of counters and say the new combination to yourself. Focusing on
a quantity in terms of its parts has important implications for developing number sense. The
ability to think about a number in terms of parts is a major milestone in the development
of number sense. Of the four number relationships we have discussed, part–whole ideas are
easily the most important.
Most part–part–whole activities focus on a single number for the entire activity.
Kindergarteners can usually begin these activities working on the number 4 or 5. As con-
cepts develop, the children can extend their work to numbers 6 to 12. It is not unusual
to find children in the second grade who have not developed firm part–part–whole con-
structs for the numbers 7 through 12, even though by that time they should be adding
up to 100. Figure 11
The following activity and its variations may be considered the “basic” part–part–whole
Assorted materials for
activity. building parts of 6.
• Use two‐color counters such as lima beans spray‐ Two column strips
painted on one side (also available in plastic).
• Make bars of connecting cubes. Make each bar
with two colors. Keep the colors together.
• Make combinations using two dot strips—strips
“Five and one” “Three and three”
of cardstock about 1 inch wide with stick‐on dots.
Dot strips
(Make lots of strips with one to four dots and fewer
strips with five to ten dots.)
• Make combinations of “two‐column strips.” These
are cut from cardstock ruled in 1‐inch squares. All Five and one
pieces except the single squares are cut from two
columns of the cardstock.
• Color rows of squares on 1‐inch grid paper. “One and three and two”
• Use arithmetic racks that have 10 beads in two Arithmetic rack
Grid paper
rows.
Standards for
Mathematical Practice
As you observe children working on the “Build It in Parts” activity, ask them to
“read” or write a number sentence to go with each of their representations. Reading ◀ 4 Model with
or writing the combinations serves as a means of encouraging reflective thought fo- mathematics
cused on the part–whole relationship. Writing can be in the form of drawings, numbers
127
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
128
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
A set of counters equal to the target amount is counted out, and the rest are put aside.
One child places the counters under a margarine tub or piece of cardstock. The child then
pulls some out into view. (This amount could be none, all, or any amount in between.)
For example, if 6 is the whole and 4 are showing, the other child says, “Four and two is
six.” If there is hesitation or if the hidden part is unknown, the hidden part is immediately
shown (see Figure 14).
Figure 14
Missing‐part activities.
You can also use a virtual format for any of the preceding activities by using the part–
part–whole model found at http://maine.edc.org/file.php/1/AssessmentResources/Add
Models_PPW‐NumLine32_L.html. Counters are dragged into the boxes that represent the
parts. The whole and either part can be hidden from view as well as the corresponding numerals.
Select a number between 5 and 12 and partner children to find combinations of two
dot cards totaling that number (see Blackline Masters 3–8). Children make a “sandwich” BLM
with the two cards by placing them back‐to‐back with the dot sides out. When they have
found at least 10 pairs, the next challenge is for the partner to name the number on the
other side. The cards are turned over to confirm. The same sandwiches can then be used
again to name the other hidden part.
129
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
Doug has a pocketful of pennies and nickels. He has 9 coins in all. He has 3 pennies. How
many nickels does Doug have?
Research has demonstrated that when kindergarten and first‐grade children are
regularly asked to solve word problems, not only do they develop a collection of number
relationships, but they also learn addition and subtraction facts based on these relationships.
The key is to allow them to figure out ways to solve the problems. Simply telling them
to solve the problems in a particular way robs them of the opportunities to build number
relationships.
130
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
Wright and his colleagues (2006) outlined a progression of three levels in children’s under-
standing of ten:
1. An initial concept of ten. The child understands ten as ten ones and does not see ten as a
unit. When children at this level work on a task involving tens, they will count by ones.
2. An intermediate concept of ten. The child understands ten as a unit composed of ten
ones but relies on materials or representations to help complete tasks involving tens.
3. A facile concept of ten. The child can solve tasks involving tens and ones without using
materials or representations. At this level children can mentally think about
two‐digit numbers as groups of tens and ones.
In order to help children begin to think about counting in ways that can move their un-
derstanding of “ten” forward, consider providing lots of purposeful opportunities for them
to count and group objects. For example, Fosnot and Dolk (2001) describe a K–1 teacher
who used the context of making necklaces using five beads of one color, then five beads of
another color, and then repeating these groups of five. Children could count by ones if they
needed to, but the teacher built in the constraint of creating necklaces with a five structure
to encourage the children to begin to count by fives. Eventually she introduced the idea of
selling the necklaces as a school fundraiser and suggested charging a penny per bead. Since
the beads are in groups of five, the children begin to work with nickels and dimes, further
Standards for
encouraging the children to begin to see five and ten as units. Mathematical Practice
Building from the idea mentioned earlier, mapping the teens number names to a ten
and one structure is an important idea (i.e., matching thirteen to “ten and three”). The ◀ 7 Look for and
activity that follows provides a way to help children visualize and reinforce the meaning of make use of structure
multidigit numbers.
131
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
Figure 16 From cardstock create a set of cards so that the tens card is twice as long as the ones card
and the hundreds card is three times as long as the ones card (see Figure 16). To start, chil-
Building numbers with
dren use the cards to create two‐digit numbers. Write a two‐digit number, say, 26, where
a set of cards.
children can see it. Children are to find the two cards that can
be used to make 26 (a 20 and a 6). Select children to demon-
strate to the class how to make the given number. Point out
600 that you can still see the 20 hiding under the overlay of the 6.
4 05007 2 numbers. You can also have children model the numbers with
materials, such as base‐ten materials or ten‐frames.
Given that their understanding of “ten” is likely to be at an initial concept level, the idea
of a single ten can be strange for a kindergarten or early first‐grade child to grasp. The dif-
ficulty of discussing “one ten and six ones” (what’s a one?) does not mean that a set of ten
should not figure prominently in the discussion of the teen numbers. The following activity
illustrates this idea.
Using a simple two‐part mat, have children count out ten counters onto one side. Next
have them put five counters on the other side. Together count all of the counters by ones.
Chorus the combination: “Ten and five is fifteen.” Turn the mat around: “Five and ten
is fifteen.” Repeat with other numbers in a random order but without changing the ten
side of the mat.
Activity 22 is designed to teach new number names and, thus, requires a certain amount
of directed teaching. Following this activity, explore numbers to 20 in a more open‐ended
manner. Provide each child with two ten‐frames drawn one under the other on a cardstock
BLM mat, or use Blackline Master 14. In random order, have children show numbers to 20 on their
mats. There is no preferred way to do this as long as the number of counters is correct.
132
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
133
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
Have children work in pairs to find patterns on the hundreds chart. Solicit ideas orally
from the class. Have children explain patterns found by others to be sure that all under-
stand the ideas that are being suggested.
There are many different patterns on the hundreds chart. In a discussion, different chil-
dren will describe the same pattern in several ways. Accept all ideas. Here are some of the
patterns they may point out:
• The numbers in a column all end with the same number, which is the same as the
number at the top.
• In a row, one number “counts” (the ones digit goes 1, 2, 3, . . . , 9, 0); the “second”
number (ones digit) goes up by ones, but the first number (tens digit) stays the same.
• In a column, the first number (tens digit) “counts” or goes up by ones.
• You can count by tens going down the right‐hand column.
• If you count by fives, you get two columns: the 5 column and the last column.
For children, these patterns are not at all obvious or trivial. For example, one child may
notice the pattern in the column under the 4—every number ends in a 4. Two minutes later
another child will “discover” the parallel pattern in the column headed by 7. That there is a
pattern like this in every column may not be completely obvious.
Although not essential, skip‐count patterns can also be explored at an early level. Skip
counts by twos, fives, and tens are the easiest and the most important. Help children see the
column patterns that these counts make.
Provide children with a hundreds chart on which some of the number cards have been
removed. (You can use a classroom pocket chart.) The children’s task is to replace the
missing numbers in the chart. Beginning versions of this activity have only a random
selection of individual numbers removed. Later, remove sequences of several numbers
from three or four different rows. Finally, remove all but one or two rows or columns.
Eventually, challenge children to replace all of the numbers in a blank chart.
The “Missing Numbers” activity can also be done with the full class. Use cardstock tabs
to cover numbers on the chart. Have children write the missing numbers as you point to
them. You may think that the adjacent numbers are too much of a clue, but the clue is itself
a help in learning the number sequence.
134
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
Calendar Activities
There are significant issues with calendar activities being considered the kind of mathemat-
ics instruction that will support young learners in reaching mathematical literacy.
Although the calendar may be helpful in developing a sense of time, the National
Research Council Committee (2009) has stated that “using the calendar does not emphasize
foundational mathematics” (p. 241) and has pointed out that the calendar does not support
the development of key mathematical relationships related to 10 because the calendar is
based on groups of seven. The committee concludes, “Doing the calendar is not a substitute
for teaching foundational mathematics” (p. 241). Therefore, doing calendar mathematics
should be thought of as an “add on” and should not take time away from developing essen-
tial pre-K–2 mathematics concepts.
135
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
To help with numbers and measures, estimate several things in succession using the
same unit. For example, suppose that you are estimating and measuring “around things”
using a string. The string is wrapped around the object and then measured in some unit
such as popsicle sticks. After measuring the distance around Demetria’s head, estimate the
distance around the wastebasket or around the globe or around George’s wrist. Each succes-
sive measure helps children with the new estimates.
Write a number on the board. Now suggest some units to go with it, and ask the children
what they can think of that fits. For example, suppose the number is 9. “What do you
think of when I say 9 dollars? 9 hours? 9 cars? 9 kids? 9 meters? 9 o’clock? 9 hand spans?
9 gallons?” Spend some time in discussion of each. Let children suggest units as well. Be
prepared to explore some of the ideas either immediately or as projects or tasks to share
with parents at home.
Activity 27 IS IT REASONABLE?
Select a number and a unit—for example, 15 feet. Could the teacher be 15 feet tall?
Could your living room be 15 feet wide? Can a man jump 15 feet high? Could three chil-
dren stretch their arms 15 feet? Pick any number, large or small, and a unit with which
children are familiar. Then make up a series of these questions.
Standards for
Mathematical Practice Once children are familiar with Activity 27, have them select the number and the
unit or things (e.g., 10 kids, 20 bananas), and see what kinds of questions children cre-
3 Construct ate. When a difference of opinion develops, capitalize on the opportunity to explore
viable arguments ◀
and find out. Resist the temptation to supply your adult‐level knowledge. Say instead,
and critique the “Well, how can we find out whether it is reasonable? Who has an idea about what we
reasoning of others
could do?”
Graphs
Graphing activities are another good way to connect children’s worlds with numbers.
Graphs can be quickly made from almost any data that can be gathered with children: fa-
vorite ice cream, color, sports team, type of pet; number of sisters and brothers; kids who
ride different buses; types of shoes; number of pets; and so on. Graphs can be connected
to content in other subjects. A unit on sea life might lead to a graph of favorite sea animals.
Once a simple bar graph is made, it is very important to take a few minutes to ask as
many number questions as are appropriate for the graph. In the early stages of number
development (grades pre-K−1), the use of graphs for number relationships and for connect-
ing numbers to real quantities in the children’s environment is a more important reason for
building graphs than the graphs themselves. The graphs focus attention on counts of real-
istic things. Equally important, bar graphs clearly exhibit comparisons between and among
136
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
numbers that are rarely made when only one number or quantity is considered at a time.
See Figure 19 for an example of a graph and questions that can be asked. At first, children
will have trouble with the questions involving differences, but repeated exposure to these
ideas in a bar graph format will improve their understanding. These comparison concepts
add considerably to children’s understanding of number.
137
E x p a n d e d L e s s o n
I Wish I Had
Content and Task Decisions language. Other possibilities are for them to show the
answer by holding up the appropriate number of fin-
Grade Level: K–1 gers. Or, if they are readers, provide a translation list of
the numbers.
Mathematics Goals
• To develop part–whole relationships by focusing on the For Children with Disabilities
missing part
• Instead of the two‐column cards, use connecting cubes.
Grade Level Guide • Use a part–part–whole mat. Let children place the “I
wish I had” number in the area representing the whole.
NCTM Common Then use the other number as a part to help structure
Curriculum Focal Points Core State Standards their thinking about the relationship.
138
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
Teacher will need: they counting on from your card? Are they using card
• Either a projection device or a large shapes instead of number of squares? You may want
demonstration set of two‐column BLM to have them put the 7 card away; however, some
cards (see Blackline Master 34) children may need to have it visible.
• Note: Several alternative materials • Listen to how confident children are as they say their
can be used, such as connecting cubes, dot plates, or combinations.
ten‐frames. • If 7 seems to present little challenge for all in the
group, make a big deal out of changing the game to
“I wish I had 8.” Alternately, if 7 seems difficult, simply
Lesson switch to 5 or 6.
Before
After
Present the focus task to the class:
Bring the class together to share and discuss the task:
• This lesson will be described for the number 7, but it
can be done for any number from 6 to 10. • “Suppose that I show the 3 card. Tell me how you
decide which card should go with it to make 7.” Get
• Have children place a 7 card in front of them and return
responses from several children. Repeat the question
all cards greater than 7 to the bag.
with another card.
• Show the class a two‐column card for any number less
• “Is there any number that is easy for you? Why?”
than 7 and say, “I wish I had 7.” The children’s task is to
find the two‐column card from their set that goes with • “Does the shape of the card help you? How?”
your card to make 7. For example, if you hold up (or place
on the floor) a 2 card, they would have to find the 5 card.
• When children have found the card that makes 7 when Assessment
combined with your card, they should hold it up si- Observe
lently. Call on a child to say the 7 combination: “Five
• Children’s success with this task depends somewhat on
and two makes seven.”
their familiarity with the two‐column cards, though
• Add context to make it more interesting. Then student it depends more on their experiences with part–part–
responses would include the context: “Five balloons and whole activities for the number 7 (or whatever number
two more balloons makes seven balloons for my party!” you have selected).
Provide clear expectations: • There are many ways that children may use to find the
correct card. They may first count the squares on your
• This lesson can be done with a small group of children
card and then select a card and count on. They may
or the full class sitting in a circle on the floor. All chil-
seem to know which card is needed but still have to
dren must be able to see the teacher materials clearly.
count to find it. Others may use a purely spatial ap-
proach, comparing the shape of your card to the 7 card
During that is in front of them and locating the missing piece.
Ongoing: • Children for whom the game is easy and who do not
• Occasionally stop and ask children how they decided on need to count or rely on the spatial characteristics of
the card so that you can get insights into their thought the cards can be said to “know 7,” at least in terms of
processes. parts and wholes.
• Do not forget to include 0 (indicate “no card” by mim- • It is not a goal to be able to recognize numbers in this set
ing the placing of a card) and also 7. without counting, but some children will quickly be able
to recognize each of the cards without counting (subi-
• For both right and wrong responses, ask whether other
tize). This provides another way to think about numbers.
children agree. Another idea is to ask “How can we tell
if that is correct?” Be sure to do this for both right and
wrong responses. Ask
• Pay attention to the methods that children use to find • “Suppose I show the 4 card. Tell me how you decide
the required card. Do they count squares? Can they which card should go with it to make 7.”
easily tell how many squares are on your card? Are • “How did you find the missing part?”
139
Developing Early Number Concepts and Number Sense
References
CCSSO (Council of Chief State School Officers). National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
(2010). Common core state standards. Retrieved from (2006). Curriculum focal points for prekindergarten
http://corestandards.org through grade 8 mathematics: A quest for coherence.
Reston, VA: Author.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2007). Early
childhood mathematics learning. In F. K. Lester, National Research Council Committee. (2009).
Jr. (Ed.), Second handbook on mathematics teaching Mathematics learning in early childhood: Paths toward
and learning (pp. 461–555). Charlotte, NC: excellence and equity. Washington, DC: National
Information Age Publishing. Academies Press.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2009). Learning and Steffe, L., & Cobb, P. (1988). Construction of
teaching early math: The learning trajectories approach. arithmetic meanings and strategies. New York, NY:
New York, NY: Routledge. Springer‐Verlag.
Fosnot, C. T., & Dolk, M. (2001). Young Wright, R., Stanger, G., Stafford, A., & Martland,
mathematicians at work: Constructing number J. (2006). Teaching number in the classroom with 4–8‐
sense, addition, and subtraction. Portsmouth, NH: year‐olds. London: Sage.
Heinemann.
Howden, H. (1989). Teaching number sense.
Arithmetic Teacher, 36(6), 6–11.
140
Developing Meanings
for the Operations
From Chapter 9 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
141
Developing Meanings
for the Operations
BigIDEAS
1 Addition can be thought of as physically or conceptually placing two or more quantities together.
2 Subtraction can be thought of as taking an amount away from a given quantity, comparing two quantities, or
finding a missing part given the whole and the other part.
3 Multiplication in grades pre-K–2 involves counting groups of equal size and determining how many are in all.
4 Division in grades pre-K–2 can be thought of as sharing equal amounts among a given number of groups or as
repeatedly measuring out the same amount from a given total.
5 The operations are related to each other. Addition names the whole in terms of the parts, and subtraction names a
missing part. Multiplication can be thought of as repeated addition. Division names a missing factor in terms of the known
factor and the product. Division can also be thought of as repeated subtraction.
6 Models can be used to solve contextual problems for all operations and to figure out what operation is involved in a
problem. Models also can be used to give meaning to number sentences.
142
Developing Meanings for the Operations
Aidan had 7 beads. After she bought some more beads, she had 15 beads. How many beads
did Aidan buy?
Most adults and older students solve the problem by subtracting 15 - 7. But young children
do not initially view this as a subtraction situation because 15 - 7 is the “opposite” operation Standards for
Mathematical Practice
or action implied in the problem. Instead they will mimic the implied action in the problem
and solve it by adding on or counting up from 7 until they reach 15. Their approach may 1 Make sense
seem less efficient than simply subtracting, but it makes sense to young children because ◀ of problems and
persevere in
it mirrors the situation in the problem. Eventually, after much experience making sense of
solving them
story problems, working with different combinations of numbers, and examining the results
of using addition and subtraction in these situations (possibly at their teacher’s request),
they will begin to generalize that they get the same result by subtracting and will join older
students and adults in using subtraction for these kinds of problems. This example illustrates
how children’s initial conceptions are different from those of adults—even with something
we consider so basic as addition and subtraction.
Children’s conceptions are the best foundation on which to build future learning.
Therefore, the perspective on addition and subtraction taken in this chapter is based
on what has been learned from numerous research studies (Gutstein & Romberg, 1995;
Carpenter, Fennema, Franke, Levi, & Empson, 1999; NRC, 2009; Verschaffel, Greer, & De
Corte, 2007; Clements & Sarama, 2009). Through this research, we are aware that children
can solve contextual or story problems with appropriate numbers by reasoning through
the relationships in the problems. We also know that different problems have different
structures that can affect the difficulty level of the problem. When teachers are familiar with
these structures, they are better able to plan and differentiate instruction.
143
Developing Meanings for the Operations
Standards for Verschaffel, Greer, & De Corte, 2007; Clements & Sarama, 2009; NRC, 2009; CCSSO,
Mathematical Practice 2010). Table 1 illustrates one categorization scheme that identifies four types of problems
7 Look for and make based on the relationships involved. The problem types include join (add to) problems, sepa-
◀
use of structure rate (take from) problems, part-part-whole (collection) problems, and comparison problems.
Table 1 Addition and Subtraction Problem Types Using the Number Family 4, 8, 12
Problem Type
and Structure Result Unknown Change Unknown Start Unknown
Physical Action Involved
Join (Add To) Sandra had 8 pennies. Sandra had 8 pennies. Sandra had some
George gave her George gave her some pennies. George
Change 4 more. How many more. Now Sandra has gave her 4 more.
pennies does Sandra 12 pennies. How many Now Sandra has 12
have altogether? did George give her? pennies. How many
pennies did Sandra
Start Result
have to begin with?
Separate (Take From) Sandra had 12 pennies. Sandra had 12 pennies. Sandra had some
She gave 4 pennies to She gave some to pennies. She gave
Change George. How many George. Now she has 4 to George. Now
pennies does Sandra 8 pennies. How many Sandra has 8 pennies
have now? did she give to George? left. How many
pennies did Sandra
Start Result
have to begin with?
144
Developing Meanings for the Operations
Join/Add To Problems. Join problems describe situations in which quantities are physi-
cally being brought together. These problems are also known as add to problems because the
action or change occurring in the problem is a result of adding or joining quantities.
Separate/Take From Problems. Separate problems are commonly known as take away
or take from problems in which part of a quantity is physically being removed or taken away.
Notice that in the separate problems, the start amount is the whole or the largest amount,
whereas in the join problems, the result is the whole.
Structure versus Operation. Each type of problem involves a number “family,” such
as 4, 8, 12, that can be related through addition or subtraction. Note that the problems are
described in terms of their structure and not as addition or subtraction problems. A different
problem results within each problem type depending on which of the three quantities in the
structure is unknown.
In most curricula, the overwhelming emphasis is on the easier join and separate
problems with the result unknown. These become the de facto definitions of addition and
subtraction: Addition is “put together” and subtraction is “take away.” But these are not the
only situations in which we use addition and subtraction, as you can see from Table 1. When
children develop these limited put‐together and take‐away definitions for addition and sub-
traction, they often have difficulty later when addition or subtraction is called for but the
structure of the problem is something other than put together or take away. It is important
that children experience all the problem types to ensure they are developing a broader un-
derstanding of addition and subtraction. You can find more examples of these problem types
in the Common Core State Standards (see Table 1 in the CCSSI Math Standards Glossary;
CCSSO, 2010, p. 88).
145
Developing Meanings for the Operations
In writing an equation for each of the problems in Table 1, you may have written
some equations where the unknown quantity is not isolated on one side of the equal sign.
For example, a likely equation for the join problem with start unknown is + 4 = 12.
This is referred to as the semantic equation for the problem since the numbers are listed
in the order that follows the sequence in the problem. When the semantic form does not
Standards for isolate the unknown, an equivalent equation can be written for the same problem. In this
Mathematical Practice case, the equation 12 - 4 = is referred to as the computational form of the equation; it
isolates the unknown and is typically how most adults think about the problem. This may be
4 Model with
◀ an efficient way to solve the problem, but children typically begin to think about and model
mathematics
the situation based on the sequence played out in the problem, which is modeled more
appropriately by the semantic equation. When the two forms are not the same, you should
help children eventually come to see the equivalence of these equations. However, initially
they should be allowed to use the semantic form of the equation if this equation better
represents their way of reasoning through the problem.
Jacob has 5 cards. His brother gives him 8 more cards. How many cards does Jacob have now?
Children who use direct modeling to find a solution model every number and action the
situation describes using actual objects, fingers, or drawings. So a child who is working
at the direct modeling level would count out 5 blocks, then count out 8 more blocks, and
then count them all to find 13—this is called a “count all” strategy. A child who is using
counting strategies does not need to model every number in the situation. So this child
might start at 8 and count up 5, holding up one finger for each number word from 9 up
to 13—this is called a “counting on” strategy. A child functioning at the derived facts level
often looks for ways to decompose the numbers in a given situation to make an easier
problem. This child may reason that because 8 is 2 away from 10, he can decompose 5 into
2 and 3, add the 2 to the 8 to get 10, and then add 3 more to get 13—this is called an “up
over 10” strategy. A child who is using derived facts may reason so quickly that it appears
that the fact is a known fact. In this case, the distinction between derived and known facts
is arbitrary.
Problem Difficulty
Structure
The various types of problems are not at all equal in difficulty for children. Problems in
which a physical action is taking place, as in join and separate problems, are easier be-
cause children can model or act out the situation. However, even within these types of
146
Developing Meanings for the Operations
problems, some problems are more difficult than others. Consider each of the following
three problems.
Maggie had 7 bracelets. She bought 8 more bracelets. How many bracelets does Maggie
now have?
Maggie had 7 bracelets. She bought some more bracelets. She now has 15 bracelets. How
many did Maggie buy?
Maggie had some bracelets. She bought 8 more bracelets. She now has 15 bracelets. How
many bracelets did Maggie start with?
Unknown start problems are more difficult than unknown change problems and unknown
change problems are more difficult than unknown result problems. Unknown start problems
are among the most difficult probably because children modeling the problems directly do
not know how many counters to put down to begin with. For
these problems, children initially use a trial‐and‐error ap- Many children have difficulty with problems in which the
proach (Carpenter, Fennema, Franke, Levi, & Empson, 1999) start is unknown because they try to model the problem
to determine the unknown start amount. The easiest of the in chronological order and they cannot make the set that
three problems is the first one, in which the start and change represents the beginning of the problem. Write a question
are known because to find the unknown result the known mark on an index card and have children use the card to
quantities are simply combined, an action that is implied in represent the unknown amount.
the problem. The result unknown problems tend to be the
most commonly used in classrooms. Make sure to provide your children a wide range of expe-
riences by also posing join and separate problems with more difficult structures.
Part-part-whole problems can be difficult for children for two reasons. First, there is no
action to model because the situation describes a conceptual bringing together of quanti-
ties, which is difficult to directly model. Second, it is a challenge for children to grasp that a
quantity can represent two things at once. For example, if the problem describes 3 cars and
4 trucks in a parking lot and asks how many vehicles are in the lot, children have to under-
stand that the cars and trucks are also part of the larger category of vehicles.
The challenge of comparison problems comes from the fact that two quantities are
being described using language that can be complex for children. Fewer, less than, more,
bigger, and greater than are the terms typically used to describe the relationships in
comparison problems. Children often have more expe-
riences with the relationships of more and bigger than,
If children struggle to make sense of the relationship be-
so you need to ensure they have opportunities to think
tween the quantities in a comparison problem, suggest they
about relationships described using fewer and less than.
cover up the number that comes before the word more/
Note that when the bigger amount is unknown, stating
fewer (e.g., George has ■ more pennies than Sandra; Sandra
the problem using the term more is easier for children
has ■ fewer pennies than George). This strategy helps them
because the relationships between the quantities and the
determine which quantity is bigger or smaller.
operation more readily correspond to each other. In the
147
Developing Meanings for the Operations
smaller unknown situation, stating the problem using the term fewer is easier for children
for the same reason. Similar to the part-part-whole problems, the lack of a physical ac-
tion in these situations makes it difficult for children to model or act out these kinds of
problems.
A school of 28 fish was swimming together in the ocean. Another school of fish decided to
join them, making a larger school of fish. The new larger school had 64 fish. How many fish
were in the second school of fish that joined the first group of fish?
Because the preceding problem has a join action, this increases the probability that children
will use a counting‐on or an add‐on approach to solve the problem. Using an open number
line to support a more efficient strategy than simply counting on by ones, a child might rea-
son as follows: Add 2 to 28 to get to 30, add 30 more to get to 60, add 4 more to get to
2 30 4
64. Then add 2 + 30 + 4 (what was added to 28 to get 64). Another child might add on
40 to 28 making 68 and then take off the 4 extra—4 from 40 is 36.
28 30 60 64
Wording of Problems
Problems that are worded so that the actions and quantities follow in a chronological or
natural story order are generally easier than those in which the problem order is inverted.
For example, in the following pair of problems, notice how the second problem matches a
natural order of events:
Mike’s grandpa gave him some money for his birthday. Mike already had 6 dollars. Now
Mike has 9 dollars. How much did Mike’s grandpa give him?
148
Developing Meanings for the Operations
Mike had 6 dollars. His grandpa gave him some money for his birthday. Now he has 9 dollars.
How much did Mike’s grandpa give him?
Join problems in which the change is unknown can be thought of in terms of “how
much more is needed” or in terms of “how much was added.” Some children find actions
that happened in the past more difficult to grasp—as in “how much was added?” These chil-
dren would find the first of the following two problems easier:
Joyce has 3 eggs in her basket. How many more eggs does she need to find to have 8
eggs?
Joyce has 3 eggs in her basket. She found some more eggs for her basket in the chicken
coop. Now she has 8 eggs. How many more eggs did Joyce find in the chicken coop?
When you think that issues with wording are the cause of difficulty, pose problems with
similar structure but with wording in which the actions and quantities follow a chronological
or natural story order. If it is the wording that is causing the difficulty, have children gain
confidence with the easier wording, but eventually they need to be challenged to make sense
of the more difficult wording. Here are some strategies to help support children when they
are challenged with more difficult wording:
• When children do not seem to know what to do with a problem, ask them first to
talk about what is happening in the problem. (See the suggestions in the later section,
“Encourage Problem Analysis.”)
• Have children use counters to act out the problem. Have them say out loud what each
set of counters stands for and explain with the counters what is happening in the prob-
lem. This can help children with similar analyses when you are not there to make these
suggestions.
• Have children draw out pictures that represent each of the quantities in the problem.
Then have them try to articulate the relationships between these quantities.
If you have English language learners (ELLs) in your classroom, you may be tempted to
avoid using story problems because of the concern that language might present a challenge
for them. However, contextual problems are good for ELLs because they connect to life
experiences. Some strategies to support comprehension of story problems include structur-
ing the sentences to use present and past tenses, using a noun‐verb word order, replacing
terms such as his/her and it with a name, and removing unnecessary vocabulary words. Also
ensure that ELLs understand the meaning of any contextual terms (e.g., chicken coop, model
boat, savings account) as well as any mathematical terms (e.g., how many, fewer, feet) used in
story problems.
Introducing Symbolism
Very young children initially have no need for the symbols +, -, and =. However, these
symbolic conventions are important. When you feel your children are ready to use these
symbols, introduce them in the discussion portion of a lesson where children have solved Standards for
story problems. Say, “You had the number 12 in your problem and the number 8 was one Mathematical Practice
of the parts of 12. You found out that the part you did not know was 4. Here is a way we 4 Model with
can write that: 12 - 8 = 4.” The minus sign should be read as “minus” or “subtract” but not ◀ mathematics
as “take away” because not all subtraction situations are take away situations. The plus sign
149
Developing Meanings for the Operations
is easier since it is typically a substitute for “and.” Alternatively, for the same problem, you
could introduce the equation as 8 + 4 = 12, especially if a child has described a counting‐up
strategy to find 4.
Some care should be taken with the equal sign. The equal sign means “is the same as.”
However, many children come to think of it as a symbol that tells you that the “answer
is coming up” or “it is time to do a computation.” It is
Use the phrase “is the same as” in place of or in conjunction interpreted in much the same way as the = key on a calcula-
with “equals” as you read equations with children. tor. That is, it is the key you press to get the answer. Find
opportunities to write equations like 12 = 8 + 4 to counter
this misinterpretation of the equal sign. So, for example, if a child has described how she
broke or decomposed 8 into 3 and 5, you could record that idea as 8 = 3 + 5. The next activ-
ity helps to emphasize the meaning of the equal sign.
Addition
When the parts of a set are known, addition is used to name the whole in terms of the parts.
This simple definition of addition serves both action situations (join and separate) and static
or no‐action situations.
Each part-part-whole model shown in Figure 1 is a model for 5 + 3 = 8. Some
of these are the result of a definite add to or joining action, and some are not. Notice
that in every example, both of the parts are distinct, even after the parts are joined. For
children to see a relationship between the two parts and the whole, the image of the
5 and 3 must be kept as two separate sets. For example, if counters are used, the two
parts should be kept in separate piles or in separate sections of a mat or should be two
distinct colors. This helps children reflect on the action after it has taken place. “These
red chips are the ones I started with. Then I added these five blue ones, and now I have
eight altogether.”
150
Developing Meanings for the Operations
5 3
5 3
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Two hops on a
number line
Eight hops on an open number line (Note the whole hop.)
In this activity, children are challenged to find different combinations for a particular num-
ber. When children are ready to deal with written symbolism, simply show them how to
write an addition number sentence (equation) for each design. Initially with “Build It in
Parts” children said a combination, such as “Four and five is nine.” Now they have a new
symbolic way to represent and record what they say.
151
Developing Meanings for the Operations
You can find an interactive number line at www.eduplace.com/kids/mw that emphasizes the unit by using
animals (a frog, a rabbit, or a kangaroo) to hop along the number line. Once at the site, select the grade
level (K, 1, or 2), e‐Manipulatives, and then Number Line. Each hop is marked with an arc and a length and
you can change the length of the hops from one to ten. You can also change the direction of the hop so
that the animal hops forward (addition) or backward (subtraction).
The next activity provides an opportunity for children to make sense of a number line.
Create a large number line on the floor of your classroom by using colored tape. (Mask-
ing tape comes in a variety of colors and will not leave adhesive on your floor.) Make sure
Standards for to start with zero and include arrows at each end of the line. Alternatively you could dis-
Mathematical Practice play a number line in the front of your room. Use a stuffed animal, like a frog or rabbit,
2 Reason abstractly for hopping along the number line, or ask a child to walk on the number line if using the
◀ floor model. Pose a variety of problem situations and talk about the movement required
and quantitatively
for each situation. To begin with, your problem situations should mirror the idea of mov-
ing a distance to emphasize the spaces (units of length) between the numbers on the
number line. For example, use a scenario of a baby rabbit making 5 hops away from its
Figure 3 mother and then making 3 hops back. The children’s task is to determine how many hops
Models for 9 - 4 as a
away the baby is from its mother. This activity can help children create a mental image for
missing‐part problem.
thinking about the meaning of addition and subtraction.
Subtraction
?
In a part-part-whole model, when the whole and one of the parts are known, subtraction
can be used to name the other part. If you start with a whole set of 8 and remove a set of 3,
9
the two sets that you know are the sets of 8 and 3. The expression 8 - 3, read “eight minus
No action three,” names the five remaining. Notice that the models in Figure 1 are models for subtrac-
tion as well as addition (except for the action). Helping children see that they are using the
Start with 9 in all. same models or pictures aids in connecting the two operations.
Remove some.
How many covered?
e Lerane
rE
Break some off. What’s Hiding in There? (Drescher, 2008) offer scenarios that can be used as contexts.
How many hidden?
Explain to the children that they will model whatever is hiding using a fixed number of
tiles placed on a mat. One child separates the tiles into two parts while another hides his
or her eyes. The first child covers one of the two parts with a sheet of paper or a large
index card, revealing only the other part (see Figure 3). The second child says the
subtraction sentence. For example, “Nine minus four [the visible part] is five [the covered
The other part of part].” The covered part can be revealed for the child to self‐check. Have children record
the bar is hidden. both the subtraction equation and the addition equation. ELLs may need sentence
prompts such as “_____ minus ______ is ________.”
152
Developing Meanings for the Operations
Subtraction as Think-Addition
Note that in Activity 4, the situation ends with two parts clearly distinct, even when there
is a remove action. The removed part remains in the activity or on the mat as a model for
an addition equation to be written after writing the subtraction equation. A discussion of
how these two equations can be written for the same model is an important opportunity
to connect addition and subtraction. This is significantly better than the traditional work-
sheet activity of “fact families” in which children are given a family of numbers, such as 3,
5, and 8, and are told to write two addition equations and two subtraction equations. Very
quickly this becomes a matter of putting the numbers in the various slots without much
meaning.
Subtraction as “think‐addition” is extremely significant for mastering subtraction
facts. Because the tiles for the remaining or unknown part are left hidden under the
cover, when children do these activities they are encouraged to think about the hidden
part: “What goes with the part I see to make the whole?” For example, if the total or
whole number of tiles is nine, and six can be seen, the child is likely to think in terms of
“6 and what makes 9?” or “What goes with 6 to make 9?” The mental activity is “think‐
addition” instead of a “count what’s left” approach. Later, when working on subtraction
facts, a subtraction fact such as 9 - 6 = should trigger the same thought pattern: “6
and what makes 9?”
Comparison Models
Comparison situations involve two distinct sets or quantities and the difference between them.
Several ways of modeling the difference relationship are shown in Figure 4. The same kind of
model can be used whether the difference or one of the two quantities is unknown.
153
Developing Meanings for the Operations
Tania is on page 8 in her book. Tomorrow she hopes to read 6 more pages. What page
will she be on if she reads that many pages?
The recycling bin in the cafeteria had 6 bottles in it. During lunch 8 more bottles were
put in the bin. How many bottles were in the bin after lunch?
Ask if anyone notices how these problems are alike. If done as a pair, some children will see
that having solved one they have essentially solved the other. You will likely need to pose
multiple problem pairs across time before children become convinced that the order does
not matter.
The following activity helps with the same idea.
154
Developing Meanings for the Operations
Give children six sums to find involving three or four addends. Prepare these on one page
divided into six sections so that there is space to write beneath each sum. Within each,
include at least one pair with a sum of 10 or perhaps a double: 4 + 7 + 6, 5 + 9 + 9, or 3 +
4 + 3 + 7. Children should discuss and show how they added the numbers. Allow children
to find the sums without any other directions.
Figure 5 illustrates how children might show what they did. As they share their solu- Standards for
tions, almost certainly there will be children who added in different orders but got the Mathematical Practice
same result. From this discussion you can help them conclude that you can add numbers
8 Look for and
in any order. But continue to find opportunities to highlight this idea because some chil- ◀ express regularity in
dren will need additional time to be convinced that the order does not matter. You are repeated reasoning
also using the associative property, but it is the commutative property that is more im-
portant here. This is also an excellent number sense activity because many children will
find combinations of 10 in these sums or will use doubles (easy facts for many children).
Learning to adjust strategies to fit the numbers is the beginning of the road to computa-
tional fluency. Note that some children will attempt to overgeneralize the commutative
property to subtraction. Use contextual situations or story problems to help children
confront this misconception. Figure 5
Using story problems with zero or with zero in the three‐addend sums (e.g., 4 + 0 + 2) Children show how they
is also a good method of helping children understand zero in addition or subtraction. added.
Occasionally children think that 6 + 0 must be more than 6 or that 12
- 0 must be 11 since “addition makes numbers bigger” or “subtraction 4+7+6 5+9+9 3+4+3+7
makes numbers smaller.” Instead of making meaningless rules, create
10 18 7 10
opportunities for discussing adding and subtracting zero using contex-
tual situations. 17 23 17
155
Developing Meanings for the Operations
Jill has 4 bags of crayons. There are 3 crayons in each bag. All together she has 12 crayons.
In a given problem, any of these three quantities can be unknown. When the total is
unknown, the problem is a multiplication situation. When either the number of groups or
the size of the groups is unknown, the problem is a division situation. But note that these
latter two situations are not alike. Problems in which the size of the groups is unknown are
called fair‐sharing or partition problems. The whole is shared or distributed among a known
number of groups to determine the size of each. If the number of groups is unknown but the
size of the equal groups is known, the problems are called measurement or repeated subtrac-
tion problems. The whole is “measured off” in groups of the given size.
There is also a subtle difference between problems that might be termed equal‐group
problems (e.g., If 3 children have 4 apples each, how many apples are there?) and those that
might be termed rate problems (e.g., If there are 4 apples per child, how many apples would 3
children have?). Rate problems may be more difficult for children because they involve a rate
rather than a number of countable objects. However, because they can be thought of in much
Standards for
Mathematical Practice the same way as the equal‐group problems, you should consider posing such problems.
Examples of the problem types for multiplication and division are shown in Table 2. Prob-
7 Look for and
◀ lems matching these structures can be modeled with sets of counters, number lines, or arrays.
make use of structure
(The term multiplicative is used here to describe all problems that involve multiplication and
division structure.) You can find more examples of these problem types in the Common Core
State Standards (see Table 2 in the CCSSI Math Standards Glossary; CCSSO, 2010, p. 89).
In multiplicative problems, one number or factor counts how many groups or parts
of equal size are involved. The other factor tells the size of each group or part. The third
number in each of these two structures is the whole or product and is the total of all of the
parts. The parts and wholes terminology is useful in making the connection to addition.
156
Developing Meanings for the Operations
There are 3 golf balls in a tube. How many balls are in 6 tubes?
A child who is directly modeling would make 6 groups with 3 counters in each group and
would count all the counters to find the answer. A child who has moved to the next level
might hold up a finger one at a time as she counts by three 6 times: 3, 6, 9, 12, 15, 18.
Another child who is not as proficient at skip counting by threes may start skip counting
and then revert to counting by ones: 3, 6, 9, 12, . . . 13, 14, 15, . . . 16, 17, 18. This child is
still using a counting strategy. When a child is using derived facts, he uses known facts to
find unknown facts. For this problem, a child might reason that 3 * 5 is 15 and 3 more is 18.
For partition division, let’s look at the following problem:
Arielle has 12 lollipops. She wants to share them equally among 4 of her friends. How many
will each friend get?
When direct modeling, a child will count out 12 counters and then separate them into 4 Standards for
groups, probably placing one counter in each of the 4 groups until all 12 counters are dis- Mathematical Practice
tributed. He would then count the counters in one of the groups to find the answer. For a 1 Make sense
child who is using a counting strategy to solve a partition division problem, the child will ◀ of problems and
typically use trial and error to determine how many might be in each group because she persevere in
is trying to mimic the action in the problem. So the child might first skip‐count by twos, solving them
keeping track of the number of twos on her fingers: 2, 4, 6, 8. Because 4 twos is not enough,
she tries another number, say, 3: 3, 6, 9, 12. When she reaches 12 with 4 fingers raised, she
realizes each friend would get 3 lollipops.
For measurement division, consider this problem:
Parker has 24 cupcakes. He wants to store the cupcakes 6 to a box. How many boxes will he need?
A child who is directly modeling will count out 24 counters and then repeatedly measure
out groups of 6 counters until no more groups of 6 are possible. He will then count the
number of groups to find the answer. For a child who is using a counting strategy, he will
count by sixes, keeping track of each count, say, by extending a finger: 6, 12, 18, 24. Looking
at 4 extended fingers, he knows Parker will need 4 boxes. If a child is using derived facts, he
may think 5 * 6 is 30, but one less 6 would be 24—so, 4 boxes.
157
Developing Meanings for the Operations
to write equations to express the total as a sum of equal addends (CCSSO, 2010). If you feel
your children are ready, introduce the multiplication sign and explain what the two factors
Standards for
Mathematical Practice mean. You can also write one sentence that expresses both concepts at once, for example,
9 + 9 + 9 + 9 = 4 * 9.
6 Attend to ◀ In the United States, the usual convention is that 4 * 8 refers to four sets of eight, not
precision
eight sets of four. (In other countries such as Japan and Korea, the convention is that 4 * 8
refers to eight sets of four.) The more important idea at this level is the informal explora-
tions with multiplication and division situations and not the introduction of symbolism.
Griffin has 11 brownies and he wants to share them with 4 people. How many brownies will
each person get?
Figure 6 Some children will handle partition problems like this one by fairly sharing, as can be seen
Remainders expressed
as fractions. in the first part of Figure 6.
It is a bit more difficult to conceptualize a measurement problem
Partition 11 ÷ 4 = 2 34 with a fractional remainder. Consider the following problems:
2 34 in each of the 4 sets
(each leftover divided in fourths)
Tania was filling crates with special oranges. Each crate holds 4 oranges.
If Tania has 11 oranges, how many crates will she need?
Toby has 11 cups of milk in a pitcher. If he pours all 11 cups into jars that
hold 4 cups each, how many jars will Toby fill?
The first problem can be modeled as 2 full crates and 34 of another crate (see
the second part of Figure 6). This requires thinking of each crate as a whole
Measurement 11 ÷ 4 = 2 34
3
2 4 sets of 4 and assumes that the child will think about a partial crate as part of the an-
(2 full sets and 34 of a set) swer. In the milk problem, there may be a greater chance of thinking about
a jar being 34 full. If you want to introduce these fractional ideas and no one
in the class suggests them, offer them yourself. You can say, “If someone said
that Toby filled two and three‐fourths jars with milk, would that make sense?
Do you think that could be a correct answer?” There is no need to be afraid
of these ideas even if they do not appear in your curriculum.
More often than not, division situations in real life do not result in a simple whole
number. In the absence of a context, a remainder can be dealt with in only two ways: It can
either remain a quantity left over or be partitioned into fractions. In Figure 6, the problem
11 , 4 is modeled to show fractions.
In real contexts, remainders sometimes have three additional effects on answers:
1. The remainder is discarded, leaving a smaller whole‐number answer.
2. The remainder can “force” the answer to the next highest whole number.
3. The answer is rounded to the nearest whole number for an approximate result.
158
Developing Meanings for the Operations
• Left over. You have 10 pieces of candy to share fairly with 3 children. How many pieces
of candy will each child receive? Answer: 3 pieces of candy and 1 left over.
• Partitioned as a fraction. You have 9 brownies and you want to fairly share with 4 chil-
dren. How many brownies will each child receive? Answer: 2 and 14 brownies.
• Discarded. A rope is 15 feet long. How many 7‐foot jump ropes can be made? Answer:
2 jump ropes.
• Forced to next whole number. If 4 children can ride in each car, how many cars are
needed to take to 23 children to the museum? Answer: 6 cars.
• Rounded, approximate result. If 6 children are planning to share a bag of 50 pieces of
bubble gum, about how many pieces will each child get? Answer: About 8 pieces for
each child.
In the beginning, children will be able to use the same models—sets and number lines—for ◀ 5 Use appropriate
tools strategically
all four operations. A model not generally used for addition but extremely important and
widely used for multiplication and division is the array. An array is any arrangement of
objects in rows and columns, such as a rectangle of square tiles or blocks. A variety of models
is shown in Figure 7.
The Common Core State Standards recommend that second graders experience using
the array model with no more than five rows and five columns to lay the foundation for
5×3=3+3+3+3+3
6×4=4+4+4+4+4+4
5×8=8+8+8+8+8
6 × 3 = 18 3 + 3 + 3 + 3 + 3 + 3 = 18
0 5 10 15 20
6×7
20
7+7+7+7+7+7
4 4 4 4 4
0 5 10 15 20
5 × 4 = 20
4 + 4 + 4 + 4 + 4 = 20
159
Developing Meanings for the Operations
multiplication (CCSSO, 2010). However, even younger children can use the array model to
begin to help them organize quantities and skip‐count. Because you want to move children
beyond counting by ones and encourage children’s use of
rows and columns as units, you need to use small quantities
The idea of organizing quantities into rows and columns
to play off their ability to subitize (quickly recognize the
might seem a bit arbitrary to children. So it is important to
amount without counting).
use contexts in which this arrangement seems natural.
The following activity uses contexts and the idea of using small
numbers of rows and columns to help children make sense of the array
model as well as move beyond counting by ones.
To use contexts to introduce the array model, find places where you live that organize ob-
jects into arrays, such as a bakery or a grocery store. Take pictures of a tray of doughnuts
or muffins or a box of oranges or apples. You can also find pictures online for this pur-
pose. (Check out the website for the Harry & David stores as one possibility.) Show these
pictures to your children and set up the task by telling them that the baker, grocer, or
person making an online order needs their help to determine how many items are on the
tray or in the box. Allow children to use a strategy that makes sense to them, which ini-
tially may be counting by ones. Look for children who are using skip counting and make
sure to have them share their strategy so that this idea is introduced to other children.
You can repeat this activity several times with different contexts and different pictures.
Keep in mind that even if you use arrays that are arranged in quantities more likely
to be subitized, some children may not be ready to move beyond counting by ones. Allow
them to count by ones but continue to provide them opportunities to work with these
kinds of arrays and to hear how other children are paying attention to and using small
amounts to efficiently skip‐count. Given time, these children will also begin to use more
efficient strategies.
Using contexts also helps to encourage children to explore division situations. Snack
time provides lots of opportunities for children to explore the notion of fair sharing or parti-
tion division as the next activity illustrates.
Read Snack Attack (Ruschak & Carter, 1990) as a lead‐in to this activity. In groups, children
work with a bag of snacks, such as crackers or cookies, to determine how many each child
in the group will get. Because the children will be handling the snacks, you can either
160
Developing Meanings for the Operations
use pictures of the snacks that have been cut out or have additional snacks on hand for
children to eat after the activity. To differentiate, you can prepare baggies of different
amounts of the snack and place children in different size groups. For example, you can
give a bag of 12 cookies to a group of 3 children; a bag of 10 cookies to 3 children; a bag
of 16 cookies to 4 children; or a bag of 15 cookies to 4 children. The scenarios depend
on your children and whether they are ready for the challenge of dealing with leftovers.
Circulate as children are engaged in this sharing activity, asking children to explain their
reasoning. You can capture their ideas on chart paper to help them share their ideas later
in a discussion. Look for ways that children dealt with leftovers as well as whether chil-
dren distributed the snacks in amounts other than by ones.
Roberts (2003) described a teacher who used Snack Attack and a similar sharing activ-
ity with her kindergartners. The article describes the various strategies children used to
fairly share their snack and how the teacher supported the children as they shared their
strategies during discussion time. You can refer to the article for more ideas. One Hun-
dred Hungry Ants (Pinczes, 1999) and Remainder of One (Pinczes, 2002) are two additional
children’s books that provide engaging contexts for children to explore multiplication
and division.
As with addition problems, children who are investigating multiplication and division
situations can benefit from a few activities with models and no context. The purpose of such
activities is to focus on the meaning of the operation and the associated symbolism, if chil-
dren are ready for symbolism. Activity 8 has a good problem‐solving spirit. The language
you use depends on what you have used with your children in the past.
Provide children with an ample supply of counters and a way to place them into small
groups. Small paper cups work well. Have children count out a number of counters to
be the whole or total set (e.g., start with 18). They then record this number. Next specify
either the number of equal sets to be made or the size of the sets to be made: “Separate
your counters into four equal‐sized sets,” or “Make as many sets of four as possible.”
Next, if ready for symbolism, have the children write the corresponding repeated ad-
dition or multiplication equation for what their materials show. You can also challenge
children to write a corresponding story problem.
Be sure to include both types of exercises: number of equal sets and size of sets. Discuss
with the class how these two situations are different yet how each is related to multiplication
and can be written as repeated addition and multiplication equations. Do Activity 8 several
times. Start with whole quantities that are multiples of the divisor (no remainders) but soon
include situations with remainders.
Vary the activity by changing the model used. Have children build arrays using square
tiles or have them draw arrays on centimeter grid paper. Present the exercises by specify-
ing how many squares are to be in the array. You can then specify the number of rows that
should be made (partition) or the length of each row (measurement). How could children
model fractional answers using drawings of arrays on grid paper?
161
Developing Meanings for the Operations
Figure 9
Two ways an array can be used to illustrate the commutative (order) property for multiplication.
Turn
3×6
Rows
6×3
6×3 3×6
3 rows of 6
Columns
6 rows of 3
162
Developing Meanings for the Operations
Maxine gave 28 stickers she no longer wanted to Zandra. Now Maxine has 73 stickers left.
How many stickers did Maxine have to begin with?
If you look through the story problems in this chapter, you will find other examples of
misleading key words.
163
Developing Meanings for the Operations
3. Many problems have no key words. Especially when you get away from the
overly simple problems found in primary textbooks, you will find that a large
percentage of problems have no key words. A child who has been taught to rely on
key words is left with no strategy. In both the addition and the multiplication
problems in this chapter, you will find numerous examples of problems with
no key words.
4. Key words cannot be used to solve multistep problems, which begin in grade
3 in the Common Core State Standards (2010). Using key words for simpler
problems does not require children to learn to read for meaning. When faced
with multistep or more complex problems in later grades, they will struggle
if they have not learned to read word problems for meaning.
Luke is saving up to buy a new model boat that costs $33. Each week his dad agrees to put
$6 in a savings account if Luke does chores around the house. How many weeks will Luke
have to work before he has enough money to buy the model boat?
Here are some questions that you might have the class discuss or answer on paper.
Similar questions can be used for most any problem:
• What is happening in this problem? Luke is working to save money to buy a boat.
• What will the answer tell us? The answer will tell us how many weeks Luke will have to
work in order to have enough money.
• Do you think it will be a big number or a small number? Well, each week he gets $6. If he
works 10 weeks, he would have $60. He only needs $33. So it must be less than 10.
• About how many weeks do you think he will have to work? [Although some children may be able
to compute the answer mentally, others may not even be able to make a good guess.]
With these questions, children are asked to focus on the problem and the meaning of the
answer. The analysis of the problem leads to an idea of the size of the answer or a rough
estimate of the answer. Knowing about how big the answer might be—even knowing if it
will be more or less than one of the numbers in the problem—is a big first step in solving
the problem. It is also useful information in judging the answer when the problem is solved.
Standards for
Mathematical Practice
164
Developing Meanings for the Operations
verbal statements to drawings to written words. You may find that children who know their
basic facts or have learned traditional methods of computing will write little more than the
computations they used. It is important to make clear that you want explanations, nearly
always using words and numbers and often using drawings as well.
The solutions shown in Figure 10 are from children aged 4 to 7. Laura, a beginning
second grader, used separate equations to represent the amounts. Emma, who was just
beginning first grade, chose to use a weekly calendar format to help her think about the
problem. She explained that the calendar helped her keep track of the amounts. She had
Figure 10
Three pre-K–2 children solve a complex problem using different types
of drawings and explanations for their work.
165
Developing Meanings for the Operations
initially drawn only circles for each day, counted the circles,
and wrote the large numeral 15 on her paper. When asked
how she got 15, she labeled each circle as she counted by
Sometimes children can become so involved in drawing ones to reach 15. Abby, a pre‐kindergarten child, drew her
detailed realistic pictures of objects mentioned in problems picture on her own after hearing the story problem read
that they lose sight of the mathematics (Crespo & Kyriakides, twice. Notice how Abby used spacing to help organize her
2007). So you will likely need to engage in a discussion with ideas. When asked how many eggs were found, she said she
children about what makes a good drawing in mathematics counted and got 15. She wrote the numeral 15 with help.
versus a good drawing for other purposes. In mathematics, As children continue to attempt to show their reason-
drawings should be simple and only include information rel- ing, they will improve both from practice and from seeing
evant to solving the problem. the methods used by others. For the examples in Figure
10, the children are showing how they solved the problem.
To extend their explanations, you might ask questions such as, “Why do you think your answer
is correct?” or “Why did you decide that addition (or whatever operation was used) was the right
thing to do in this problem?”
166
E x p a n d e d L e s s o n
167
Developing Meanings for the Operations
168
Developing Meanings for the Operations
References
CCSSO (Council of Chief State School Officers). Gutstein, E., & Romberg, T. A. (1995). Teaching
(2010). Common core state standards. Retrieved from children to add and subtract. Journal of Mathematical
http://corestandards.org Behavior, 14, 283–324.
Christelow, E. (2004). Five little monkeys play hide‐ Pinczes, E. (1999). One hundred hungry ants. New
and‐seek. New York, NY: Clarion. York, NY: Houghton Mifflin.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2007). Early Pinczes, E. (2002). Remainder of one. New York,
childhood mathematics learning. In F. K. Lester, NY: Houghton Mifflin.
Jr. (Ed.), Second handbook on mathematics teaching
Roberts, S. (2003). Snack math: Young children
and learning (pp. 461–555). Charlotte, NC:
explore division. Teaching Children Mathematics,
Information Age Publishing.
9(5), 258–261.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2009). Learning and
Ruschak, L., & Carter, D. (1990). Snack attack:
teaching early math: The learning trajectories approach.
A tasty pop‐up book. New York, NY: Simon &
New York, NY: Routledge.
Schuster.
Crespo, S., & Kyriakides, A. (2007). To draw or
Schifter, D., Bastable, V., & Russell, S. J. (1999).
not to draw: Exploring children’s drawings for
Making meaning for operations: Facilitator’s guide
solving mathematics problems. Teaching Children
(Developing mathematical ideas: Number and
Mathematics, 14(2), 118–125.
operations, part 2). Parsippany, NJ: Dale Seymour.
Drescher, D. (2008). What’s hiding in there?
Verschaffel, L., Greer, B., & De Corte, E. (2007).
Edinburgh, Scotland: Floris.
Whole number concepts and operations. In
Fosnot, C. T., & Dolk, M. (2001). Young F. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on
mathematicians at work: Constructing number mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 557–628).
sense, addition, and subtraction. Portsmouth, NH: Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of
Heinemann. Mathematics.
169
This page intentionally left blank
Helping Children
Master the Basic Facts
BigIDEAS
1 Number relationships provide the foundation for strategies that help children to remember basic facts or figure
out unknown facts from those already known. For example, knowing how numbers are related to 5 and 10 helps children
master facts such as 3 + 5 (think of a ten‐frame) and 8 + 6 (since 8 is 2 away from 10, take 2 from 6 to make 10 + 4 = 14).
2 Finding the total number of objects arranged in equal‐sized groups helps children to build a foundation for
multiplication. This situation can be represented using repeated addition.
3 Mastery of the basic facts is a developmental process. Children move through stages starting with counting, then
moving to more efficient reasoning strategies and eventually to quick recall. Instruction must help children move through
these phases without rushing them to memorization.
4 Children who are not developing fluency with the basic facts may need to revisit foundational ideas, such as
number relationships. Simply continuing to drill will not address the cause of their difficulty and can negatively affect their
confidence and attitude toward and success in mathematics.
Basic facts for addition refers to combinations in which both addends are
less than 10. Subtraction facts correspond to the addition facts. Thus,
15 - 8 = 7 is a subtraction fact because both the corresponding addition
parts are less than 10.
Mastery of a basic fact means that a child can give a quick response
(in about 3 seconds) without resorting to nonefficient means, such as
counting. According to the Common Core State Standards (CCSSO,
2010), by the end of second grade, children should have mastered their
basic addition and subtraction facts. It is critical that children know
their basic facts well, but mastery does not just start in second grade.
If you teach in grades pre-K-1, this chapter is also for you because you
have a very important role in helping children to be ready to master
their basic addition and subtraction facts by the end of second grade.
From Chapter 10 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
171
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
Aspects of number sense that are critical to children developing fluency with basic facts
begin in pre‐kindergarten and continue throughout the primary grades. The Common Core
State Standards (CCSSO, 2010) indicate that kindergartners should represent and solve ad-
dition and subtraction situations (putting together and adding to; taking apart and taking
from) within 10 using any method that makes sense to them, such as using objects, their
fingers, drawings, or mental images. First graders extend addition and subtraction to a wider
variety of situations and to within 20, still using any method that makes sense to them.
This chapter explains strategies for helping children learn their facts, including instruc-
tional approaches to use and instructional approaches to avoid. It may surprise you to hear
that flash cards and timed tests are not the answer. Focusing on number sense is key to
teaching basic facts effectively.
Memorization
Some textbooks and teachers move from presenting concepts of addition and subtraction
straight to memorization of facts, feeling that developing strategies is not essential to learning
facts (Baroody, Bajwa, & Eiland, 2009). This “passive storage view” (the idea that children can
just store the facts when they are practiced extensively) means that children have 100 separate
addition facts for the various combinations of 0 through 9 that must be memorized and prac-
ticed frequently. They may even have to memorize subtraction separately, bringing the total to
over 200! There is strong evidence that this method simply does not work. Too many fourth
and fifth graders have not mastered addition and subtraction facts and continue to count on
their fingers. You may be tempted to say that you learned your facts in this manner, as did
many other children. However, studies as long ago as 1935 found that despite of the amount
of isolated drill that they experienced, children independently develop a variety of different
thought processes or strategies for basic facts (Brownell & Chazal, 1935). You may even be
aware that you found your own strategies for facts that were difficult to remember. Unfortu-
nately, drill does not encourage or support the refinement of these strategies. Baroody (2006,
p. 27) notes that this approach to basic fact instruction works against the development of the
five strands of mathematical proficiency, pointing out the following limitations:
• Inefficiency. There are too many facts to memorize in a rote fashion.
• Inappropriate applications. Children misapply the facts and don’t check their work.
• Inflexibility. Children don’t learn flexible strategies for finding sums and, there-
fore, continue to count by ones. (Reprinted with permission from Why Children Have
Difficulties Mastering the Basic Number Combinations and How to Help Them, copyright
2006, by the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. All rights reserved.)
When taught basic facts via rote memorization, many children with learning disabilities
continue to use counting strategies because they do not independently develop thought pro-
cesses or other strategies that move beyond counting (e.g., Mazzocco, Devlin, & McKenney,
172
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
2008). However, they can be very successful in learning their basic facts when the empha-
sis is on using strategies. In addition, drill can cause unnecessary anxiety and undermine
children’s interest and confidence in mathematics. Connecting to what children know and
building on that knowledge allows all children to learn the basic facts and to learn them for
life.
Guided Invention
With this approach, basic fact instruction and mastery are intricately tied to children’s col-
lection of number relationships (Gravemeijer & van Galen, 2003). Some children may think
of 6 + 7 as “double 6 is 12 and one more is 13.” Others in the same class may recognize that Standards for
7 is 3 away from 10, so they take 3 from 6 to add to 7 to make 10, and then add the remain- Mathematical Practice
ing 3 to get 13. Still other children may take 5 from each addend to make 10 and then add
7 Look for and
the remaining 1 and 2 to get 13. The crucial point is that children are using number combi- ◀ make use of structure
nations and relationships that make sense to them.
Gravemeijer and van Galen (2003) called this approach guided invention because not all
children will independently develop many of the strategies that are efficient without some
guidance. That is, we cannot put all of our efforts on number relationships and the meanings
of operations and hope that fact mastery will magically happen. Instead you need to pur-
posefully design tasks and problems that will promote the
invention of effective strategies by children. And then you
need to ensure that these strategies are shared and clearly Having children share their reasoning in think‐alouds can
articulated in the classroom. help others understand the decisions they made.
173
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
Figure 1
The developmental process for basic fact mastery for addition and subtraction.
Addition Subtraction
Counting Direct modeling (counting objects and fingers) Counting objects
• Counting all • Separating from
• Counting on from first • Separating to
• Counting on from larger • Adding on
Counting abstractly
• Counting down
• Counting up
Source: Reprinted with permission from First‐Grade Basic Facts: An Investigation into Teaching and Learning
of an Accelerated, High‐Demand Memorization Standard. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
copyright 2008, by the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. All rights reserved.
2. Reasoning strategies. Using known facts and relations to logically determine the answer
for an unknown combination. (Example: For 4 + 7, the child knows that 3 + 7 is 10,
so 4 + 7 is one more or 11.)
3. Mastery. Producing answers quickly and accurately. (Example: For 4 + 7, the child
quickly responds, “11; I just know it.”) (Reprinted with permission from Why Children
Have Difficulties Mastering the Basic Number Combinations and How to Help Them, copyright
2006, by the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. All rights reserved.)
Figure 1 outlines the developmental methods children use to solve basic addition and
subtraction problems. In this chapter, we will focus on reasoning strategies and how to teach
children to use reasoning to master the basic facts (phases 2 and 3).
174
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
Reasoning Strategies
The purpose of developing reasoning strategies is to help children move from count-
ing to more efficient ways of recalling facts quickly and accurately. In order for you to
support your children in using effective strategies, you need to have a command of as many
successful strategies as possible. With this knowledge you will be able to recognize effec-
tive strategies as your children develop them and then help others make sense of their
peers’ ideas.
Rachel sold 9 boxes of Girl Scout cookies on Friday and 6 boxes on Saturday. How many
boxes did she sell?
Suppose that Sammie explains that because 9 is 1 away from 10, she took 1 from 6 to Standards for
Mathematical Practice
make 9 into 10 and then added the remaining 5 to get to 15. Make explicit the expectation
that children are to attend to their classmates’ ideas. You can ask another child to explain 3 Construct
what Sammie just shared. You can also explore with the class what other facts might work ◀ viable arguments
and critique the
with Sammie’s strategy. reasoning of others
Continue to develop and use story problems that are readily solved using the Make 10
strategy to give children more opportunities to make the strategy their own.
Posing a story problem such as the preceding one each day, followed by a brief dis-
cussion of the strategies that children used, can improve children’s accuracy and efficiency
with basic facts (Rathmell, Leutzinger, & Gabriele, 2000).
Research has found that when a strong emphasis is placed
on children solving problems, they not only become better Children with disabilities may find it difficult to keep all of
problem solvers but they also master more basic facts than their peers’ ideas in working memory, so display the ideas on
children in a drill program (National Research Council, a board or on chart paper as an effective support.
2001).
175
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
Some teachers hesitate to use story problems with ELLs or children with disabilities
because of the additional language or reading required, but because language supports un-
derstanding, using story problems is important for all children. It is crucial, however, that
the contexts you use are relevant and understood by all the children.
176
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
Standards for
Activity 1 HOW MANY FEET IN THE BED? Mathematical Practice
-La
nguag
4 Model with
Read How Many Feet in the Bed? by Diane Johnston Hamm. On the second time ◀ mathematics
nglish
e Lerane
rE
through the book, when a new person gets in the bed, ask children how many more
fo rs
feet are in the bed. Have children record the equation (e.g., 4 + 2) and tell how
many feet in all. Standards for
As family members get out of the bed, two less can be explored. If children continue Mathematical Practice
to count all, let them use a number line or ten‐frame. Find opportunities to make the 8 Look for and
connection between counting on two and adding two using a number line or ten‐frame. ◀ express regularity in
For ELLs, clarify the meaning of foot (it is used here as a body part, not as a unit of mea- repeated reasoning
sure) and be sure to they know what the phrases “two more” and “two less” mean. You
can further support ELLs and children with disabilities by acting out the story with chil-
dren in the classroom.
The different responses will provide you with a lot of information about children’s Figure 2
number sense. As children are ready to use the two‐more‐than idea without counting all,
One more and two more
they can begin to practice with activities such as the following.
activities.
Two Dice
+2 5
“Five and two
more is seven”
Activity 2 ONE MORE THAN AND TWO
MORE THAN WITH DICE AND SPINNERS
+1 +1 7 6
Make a die labeled +1, +2, +1, +2, “one more,” and
“two more.” Label another die with the values 3, 4, 5, Spinner and Die
6, 7, and 8, or whatever values children need to practice. After
each roll of the dice, children should say the complete fact:
“Four and two is six.” Alternatively, use a spinner with “1 2
more” on one half and “2 more” on the other half in place of more
the first die (see Figure 2). For children with disabilities, you 1 “Eight plus one
may want to start with a die that just has +1 and “one more” more is nine.”
and then move to a +2 and “two more” die on another day. 8
This approach will help these children focus on and practice 3 5
one approach.
Activity 2 can be modified for almost all of the strategies in the chapter. + 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
0 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1 1
Adding Zero
2 2
Nineteen facts shown in the accompanying chart have zero as one of the addends. 3 3
Though such problems are generally easy, some children overgeneralize the idea that 4 4
5 5
answers to addition problems are bigger than the addends. Word problems involving
6 6
zero and subsequent discussions will be especially helpful in avoiding this overgen- 7 7
eralization. In the discussion, use drawings that show two parts with one part empty. 8 8
The following activity provides children opportunities to generalize from a set 9 9
of problems, which reinforces reasoning.
177
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
Using 5 as an Anchor
Using an anchor (5 or 10) is a reasoning strategy that builds on children’s knowledge of
number relationships and so is a great way to reinforce number sense while learning the
basic facts. Using 5 as an anchor means looking for fives in the numbers in the problem. For
example, in 7 + 6, a child may think of 7 as 5 + 2 and 6 as 5 + 1. The child would add 5 + 5
and then the extra 2 from the 7 and the extra 1 from 6, adding up to 13.
Five‐ and ten‐frames and the arithmetic rack can help children visualize numbers as
5 and some more. The following activity extends the use of these visual models to support
children’s reasoning strategies for addition.
Activity 4 FLASH
Project two ten‐frames on the board. Without letting children see, place counters on
each ten‐frame so that the top row is full (five counters) and the extras are in the bottom
row of each ten‐frame. Uncover the two ten‐frames for about 3 to 5 seconds and then
BLM
recover. First, ask children to share how many counters they saw and accept all answers.
Then ask children to explain how they saw it. Again quickly uncover and then cover the
ten‐frames and ask if anyone wants to change their answer. Finally, uncover the ten‐
frames and have children explain how they saw the counters. Alternatively, you can use
two ten‐frame cards from Blackline Master 18 or two arithmetic racks.
Make 10
Perhaps the most important strategy for children to know is the Make 10 strategy, or the
combinations that make 10. The ten‐frame and the arithmetic rack are useful tools for
creating visual images of the number relationships. The next activity is a good way to intro-
duce the Make 10 strategy. Story problems using two numbers that make 10 or that ask how
many are needed to make 10 can provide children additional opportunities to develop the
Make 10 strategy.
178
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
Knowing number combinations that make 10 also builds a foundation for working on
addition with larger numbers. For example, when adding 28 + 7, a child can use the Make
10 strategy by adding 2 to 28 to get to 30 and then adding 5 more. This strategy can even
be extended to make 100.
Up Over 10
This strategy is also known as Make 10 Extended. Many facts have sums greater than
10, and all those facts can be solved using the Up Over 10 strategy, making this a very
useful strategy. With this strategy, children use their known facts that equal 10 and
then add the rest of the number onto 10. For example, to solve 6 + 8, children may
recognize that 8 is 2 from 10; so they take 2 from the 6 and add the 2 to 8 to make 10
and then add on the remaining 4 to get 14. This process is called Break Apart to Make
Ten, or BAMT (Sarama & Clements, 2009), and aligns with children’s previous experi-
ences with ten‐frames and arithmetic racks. If you are trying to encourage children to
come up with this strategy on their own, pose problems that have at least one addend
of 8 or 9.
Notice that many of the basic addition facts can be solved using the Up Over 10 strat-
egy (about a third). Moreover, this strategy can be later applied to adding up over other
benchmark numbers such as 20 and 50. Therefore, you should spend a significant amount of
time and attention to helping children develop this strategy.
Give children a mat with two ten‐frames. Flash cards are placed next to the ten‐frames,
or a fact can be given orally. Children should cover each frame with counters to represent
the problem. For example, for the problem 9 + 6, children would cover nine places on one
frame and six on another. Then children are to decide on a way to move the counters so
that they can find the total without counting. Ask children to explain what they did and Standards for
connect their ideas to the new equation. Mathematical Practice
For example, 9 + 6 may become 10 + 5 by moving one counter to the first 4 Model with
◀
ten‐frame. mathematics
179
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
Move 1
to make 10.
7 7 plus 9 is
+9
the same as
10 and 6:
16.
Note that children may have many other ways of using 10 to add with 8 or 9. For example,
with the fact 9 + 5, some will add 10 + 5 and then subtract 1. This is a perfectly good strategy
that uses 10.
+ 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Doubles
0 0 There are ten doubles facts from 0 + 0 to 9 + 9, as shown in the chart here. These
1 2 facts can be anchors for other facts.
2 4
3 6
Many children find doubles easier to grasp than other facts. However, all chil-
4 8 dren, and especially children with disabilities, can benefit from using and creating
5 10 picture cards for each of the doubles as shown in Figure 5. Story problems can focus
6 12 on pairs of like addends:
7 14
8 16
9 18 Alex and Zack each found 7 seashells at the beach. How many did they find together?
e Lerane
rE
“double maker” (2 * __) into the calculator. Then one child says a double—for ex-
fo rs
ample, “Seven plus seven.” The other child should press 7 on the calculator, say what the
double is, and then press = to see the correct double (14) on the display. The children then
switch roles. For ELLs who are just learning English, invite them to say the double in their
native language or in both their native language and English. (Note that the calculator is
also a good way to practice +1 and +2 facts.)
S M T W T F S 9
4 +9
1 2 3
7 +4
4 5 6 7 8 9 10
11 12 13 14 15 16 17
+7
18 19 20 21 22 23 24
25 26 27 28 29 30 31
5
+5
S 8
N
6 AYO +8
+6 CR
180
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
Flash
7 Cards ble
6 3 4 +6 5 Dou
+1
+6 +3 +4 +4 9
6
3 +8
8 1 9 +5
+4 5 7
+8 +1 +9 +6 + 8 Double
+1
7
2 7 5
+2 +7 +5 +8 Put the 6
near-double 7 +6
on the double +6
fact that helps.
Near‐Doubles
+ 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
Near‐doubles are also called the “doubles‐plus‐one” or “double‐minus‐one” facts
0 1
and include all combinations in which one addend is one more or one less than 1 1 3
the other. This strategy uses a known fact to derive an unknown fact. Double the 2 3 5
smaller number and add 1 or double the larger number and subtract 1. Be sure chil- 3 5 7
dren solidly know the doubles before you focus on this strategy. 4 7 9
5 9 11
To introduce the strategy to the class, write several near‐doubles facts on the 6 13
11
board. Place the smaller addend first for some of the problems and second for oth- 7 13 15
ers. Have children solve problems independently and then discuss their strategies. 8 15 17
Some may double the smaller number and add one and others may double the 9 17
larger and subtract. If no one uses a near‐double strategy (they may use the Up
Over 10 strategy), write the corresponding doubles for some of the facts and ask how these
facts could help. This strategy is more difficult for students to recognize and, therefore, may
not be a strategy that all children find useful. In that case, do not force it.
Create a display (on a board or on paper) that illustrates the doubles and prepare cards with
near‐doubles (e.g., 4 + 5) (see Figure 6). Ask children to take a near‐doubles card and to find
the doubles fact on the display that could help them solve the fact on the card and place the
card on that spot. Ask children if there are other doubles that could help as well.
181
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
tions between addition and subtraction by posing more join and separate story problems
with start and change unknowns as well as more part-part-whole and comparison story
problems.
Subtraction as Think‐Addition
In Figure 7, subtraction is modeled in such a way that children are encouraged to think,
Figure 7 “What goes with this part to make the total?” When done in this think‐addition man-
Using a think‐addition
ner, children use known addition facts to produce the unknown quantity or part. If this
model for subtraction
important relationship between parts and wholes—between addition and subtraction—can
facts.
be made, subtraction facts will be much easier. As with addition
facts, it is helpful to begin with facts whose totals (minuends) are
Connecting Subtraction to Addition Knowledge
10 or less (e.g., 8 - 3, 9 - 7) before working on facts that have a
1. Count out 13 and total higher than 10 (e.g., 13 - 4).
cover.
The value of think‐addition cannot be overstated. However, if
children are to effectively use the think‐addition strategy, they first
must master the basic addition facts. Evidence suggests that children
learn very few, if any, subtraction facts without first mastering the
2. Count and corresponding addition facts. So, for example, mastery of 3 + 5 is
remove 5. prerequisite knowledge for learning the facts 8 - 3 and 8 - 5.
Keep these Story problems that promote think‐addition are those that
in view.
sound like addition but have a missing addend: join, start unknown;
join, change unknown; and part–part–whole, part unknown. Consider
3. Think: “Five and what
makes thirteen?” 8! 8 left. this problem:
13 minus 5 is 8.
4. Uncover. Janice had 5 fish in her aquarium. Grandma gave her some more fish.
Then she had 12 fish. How many fish did Grandma give Janice?
8 and 5 is 13. Notice that the action is join and, thus, suggests addition. There is
a high probability that children will think, “Five and how many more
make 12?” In the discussion in which you use problems such as this,
your task is to connect this thought process with the subtraction fact, 12 - 5. Children may
use the Up Over 10 strategy to solve this problem, just as they did with the addition facts. For
example, they may think, “It takes 5 to get to 10 and 2 more to get to 12, so that’s 7.”
Down Over 10
You may have used a think‐addition strategy for any of the problems in the previous Stop
and Reflect. Or you may have started with the 14 and counted down to 10 (4) and then
down 1 more to 9 for a total difference of 5. This reasoning strategy is called Down Over 10.
If you did not use this strategy, try it with one of the examples.
This strategy is based on a derived fact strategy because children use what they know
(14 minus 10 is 4) to figure out a related fact (14 - 9). This strategy helps children move to
mastery while supporting their number sense.
182
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
One way to develop the Down Over 10 strategy is to give children five or six pairs of
facts in which the difference for the first fact is 10 and the second fact is 8 or 9 (e.g., 16 - 6
and 16 - 7 or 14 - 4 and 14 - 6). Have children solve each pair of problems and discuss their
strategies. If children do not naturally see the relationship, ask them to think about how the
first fact can help them solve the second. Reinforce the Down Over 10 strategy by posing
story problems such as the following:
Bekah had 15 bracelets. She gave 6 to a friend. How many does Bekah have left?
Take from 10
This strategy capitalizes on children’s knowledge of the combinations that make 10 and it
works for all subtraction problems in which the starting value (minuend) is more than 10.
For example, consider the problem 16 - 8. Children would take the minuend (16) apart into
10 + 6, and then subtract 10 - 8 (because they know this fact). They would then add the 2
back to the 6 to get 8. Try this strategy on these examples:
15 - 8 = 17 - 9 = 14 - 8 =
This is a great reasoning strategy, although it may seem uncomfortable at first. It may
be helpful to use story problems to make it easier to keep track of the quantities. For ex-
ample, for 14 - 8, you might use a story problem such as “Andy had 14 toy animals. He gave
8 to a friend. How many does Andy have left?” To reason through the strategy, children
can either use manipulatives or imagine taking the 14 toy animals and splitting them into a
group of 10 and a group of 4. Then they have to take 8 from the group of 10 to get 2. Then
they add the 2 to the 4 to see they have 6 left.
The following activity is good for helping children realize that if they just don’t know a
fact, they can rely on reasoning.
e Lerane
rE
fo rs
any fact that you want students to think about), how could you figure it out with-
out counting? Encourage children to come up with more than one way (hopefully
using the strategies suggested earlier). To support children’s communication skills,
especially ELLs and children with disabilities, use a think‐pair‐share approach in which
children discuss their ideas with a partner before they share them with the class.
183
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
For a virtual site that connects arrays to multiplication facts, go to http:// nlvm.usu.edu/en/nav
/category_g_2_t_1.html and click on “Rectangle Multiplication.”
Doubles
Facts that have 2 as a factor are equivalent to addition doubles and should already
+ 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 be known by children by the time you begin introducing them to multiplication.
0 0 So the goal is to help children realize that 2 * 7 is the same as double 7, as is 7 * 2.
1 2
Start with story problems in which 2 is the number of groups. Later, use problems
2 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18
3 6 in which 2 is the size of each group.
4 8
5 10 Our field trip is in 2 weeks. How many days will we need to wait? [two groups of 7]
6 12
7 14
8 16
A kindergarten class was making clay animals. If there were 7 rabbits made, how
9 18
many rabbit ears were there? [seven groups of 2]
Fives
Facts with 5 as the first or second factor can be related to skip counting by fives. Practice
counting by fives to at least 45. Connect counting by fives with arrays that have rows of 5
dots (see Figure 8a). Point out that such an array with six rows is a model for 6 * 5, eight
184
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
12
11 1
5
10 2
10
15 5
9 3 ×7
20
25
8 4
30
7 5
6
rows is 8 * 5, and so on. As illustrated in the following activity, you can also connect fives
facts to telling time on a clock to the nearest five minutes, an expectation for second graders
in the Common Core State Standards.
+ 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
0 0
Activity 11 CLOCK FACTS 1 5
2 10
Focus on the minute hand of the clock. When it points to a number, how many 3 15
4 20
minutes after the hour is it? Point to numbers 1 to 9 on a large clock face in 5 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45
random order. Children respond with the minutes after. (See Figure 8b.) Connect 6 30
this idea to the multiplication facts with 5. In this way, the fives facts become the 7 35
“clock facts.” 8 40
9 45
185
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
As stated in the Common Core State Standards, the expectation is that by the end of sec-
ond grade children will know from memory their basic facts for addition and subtraction.
This achievement is a result of repeated experiences with reasoning strategies over time
and not because of time spent memorizing. Children can know something from memory
through immersion in the reasoning strategies, game playing, and life experiences, without
ever having to memorize a fact from a flash card. Drill without moving children from count-
ing strategies (phase 1) to reasoning strategies (phase 2) to mastery (phase 3) has repeat-
edly been found to be ineffective. The distinction between mastering basic facts via rote
memorization and mastering basic facts via an emphasis on reasoning strategies is the most
significant message in this chapter.
Effective Drill
The fact that drill—repetitive non‐problem‐based activity—can strengthen memory and
retrieval capabilities (Ashcraft & Christy, 1995) is important to consider once children
are effectively using reasoning strategies that they understand but that they have not yet
memorized. For drill to be effective, pacing and focus are crucial. Too often children
become frustrated and overwhelmed because drill includes too many facts too quickly.
Children will progress at different rates—gifted children tend to be good at memorizing
whereas children with intellectual disabilities have difficulty memorizing (Forbringer &
Fahsl, 2010).
When working on moving children to phase 3 (know from memory), use groups of
related facts. By organizing the work around related facts, if a child needs any further discus-
sion and illustrations, the focus will be beneficial. For example, if given a stack of *1 facts,
some children will quickly learn these facts by noting the generalization described earlier.
However, some children—in particular, children with disabilities—may need more discus-
sion and illustration to support their learning.
There are several websites and software programs that provide children opportunities to drill basic facts.
These programs can be a great support for children who are near mastery or who are maintaining mastery
because they can be fun. Try to use websites or programs for which you can target groups of related facts,
rather than ones that drill on all the facts at one time. Fun 4 the Brain (www.fun4thebrain.com) and Math
Fact Café (www.mathfactcafe.com) are two such websites.
186
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
Activity 12 SALUTE!
nguag
-La
Place children in groups of three and give each group a deck of cards (omit face
nglish
e Lerane
rE
cards and use aces as ones). Two children draw a card without looking at it and
fo rs
place it on their forehead facing outward so the others can see it. The child with
no cards tells the sum of the two cards. The first of the other two to correctly say
the number on their forehead “wins” the round. For ELLs, children with disabilities, or
reluctant learners, speed can increase anxiety and inhibit participation. You can remove
speed of response by having each child write down the card they think they have
(within 5 seconds) and they get a point if they are correct. This activity can be differen-
tiated by including only certain cards (e.g., addition facts using only the numbers 1
through 5).
Show children families of numbers with the sum circled, as in Figure 9a. Ask why
they think the numbers go together and why one number is circled. When this num-
ber family idea is understood, draw a different card and cover one of the numbers
with your thumb, asking, “What is missing?” Ask children how they figured it out.
After you have modeled this process, children can do this with partners. Alternatively,
you can create cards with one number replaced by a question mark, as in Figure 9b.
When children understand this activity, you can introduce missing‐number cards such
as those in Figure 9c. Ask children to name the missing number and explain their
thinking.
6 8
3 7
13
4 11 4
8 5 5
9 6 12
14
(a)
Why do these numbers belong together?
8
Why is one circled?
7 9
? ? 10 4 14 3 6
6
? 13 (c)
3 4
These missing-number
(b)
cards are just like the
Which number is missing? number families. Say the
How can you tell what it is? missing number.
187
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
Figure 10
Missing‐number handouts. The blank version can be used to fill in any sets of facts you
wish to emphasize (see Blackline Master 11). (Note that the columns are not labeled on
student pages.)
Two fact
families
Make-ten Near- (7, 8, 15)
facts doubles (4, 8, 12)
4 8 5 6 4 12
9 6 13 7 15 8
8 7 15 8 12 4
15 6 5 11 7 8
5 13 7 15 12 8
8 17 9 4 12 8
6 8 17 8 4 12
3 9 11 6 8 15
9 16 5 4 15 7
6 8 3 7 7 8
7 16 9 5 4 12
3 9 6 13 4 8
8 8 17 9 8 15
188
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
189
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
2. Don’t use public comparisons of mastery. Avoid using public comparisons, such as a bulletin
board display that shows which children have mastered which basic facts. Similarly,
avoid public competition with flash cards. It is great to celebrate children’s successes,
but avoiding public comparisons between children can go a long way in preventing
children’s anxiety as they learn mathematics.
3. Don’t proceed through the facts in order from 0 to 9. Instead work on collections of facts
based on strategies and conceptual understanding to emphasize connections and
sense making.
4. Don’t work on all the facts all at once. Select a strategy (starting with the easier ones) and
then work on memorization of that set of facts (e.g., doubles). Be sure children really
know these facts before moving on; otherwise, they may become confused and your
goal for them to master all the facts will backfire. Because some children will learn
some facts more quickly than other children, you will need to differentiate.
5. Don’t move to quick‐recall activities too soon. Quick recall or mastery can be attained only
after children have a robust collection of reasoning strategies to apply as needed.
6. Don’t use facts as a barrier to good mathematics. Mathematics is not solely about
computation. Mathematics is about reasoning, using patterns, and making sense of
things. Children who have mastered their basic facts do not necessarily reason better
than those who, for whatever reason, have not yet mastered their facts. There is no
reason that a child who has not yet mastered all basic facts should be excluded from
any mathematical experiences.
7. Don’t use fact mastery as a prerequisite for calculator use. Insisting that children master
the basic facts before allowing them to use a calculator denies them important
learning opportunities. For example, if the learning objective for a lesson is for
children to explore even and odd numbers, then they might be building different
array representations of numbers, recording different parts of the representations
while they look for patterns. A child who has not yet developed fluency could
get too bogged down in computation without a calculator. With a calculator, the
same child can participate and hopefully attain the learning goals of the lesson.
190
E x p a n d e d L e s s o n
Missing‐Number Cards
Content and Task Decisions how they thought about it. Encourage such children
to illustrate their solution on a hundreds chart or with
Grade Level: 1–2 double ten‐frames.
• The phrase “fact families” or “families of values” may
Mathematics Goals sound odd to ELLs because they probably know a dif-
• Practice part–part–whole thinking in developing con- ferent meaning of the word family. Use the everyday
nections between addition and subtraction meaning to connect to the grouping of the numbers in
the missing‐number sets.
• Use think‐addition as a strategy for solving subtraction
facts
For Children with Disabilities
Grade Level Guide • If children are struggling, you may want to use a part–
NCTM Common part–whole mat along with counters to model the ar-
Curriculum Focal Points Core State Standards rangements of the fact families.
191
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
• Pass out the missing‐part recording sheets prepared • For children who struggle with missing parts, it is im-
with number families on which children can practice. portant to see how fluent they are with the corre-
Their task is to fill in the missing number on each sponding missing‐whole cards. You can similarly check
“card” of the recording sheet. their mastery of addition facts.
• Prepare a page of addition and subtraction facts that
Provide clear expectations: correspond to the facts on the missing‐part recording
• This activity may be presented in small groups to ad- sheets. Children who complete the missing‐part record-
dress specific weaknesses. ing sheets fluently should also know the corresponding
addition and subtraction facts.
192
Helping Children Master the Basic Facts
References
Ashcraft, M. H., & Christy, K. S. (1995). The Gravemeijer, K., & van Galen, F. (2003). Facts
frequency of arithmetic facts in elementary texts: and algorithms as products of students’ own
Addition and multiplication in grades 1–6. Journal mathematical activity. In J. Kilpatrick,
for Research in Mathematics Education, 26, 396–421. W. G. Martin, & D. Schifter (Eds.), A research
companion to Principles and Standards for School
Baroody, A. J. (1985). Mastery of the basic number
Mathematics (pp. 114–122). Reston, VA: National
combinations: Internalization of relationships or
Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
facts? Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
16(2), 83–98. Henry, V. J., & Brown, R. S. (2008). First‐grade
basic facts: An investigation into teaching
Baroody, A. J. (2003). The development of
and learning of an accelerated, high‐demand
adaptive expertise and flexibility: The integration
memorization standard. Journal for Research in
of conceptual and procedural knowledge. In A. J.
Mathematics Education, 39(2), 153–183.
Baroody & A. Dowker (Eds.), The development of
arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive Kamii, C. K., & Anderson, C. (2003).
expertise (pp. 1–34). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Multiplication games: How we made and used
them. Teaching Children Mathematics, 10(3),
Baroody, A. J. (2006). Why children have difficulties
135–141.
mastering the basic number combinations and how
to help them. Teaching Children Mathematics, 13(1), Lewis, T. (2005). Facts + fun = fluency. Teaching
22–31. Children Mathematics, 12(1), 8–11.
Baroody, A. J., Bajwa, N. P., & Eiland, M. (2009). Mazzocco, M. M. M., Devlin, K. T., & McKenney,
Why can’t Johnny remember the basic facts? S. J. (2008). Is it a fact? Timed arithmetic
Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 15, performance of children with mathematical
69–79. learning disabilities (MLD) varies as a function
of how MLD is defined. Developmental
Bley, N. S., & Thornton, C. A. (1995). Teaching
Neuropsychology, 33(3), 318–344.
mathematics to students with learning disabilities (3rd
ed.). Austin, TX: Pro‐Ed. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
(2006). Curriculum focal points for prekindergarten
Brownell, W., & Chazal, C. (1935). The effects of
through grade 8 mathematics: A quest for coherence.
premature drill in third grade arithmetic. Journal
Reston, VA: Author.
of Educational Research, 29(1), 17–28.
National Research Council. (2001). Adding it up:
CCSSO (Council of Chief State School Officers).
Helping children learn mathematics. J. Kilpatrick, J.
(2010). Common core state standards. Retrieved from
Swafford, & B. Findell (Eds.). Washington, DC:
http://corestandards.org
National Academies Press.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2009). Learning and
Rathmell, E. C., Leutzinger, L. P., & Gabriele, A.
teaching early math: The learning trajectories approach.
(2000). Thinking with numbers. Cedar Falls, IA:
New York, NY: Routledge.
Thinking with Numbers.
Forbringer, L., & Fahsl, A. J. (2010). Differentiating
Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2009). Early childhood
practice to help students master basic facts. In D. Y.
mathematics education research: Learning trajectories for
White & J. S. Spitzer (Eds.), Responding to diversity:
young children. New York, NY: Routledge.
Grades pre‐K–5 (pp. 7–22). Reston, VA: National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Verschaffel, L., Greer, B., & De Corte, E. (2007).
Whole number concepts and operations. In F. Lester
Fuson, K. C. (1984). More complexities in
(Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics
subtraction. Journal for Research in Mathematics
teaching and learning (pp. 557–628). Reston, VA:
Education, 15(3), 214–225.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Fuson, K. C. (1992). Research on whole number
Wallace, A. H., & Gurganus, S. P. (2005). Teaching
addition and subtraction. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.),
for mastery of multiplication. Teaching Children
Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and
Mathematics, 12(1), 26–33.
learning (pp. 243–275). New York, NY: Macmillan.
193
This page intentionally left blank
Developing Whole‐Number
Place‐Value Concepts
BigIDEAS
1 Sets of ten (and tens of tens) can be perceived as single entities or units. For example, “three sets of ten and two
singles” is using base‐ten language to describe 32 single objects.
2 The positions of digits in numbers determine what they represent—which size group they count. This is the major
organizing principle of place‐value numeration and is central to developing number sense.
3 There are patterns to the way that numbers are formed. For example, each decade has a symbolic pattern reflective
of the 0‐to‐9 sequence (e.g., 20, 21, 22, . . . 29).
4 The groupings of ones, tens, and hundreds can be taken apart in different but equivalent ways. For example, beyond
the typical way to decompose 256 into 2 hundreds, 5 tens, and 6 ones, it can be represented as 1 hundred, 14 tens, and
16 ones as well as 25 tens and 6 ones. Decomposing and composing multidigit numbers in flexible ways is a necessary
foundation for computational estimation and exact computation.
5 Children progress through three levels of understanding the concept of “ten” starting with understanding ten not as
a unit but only as ten ones. They then move to seeing ten as a unit but rely on physical or mental reconstructions of models
to help them work with units of ten. Finally, they are able to easily work with units of ten without the need of physical or
mental reconstructions of base‐ten models.
6 Children’s ability to label the tens place and the ones place or to count by tens does not guarantee that they
understand that one ten is the same as ten ones.
From Chapter 11 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
195
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
numbers between 11 and 19 by putting them together (composing) and taking them apart
(decomposing) into tens and ones using materials and drawings. By second grade, these ini-
tial ideas are extended to three‐digit numbers.
As a significant part of this development, children should engage in composing and
decomposing numbers in a wide variety of ways as they solve addition and subtraction
problems with two‐ and three‐digit numbers. In other words, there is no need to separate
place‐value instruction from computation instruction. Children’s efforts with the invention
of their own computation strategies will both enhance their understanding of place value
and provide a firm foundation for flexible methods of computation.
The children just described know that there are 35 tiles “because I counted them.”
Writing the number and saying the number are usually done correctly, but their under-
standing of 35 derives from and is connected to the count‐by‐ones approach. Children do
not easily or quickly develop a meaningful use of groups of ten to represent quantities.
Even if children can tell you that in the numeral 35 the 3 “is in the tens place” or that
there are “5 ones,” they might just know the name of the positions without understanding
196
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
197
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
198
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
Figure 2
Groupings by 10 are matched with numerals, which are recorded in labeled places and
eventually written in standard form.
TENS ONES
5 3
53
5 tens 3 ones
5 3
Other languages often use base‐ten terminology, so this can be a good cultural connection
for children (e.g., 17 in Spanish is diecisiete, literally meaning “10 and 7”).
199
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
Cubes:
Ten single cubes form
a bar of 10.
Hundreds: ten bars on
cardboard backing. As children begin to make groupings of ten, start introduc-
ing the language of “tens” by matching the objects being
Bundles of sticks (wooden craft sticks, coffee stirrers): used, such as “cups of ten and ones” or “bundles of ten and
If bundles are left intact, these are a pregrouped model.
singles.” Then move to a general phrase, such as “groups of
Hundreds: ten bundles grouped with a rubber band.
ten and ones.”
200
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
The little ten‐frames (see Blackline Masters 17–18) are less common but are very effec-
tive. If children have been using ten‐frames to think about numbers to 20, the value of the BLM
filled ten‐frame may be more meaningful than the ten rods and squares of base‐ten materi-
als. Although the ones are fixed on the cards, this model has the distinct advantage of always
showing the distance to the next decade. For example, when 47 is shown with 4 ten cards
and a seven card, a child can see that three more ones will make five full cards, or 50.
A significant disadvantage of the pregrouped physical models is the potential for children
to use them without reflecting on the ten‐to‐one relationships or without really understand-
Standards for
ing what they are doing—this is especially true if children have not had adequate experience Mathematical Practice
working with groupable models.
For example, if children are told to trade 10 ones for a ten, it is quite possible for ◀ 5 Use appropriate
them to make this exchange without attending to the “ten‐ness” of the piece they call a ten. tools strategically
Similarly, children can learn to “make the number 42” by simply selecting 4 tens and 2 ones
pieces without understanding that if the pieces all came apart there would be 42 ones pieces
that could be counted by ones.
Electronic versions of pregrouped base‐ten manipulatives (such as the Base Blocks applet at http://nlvm
.usu.edu/en/nav/frames_asid_152_g_3_t_2.html) are computer representations of the three‐dimensional
base‐ten blocks. With simple mouse clicks, children (including those with disabilities) can place ones, tens,
hundreds, or thousands on the screen. To break a piece into 10 smaller pieces, simply move the piece to
the right one column. Lasso 10 pieces and they are automatically grouped into a larger base-ten piece. As
pieces are moved onto the place value mat, a corresponding numeral that shows the current quantity is
displayed. You can also adjust the columns to display two, three, or four place values.
Compared to real base‐ten blocks, these virtual materials are free, can be easily grouped and
ungrouped, can be shown to the full class on a projection device, and are available in “endless” supply—
even the thousands blocks. Even with all these advantages, the computer models, like the physical models,
are only a representation for children who understand the relationships involved.
Nonproportional Models
Nonproportional models (e.g., money, an abacus) can be used by children who understand
that 10 units make “a ten.” These are models, such as money, in which the ten is not physi-
cally ten times larger than the one. Many children can grasp place‐value relationships using
pennies, dimes, and dollars to represent the ones, tens, and hundreds on their place‐value
mat. Using coin representations, they can display amounts and exchange 10 dimes for a dol-
lar and represent and carry out a variety of calculations. However, like a bead frame that has
the same‐sized beads on wires in different columns (i.e., an abacus) or chips that are assigned
different place values by color, nonproportional representations are not used to introduce
place‐value concepts. They are used once children have a conceptual understanding of the
numeration system and need additional reinforcement.
201
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
oral and written names for numbers (the rest of Figure 3) is discussed separately. How-
ever, in the classroom, the oral and written names for numbers can and should be devel-
oped in concert with conceptual ideas.
Grouping Activities
Reflect for a moment on how strange it must sound to say “seven ones.” Certainly, children
have never said they were “seven ones” years old. The use of the word ten as a singular group
name is even more mysterious. Consider the phrase “Ten ones makes one ten.” The first
ten carries the usual meaning of 10 things, the amount that is 1 more than 9 things. But the
other ten is a singular noun, a thing. How can something the child has known for years as
the name for a lot of things suddenly become one thing? And if you think this is confusing
for native speakers, imagine the potential difficulty for ELLs.
Because children come to their development of base‐ten concepts with a count‐by‐ones
idea of number, you must begin there. You cannot arbitrarily impose grouping by ten on
children. Children need to experiment with showing amounts in groups of like size and
eventually come to an agreement that ten is a very useful size to use. The following activ-
ity could be done in first grade and is designed as an example of a first effort at developing
grouping concepts.
One teacher challenged her children to find a good way to count all the linking cubes
being held by the class after each child had been given a cube for each of their pockets.
The first suggestion was to count by sevens. That was tried but did not work very well
because none of the children could count by sevens. In search of a more efficient way, the
next suggestion was to count by twos. This did not seem to be much better than counting
by ones. Finally, they settled on counting by tens and realized that this was a pretty good
method.
This and similar activities provide you with the opportunity to suggest that materi-
als be arranged into groups of tens before the “fast” way of counting is begun. Remember
that children may count “ten, twenty, thirty, thirty‐one, thirty‐two” but not fully realize the
“thirty‐two‐ness” of the quantity. To connect the count‐by‐tens method with their under-
stood method of counting by ones, the children need to count both ways and discuss why
they get the same result.
The idea in the next activity is for children to make groupings of ten and record or say
the amounts. Number words are used so that children will not mechanically match tens and
ones with individual digits. It is important that children confront the actual quantity in a
manner meaningful to them.
202
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
Prepare bags of different types of objects such as toothpicks, buttons, beans, plas-
tic chips, linking cubes, Popsicle sticks, or other items. Children should have a re-
cording sheet similar to the top example in Figure 5. The bags can be placed at stations
around the room or given to pairs of children. Children empty the bags and count the
contents. The amount is recorded as a number word. Then the objects are grouped in as
many tens as possible. The groupings are recorded on the form. After returning the ob-
jects to the bags, bags are traded or children move to another station. Note that children
with disabilities may initially need to use a ten‐frame to support their counting. Then the
use of the ten‐frame should eventually fade.
Variations of the “Groups of Ten” activity are suggested by the other recording sheets
in Figure 5. On the “Get This Many” sheet, children count the dots and then count out the
corresponding number of objects. Provide small cups to put the groups of ten in. Notice
that the activity requires children to first count the set in a way they understand (e.g., count
Name
Bag of Number word
Tens
Toothpicks
Singles
Tens
Beans
Singles
Tens
Washers
Singles
Tens Ones
Tens Extras
Tens Ones
203
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
by ones), record the amount in words, and then make the groupings. The “Fill the Tens”
and “Loop This Many” sheets begin with a verbal name (number word), and children must
count the indicated amount and then make groups.
The following activity is another variant of the grouping activities but includes an esti-
mation component that adds interest, makes the activity more problem based, and contrib-
utes to number sense. Listening to children’s estimates is a useful assessment opportunity
that tells you a lot about children’s concepts of numbers.
Show children a length that they are going to measure—for example, the length of a child
lying down or the distance around a sheet of newspaper. At one end of the length, line up
10 units (e.g., 10 linking cubes, toothpicks, rods, or blocks). On a recording sheet (see Fig-
ure 6), children write down an estimate of how many groups of 10 and ones they think will
fit into the length. Next they find the actual measure, placing units along the full length.
These are counted by ones and also grouped in tens. Both results are recorded. Estimating
the groups of ten requires children to pay attention to the ten as a group or unit.
Children can work in pairs to measure several lengths around the room. A similar
estimation approach could be added to “Groups of Ten” (Activity 2), where children first
estimate the quantity in the bags.
NAME Jessica
OBJECT ESTIMATE ACTUAL
Thirty-two
Number Word
Number Word
204
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
Show children any quantity with 150 to 1000 items. For example, you might use a jar of
lima beans, a clear bag full of plastic necklace beads, a long chain of connecting links or
paper clips, or a box of Styrofoam packing peanuts. Suppose you are using the bag of
plastic beads. First, have children make and record estimates of how many beads are in
the bag. Discuss with children how they determined their estimates. Then give portions
of the beads to pairs or triads of children to put into cups of 10 beads. Collect leftover
beads and put these into groups of ten as well. Now ask, “How can we use these groups
of ten to tell how many beads we have? Can we make new groups from the groups of
ten? What is 10 groups of ten called?” If using cups of beads, be prepared with some
larger containers or baggies into which 10 cups can be placed. When all groups are made,
count the hundreds, the tens, and the ones separately. Record the totals on the board as
“4 hundreds + 7 tens + 8 ones.”
In this activity, it is important to use a groupable model so that children can see how the
10 groups of ten are the same as 100 individual items. This connection is often too implicit in
the display of a hundreds flat or a paper hundreds square in the pregrouped base‐ten models.
Equivalent Representations
An important variation of the grouping activities is aimed at the equivalent representations
of numbers. After children have just completed the “Groups of Ten” activity (Activity 2)
with a bag of objects, pose the following task.
What is another way you can show 42 besides 4 groups of ten and 2 singles? Let’s see how
many ways you can find.
Interestingly, most children will go next to 42 singles. The following activities focus on cre-
ating other equivalent representations.
Show a collection of materials that is only partly grouped in sets of ten. For example, you
may have 5 chains of 10 links and 17 additional links. Be sure the children understand
that each chain is a group of 10 links. Have children count the number of chains, and
also count the singles, in any way they wish to count. Ask, “How many in all?” Record all
responses and discuss how they got their answers. Next change the groupings (make a
205
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
ten from the singles or break apart one of the tens) and repeat the questions and discus-
Standards for
Mathematical Practice sion. Do not change the total number from one time to the next. Once children begin to
understand that the total does not change, ask in what other ways the items could be
8 Look for and
◀ grouped if using tens and singles.
express regularity in
repeated reasoning
If you teach second grade, equivalent representations for hundreds as groups of tens can
help children with the concept of a hundred as 10 tens. The next activity is similar to “Can
You Make the Link?” but is done using pregrouped materials and includes hundreds.
Children work in groups or pairs. First, they show 463 on their desks with base‐ten materi-
als in the standard representation. Next, they find and record at least three other ways of
representing this quantity.
206
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
Base‐ten riddles can be presented orally or in written form. In either case, children should use
base‐ten materials to help solve the riddles. The examples here illustrate a variety of differ-
ent levels of difficulty. After children solve the following riddles, have them write new ones.
Use a 10 * 10 array of dots on the projector. Cover up all but two rows, as shown in Fig-
ure 8a. “How many tens? (2.) Two tens is called twenty.” Have the class repeat. Show an-
other row. “Three tens is called thirty. Four tens is forty. Five tens could have been fivety
but is just fifty. How many tens does sixty have?” The names sixty, seventy, eighty, and
ninety all fit the pattern. Slide the cover up and down the array, asking how many tens
and the name for that many. ELLs may not hear the difference between fifty and fifteen,
sixty and sixteen, and so on, so explicitly compare these words and clearly enunciate and
even overemphasize the word endings.
Use the same 10 * 10 array to work on names for tens and ones. Show, for example,
four full lines, “forty.” Next expose one dot in the fifth row. “Four tens and one. Forty‐
one.” Add more dots one at a time. “Four tens and two. Forty‐two.” “Four tens and
three. Forty‐three.” This is shown in Figure 8b. When that pattern is established, repeat
with other decades from 20 through 90.
207
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
Figure 8
BLM
10 * 10 dot arrays are used to model sets of 10 and singles (Blackline Master 12).
(a) (b)
“Four tens—forty”
“Two tens—twenty” “Four tens and three—forty-three”
Repeat this basic approach with other base‐ten models. The next activity shows how
this might be done.
e Lerane
rE
fo
arrangement. Ask how many tens. Add a ten or remove a ten and repeat the question.
rs
Next add some ones. Always have children give the base‐ten name and the standard name.
Continue to make changes in the materials displayed by adding or removing one or
two tens and by adding and removing ones. Avoid the standard left‐to‐right order for
tens and ones so the emphasis is on the names of the materials, not the order they are in.
Reverse the activity by having children use base‐ten blocks at their desks. For example, say,
“Make 63.” The children make the number with the models and then give the base‐ten name (6
tens and 3 ones) and standard name (63).
Standards for
Mathematical Practice Activity 10 TENS, ONES, AND FINGERS
2 Reason abstractly
◀ Ask your class, “Can you show 6 fingers (or any amount less than 10)?” Then ask, “How can
and quantitatively you show 37 fingers?” Some children will figure out that at least four children are required.
208
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
Line up four children, and have three hold up 10 fingers while the fourth child holds
up 7 fingers. Have the class count the fingers by tens and ones. Ask other children to
show different numbers. Emphasize the number of sets of 10 fingers and the single fin-
gers (base‐ten language) and pair this with standard language.
Use the standard names to say the following numbers: 203, 23, and 230. As you say a num-
ber, have children write the number and then model the number with base‐ten materials.
Then repeat the numbers orally using base‐ten language and have children check their mod-
els and written numerals.
Children frequently incorrectly write 2003 for two hundred three. The emphasis on the
meaning in the oral base‐ten language will be a significant help. ELLs may need additional
time to think about how to say and write numerals because they are translating all the terms
within the number.
Written Symbols
Place‐value mats are simple mats divided into two or three sections to hold ones and tens or
ones, tens, and hundreds pieces as shown in Figure 9. You can suggest to children that the
mats are a good way to organize their materials when working with base‐ten blocks. Explain
that the standard way to use a place‐value mat is with the space for the ones on the right and
tens and hundreds places to the left.
Although not commonly seen in textbooks, it is strongly recommended that two ten‐
BLM
frames (see Blackline Master 15) be drawn in the ones place as shown. That way, the amount of
ones on the ten‐frames is always clearly evident, eliminating the need for repeatedly counting
209
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
Figure 9 the ones. The ten‐frame also makes it very clear how many
Place‐value mats with two ten‐frames in the ones place to
additional counters would be needed to make the next set
organize the counters and promote the concept of groups of ten. If children are modeling two numbers at the same
of ten. time, one ten‐frame can be used for each number.
Be aware of how easy it is for a child to show a num-
Strips and squares show ber on a mat using base‐ten blocks and learn to write the
237 on three-place mat.
number without any understanding of what the number
represents. First‐ and second‐grade textbooks often show
a picture of base‐ten materials and have children record
numbers in this manner:
4 05007 2 nections between models, oral names (base ten and standard), and
written forms. The activities can be done with two‐ or three‐digit
numbers depending on children’s needs.
e Lerane
rE
tor or simply draw on the board using the square‐stick‐dot method.) Children say
fo rs
the amount shown in base‐ten language (“four hundreds, one ten, and five ones”) and
then in standard language (“four hundred fifteen”); next they enter it on their calculators.
Have someone share his or her display and defend it. Make a change in the materials and
repeat. You can also do this activity as “Show It/Press It” and start by saying the standard
name for a number (with either two or three digits). At their desks, children use base‐ten
materials to show that number and press it on their calculators (or write it). Again, pay spe-
cial attention to numbers in the teens and the case of zero tens. ELLs may need additional
time to think of the words that go with the numbers, especially as the numbers get larger.
210
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
These activities are especially good for numbers that pose problems for children, such
as the teens and three‐digit numbers with zero tens. If you show or say 7 hundreds and 4
ones, the class says, “seven hundreds, zero tens, and four ones—seven hundred (slight pause)
four.” The pause and the base‐ten language suggest the correct three‐digit number to press,
write, and model.
The next activity is a wonderful challenge for children in early stages of place‐value
development. It can also be used as an assessment to see whether children really understand
the value of digits in two‐ and three‐digit numbers.
211
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
A third interview can also be revealing. Ask the child to write the number that represents 5 tens, 2
ones, and 3 hundreds. Note that the task does not give the places in order. What do you think will be the
common misunderstanding? If the child does not write 352, then ask the child to show you the number
with base‐ten materials. What information can you obtain from the results of this interview?
BLM
Figure 11
A hundreds chart Patterns and Relationships with Multidigit Numbers
(Blackline Master 10).
Now we move beyond a snapshot view of individual numbers to
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 look at patterns in our number system and how numbers are re-
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 lated. In particular we are interested in helping children develop an
understanding of the relationships of numbers to special numbers
21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 called benchmark numbers. These ideas begin to provide a basis for
31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 computation.
41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50
The Hundreds Chart
51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
The hundreds chart (Figure 11) is such an important tool in the
61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 development of place‐value concepts that it deserves special at-
tention. K–2 classrooms should have a hundreds chart displayed
71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80
prominently.
81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 A pocket‐chart version of a hundreds chart is very useful. You
can hide a number in the chart by inserting a blank card in front
91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 of the number in the pocket. You can also insert colored pieces
of paper in the slots to highlight various number patterns such as
skip-counting patterns. You can also remove some or all the num-
BLM ber cards and have children return them to their correct positions. A blank 10 * 10 grid (see
Blackline Master 9) serves as an empty hundreds chart on which you can write numbers.
In kindergarten and first grade, children can count and recognize two‐digit numbers
with the hundreds chart. In first and second grades, children can use the hundreds chart to
develop base‐ten understanding, noticing that jumps up or down are jumps of ten, while
jumps to the right or left are jumps of one.
There are lots of patterns on the hundreds chart. In a discussion, different chil-
dren will describe the same pattern in several ways. Accept all ideas. You might have
children explore various skip‐count patterns.
You can use online versions of a hundreds chart, one of which (called Hundred Square) can be found at
www.crickweb.co.uk/ks2numeracy‐tools.html. This online tool allows you to hide numbers behind opaque
screens as well as highlight particular numbers or patterns using transparent colored screens. You can also
change where the hundreds chart begins (e.g., 0 or 1) and change the increments between numbers to
explore skip‐counting patterns.
212
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
Have children skip‐count together by twos, threes, or fours. Then have children record
a specific skip‐count pattern on their own copy of the hundreds chart by coloring in
each number they count. Discuss the patterns on the chart as well as the patterns in the
numbers.
Replacing the number cards from a blank chart is a good station activity for two chil-
dren to try. By listening to how children determine the correct places for numbers, you can
assess how well they have constructed an understanding of the 1‐to‐100 sequence.
Begin with a blank or nearly blank hundreds chart. Circle a particular missing
number. Children are to fill in the designated number and its “neighbors”—the
numbers to the left, to the right, above, and below. This can be done with the full class
on a projector or with blank hundreds chart worksheets. Especially for children with
disabilities, it is important to use a “think‐aloud” to describe what key features of the
numbers you think about as you determine the missing number and its neighbors. After
children become comfortable naming the neighbors of a number, ask what they notice
about the neighboring numbers. The numbers to the left and right are one more and one
less than the given number. Those above and below are ten less and ten more, respec-
tively. What about those numbers on the diagonal? By discussing these relationships on
the chart, children begin to see how the sequence of numbers is related to the numerical
relationships.
Notice that children will first use the hundreds chart to learn about the patterns in the
sequence of numbers. Many children, especially at the kindergarten or first-grade level, will
not understand the corresponding numeric relationships such as those discussed in the last
activity. In the following activity, number relationships on the chart are made more explicit
by including the use of base‐ten materials.
Use any base‐ten model for two‐digit numbers with which the children are familiar. The
little ten‐frame cards are recommended (see Blackline Masters 17–18). Give children one Standards for
or more numbers to first make with the models and then find on the chart. Use groups of Mathematical Practice
two or three numbers either in the same row or the same column. Ask children, “How are
2 Reason abstractly
the numbers in the row (or column) alike? How are they different?” ◀
and quantitatively
Indicate a number on the chart. Ask children, “What would you have to change to
make it into each of its neighbors (the numbers to the left, to the right, above, and
below)?”
It is also helpful for children to have a chart that extends to 200, even in the first grade.
Perhaps a more powerful idea is to extend the hundreds chart to 1000.
213
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
Provide children with several sheets of the blank hundreds charts (see Blackline Master 9).
BLM Assign groups of three or four children the task of creating a 1‐to‐1000 chart. The chart is
made by taping 10 blank hundreds charts together in a long strip. Children should decide
how they are going to divide up the task of filling in the chart with different children
working on different parts of the chart.
The thousands chart should be discussed as a class to examine how numbers change as
you count from one hundred to the next, what the patterns are, and so on. In fact, the earlier
hundreds chart activities can all be extended to a thousands chart.
Activity 16 WHO AM I?
Sketch a line (or use a piece of cash register tape) and label 0 and 100 at opposite ends.
Mark a point with a “?” that corresponds to your secret number. (Estimate the position the
best you can.) Children use estimation to try to identify your secret number. For each esti-
mate, place and label a mark on the line. Continue marking each estimate until your secret
number is discovered. Have children explain how they are making their estimates. High-
light any use of benchmark numbers in their estimations. As a variation, the endpoints can
be other than 0 and 100. For example, try 0 and 1000, 200 and 300, or 500 and 800.
21 38 47 75
0 ? 100
e Lerane
rE
numbers. Show children different points labeled with letters; ask what numbers
fo rs
these points might be and why the children think that. In the example shown
here, B and C are less than 100 but probably more than 60. E could be about 180.
214
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
A BC D E
50 200
You can also ask where 75 might be or where 400 is. About how far apart are A
and D? Why do you think D is more than 100? For ELLs and children with disabilities, say
as well as write the numbers on a note card, or ask children to both write and say the
numbers.
The key purpose of the following activities is to provide opportunities for children to
apply their emerging understanding of place value to computation.
Say or write a number between 50 and 100. Children respond with “50 and __________.”
For 63, the response is “50 and 13.” Any benchmark number can be used instead of 50.
For example, you could use any number that ends in 50 (e.g., 450). You could also use
numbers such as 70 or 230.
*Reprinted with permission from Principles and Standards for School Mathematics, copyright 2000, by the National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics. All rights reserved.
215
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
Benchmark numbers are often used in computational strategies to make the computa-
tions easier to do. The next activity is aimed at what may be one of the most important
benchmark numbers, 100.
Two children work together with a set of little ten‐frame cards. One child makes a
two‐digit number. Then both children work mentally to determine what goes with the
ten‐frame amount to make 100. They write their solutions on paper and then check by
making the other part with the cards to see if the total is 100. Children take turns mak-
ing the original number. Figure 12 shows three different thought processes that children
might use.
28 and 70 is 98 and 2
makes 100, 72.
What you might have done in finding the other part of 83
was subtract 36 from 83. Or you might have added up from
Has to be 70-something
36. Either way, notice that you did not regroup. Most likely
because 80 more is too much.
70 and 2 goes with the 8, 72. you did it in your head, possibly using benchmark numbers.
With more practice you (and children as early as the second
grade) can do this without the aid of the cards.
Compatible numbers for addition and subtraction are
numbers that go together easily to make benchmark numbers.
Numbers that make tens or hundreds are the most common
Figure 13 Compatible pair searches. examples. Compatible sums also include numbers that end in
5, 25, 50, or 75, because these numbers are easy to work with
Using fives to
as well. The teaching task is to get children accustomed to
Make 10 make 100
6 7 looking for combinations that work together and then looking
2 25 5 85 65
9 45 for these combinations in computational situations.
5 3 75
35
1 4 5 8 95 15 55
216
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
The next activity has children apply some of the same ideas about benchmark numbers
that we have been exploring.
e Lerane
rE
With these three numbers as referents, ask questions such as the following,
encouraging discussion of all responses:
For ELLs, modify this activity by using prompts that are similar to each other. Chang-
ing the prompts each time increases the linguistic demand for these children. ELLs and
children with disabilities will also benefit from using a visual, such as a number line, and
from writing the numbers rather than just hearing them or saying them.
The following activity takes the next step to making addition more explicit.
Children press any number on the calculator (e.g., 17), then + 10. They say the
sum before they press = . Then they continue to add 10 mentally, challenging
themselves to say the number before they press = . Challenge them to see how far they
can go without making a mistake.
You can adjust the difficulty level by adjusting the numbers. You may want to
begin with a starting number that is less than 10 (e.g., 6) for children with disabilities
or start with a larger number, such as 98 or 327, for children who need a challenge.
The constant addend ( + 10 in the preceding example) can also be changed to any
number with one, two, or three digits. Some children will even find jumps of 5 to be
challenging if the starting number is not a multiple of 5. Skip counting by 20 or 25
will be easier than counting by 7 or 12 and will help develop important patterns and
relationships.
You can also go in reverse. That is, enter a number such as 53 in the calculator and
press – 6. As before, children say the result before pressing = . Each successive press will
subtract 6 or whatever constant was entered.
Children can work together in pairs quite profitably on this activity. The activity can
be used repeatedly and varied according to the skill level to challenge children while they
improve their mental skills with numbers.
This next activity combines base‐ten representations with symbolism.
217
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
Figure 14 Figure 15
Flexible counting on or addition using Take away or take from subtraction using both
both models and numerals. models and numerals.
Take
30 away
30
56
Take
45 85 away
Take
away
232
470
Take
673 away
745
As illustrated in Figure 14, prepare a worksheet or display that includes a numeral and
some base‐ten pieces. Use small squares (hundreds), sticks (tens), and dots (ones) for base‐
ten pieces to keep drawings simple. Children then mentally compute the totals.
Figure 15 is a take‐away version of the same activity. As shown, the amount removed
can be represented either by a numeral or the squares and sticks. You may want to start
with the removed amount represented by the numeral first before using the square-stick-
dot representation for the removed amount.
If this activity is done as a whole class, discuss each task before going on to the next. If
you use a worksheet format, include only a few examples and have children write how they
solved them. But it is still important to have a discussion with the class.
The next activity extends the use of the hundreds chart by using it for addition.
BLM
Activity 24 HUNDREDS CHART ADDITION
Standards for Display a hundreds chart (or thousands chart) for all children to see or, alternatively, give
Mathematical Practice
children their own individual charts (see Blackline Master 10). Children use the hundreds
3 Construct chart to add two numbers (e.g., 38 and 24). There are many ways that children can use
viable arguments ◀ the hundreds chart to add two numbers so class discussions are a must. Have children
and critique the work on one sum at a time and then have a discussion to compare the different methods
reasoning of others
children used.
218
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
The hundreds chart can be thought of as a stacked number line—one that accentuates
the distance from any number to the next multiple of 10. A jump down a row is the same
as adding 10, and a jump up a row is 10 less. To begin, pose problems with relatively small
second numbers such as the following:
17 + 14 23 + 12 35 + 13 78 + 15
Many children will initially count on by ones from the first number, which is an indica-
tion that they may not understand how to count by tens from any starting value (an impor-
tant place‐value concept). A child who is using ten as a unit might explain for the problem
17 + 14 that she started at 17 on the hundreds chart and added 10 by jumping down a row
to 27, and then added 3 more by counting over 30, and then one more to 31. Alternatively, a
child might add 17 + 14 by adding 10 + 10 by starting at 10 and jumping down a row to 20,
then moving over 7 spaces to 27, and then counting 4 more spaces to 31.
The following activity is similar to “Hundreds Chart Addition” but explores the idea of
adding up as a method of subtraction.
Children must have access to a hundreds chart for this activity. Give them two numbers.
Their task is to determine how much from one number to the other.
In “How Much Between?” the choice of the two numbers has an impact on the strate-
gies children will use. The easiest pairs are ones in the same column on the hundreds chart
(e.g., 26 and 76), which is a good place to begin. When the larger number is in a different
column from the smaller number (e.g., 24 and 76), children can add on tens to get to the tar-
get number’s row and then add or subtract ones. There are also other possible approaches.
Consider the numbers 26 and 72, where the column of 72 is to left of the column that 26 is
in. A child might count by tens from 26 to get to 66 (the row before 72) and then count by
ones to get to 72. Or a child might count by tens from 26 to get to 76 and then count back
by ones to get to 72.
You may be so accustomed to thinking about addition and subtraction computation as
involving “regrouping”—the trading of 10 ones for a ten or vice versa—that you tend to be-
lieve that regrouping is an integral part of computation. In fact, virtually all invented strate-
gies for computation as well as mental strategies involve no regrouping at all. Rather, what
happens in most cases might be called “bridging a ten.” The row structure of the hundreds
chart is especially useful in developing this understanding of bridging across ten. Children
should have ample opportunities to develop their ideas in activities like the ones in this
section. Many children are likely still developing their ideas about numbers and distances
between them. These ideas are as much about place‐value understanding as about addition
and subtraction.
219
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
How many cartons of chocolate and plain milk are served in the cafeteria each month?
Collecting data for such problems and grouping them into tens and hundreds will help re-
inforce the value of grouping to count and compare quantities.
The particular way you bring numbers and the real world together in your class is up
to you. But do not underestimate the value of connecting the real world to the classroom.
220
E x p a n d e d L e s s o n
• To connect a count-by-ones understanding to a count • As the two children actually measure the length in the
based on the number of groups of 10 and leftovers for Before component of the lesson, ask them to share
quantities to 100 aloud how they are deciding where to put the connect-
ing cubes or other unit.
• To measure lengths using nonstandard measures
221
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
• Explain that you want to make an estimate of how Provide clear expectations:
long the item is in terms of connecting cubes. Accept • Have the children work in groups of two to four. Tell them
estimates and record them where the children can see. that they will work together to measure each object, but
Expect the children’s guesses to be quite varied. Then each child should make his or her own estimate. Over the
suggest that it might be helpful to estimate in terms of course of the three stations, each child should have the
groups of 10 units and leftovers. Show the children a opportunity to measure an object using individual units
bar of 10 connecting cubes. and to measure using groups of 10 and leftovers.
• Hold the 10 units at one end of the length to be mea-
sured and accept children’s new estimates. Write the During
first child’s estimate on the projected recording sheet.
Initially:
Explain that an estimate is what you think it might be
by looking at the 10 units; it is not just a wild guess. • Observe that the children understand the task and are
in the process of making a reasonable estimate before
• Pass out the recording sheets and have the children re-
measuring each object.
cord their own estimates in the first box. Ask several
children what their estimates are. • Be sure that the children are making and recording es-
timates by comparing the length to the provided group
• Have two children use individual units (e.g., connecting
of 10. They are not to change their estimates.
cubes) to measure the length. It is important that they
line the units end to end along the entire length so that Ongoing:
when they have finished they will have as many actual
• Check that the children are lining individual units along
units as required for the measure. Have two children
the edge of the object without gaps or overlaps when
put the units into groups of 10. Count the groups of 10,
measuring.
and count any leftovers separately. Record this in blanks
labeled “Actual” on the projected recording sheet and • Pay attention to how the children count the total num-
have the children do likewise on their papers. ber of units. Some may already know that 4 tens and 6
leftovers is 46. However, many will count by ones. Chal-
• Finally, ask the children how many units there are. Have
lenge the children who just count groups: “Are you
the class count the entire group by ones as you set them
sure you will get 46 if you count them all by ones?”
aside or point to each. Write the number word and the
number (e.g., thirty-four 34 ) on the projected recording
After
sheet and the unit (e.g., 34 connecting cubes or 34 tooth-
picks). Have children write this on their recording sheets. Bring the class together to share and discuss the task:
• Discuss what it means to estimate—it is not the same as
Present the focus task:
a guess. Ask, “How did using a group of 10 units help
• At three stations, children are to see if they can make you make an estimate? How does counting the groups
reasonable estimates of lengths in terms of groups of of tens and leftovers help tell you how many units you
10 and leftovers. had?” This last question is the key to this lesson. Avoid
• They then check their estimates by actually measuring, telling children how to relate the groups and leftovers
making and counting groups of 10 and leftovers, and to the actual number.
finally counting all the units.
• Explain that there are measuring kits and a length for
each station. For each length, children are to:
Assessment
• Hold the 10 units at one end of the length and esti- Observe
mate the measure of the length in terms of 10 and • Look for children who do not make connections between
leftovers. Each child should record his or her esti- the groups and leftovers and the actual counts. These
mate on the recording sheet. children have not yet developed base-ten concepts.
• They are then to measure the lengths using individ- • Those who confidently state the total when they have
ual units—laying them end to end with no gaps or the number of groups and leftovers have indicated at
overlaps. least a beginning understanding of base-ten concepts.
• When they have placed units along the full length,
they should make sets of 10, then count and record Ask
the number of groups of 10 and leftovers. • How did using a group of 10 units help you make an
• Finally, they should count all of the units and record estimate?
this as a number word and as a number. Refer to your • How does counting the groups of tens and leftovers
example. help tell you how many units you had?
222
Developing Whole‐Number Place‐Value Concepts
References
Dougherty, B., Flores, A., Louis, E., & Sophian, Ross, S. H. (1986). The development of children’s
C. (2010). Developing essential understanding of place‐value numeration concepts in grades two through
number and numeration for teaching mathematics five. Presented at the annual meeting of the
in prekindergarten–grade 2. Reston, VA: National American Educational Research Association, San
Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Francisco. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service
No. ED 2773 482)
Fuson, K. C. (2006). Research on whole number
addition and subtraction. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Ross, S. H. (1989). Parts, wholes, and place value:
Handbook of research on mathematics teaching A developmental perspective. Arithmetic Teacher,
and learning (pp. 243–275). Charlotte, NC: 36(6), 47–51.
Information Age Publishing.
Ross, S. R. (2002). Place value: Problem solving
Kamii, C. K. (1985). Young children reinvent and written assessment. Teaching Children
arithmetic. New York, NY: Teachers College Press. Mathematics, 8(7), 419–423.
Labinowicz, E. (1985). Learning from children: New Wright, R., Martland, J., Stafford, A., & Stanger,
beginnings for teaching numerical thinking. Menlo G. (2008). Teaching number: Advancing children’s
Park, CA: AWL Supplemental. skills and strategies. London: Sage.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
(2000). Principles and standards for school
mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.
223
This page intentionally left blank
Building Strategies for
Whole‐Number Computation
BigIDEAS
1 Flexible methods of addition and subtraction involve taking apart (decomposing) and combining (composing)
numbers in a wide variety of ways. Most of the decomposition of numbers is based on place value or “compatible”
numbers—number pairs that work easily together, such as 25 and 75.
2 Invented strategies are flexible methods of computing that vary with the numbers and the situation involved.
Strategies can be invented by a child, a peer, or the class as a whole; they may even be suggested by the teacher.
3 Flexible methods for computation require a strong understanding of the operations and properties of the operations,
especially the commutative (turn‐around) property and the associative property. How addition and subtraction are related as
inverse operations is also important.
4 The standard algorithms are clever strategies for computing that are based on performing the operation on one
place value at a time with transitions to an adjacent position (trading or regrouping). Standard algorithms tend to cause
children to think in terms of digits rather than the composite number that the digits make up.
5 Multidigit numbers can be built up or taken apart in a variety of ways. These parts can be used to create estimates
in calculations rather than using the exact numbers involved. For example, 17 can be thought of as 15 and 2 or 3 less
than 20.
From Chapter 12 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
225
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
According to the Common Core State Standards, children in grades K-2 should solve
addition and subtraction problems with numbers appropriate for their grade level (within
10 for kindergartners; within 100 for first graders; and within 1000 for second graders).
First graders are expected to add two‐digit numbers to one‐digit numbers or to a multiple
of 10. Second graders are expected to add two‐digit and three‐digit numbers. The solution
methods range from using concrete models or drawings to strategies based on place value,
meanings of operations, and number sense. To support the development of flexible addition
and subtraction strategies, the Common Core State Standards also expect children to be able
to compose and decompose numbers (numbers less than 20 for kindergartners; numbers less
than 100 for first graders; and numbers less than 1000 for second graders).
Addition and subtraction strategies that build on decomposing and composing numbers
in flexible ways contribute to children’s overall number sense. In most everyday instances,
these alternative strategies for computing are easier and faster than the standard algorithms
(procedures for computing) and can often be done mentally.
Consider the following problem.
Mary has 114 spaces in her photo album. So far she has 89 photos in the album. How many
more photos can she put in before the album is full?
Here are just four of many methods that have been used by children in the primary
grades to solve the computation in the photo album problem:
• 89 + 11 is 100. 11 + 14 is 25.
• 90 + 10 is 100 and 14 more is 24 plus 1 (because we should have started at 89, not 90)
is 25.
226
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
Figure 1
A general instructional sequence for three types of computational strategies.
Direct Modeling
Counts by ones.
Invented Strategies
Supported by written recordings.
After the standard algorithms are
Mental methods when appropriate. added to children’s repertoire of
available strategies, reinforce the idea
that just like the other strategies, they
may be more useful in some instances
than in others. Children should continue
Standard Algorithms to discuss which methods seem best
in which situations.
Usually require guided development.
Use base-ten materials to model
the steps. Justify that the algorithm
produces the correct answer. Should
be considered just another strategy.
• Take away 14 to get to 100, then take away 10 more to get to 90, and then 1 more to get
to 89, or take away 25 in all.
• 89, 99, 109 (that’s 20). 110, 111, 112, 113, 114 (keeping track on fingers) is 25.
Strategies such as these can be done mentally, are generally faster than the standard
algorithms, and make sense to the person using them. Every day, children and adults resort
to often error‐prone, standard algorithms when other, more meaningful methods would be
faster and less susceptible to error. Flexibility with a variety of computational strategies is
an important tool for successful daily living. It is time to broaden our perspective of what it
means to compute.
Figure 1 lists a general instructional sequence for three types of computing. The direct
modeling methods can, with guidance, develop into an assortment of more flexible and use-
ful student‐invented strategies, many of which can be carried out mentally. The standard
algorithms remain in the mainstream curricula; however, an emphasis on a variety of strate-
gies is critical to developing number sense as well as procedural proficiency. The standard
algorithms should be seen simply as another strategy that can be added to children’s reper-
toire of available strategies.
Direct Modeling
The developmental step that usually precedes invented strategies is called direct modeling:
The use of manipulatives, drawings, or fingers along with counting to directly represent the
meaning of an operation or story problem. Figure 2 shows an example of direct modeling in
which a child has modeled the numbers in the problem using counters and then counted by
ones to find the answer.
227
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
Invented Strategies
We refer to any strategy, other than the standard algorithm, that does not involve the use
of physical materials or counting by ones as an invented strategy (Carpenter, Franke, Jacobs,
Fennema, & Empson, 1998). In the expectations for first and second graders, the Com-
mon Core State Standards (CCSSO, 2010) describe these as “strategies based on place value,
properties of operations, and/or the relationship between addition and subtraction” (pp. 16,
19). More specifically, children are expected to “develop, discuss, and use efficient, accurate,
and generalizable methods to compute sums and differences of whole numbers in base‐ten
notation, using their understanding of place value and the properties of operations” (p. 17).
At times, invented strategies become mental methods after ideas have been explored, used,
and understood. For example, after some experience, children may be able to do 75 + 19
mentally (75 + 20 is 95, less 1 is 94). For 648 + 257, children may need to write down inter-
mediate steps to aid in memory as they work through the problem. (Try that one yourself.)
In the classroom, written support is often encouraged as strategies develop. Written records
of thinking are more easily shared and help children focus on the ideas. Distinctions among
written, partially written, and mental computation are not important, especially in the de-
velopment period.
A number of research studies have focused attention on how children handle computa-
tional situations when they have been given options for multiple strategies (see, for example,
228
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
Keiser, 2010; Rittle‐Johnson, Star, & Durkin, 2010; Verschaffel, Greer, & De Corte, 2007).
“There is mounting evidence that children both in and out of school can construct methods
for adding and subtracting multi‐digit numbers without explicit instruction” (Carpenter et
al., 1998, p. 4). But not all children invent their own strategies. So strategies invented by
class members are shared, explored, and tried out by others. However, children should not
be permitted to use any strategy without understanding it.
229
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
• Invented strategies are the basis for mental computation and estimation. When in-
vented strategies are the norm for computation, there is no need to talk about mental
computation as if it were a separate skill. As children become more and more proficient
with these flexible methods, they find they are able to use them mentally without having
to write down even intermediate steps.
• Flexible methods are often faster than the standard algorithms. Consider 300 - 98.
A simple invented strategy might use 300 - 100 = 200. Adding 2 back (because we
subtracted 2 too many), we get 202. This is easily done mentally, or even with some
recording, in much less time than the steps to the standard algorithm. Those who be-
come adept with invented strategies will consistently perform addition and subtraction
computations more quickly than those using a standard algorithm.
• Strategy invention is itself an important process of “doing mathematics.” Children who
invent a computational strategy or who adopt a meaningful strategy developed by a class-
mate are involved in the process of sense making. This development of procedures is a
process that is often hidden from children. By engaging in this aspect of mathematics, a
significantly different and valuable view of “doing mathematics” is revealed to learners.
• Children who use invented strategies perform similarly or outperform their counter-
parts who are taught only standard algorithms. Children in other countries such as the
Netherlands are not taught to use standard algorithms, and they perform significantly
better than U.S. children on international measures of proficiency (Fleischman, Hop-
stock, Pelczar, & Shelley, 2010).
Mental Computation
A mental computation strategy is simply any invented strategy that is done without record-
ing steps. What may be a mental strategy for one child may require written steps by another
child. Initially, children may not be ready to do computations mentally, as they may still be
at the direct modeling stage or need to notate parts of the problem as they think it through.
As children become more adept, they can and should be challenged to do appropriate com-
putations mentally. You may be quite amazed at the ability of children (and at your own
ability) to do mental mathematics.
Try this example using mental mathematics:
342 + 153 + 481
Standard Algorithms
More than a century of tradition combined with pressures from families who were taught
only the standard algorithm may result in thinking that there is only one best approach and
one “right” algorithm. Arguments for a single algorithm generally revolve around efficiency
and the need for methods that work with all numbers. For addition and subtraction, how-
ever, well‐understood and practiced invented strategies are sometimes more efficient and
often more accurate.
Although teaching only the standard algorithm does not allow children to explore other
useful approaches, including it among the strategies children learn is important. The main
230
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
focus in teaching the standard algorithm is not learning a memorized series of steps but
making sense of it as a process.
231
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
Figure 3 12 ones), and then subtract 7 from the 12, and so forth. In the
“equal addition” approach (see Figure 3), you add 10 to the
The “equal addition” algorithm.
ones place (2) in 62 to get 12 ones. You would then counteract
the addition of 10 to the minuend by adding 10 to the 27 (sub-
trahend), making 37. Now you subtract the 37. This may sound
confusing to you, but try it. Especially when there are zeros in
the minuend (e.g., 302 - 178), you may find this an easier ap-
proach than our “standard” algorithm. More importantly, your
possible confusion can give you a sense of how your children
(and their families) may react to a completely different proce-
dure from the one they know and find successful.
232
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
Figure 4
Two methods of recording children’s thought processes on the board so that the class can follow
the strategy.
S: Yes, 80 and 20 is 100. Then I added the other S: It was easier to think about 80 than 84. I will save
20 and got 120. the other part of 8 until later. Then I jumped back 60
to get 20.
T: (writes the equations on the board)
60 4
S: Then I added the 6 and the 7 and got 13.
T: (writes this equation)
20 80 84
S: Then I added the 120 to the 13 and got 133.
S: Then I jumped back 4.
T: Indicates with joining lines.
47 86 4 60 4
20 20 7 80 6
80 + 20 = 100 100 + 20 = 120 16 20 80 84
6 + 7 = 13 T: Why 4?
S: That was how much I still had left over from 68.
133
233
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
The shortcut strategy involves the flexible adjustment of numbers. For example, just as
children used 10 as an anchor in learning their basic facts, they can move numbers such as
38 or 69 to the nearest 10 and then take the 2 or 1 off to compensate later. In this case, 40 +
70 equals 110, and then take off the three extras to get 107. As another example, 51 - 37 can
be thought of as 51 + 6 to get 57 and 57 - 37 = 20; then subtract 6 (because you added 6 to
make the problem easier) to get 14. So 51 - 37 = 14.
As these examples suggest, the numbers involved in a computation will tend to influ-
ence how children approach a problem. The type of story problems used can also influence
strategies children use. Therefore, it is important to think carefully about the type of story
problem you pose as well as the numbers you use in problems.
Quickly review the Up Over 10 and Down Over 10 strategies from basic facts using ten‐
frames. Then pose an addition or subtraction story problem that crosses a decade num-
ber and involves a change or difference of less than 10. The following problems are
examples.
• Tommy was on page 47 of his book. Then he read 6 more pages. What page did he
end up on?
• How far is it from 68 to 75?
• Meghan had 42 cents. She bought a small toy for 8 cents. How much money does
she have left?
Two children can work together to determine how to quickly find the total.
Listen for children who are counting on or counting back by ones without paying at-
tention to the ten. For these children, suggest that they use either a hundreds chart or the
Standards for little ten‐frames, shown in Figure 5, to support their thinking. Also find out how they solve
Mathematical Practice fact combinations such as 8 + 6 and 13 - 5. The use of tens for these facts is essentially the
7 Look for and ◀ same as for the higher decade problems. Have children who are using the ten to solve these
make use of structure problems share their strategy. For 47 + 6 you should expect something like I added 3 from the
6 to the 47 to get to 50. Then I added the remaining 3 to get to 53.
234
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
Figure 6 illustrates four different strategies for the following story problem, which in-
volves addition of 2 two‐digit numbers. The recording methods in Figure 6 are suggestions.
Two Scout troops went on a field trip. There were 46 Girl Scouts and 38 Boy Scouts. How
many Scouts went on the trip?
The shortcut and compensation strategies that focus on making ten are useful when one
of the numbers ends in 8 or 9. To promote these strategies, present problems with addends
like 39 or 58. Note that it is only necessary to adjust one of the two numbers.
235
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
Figure 6 Four different invented strategies for addition with two‐digit numbers.
Add Tens, Add Ones, Then Combine Move Some to Make Tens 46 + 38
46 + 38 46 + 38 44 + 40
40 + 30 = 70 84
40 and 30 is 70. 6 and 8 is 14. Take 2 from the 46 and put it with the 38 to make 40.
70 and 14 is 84.
6 + 8 = 14 Now you have 44 and 40 more is 84.
the 46 44
from
84 2—
Add on Tens, Then Add Ones
46 + 38 38 40 84
46 and 30 more is 76. Then I added on 46 + 30 —› Use a Nice Number and Compensate
the other 8. 76 and 4 is 80 and 4 is 84. 76 + 8 —› 80, 84 46 + 38 46 + 40 —›
46 and 40 is 86. That’s 2 extra, so it’s 84. 86 – 2 —› 84
10 10 10 4 4 40
46 76 80 84 2
46 84 86
The next activity provides children opportunities to add multidigit numbers outside a
story problem context.
Children work in pairs for this activity. Without communicating to each other,
each child writes down a two‐digit number. (You can also use three‐digit numbers
depending on the children.) The children work together to find out the sum of the two
numbers. Once a sum is determined, they should use little ten‐frame cards to represent
the two numbers they started with and use those to check their sum. Children with dis-
abilities may initially need to use the little ten‐frames to help them determine the sum.
This activity could also be completed by displaying two numbers on a projector and
having children work in pairs at their desks. Follow up by having children share their
strategies for finding the sum.
The following activity encourages children to think about different ways to adjust num-
bers as they solve problems.
236
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
Suppose you want to help children think about adjusting numbers by using 10 as an
anchor. Create a series of problems using numbers that will increase the likelihood that
children will gravitate toward this idea, but do not require children to use this idea and
do not be disappointed if they do not use this idea. The point is to try to help children
become more aware of different ways to adjust numbers. Following is one possible series
of problems:
50 + 30 48 + 30 50 + 32 51 + 28 20 + 60 18 + 58
Project one problem at a time and give children time to solve it before you ask first
for answers and then for explanations. Record children’s strategies so that you can refer
back to them when appropriate. If no child suggests the idea of using 50 + 30 or 20 + 60
to help solve the subsequent related problems, you may want to challenge them to deter-
mine how they could use that problem to help them solve the others.
Note that there are multiple ways to add the numbers from the preceding activity and
there are multiple ways to use 10 as an anchor to solve the series of problems. For example,
for 51 + 28, a child may change 51 to 50 and 28 to 30, add 50 + 30 to get 80, and then adjust
for the 30 and 50—subtract 2 to adjust for adding 30 (instead of 28) and add 1 for adding
50 (instead of 51)—to get 79. Other children may think that 51 is really close to 50 and
they move 1 to the 28 and add 50 + 29 to get 79. Still other children may add the tens (50 +
20) and then the ones (1 + 8) to get 79. This last strategy did not use 50 + 30, which is fine.
Again, your goal is to help children develop strategies that are efficient and that make sense
to them. Listening to how some of their classmates used 50 + 30 can help others be more
aware of different ways to adjust numbers.
Subtracting by Counting Up
This is an amazingly powerful way to subtract. Children working on the think‐addition strat-
egy for their basic facts can also be solving problems with larger numbers. The concept is
the same. For example, for 45 - 19, the idea is to think, “How much do I add to 19 to get to
45?” Notice that this strategy is not as efficient for 45 - 6. Using join with change unknown
problems or missing‐part problems will encourage the counting‐up strategy. Here is an ex-
ample of each.
Sam had 46 baseball cards. He went to a card show and got some more cards for his collec-
tion. Now he has 73 cards. How many cards did Sam buy at the card show?
Juanita counted all of her crayons. Some were broken and some were not. She counted 73 Standards for
Mathematical Practice
crayons in all. 46 crayons were not broken. How many were broken?
1 Make sense
◀
Figure 7 shows invented strategies for these story problems. As you can see, using tens of problems and
is also an important part of these strategies. Simply asking for the difference between two persevere in
solving them
numbers may also prompt these strategies.
237
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
Add Tens to Get Close, Then Ones Add Ones to Make a Ten, Then Tens and Ones
46 + 20 = 66
73 – 46 66 + 4 = 70 73 – 46 46 + 4 —› 50
46 and 20 is 66. (30 more is too much.) 46 and 4 is 50. 50 and 20 is 70 and 3 50 + 20 —› 70
Then 4 more is 70 and 3 is 73. That’s 20
70 + 3 = 73 more is 73. The 4 and 3 is 7 and 20 is 27.
20 + 4 + 3 = 27 70 + 3 —› 73
and 7 or 27.
4 + 20 + 3 = 27
27
20
4 3 4 10 10 3
46 66 70 73 46 50 60 70 73
Add Tens to Overshoot, Then Come Back
73 – 46 46 + 30 —› 76 – 3 —› 73 46 + 4 —› 50
46 and 30 is 76. That’s 3 too Similarly, 46 and 4 is 50.
much, so it’s 27.
30 – 3 = 27 50 and 23 is 73.
50 + 23 —› 73
23 and 4 is 27. 23 + 4 = 27
27
30 23
4
-3
46 73 76 46 50 73
Take‐Away Subtraction
Using take‐away subtraction is considerably more difficult to do mentally. However, take‐
away strategies are common, probably because many textbooks emphasize take‐away as the
meaning of subtraction. Four different strategies are shown in Figure 8 for the following
story problem.
There were 73 children on the playground. The 46 second‐grade students came in first. How
many children were still outside?
The two methods that begin by taking tens from tens are reflective of what most chil-
dren do with base‐ten pieces. The other two methods leave one of the numbers intact and
subtract from it. When the subtracted number is a multiple of 10 or close to a multiple of
10, take‐away subtraction can be an easy method to do mentally. Try 83 - 29 in your head by
first taking away 30 and adding 1 back. Some children will become confused when they hear
a classmate describe this strategy for 83 - 29. In particular, they do not understand why you
add 1 back. They think because you added 1 to 29 to make it 30, then you should subtract 1
from the answer. Use a story problem with the numbers and let them act it out so that they
can see that when they take away 30, they took 1 too many away and that is why they need
to add 1 back.
Remember the “equal addition” algorithm that was described in the section on standard
algorithms? There are some children who use this strategy as an invented strategy with
take‐away subtraction. For example, for 32 - 17, children might think that 32 is 2 away from
30, so if they subtract 2 from 32 to get 30, they need to subtract 2 from 17 to get 15. Now
the problem is 30 - 15 or 15.
238
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
Take Tens from the Tens, Then Subtract Ones Take Away Tens, Then Ones
73 – 46 73 – 46 73 – 40 —> 33 – 3
70 minus 40 is 30. 73 minus 40 is 33. Then take away 6:
Take away 6 more 70 – 40 —› 30 – 6 3 makes 30 and 3 more is 27. 30 – 3 —> 27
is 24. 24 + 3 —› 27 –40
Now add in the –3 –3
3 ones 27.
27 30 33 73
–6 –40
3 Take Extra Tens, Then Add Back
73 – 46 73 – 50 —> 23 + 4
24 27 30 70
73 take away 50 is 23. That’s 4 too many.
23 and 4 is 27. 27
50
Or took 4
70 minus 40 is 30. I can 70 – 40 = 30 extra +4
take those 3 away, but I (73 – 3 = 70)
need to take away 3 more 30 – 3 = 27 23 27 73
from the 30 to make 27.
Add to the Whole If Necessary
–40 +3
73 – 46 73 – 46—›
–3 –3
Give 3 to 73 to make 76. 76 take away
46 is 30. Now give 3 back 27.
76 – 46 —› 30
27 30 70 73
46 – 3 —› 27
-3
+3
27 30 73 76
For many subtraction problems, especially those with three digits, an adding‐on ap-
proach is significantly easier than a take‐away approach. Try not to force the issue with
children who do not use an add‐on strategy. However, you may want to revisit some simple
missing‐part activities that are more likely to encourage that type of thinking.
Children work in pairs for this activity. Without communicating to each other,
one child writes down a number less than 50 while the other child writes down a
number greater than 50. You may choose to limit the size of the second number (e.g., less
than 100; less than 500) depending on the children. The children work together to find
out how much more must be added to the smaller number to get to the larger number.
Once an answer is determined, they should use little ten‐frame cards to represent the
smaller number and the amount they found to see whether the total matches the larger
number. Children with disabilities may initially need to use the little ten‐frames to help
support their work in finding out how much more to add on.
239
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
“How Far to My Number?” can also be done by displaying two numbers on a projector
and having children work in pairs at their desks. Another idea to help children think about
using an adding‐on approach is to show a number such as 28 with little ten‐frame cards and
ask, “What goes with 28 to make 53?” You can do the same with three‐digit numbers with or
without the use of models. As always, it is important to have children share their strategies
during a discussion.
Bridging
For most of the strategies, it is easier to add or subtract when bridging is not required. Try
each strategy with 34 + 52 or 68 - 24 to see how it works. Easier problems instill confidence.
They also permit you to challenge children with a “harder one.” There is also the issue of
bridging 100 or 1000. Try 58 + 67 with different strategies. Bridging across hundreds is also
an issue for subtraction. Problems such as 128 - 50 or 128 - 45 are more difficult than ones
that do not cross a hundred.
Larger Numbers
The Common Core State Standards recommend that second graders add and subtract three‐
digit numbers using a variety of strategies. Try seeing how you would do these without using
the standard algorithms: 487 + 235 and 623 - 247. For subtraction, a counting‐up strategy is
usually the easiest. Occasionally, children will use other strategies with larger numbers. For
example, “chunking off ” multiples of 50 or 25 is often a useful method. For 462 + 257, pull
out 450 and 250 to make 700. That leaves 12 and 7 more, making 719.
Pose a problem to the class that requires regrouping across zero. The
problem should be one that will be easy for children to check using
an invented strategy, such as 103 - 78. Children work in pairs using
base‐ten models and place‐value mats. Once they have identified an
answer, they should check their answer using an invented strategy.
Figure 11 Help children record on paper each step they do on their place‐value mats.
242
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
If they did not get the same answer using base‐ten models and their invented strategy, en- Figure 12
courage them to try to determine why. Follow up with a discussion that starts with children An alternative
sharing their ideas. recording scheme for
addition. Notice that
this can be used from
left to right as well as
from right to left.
358
There are several versions of base‐ten blocks online. One example called “Base Blocks Addition” is found at 276
the National Library of Virtual Manipulatives (http://nlvm.usu.edu/en/nav/grade_g_1.html). You can create
any problem up to four digits or you can let the applet generate problems. When you want to regroup,
500
simply drag a rectangle around the pieces you wish to join. 120
14
634
Develop the Written Record
The process of recording each step as it is done is the same as was suggested for addition.
You can also use the same recording sheets (see Figure 10).
When children can explain the use of the symbols involved in the recording process,
move them away from the use of the physical materials and on to a completely symbolic
level. Again, be attentive to problems with zeros.
If children are permitted to follow their natural instincts and begin with the big pieces
(from the left instead of the right), recording schemes similar to that shown in Figure 14 are
possible. The trades are made from the pieces remaining after the subtraction in the column
to the left has been done. In this case, a “regroup across zero” situation will still occur in
problems like 462 - 168. Try it.
It does
not matter
45 which ones
come off.
–27 Put the
leftovers
2 2 together.
7 7
Not enough ones to take off 7. And now I can take off 2 tens.
Trade a ten for 10 ones.
45
–27
18
2 7
2 7
Now there are 15 ones. That’s 18 left.
I can take 7 off easily.
243
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
Figure 14 Once children understand the standard algorithms for addition and subtraction of mul-
tidigit numbers, it is important to provide them opportunities to determine which strategy
A left‐hand recording
scheme for subtraction. from a variety of strategies might be more useful, given the specific numbers involved. The
Other methods can also following activity provides this experience.
be devised.
1314
734
275
500 “Base Blocks Subtraction” is an applet found at the National Library of Virtual Manipulatives (http://nlvm
.usu.edu/en/nav/grade_g_1.html). You can create any problem up to four digits or you can let the applet
460 generate problems. The applet uses blue blocks to represent the top number and red blocks to represent
59 the bottom number (the number being subtracted). When the blue blocks are dragged onto the red blocks,
459 they disappear. Although you can begin in any column, if you start in the ones column and trade when
necessary, the applet reinforces the standard algorithm by displaying a corresponding written record.
Project a list of strategies related to how children might solve addition (or subtraction)
problems. This could include whatever names you have used to label the invented strate-
gies as well as the standard algorithm. Tell children you are going to project a problem
for them to see, but they are NOT to solve it—they are simply to tell which of the various
methods they would choose to solve the problem and be ready to explain why. After
they have made their choice, have children raise their hand as you say each method to
indicate that is the method they have selected. Then tell them they are going to solve the
given problem using their selected method and, once they are finished, they are to raise
their thumb and hold it to their chest to indicate they are finished. Have children share
solutions for each different method. Then ask which method seemed to work best for
this situation. Make sure to use a variety of problems whose numbers lend themselves to
different strategies.
Helping children develop flexible methods of adding and subtracting strengthens their
understanding of place value, number relationships, and operations. Comparing methods
used, including the standard algorithm, supports children in better understanding these
methods, which results in their making fewer computational errors. This assortment of
computational methods will serve children well in the real world.
244
E x p a n d e d L e s s o n
245
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
• They should provide enough information on their paper • One good visual representation is to use an empty
so that, if they shared it with other second‐grade chil- number line. You can use the number line when a child
dren, other children would understand what they did. suggests a strategy that involves skip counting by tens
• Tell children to continue on to the second and third (e.g., “I started with 35, added 10 and 10 and 10 to get
problems after they finish the first one. to 65, then counted by five to 70, and then by ones to
72, and got 37.”). Your number line on the board may
look like this:
During
Initially: 37
• Observe that children have access to the materials
needed to solve the problem. 30
10 10 10 7
• Observe that each child understands the question and is
5
in the process of attempting to solve the first situation. 2
Ongoing: 35 45 55 65 70 72
using and the models they are using to solve the prob-
lem. See the “Assessment” section of this lesson for de-
• If there are different answers, allow the children with
tails. Keep these in mind for selecting who will share in
different answers to explain their thinking, and the
the After phase of the lesson.
class as a whole can determine if the answer makes
• As children work, ask them to tell you what the prob- sense. The responsibility for deciding what is correct
lem is asking and how they are thinking about solving falls to the class, not you.
it. See the “Assessment” section at the end of the les-
• Repeat the process for the second and third problems.
son for details.
• If a child is stuck, try to make a suggestion that builds on
the child’s ideas. For example, if the child says, “I want
to take 35 from 72, but I don’t know how to start,” you Assessment
might ask the child if the hundreds chart might help. Or
you might suggest that the child try one of the ideas that Observe
was mentioned in the Before phase of the lesson. After • Use a checklist to identify children using strategies that
making a suggestion, walk away and check back later. involve counting by ones. You may need to meet with
• If you notice an error, rather than correct it, ask children these children in a small group to encourage them to
to explain how they solved the problem (children often use skip counting or landmark numbers as a faster,
catch their mistakes while explaining and showing). more efficient strategy.
• Look to see whether children are using different strate-
After gies across the problems or using the same strategy.
246
Building Strategies for Whole‐Number Computation
References
Carpenter, T. P., Franke, M. L., Jacobs, V. Klein, A. S., Beishuizen, M., & Treffers, A. (2002).
R., Fennema, E., & Empson, S. B. (1998). A The empty number line in Dutch second grade. In
longitudinal study of invention and understanding J. Sowder & B. Schapelle (Eds.), Lessons learned from
in children’s multidigit addition and subtraction. research (pp. 41–44). Reston, VA: National Council
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29(1), of Teachers of Mathematics.
3–20.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Fleischman, H. L., Hopstock, P. J., Pelczar, M. (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics.
P., & Shelley, B. E. (2010). Highlights from PISA Reston, VA: Author.
2009: Performance of U.S. 15‐year‐old students
National Research Council. (2001). Adding it up:
in reading, mathematics, and science literacy
Helping children learn mathematics. J. Kilpatrick, J.
in an international context (NCES 2011–004).
Swafford, & B. Findell (Eds.). Washington, DC:
Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing
National Academies Press.
Office.
Torbeyns, J., De Smedt, B., Ghesquiere, P., &
Gravemeijer, K., & van Galen, F. (2003). Facts
Verschaffel, L. (2009). Acquisition and use of
and algorithms as products of students’ own
shortcut strategies by traditionally schooled
mathematical activity. In J. Kilpatrick, W. G. Martin,
children. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 71,
& D. Schifter (Eds.), A research companion to Principles
1–17.
and Standards for School Mathematics (pp. 114–122).
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Verschaffel, L., Greer, B., & De Corte, E. (2007).
Mathematics. Whole number concepts and operations. In
F. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on
Kamii, C. K., & Dominick, A. (1998). The harmful
mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 557–628).
effects of algorithms in grades 1–4. In L. J. Morrow
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of
(Ed.), The teaching and learning of algorithms in school
Mathematics.
mathematics (pp. 130–140). Reston, VA: National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
247
This page intentionally left blank
Promoting Algebraic
Reasoning
From Chapter 13 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
249
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
thinking should be connected to the focal points at every grade level with the primary top-
ics being the use of patterns leading to generalizations (especially with operations), the
study of change, and a rudimentary concept of function. In the Common Core State Stan-
dards (CCSSO, 2010), the close connection between arithmetic and algebra is evident by an
identified domain called “Operations and Algebraic Thinking.” This chapter on promoting
algebraic reasoning follows the number and operation chapters so that you can see the close
relationship between number concepts, operations, and algebraic reasoning.
Nine frogs are sitting on two lily pads, one small lily pad and one big lily pad. Show the dif-
ferent ways that the nine frogs could sit on the two lily pads.
This problem can also provide children with the occasion to notice generalizable character-
istics, such as increasing the number on the big lily pad by one means reducing the number
on the small lily pad by one. To facilitate this opportunity, children can be challenged to find
all the ways the frogs can be on the two lily pads. The sig-
nificant algebraic question is how to decide when all the solu-
tions have been found. To begin with, children will just reply
In these kinds of problems, children often do not consider
they cannot think of any more ways. By asking questions to
the combinations with zero. If this happens, simply ask,
focus children’s attention on the relationship between adja-
“Could one of the lily pads be empty?”
cent solutions (e.g., 2 + 7 and 3 + 6) (asking questions, not
250
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
telling!), children will eventually begin to strategically use each number from 0 to 9 for one
lily pad and the corresponding number for the other lily pad.
Continue to work toward generalization by looking at other quantities of frogs (e.g.,
how many ways for 5 frogs? for 6 frogs?), asking children what patterns they notice across
these cases. When a child explains that for each number 0 to 9 there is one solution, he or Standards for
she is no longer partitioning 9 into parts to find solutions but is making a generalization Mathematical Practice
about how to determine the number of solutions without having to list them. For example, 8 Look for and
the child might explain, “There is always one more way than the number of frogs.” ◀ express regularity in
This reasoning can be applied to other numbers and in other contexts, which is an im- repeated reasoning
portant step in generalization. The children’s book The Sleepover (Fosnot, 2007) describes
a context of eight children rearranging themselves on a pair of bunk beds that would offer
another opportunity for children to grapple with the idea of finding all the possible combi-
nations for a number. (Fosnot’s book The Sleepover can be found at www.heinemann.com
/products/E01084.aspx.)
The following activity purposefully focuses children’s attention on the adjacent facts in
an effort to help them generalize and reason more strategically.
Have children select a number, such as 7, and add it to itself. The task is to find out what
happens to the sum if you add 1 to one of the sevens and subtract 1 from the other
seven (e.g., 8 + 6). Ask children, “Does this work with other numbers? Does it only work
if you start with a number plus itself? Can you explain why it works? What else can you
find out?”
Activity 1, “One Up and One Down with Addition,” poses a significant task for first and
second graders. Some children may wonder if it works for “really big numbers.” Suggest they
test their ideas with a calculator. It is also useful to explore the idea of two up and two down,
and so on. Of course, the results are the same as long as the same amount is added and sub-
Standards for
tracted from each number. The result of this exploration can be useful when learning basic Mathematical Practice
facts (6 + 8 is the same as 7 + 7 or double 7). Children may want to know if the one‐up/one‐
down idea works for subtraction. (It does not.) But there is an important pattern to discover: If 2 Reasoning
both numbers change in the same direction, up or down, the result is the same. (This is called ◀ abstractly and
quantitatively
“equal additions.”) Recognizing this allows us to change 12 – 8 to 10 – 6, or 83 - 48 to 85 - 50,
changing potentially difficult problems to easier ones.
Generalizing can be extended to symbols, even with first and second graders. For example,
in the frogs and lily pad problem, the teacher might respond to the child who states that there
will always be one more solution than the number of frogs by asking, “How about if there were
n frogs?” Children will typically respond using their own words with something like, “It would
just be one more than that.” Challenge them to determine how they might write this if n indi-
cates the number of frogs. Children will struggle with this question but it extends the oppor-
tunity for them to think about the generalization and how they might communicate about it.
Contexts like the frogs on lily pads can provide opportunities to address commutativ-
ity. For example, some children might consider pairs of solutions such as 2 + 7 and 7 + 2
as different solutions because of the contextual situation; that is, 2 + 7 could represent
2 frogs on the small lily pad and 7 frogs on the big lily pad and then 7 + 2 would represent
7 frogs on the small lily pad and 2 frogs on the big lily pad. Others may argue that they are
the “same,” meaning that although 2 + 7 and 7 + 2 are modeling different situations, they
still result in 9 frogs on lily pads. It is important to allow children to work through this idea
251
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
Standards for of commutativity so look for multiple places to revisit the idea. Consider the suggestion in
Mathematical Practice
the next paragraph.
8 Look for and Slight shifts in how arithmetic problems are presented can open up opportunities for
express regularity in ◀ generalizations (Blanton, 2008). For example, instead of a series of unrelated addition prob-
repeated reasoning lems, consider the following list:
4+7 7+4 23 + 15 15 + 23
Once children have solved these problems, you can focus their attention on the addends, ask-
ing questions such as, “What do you notice?” “Will this always be true?” and “How could we
write that using symbols?” In their own words, children will explain that the numbers can be
added in any order. Even if children show some understanding of commutativity of single‐
digit addends, it is important to help them recognize the generalizability of the property with
larger numbers.
The next activity continues to have children explore patterns involving place value and
addition.
Provide children with a hard copy of a hundreds chart (see Blackline Master 10) or you
BLM can have them use an interactive virtual hundreds chart (see, for example, www.crickweb
.co.uk/ks2numeracy‐tools.html). Have children select any four numbers in the hundreds
252
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
chart that form a square. They are to add the two numbers on each diagonal as in the
example shown here.
47 48 49 50
57 58 59 60
67 68 69 70
77 78 79 80
Children should explore other diagonal sums on the chart. Expand their search to diagonals
of any rectangle. For example, the numbers 15, 19, 75, and 79 form four corners of a rect-
angle. The sums 15 + 79 and 19 + 75 are equal. Challenge children to figure out why this
happens. (See Figure 1.)
Figure 1
Diagonals on a hundreds chart. For any four numbers
forming a rectangular arrangement on the hundreds
chart, the sum of the corner numbers on one diagonal
Here are some additional tasks you might explore on a
equals the sum of the corner numbers on the other
hundreds chart.
diagonal.
• When skip counting, which numbers make diagonal
patterns? Which numbers make column patterns? Can
you describe a rule for explaining when a number will 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
have a diagonal or column pattern?
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
• If you move down two and over one on the hundreds
chart, what is the relationship between the original 21 29
number and the new number?
39
• Can you find two skip‐count patterns with one color
marker “on top of” the other (i.e., all of the shaded val- 49
ues for one pattern are part of the shaded values for
the other)? How are these two skip‐count numbers re- 59
lated? Is this true for any pair of numbers that have this 69
relationship?
79 80
These examples extend number concepts to algebraic rea-
soning. Asking questions such as “When will this be true?” 90
and “Why does this work?” requires children to generalize
and thereby strengthen their understanding of the number 100
concepts they are learning.
253
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
8+4= +5
How do you think children in the early grades or in middle school typically answer this
question? ■
For this activity, each pair of children will need eight note cards labeled with the equa-
tions 10 + 1 to 10 + 8. They lay those cards out on their desks face up.
254
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
10 + 1 10 + 3
10 + 2
10 + 4 10 + 5
10 + 6
10 + 7 10 + 8
They will also need a deck of playing cards with all the face cards, aces, and tens
removed. Each child draws one playing card from the deck. Together the partners decide
which note card is equivalent to the sum of their playing cards. They place the playing
cards behind the identified note card. If the sum of the playing cards is less than 10, they
slide the cards back in the deck in random places. Have children write the two expressions
as an equation (e.g., 5 + 8 = 10 + 3) to reinforce the idea that these quantities are equal.
Children can also play this game independently.
To begin with, children struggle to understand how amounts that look different can ac-
tually be equivalent. In other words, they wonder why the numbers do not have to be iden- Standards for
tical. Consequently, children will initially need to add the numbers on each side to verify Mathematical Practice
for themselves that they are the same quantity. As they come to understand compensation 7 Look for and make
(e.g., how part of one number can be moved to another number), they begin to understand ◀ use of structure
how equivalent quantities do not have to be identical. Once you know children can use com-
pensation, challenge them to find the equivalent expression without computing first.
The next activity continues to challenge children to find different ways to express
equivalent amounts.
Challenge children to find different ways to express a particular number, say, 6. Give a
few examples, such as 3 + 3 or 12 - 6. Encourage children to use both addition and sub-
traction in the same expression. Ask questions such as, “How many ways can you make
6 using at least one number greater than 10?” Have children write equations using their
expressions (e.g., 12 - 6 = 0 + 6; 15 - 9 = 2 + 4). In your discussion, emphasize that each
expression is a way of representing the same quantity. Adding a context (e.g., trading
cards) can support children’s reasoning.
Why is it so important that children in grades pre-K–2 correctly understand the equal
sign? First, it is important for children to understand and symbolize relationships in our number
system and the equal sign is a principal method of representing these relationships. For ex-
ample, 8 + 5 = 8 + 2 + 3 shows a basic fact strategy. When ideas that are initially and informally
developed through arithmetic are generalized and expressed symbolically, children have access
to powerful relationships for working with other numbers in a generalized manner. A second
255
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
reason, although removed from the pre-K–2 classroom, is that when older students have a poor
understanding of the equal sign, they typically have difficulty working with algebraic expressions
(Knuth, Stephens, McNeil, & Alibali, 2006). Helping pre-K–2 children develop a solid under-
standing of the equal sign can in turn help them avoid such difficulties in later grades.
Ask children to raise their arms to look like a seesaw. Explain that you have softballs,
all weighing the same, and tennis balls, all weighing the same. The softballs are
-La
g
n uag heavier than the tennis balls. (Have some softballs and tennis balls in case children,
nglish
e Lerane
especially ELLs, are not familiar with these items.) Tell children to imagine that you
rE
fo rs
have placed a softball in each of their left hands. Ask them what would happen to
their seesaw (children should bend to the left side). Tell children to imagine that you place
another softball in their right hands (children should level off). Next with the softballs still
there, ask them to imagine a tennis ball added to the left. Finally, say you are adding an-
other tennis ball in the left hand again. Then ask them to imagine a tennis ball moving over
to the right hand. This is a particularly important activity for children with disabilities who
may be challenged with the abstract idea of balancing values of expressions.
After acting out the seesaw several times, you can ask children to create and share their
observations. For example, one child may share, “If you have a balanced seesaw and add
something to one side, it will tilt to that side.” Another child may explain, “If you take away
the same object from both sides of the seesaw, the seesaw won’t change.”
Standards for Tell children that the cubes weigh the same and the balls weigh the same. Then ask
Mathematical Practice the children to think about what they know about the shapes. Have children share with
a partner what they think they know. If children do not focus on the weights, ask a more
2 Reason abstractly
◀ directed question such as, “What do you know about how the weights of the balls and
and quantitatively
the cubes compare?” Have children explain their thinking.
256
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
Figure 2 shows a series of other examples for the pan balance. Two or more balances for
a single problem provide different information about the shapes or variables. Problems of
this type can be adjusted in difficulty for children in grades K–2. When no numbers are in-
volved, as in the top three examples in Figure 2, children can find combinations of numbers
for the shapes that make the pans balance. The different shapes represent different variables
and so would have different values. There are often different paths to finding a solution.
To create your own pan balance problems, start by giving values to two or three shapes.
Place shapes in groups and add the values. Be sure your problems can be solved!
8 12
6 10 7
257
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
Figure 3
Using expressions and variables in equations and
inequalities. The two‐pan balance helps develop
the meaning of =, <, and >.
You can find more “Pan Balance” explorations at http://illuminations.nctm
.org/ActivityDetail.aspx?id=33. Use the directions under “Explorations” so that
(a)
you can help your children strategically explore the tasks found at this site.
12 – 7 3+4
12 – 7 < 3 + 4
Tilt! After children have experiences with tasks involving shapes,
they can explore numbers and then variables using pan balances.
4+4+4 6+6
455 + 197 460 + 192 On the board, draw or project a simple two‐pan bal-
ance. In each pan, write a numeric expression and ask
which pan will go down or whether the two will balance (see
455 + 197 460 + 192 Figure 3a). Challenge children to write their own expressions
Can you determine whether the expressions for each side of the scale to make it balance. Include examples
balance without doing the addition? such as the third and fourth balances for which children can
analyze the relationships on both sides (as opposed to doing
(b) the computation) to determine whether the pan tilts or bal-
64 − 26 63 − 27
ances. For children with disabilities, rather than have them
write their own expressions, initially have them identify from
64 − 26 63 − 27 a small collection of cards with expressions the ones that will
make the scale balance.
Can you determine whether the expressions
balance without doing the subtraction?
e Lerane
rE
you mean by a true equation and a false equation (e.g., 2 + 3 = 5 is a true equa-
fo rs
tion; 4 + 3 = 2 is a false equation). Then put several simple equations on the board,
some true and some false. Here are some examples:
5+2=7 4+1=6
8 = 10 - 1 7 = 12 - 5
To begin with, keep the computations simple. The children’s task is to decide which
of the equations are true and which are false. For each response, they must explain their
reasoning.
258
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
After this initial exploration, have children explore equations that are in a less famil-
iar form:
3+7=7+3 10 - 3 = 11 - 4 9 + 6 = 0 + 14
8=8 15 + 7 + 3 = 16 + 10
Listen to the types of reasons children use to justify their answers and plan additional
equations accordingly. ELLs and children with disabilities will benefit from first explaining
(or showing) their reasoning to a partner as a low‐risk speaking opportunity and then
sharing with the whole group.
Children will typically agree about equations when there is an expression on one side
and a single number on the other, although initially equations such as 7 = 12 - 5 may gener-
ate discussion. For equations with no operation (e.g., 8 = 8), the discussion may be lively.
Children often believe there must be an operation on one side and an “answer” on the other.
Reinforce that the equal sign means “is the same amount as” by using that language as you
read the symbol.
After children have experiences with true/false sentences, introduce open sentences—
that is, equations with a box or letter to be replaced by a number. To develop an understand-
ing of open sentences, encourage children to look at the number sentence as a whole and
describe in words what the equation represents.
Write several open sentences on the board. These can be similar to the true/false sen-
tences that you have been exploring. Here are some examples:
5+2= 4+ =6 4+5= -1
6-n=7-4 n+5=5+8 15 + 27 = n + 28
Ask children to decide what number can replace the box (or letter) to make the sen-
tence true. They should be ready to explain their reasoning.
Relational Thinking
Once children understand that the equal sign means that the quantities on both sides are the Standards for
same amount, they can use relational thinking to solve problems. Relational thinking occurs Mathematical Practice
when a child observes and uses number relationships between the two sides of the equal sign 2 Reason abstractly
instead of actually computing the amounts. Relational thinking of this sort is the first step ◀ and quantitatively
toward generalizing relationships found in arithmetic to relationships used when variables
are involved.
To illustrate this kind of thinking, consider the two explanations for placing an 8 in the
box for 7 + = 6 + 9.
Explanation 1: Since 6 + 9 is 15, I need to figure out 7 plus what equals 15. It is 8,
so the box is 8.
Explanation 2: Seven is one more than the 6 on the other side. That means that the box
should be one less than 9, so it must be 8.
259
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
The first child computed the sum on one side of the equation and then used the sum to
determine the missing part on the other side. The second child used a relationship between
the expressions on either side of the equal sign. This child did not need to compute the
values on each side.
To facilitate relational thinking and the meaning of the equal sign, continue to have
children explore increasingly complex true/false and open sentences that are designed to
elicit relational thinking rather than computation. Although not a guarantee, posing prob-
lems with larger numbers that make computation difficult (not impossible) can prompt chil-
dren to try a relationship approach. Here are some examples to consider.
TRUE/FALSE:
674 - 369 = 664 - 379 37 + 54 = 38 + 53 376 - 329 = 76 - 29
OPEN SENTENCES:
73 + 56 = 71 + 126 - 37 = - 40 68 + 58 = 57 + 69 + n
The following activity will help to solidify children’s understanding of the equal sign.
260
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
After children have had ample time to discuss true/false and open sentences, ask them to
make up their own true/false sentences that they can use to challenge their classmates.
Each child should write a collection of three or four sentences with at least one true and
at least one false sentence. Encourage them to include one “tricky” one. Their equations
can be either traded with a partner or shared with the whole class. Repeat for open
sentences.
When children write their own true/false sentences, they often are intrigued with the
idea of using large numbers and lots of numbers in their sentences, especially if you have
challenged them to include “tricky” sentences. Support their efforts, as these kinds of prob-
lems tend to help move children toward relational thinking.
Rebekah had 5 apples in her basket. She picked some more after lunch. Then she had 13.
How many apples did she pick after lunch?
Children may recognize this as a difference and realize the answer is 8. The equation that
fits this story is 5 + ___ = 13. Note the change is unknown. Therefore, this is a missing‐part
situation. This notion of missing addend is a precursor for children’s work with variables
261
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
and algebraic expressions and equations. You can begin to use variables instead of leaving a
blank space: 5 + a = 13.
When writing equations for story problems, different equations may occur. For ex-
ample, consider this story problem:
If Gabbie has 12 cards and Karl has 5 cards, write an equation for how many more cards
Gabbie has than Karl.
Notice the instructions did not ask children to solve the problem but rather to write an equa-
tion. Some children might write 12 - 5 = while others may write 5 + = 12. The latter
Standards for equation can be interpreted as “Karl’s 5 cards plus some more cards are the same as Gabbie’s
Mathematical Practice 12 cards.” Facilitate a discussion with children to help them understand that both equations
show the same relationships and either is correct. Although either is correct, it is important
4 Model with ◀ that children can write and justify both equations, connecting the equations to the story.
mathematics
Within a context, children can even explore three variables, each one standing for an
unknown value as in the following activity (based on Maida, 2004).
Children can figure out the cost of three toys, given the following three facts:
1. + = $3
Ask children to look at each fact and make observations
that can help them figure out the cost of each toy. For
2. + = $4 example, they may notice that the soccer ball costs $1
more than the teddy bear. Help children write this
3. + = $5 observation as the other statements. Continue until
these discoveries lead to finding the cost of each toy.
Encourage children to use manipulatives to represent
and explore the problem.
NUMBER TASK:
Sandra has 8 pennies. George has 4 more pennies than Sandra. How many pennies does
George have?
ALGEBRA TASK:
Sandra has some pennies. George has 4 more pennies than Sandra. How many pennies
does George have?
262
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
263
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
Standards for
Table 1 Properties of the Operations for Pre-K–2
Mathematical Practice
Symbolic How Children Might Describe
7 Look for and Name of Property Representation the Pattern or Structure
◀
make use of structure
Commutative a+b=b+a “When you add two numbers in any order,
you’ll get the same answer.”
Additive Identity a+0=0+a=a “When you add zero to any number, you get
the same number you started with.”
Inverse Relationship of If a + b = c then c - b = a “When you have a subtraction problem you can
Addition and Subtraction and c - a = b ‘think addition’ by using the inverse.”
*The additive inverse property is usually written as a + (-a) = 0, but the symbolic representation a - a = 0
will make more sense to elementary children.
264
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
Tell a story about two twin sisters (or brothers). The twins always shared whatever they
had equally. If they found some pretty seashells, they would count them out and share
them so that each had the same number of shells. When Mom gave them cookies for
lunch, they would be sure that each got the same number of cookies. Sometimes they
were not able to share things fairly because there would be something left over. This
happened once when Dad gave them five marbles. (Discuss why they could not share five
marbles equally.) Whenever there was a leftover, the twins put the extra item in a special
box of “leftovers” that they decided to keep for their baby brother.
After discussing the way that the twins shared things, ask children to find out what
numbers of things they could share fairly and what numbers would have a leftover when
they shared them. Assign three or four numbers from 6 to 40 to pairs of children. Their
task is to decide which of their numbers could be shared fairly and which would have a
leftover. Provide linking cubes to help them in their investigation. Collect the information
and make lists of numbers that can be shared and those that will have a leftover. Examine
the lists as a class and ask for observations. Select numbers not in the list and ask if chil-
dren can tell which list they belong in and why they think so.
The critical portion of the last activity is the discussion. Notice how the children used
the linking cubes in their investigation. Some children counted out the number of cubes
and then distributed the cubes one by one to each twin. They either kept track of the fair
265
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
Figure 6 sharing (e.g., one for this twin, one for that twin; two for this twin, two for that twin,
and so on) or they had to count at the end to determine if each twin received the same
Sharing cubes fairly with
two people. amount. Others may have organized their cubes by pairing them together to make sure
that each twin received the same amount (see Figure 6). Have children share how they
Six cubes Nine cubes used their linking cubes and ask which ways (or to identify a way to) help them quickly
shared with shared with see that the twins received the same amount. Based on this activity, an even number is an
two people two people
amount that can be shared fairly or split into two equal parts with no leftovers. An odd number is
one that is not even or cannot be split into two equal parts. Challenge the children to use their
own words to describe the numbers. The number endings of 0, 2, 4, 6, and 8 are only an
interesting and useful pattern or observation and should not be used as the definition of
an even number.
The next activity focuses on the same concept of even and odd but this time children
Extra start with visual representations of even and odd numbers.
Create sets of “two‐column cards” on card stock (see Figure 7) for groups of three to four
BLM
children (see Blackline Master 34). In their groups, children are to see how many things
they can find to tell about the pieces. (For example: There is a piece for each number 1 to
10. Some are like rectangles. Some have a square sticking out.) For those who might need
a start, suggest that they put the pieces in order from one square to ten. After having
some time to explore and share ideas they have noticed, have children sort their pieces
into two sets. It is very likely that some group will sort the pieces as shown in Figure 7. (If
no one sorts the pieces this way, do so on a projector and ask what rule you are using to
sort them.) Use children’s language to describe the pieces representing the odd numbers.
For example, someone may describe the seven piece as having a “bump.”
Next, assign the groups of children three or four numbers between 11 and 50 and
have them decide whether two‐column cards for these numbers would have bumps or
no bumps (or whatever language children used). Have linking cubes available for their
investigation. Have them use words and pictures to explain their conclusions.
266
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
Repeating Patterns
The concept of a repeating pattern and how a pattern is extended or continued can be intro-
duced to the full class in several ways. One possibility is to draw simple shape patterns on the
board and extend them in a class discussion. Oral patterns can be recited. For example, “do,
mi, mi, do, mi, mi, . . .” is a simple singing pattern. Body movements such as moving the arm
up, down, and sideways can be used to make patterns: up, side, side, down, up, side, side, down.
There are several websites that offer children opportunities to explore repeating patterns. For example, PBS
Kids offers an interactive site for young children (www.pbs.org/teachers/connect/resources/7661/preview).
NCTM’s Illuminations Activity called “Patch Tool” (http://illuminations.nctm.org/ActivityDetail.aspx?ID=27)
allows children to flip and rotate shapes to create various patterns, which informally introduces them
to geometric transformations. The National Library of Virtual Manipulatives also has several applets that
support the exploration of repeated (and growing) patterns, including Attribute Trains, Color Patterns, and
Pattern Blocks (http://nlvm.usu.edu/en/nav/grade_g_1.html).
267
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
An important concept in working with repeating patterns is for children to identify the
core of the pattern (Warren & Cooper, 2008). The core of a repeating pattern is the shortest
string of elements that repeats. Notice in Figure 8 that the core is always fully repeated and
never only partially shown.
Children can explore patterns with all sorts of materials, as shown in the following
activity.
Using the board or a projector, show six or seven patterns with different materials or
pictures. Half of the class closes their eyes while the other half uses the A, B, C method to
read a pattern that you point to. After hearing the pattern, the children who had their
eyes closed examine the patterns and try to decide which pattern was read. If two of the
patterns in the list have the same structure, the discussion can be very interesting.
Standards for A significant step forward mathematically is to see that two patterns constructed with
Mathematical Practice
different materials are actually the same pattern. For example, the third pattern in Figure 8
7 Look for and and the third pattern in Figure 9 can both be “read” A‐A‐B‐B‐A‐A‐B‐B, and the pattern
◀ above those in both figures is A‐B‐C‐D‐A‐B‐C‐D. Translating two or more essentially alike
make use of structure
patterns to a common format helps children to move beyond the materials making up the
pattern to see the fundamental mathematical structure involved. Using some form of sym-
bolism (in this case, the alphabet) to represent the structure of a pattern helps children
generalize the pattern.
268
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
269
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
270
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
Number Patterns With a large empty box, you can have a volunteer inside a
Our number system is also full of wonderful patterns. Num- function machine generating the output. This can become
bers not only offer children an opportunity to explore pat- a fun daily routine, switching who is the function machine
terns but also to learn to expect, see, and use patterns in all operator.
of mathematics.
The simplest form of a number pattern is a string of numbers that follows some rule for
determining how the string continues. Consider the following activity, which includes some
repeating patterns and some growing patterns as well as some other kinds of relationships.
Skip counts by 2, 5, and 10, already a part of most K–2 curricula, can be an excellent
source of patterns and can help children reason about number relationships (e.g., comparing
skip counting by twos and by threes leads to revealing that both counts include 6, 12, 18, . . .).
Show children five or six numbers from a number pattern. The task is to extend the
pattern for several more numbers and to explain the rule for generating the pat-
tern. The difficulty of the task depends on the number pattern and how familiar
the children are with searching for patterns. Here are some recommended patterns
to try.
Most of the preceding examples also have variations you can try. Challenge
children to make up their own number pattern rules.
271
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
The calculator can skip‐count by any amount beginning anywhere. For example, to
count by threes, enter 0 + 3 = . ( The 0 isn’t necessary but is a good idea with young
children.) Successive presses of the = key will count on from 3 by threes. To skip count
by threes from a different number, say, 16, simply press 16 + 3 and continue pressing the
= key. What is happening is that the calculator “remembers” the last operation, in this case
“ + 3,” and adds that to whatever is currently in the window whenever the = key is pressed.
The = will continue to have this effect until an operation key is pressed.
If you have not done so previously, teach children how to make their calculators count
by twos. When the children are able to do this without pressing a new operation key—
which is what they have a tendency to do—show them how to change the beginning
number. For example, press 5 + 2 = , = , = , . . . . They can return to beginning the count
at 5 (or any other number) by pressing 5 and continuing to press = .
Now challenge children working in pairs to say the next number before they press
the = key. Have them hold their finger over the = button and say the next skip count,
press the = to confirm or correct, and then continue, always trying to say the next count
before pressing the key.
As children skip count on the calculator, discuss the patterns that they see. For example,
when counting by twos starting with 0, they see numbers ending in 0, 2, 4, 6, and 8, and
then that pattern repeats. Note that this same sequence appears if you begin with any even
number, such as 34 (although the sequence begins with 4 instead of 0). If counting by twos
beginning with 1, the numbers are odd: 1, 3, 5, 7, and 9.
When children have become comfortable skip counting by small numbers, suggest that
they try a “big” number. Suggest they count by 20 or 50. These large numbers will seem
very difficult to children, who will then be surprised to recognize familiar patterns in the
tens and hundreds places. Continue as appropriate with challenges to count by 25 or by 30
Standards for or 40. Other good skip‐count numbers are 9, 11, 12, 15, or 21.
Mathematical Practice
Focusing on patterns in skip counting will support children’s use of invented strategies
1 Make sense for multiplication. In fact, the key to exploring patterns in the elementary curriculum is not
of problems and ◀ to identify patterns for patterns’ sake but to strengthen children’s understanding of number
persevere in
relationships and properties. The more often you can ask children, “Did you notice a pat-
solving them
tern?” the more they are considering and making sense of the mathematics they are doing.
272
E x p a n d e d L e s s o n
Content and Task Decisions a balance, use a two‐sided mat with an equal sign in
the middle to have the children demonstrate making
Grade Level: K–1
equations that show that what is on the left side of the
mat is the same as (or equal to) what is on the right
Mathematics Goals
side of the mat.
• To discover and explore number patterns within the
context of addition
Materials
• To understand how complementary changes in two ad-
Each child will need:
dends leave the sum unchanged
• Counters
• Calculators (optional)
Grade Level Guide
NCTM Common
Curriculum Focal Points Core State Standards
Lesson
In a focal point connection In the domain of Operations Before
under “Algebra” at both and Algebraic Thinking, Present the focus task to the class:
kindergarten and grade 1, kindergartners add by using
• Explain this problem: “The other day, a friend of mine
children work with number a variety of ways to represent
was thinking about adding 7 + 7.” Write 7 + 7 on the
patterns to explore the problems.
board. “She wondered what would happen if she made
relationships that will
In first grade, children the first 7 one more and the second 7 one less.” With
eventually result in the
add using the relationship the children’s help, write this new sum, 8 + 6, on the
creation of generalizable
between addition and board. Have the children complete each equation.
rules.
subtraction and are expected • Ask, “Why do you think the answers are the same?”
to understand the meaning Have the children offer their ideas. Ask, “Do you think
of the equal sign. this same plan will work if we started with 5 + 5 or
8 + 8?” Discuss this briefly.
Consider the Children’s Needs • To differentiate the task, some children could use coun-
Children need not know their addition combinations to en- ters to try to figure out why 7 + 7 and 8 + 6 have the
gage in this lesson. However, they should have been exposed same answers. Then try another sum, such as 5 + 5, and
to the plus and equal signs and understand how an addition see if it works for that sum also.
equation is a representation of two parts of a whole. • For more advanced children, the task is to use counters
and try to figure out why 7 + 7 and 8 + 6 have the same
For English Language Learners
answers. Will it work for any double (e.g., 5 + 5, 6 + 6,
• Be sure that the children know the words “up” and 8 + 8, and so on)? What if the two numbers you begin
“down,” which can be acted out by asking children to with are not the same?
stand up and sit down (emphasizing the words up and
• The children should use pictures, numbers, and words
down).
to show their thinking. If others pick up the papers,
• As needed, provide translations for the numbers. they should be able to understand the children’s
ideas.
For Children with Disabilities
• Use a mathematics balance (a plastic “number line” on Provide clear expectations:
a fulcrum). This can be used to dramatically model the • You may want to have children work in pairs. Work
movement of one hanging weight and the effect it has could be shown on large chart paper to be shared with
on the balance of the equation. If you do not have such the class.
273
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
274
Promoting Algebraic Reasoning
References
Ball, D. L., & Bass, H. (2003). Making mathematics Mann, R. L. (2004). Balancing act: The truth
reasonable in school. In J. Kilpatrick, W. G. Martin, behind the equals sign. Teaching Children
& D. Schifter (Eds.), A research companion to Mathematics, 11(2), 65–69.
Principles and Standards for School Mathematics (pp.
McNamara, J. C. (2010). Two of everything.
27–44). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of
Teaching Children Mathematics, 17(3), 132–136.
Mathematics.
McNeil, N. M., & Alibali, M. W. (2005).
Blanton, M. L. (2008). Algebra in the elementary
Knowledge change as a function of mathematics
classroom: Transforming thinking, transforming practice.
experience: All contexts are not created equal.
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Journal of Cognition and Development, 6, 285–306.
Carpenter, T. P., Franke, M. L., & Levi, L. (2003).
McNeil, N. M., Grandau, L., Knuth, E. J., Alibali,
Thinking mathematically: Integrating arithmetic
M. W., Stephens, A. C., Hattikudur, S., & Krill, D.
and algebra in elementary school. Portsmouth, NH:
E. (2006). Middle school students’ understanding
Heinemann.
of the equal sign: The books they read can’t help.
Carraher, D. W., & Schliemann, A. D. (2007). Cognition & Instruction, 24(3), 367–385.
Early algebra and algebraic reasoning. In F. Lester
Molina, M., & Ambrose, R. C. (2006). Fostering
(Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics
relational thinking while negotiating the meaning
teaching and learning: A project of the National
of the equals sign. Teaching Children Mathematics,
Council of Teachers of Mathematics (Vol. II, pp. 609–
13(2), 111–117.
705). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Falkner, K. P., Levi, L., & Carpenter, T. P. (1999).
(2006). Curriculum focal points for prekindergarten
Children’s understanding of equality: A foundation
through grade 8 mathematics: A quest for coherence.
for algebra. Teaching Children Mathematics, 6(4),
Reston, VA: Author.
232–236.
Numeroff, L. (1985). If you give a mouse a cookie.
Fosnot, C. (2007). The sleepover. Portsmouth, NH:
New York, NY: Harper Collins.
Heinemann.
RAND Mathematics Study Panel. (2003).
Fosnot, C. T., & Jacob, B. (2007). Young
Mathematical proficiency for all students: Toward
mathematicians at work: Constructing algebra.
a strategic research and development program in
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
mathematics education (Issue 1643). Santa Monica,
Fosnot, C. T., & Jacob, B. (2010). Young CA: Rand Corporation.
mathematicians at work: Constructing algebra.
Schifter, D. (1999). Reasoning about operations:
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers
Early algebraic thinking, grades K through 6. In
of Mathematics.
L. Stiff & F. Curcio (Eds.), Developing mathematical
Hong, L. (1993). Two of everything. Morton Grove, reasoning in grades K–12 (pp. 62–81). Reston, VA:
IL: Albert Whitman & Company. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Kaput, J. J. (2008). What is algebra? What is Schifter, D., Monk, G. S., Russell, S. J., & Bastable,
algebraic reasoning? In J. J. Kaput, D. W. Carraher, V. (2007). Early algebra: What does understanding
& M. L. Blanton (Eds.), Algebra in the early grades. the laws of arithmetic mean in the elementary
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of grades? In J. Kaput, D. Carraher, & M. Blanton
Mathematics. (Eds.), Algebra in the early grades. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Knuth, E. J., Stephens, A. C., McNeil, N. M.,
& Alibali, M. W. (2006). Does understanding Warren, E., & Cooper, T. J. (2008). Patterns that
the equal sign matter? Evidence from solving support early algebraic thinking in elementary
equations. Journal for Research in Mathematics school. In C. E. Greenes & R. Rubenstein (Eds.),
Education, 37(4), 297–312. Algebra and algebraic thinking in school mathematics:
70th NCTM yearbook (pp. 113–126). Reston, VA:
Maida, P. (2004). Using algebra without realizing
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
it. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 9(9),
484–488.
275
This page intentionally left blank
Exploring Early Fraction
Concepts
From Chapter 14 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
277
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
Part–Whole
One of the most commonly used meanings of fractions is the part–whole relationship. In
early childhood classrooms, part–whole fractions are typically represented by shading part
of a whole that has been partitioned into equal parts ( 34 of a rug). Part–whole situations can
also be described as part of a group of children ( 13 of the class brought their lunch) or as part
of a length (we walked 112 miles). The first example (shading part of a whole to indicate area)
and the third example (walked 112 miles) use continuous quantities, that is, quantities that
are measured and that can be cut into as many equal parts as we wish. The second example
( 13 of the class) uses discrete quantities, that is, quantities that represent objects that cannot
be divided further (e.g., a person, a chair, a car).
Although the part–whole meaning of fractions is important, too often instruction based
only on the part–whole relationship leaves children with little sense that fractions are num-
bers. Also fractional amounts arise naturally from partitioning amounts in everyday life. For
these reasons, initial instruction should focus on the next meaning of fractions: equal sharing.
Equal Sharing
Equal sharing is an idea that young children understand intuitively because of their ex-
Standards for periences sharing things with brothers, sisters, friends, and so on. Consider the idea of four
Mathematical Practice
children fairly sharing two sticks of clay so they can make clay animals. Fractions emerge
2 Reason abstractly naturally from this scenario. Because of the meaningful connections that can be made, early
◀
and quantitatively fraction instruction should build from young children’s experiences of sharing and partition-
ing (Empson & Levi, 2011; Lamon, 2012; Siegler et al., 2010).
Equal sharing situations can use continuous or discrete quantities. The example with
two sticks of clay is an example of a continuous quantity because theoretically we could cut
the stick into as many equal pieces as we want. For equal sharing situations that use discrete
quantities, it is best with young children to use story problems that have whole-number
solutions with no remainders. Consider these story problems:
• There are 12 chairs and 4 tables in a classroom. If we want the same number of chairs
around each table, how many chairs will be placed around a table?
• There are 14 chairs and 4 tables in a classroom. If we want the same number of chairs
around each table, how many chairs will be placed around a table?
The solution for both problems (how many chairs will be placed around a table) is a whole
number (3). But in the second problem there are 2 chairs that cannot be placed and, because
chairs are discrete objects, they cannot be split or cut into smaller parts that can be shared. If
your goal is to have children think about fractional parts, you want them to be able to share
fairly all the objects in the situation. So it is important to use continuous quantities when
there are remainders so those can be cut into as many equal parts as needed.
278
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
Measurement
Although the Common Core State Standards recommend limiting linear measurement
in grades K–2 to whole units (e.g., inches, feet, centimeters, meters), fractions often
emerge naturally in many measurement situations. Linear measurement involves iden-
tifying a unit of length and then using that unit to determine the length of an object.
For example, using the unit of an inch, a child could use multiple copies of an inch to
determine that a pencil is 5 inches long. Similarly for fractions, a child can use a fraction
strip that represents the unit fraction 13 to count or measure to show that it takes four
units to reach 43 .
Measurement also includes time. The most obvious place to relate fractions and time is
when telling time to the half hour and quarter hour. This would involve identifying the unit
of time as an hour. Making the hour unit explicit is key. Emphasize how the minute hand
sweeps a full turn around an analog clock face to measure an hour. Halfway measures half
that time or half of an hour. Half of that measures a quarter of the time by splitting the hour
into four equal quantities of time. When second graders progress to telling time in smaller
increments of 5-minute intervals, the connection between time and fractions becomes less
obvious.
Measurement situations by their very nature consist of measuring a quantity that
we could cut into as many equal-sized pieces as we need and so involve continuous
quantities.
Partitioning
Partitioning can be thought of as splitting or cutting a quantity equally. Young children are
engaged in the act of partitioning from an early age when they split a group of 6 into 3 and 3.
Connecting this informal knowledge to fair sharing and then to more formal fraction con-
cepts is key to providing effective initial fraction instruction in grades pre-K–2. You can do
this by posing story problems that involve equal sharing. In fact, using story problems is the
primary method for facilitating children’s fraction learning at the pre-K–2 level. Consider
the following example that uses a continuous quantity (candy bars) that can be cut into as
many equal-sized pieces as we wish.
Six children want to share 5 candy bars fairly. How much will each child get?
When children answer questions such as “how much will each child get?” the idea that frac-
tions are numbers will be reinforced.
279
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
Iterating
In whole-number learning, counting precedes learning to add and subtract. This is also true
of fractions. Children should come to think of counting fractional parts in much the same
way they might count apples or any other objects. Counting fractional parts to see how
multiple parts compare to the whole helps children to understand the relationship between
the parts of the whole. Children should be able to answer the question, “How many fourths
are in one whole?” just as they know how many ones are in ten. This counting a repeated
amount (e.g., unit fraction) is called iterating. Understanding that 34 can be thought of as a
count of three parts called fourths is an important idea for children to develop (Post, Wachs-
muth, Lesh, & Behr, 1985; Siebert & Gaskin, 2006; Tzur, 1999).
Area Models
Circular fraction pieces are by far the most commonly used area model. One of the ad-
vantages of the circular model is that it emphasizes the part–whole concept of fractions
and, in particular, the relative size of a part to the whole (Cramer, Wyberg, and Leavitt,
2008). What is being compared is the area of the part to the area of the whole. Because
we can cut area into as many equal-sized pieces as we want and because area is measured,
it falls into the category of a continuous quantity. Note that when drawing circles, chil-
dren (and adults) can find it difficult to partition the circle into reasonably equal-sized
parts. There are many other area models including pattern blocks, geoboards, color tiles,
and fraction bars. Regions, such as rectangles, can also be drawn on blank or grid paper.
The other physical models in Figure 1 demonstrate how different shapes can represent
the whole.
Length Models
With length models, lengths or linear measurements are compared instead of areas. In this
model, a unit of length is compared to the whole length. We can cut a length into as many
equal-sized pieces (units) as we want; therefore, length models represent continuous quanti-
ties. Length models appropriate for pre-K–2 include fraction strips, paper strips (e.g., adding-
machine tape), Cuisenaire rods, and line segments (see Figure 2). All of these models provide
flexibility because any length can represent the whole.
Researchers have identified number line models as useful in fraction instruction be-
cause they emphasize the idea that a fraction represents a quantity (Siegler et al., 2010).
Locating fractions on a number line also highlights their relationship to other numbers, in-
cluding other fractions. The number line has been shown as extremely effective with young
children when working with whole numbers (e.g., Booth & Siegler, 2008; Fosnot & Dolk,
2001; Siegler & Ramani, 2009). Using this model with whole numbers can help prepare
280
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
Rectangular regions
Any piece can be
selected as the whole.
One-half or
One-third two-fourths
these children to be ready to use this model with fractions in later grades. Using a modified
version, line segments, for fraction instruction in grades pre-K–2 is ideal.
Set Models
The whole in a set model is understood to be a set of objects, and subsets of the whole make
up fractional parts. For example, 3 red counters are one-fourth of a set of 12 counters. The
set of 12, in this example, represents the whole or 1. The idea of referring to a collection of
counters as a single entity can make set models difficult for young children. To help children
see the set of objects as a whole, put a loop of yarn around the set. Another challenge with
set models is that children may focus on the size of the subset rather than the number of
equal-sized subsets in the whole. For example, if 12 counters make a whole, then a subset of 4
counters is one-third, not one-fourth, because 3 equal-sized subsets make the whole. Discrete
objects, like two-color counters, are frequently used for set models. They can easily be flipped
to change their color to model various fractional parts of a whole set (see Figure 3).
281
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
2
Two-color counters show 3 . Two-color counters in a loop
1
of yarn show 4.
Note that in the Common Core State Standard for Mathematics, halves and fourths are
developed in first grade prior to thirds in second grade. This is done because successive
halving of parts is a natural process for young children. Once children have been successful
dealing with and explaining halves and fourths, pose sharing tasks that involve eighths. They
will likely rely on their halving strategy to find a solution. Likewise, once children have
Standards for demonstrated an understanding of thirds, pose sharing tasks that involve sixths.
Mathematical Practice In addition to helping children use the words halves, thirds, fourths, and quarters, be sure
to make regular comparisons of fractional parts to the whole. Make it a point to use the term
6 Attend to
precision
◀ whole, one whole, or simply one so that children have a language that they can use regardless
of the model involved.
than having the structure imposed on them. In this approach children do not begin with Standards for
Mathematical Practice
traditional part–whole tasks of identifying halves, thirds, fourths, and so on. These fractional
parts and children’s understanding of part–whole fractions emerge as a result of their fair 1 Make sense
sharing in meaningful contexts. ◀ of problems and
Sharing tasks are generally posed in the form of a story problem involving a given num- persevere in
solving them
ber of objects that are to be shared equally among a given number of people. The problems
and variations that follow are adapted from Empson (2002).
Four children are sharing 10 brownies so that each one will get the same amount. How
much will each child get?
For 10 brownies and 4 sharers, many children will deal out 2 brownies to each child and
then halve each of the remaining brownies (see Figure 4).
These kinds of story problems include the following features:
• The objects in the story are easy to draw and partition (e.g., apples, bagels, cookies,
brownies, quesadillas, pancakes, sticks of gum, sandwiches, pizzas, crackers, cake, candy
bars, sticks of modeling clay, pretzel sticks, and so on).
Standards for
• The story problem does not contain fractions. The fractions emerge as a result of the Mathematical Practice
relationship between the number of objects to be shared and the number of people
2 Reason abstractly
sharing. This allows children to make connections between their whole-number knowl- ◀
and quantitatively
edge and their developing fraction knowledge.
• Children can solve these kinds of problems without formal instruction and without
knowing anything about fraction symbols. They can represent how much one person
gets by shading in one share or showing one share with manipulatives.
You can differentiate these tasks by changing the numbers involved. Consider these
variations in numbers:
• 5 brownies shared with 2 children
• 2 brownies shared with 4 children
• 5 brownies shared with 4 children
• 4 brownies shared with 8 children
• 3 brownies shared with 4 children
Figure 4 Ten brownies shared with four children. Each child gets 212 brownies.
283
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
When the numbers allow for some items to be distributed whole (five shared with two),
some children will first share whole items and then cut up the leftovers. Others will slice
every piece in half and then distribute the halves. When there are more sharers than items,
some partitioning must happen at the beginning of the solution process. When children
who are still using a halving strategy try to share five things among four children, they will
eventually get down to two halves to give to four children. For some, the solution is to cut
each half in half; that is, “each child gets a whole (or two halves) and a half of a half.”
(a) Four candy bars shared with six children: Partitioning into 3 and 6 equal parts is challenging for
children because they naturally want to use a halving
1 4
1 2 3 4 5 6 2 5 strategy that does not work in these cases. Figure 5
3 6 shows some different sharing solutions that might be
observed.
Cut all the bars in half. Considerable research has been done with chil-
Cut the last two halves into three parts.
Each child gets a half and sixth. dren from first through eighth grades to determine
how they go about the process of forming fair shares
(b) Four pizzas shared with three children: and how the tasks posed to children influence their
responses (e.g., Empson & Levi, 2011; Lamon, 2012;
A B Mack, 2001; Pothier & Sawada, 1990; Siegler et al.,
2010). Researchers recommend following a progression
of sharing tasks that builds on children’s understanding
A B C C of whole-number quantities and informal partitioning
Pass out whole pizzas. strategies (e.g., Empson & Levi, 2011; Siegler et al.,
Cut the last pizza in three parts. 2010). Table 1 captures the following progression and
Each child gets 1 whole and one-third. provides examples of corresponding story problems.
284
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
1. Problems whose solutions are whole Three children want to fairly share 15 grapes. How
numbers. many grapes will each child get?
2. Problems with 2, 4, or 8 sharers and Two children want to share 5 quesadillas so that
whose solutions are mixed numbers (greater everyone gets the same amount. How much will each
than 1). child get?
3. Problems with 2, 4, or 8 sharers and whose Four children want to share 3 cookies so that
solutions are less than 1. everyone gets the same amount. How much will each
child get?
4. Problems with 3 sharers and whose solutions Three children want to share 10 sticks of clay to make
are mixed numbers (greater than 1). clay animals. If everyone gets the same amount, how
much does each child get?
5. Problems with 3 sharers and whose solutions Three children want to equally share 2 pizzas. How
are less than 1. much does each child get?
are discrete objects. In the Before and the After portions of the lessons, emphasize
that all of the objects must be shared and shared equally. This is an important idea
to stress because once the scenarios require partitioning quantities into smaller
pieces, some children will either not share all of the objects or not share them
equally.
2. Use equal sharing problems with 2, 4, or 8 sharers and whose solutions are mixed numbers.
Because children’s initial strategies for sharing single objects involve halving,
begin with 2, 4, and then 8 sharers. Using problems that have solutions that are
larger than 1 enable children to relate their understanding of whole numbers to
fractions and they actually find them easier to solve than problems whose solutions
are less than 1.
3. Use equal sharing problems with 2, 4, or 8 sharers and whose solutions are less than
1. Again, problems with 2, 4, or 8 sharers capitalize on children’s halving strategies.
Problems with solutions less than 1 tend to be more difficult for young
children.
4. Next, use equal sharing problems with 3 sharers and whose solutions are mixed numbers.
For 3 sharers, children have to anticipate how to slice or cut the objects. Many
children will attempt to use a repeated halving strategy or trial and error. Moving
to 3 (and then 6 sharers) will force children to confront their tendencies to use
halving strategies.
5. Use equal sharing problems with 3 sharers and whose solutions are less than 1. Again,
children have to anticipate how to partition the objects. Standards for
Mathematical Practice
When using story problems to help children construct mathematical ideas, having
children share their solutions and explain their reasoning to each other is key. Make sure to 3 Construct
look for significant ideas in children’s work while they are solving the story problems that ◀ viable arguments
and critique the
you can then highlight in the discussion (After) portion of the lesson. reasoning of others
285
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
As in Figure 6, show examples and nonexamples of fair shares. (Note that exam-
Standards for
Mathematical Practice ples and nonexamples are very important to use with children with disabilities.)
Have children identify the amounts that are equally partitioned and those that are not.
3 Construct
◀ For each response, have children explain their reasoning.
viable arguments The activity should be done with a variety of models, including length and set models.
and critique the
reasoning of others The Common Core State Standards set as an expectation for second graders that they will
recognize that “equal-sized parts” does not necessarily mean the same shape when using
an area model. So if you teach second grade, make sure to include examples that illustrate
this concept. Many children, in particular children with disabilities, will need to cut and move
pieces around to check to make sure the parts are indeed equal in size.
In the “Fair and Unfair Shares” activity, the wholes are already partitioned either cor-
rectly or incorrectly; the children are not involved in the partitioning. So the most impor-
tant part of this activity is the discussion. Use the discussion to reinforce the idea that all the
pieces need to be shared and shared equally.
286
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
In the following activity, children create designated equal shares Figure 6 Children learning about
using physical models as opposed to drawing their own representations. fractions through partitioning need to
You may want to start with an activity like this before you move to asking recognize when shares are not equal.
children to draw and partition their own representations.
e Lerane
rE
one whole and have them find thirds or fourths or other frac-
fo rs
Activities such as Activity 2 are especially interesting when different wholes can be designated
in the same physical model. That way, a given fractional part does not get identified with a
special shape or color but instead with the relationship of the part to the designated whole.
Fourths
One Blue
Thirds
Sixths
Fourths Cuisenaire rods
Fourths
287
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
Wow! Ten-fourths:
How many Activity 4 MORE, LESS, OR
quesadillas is that?
EQUAL TO ONE WHOLE
10 fourths
Give children a collection of fractional parts (all
the same size pieces) and indicate the kind of frac-
Is ten-thirds the same
as ten-fourths? Is it as tional part they have. (Use either area or length
much as five-fourths? models. Set models are too difficult for young chil-
dren to use for iteration.) Parts can be drawn on
10 thirds
a worksheet or physical models can be placed in
288
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
plastic bags with an identifying card. For example, if done with Cuisenaire rods or fraction
strips, the collection might have seven light green rods/strips with a caption or note indicat-
ing “each piece is an eighth.” The task is to decide whether the collection is less than one
whole, equal to one whole, or more than one whole. Ask children to draw pictures and/
or use fraction words to explain their answer. Adding a context, such as people sharing
candy bars, pizzas, or sticks of clay, can help children understand and reason through the
problem.
• Marley, Zack, Rita, and Hannah want to share 5 pretzel sticks equally. How much will each
of them get? (length)
• Marley and Zack want to share 11 grapes equally. How much will each of them get? (set)
• Marley, Zack, and Mia want to share 7 pieces of poster board equally. How much will
each of them get? (area)
The objects to be shared can be drawn on paper as rectangles, circles, or line segments
along with a statement of the problem. Some children may need to cut and physically dis-
tribute the pieces, so another possibility is to cut out construction paper circles or rectangles
to represent the objects to be shared. You could use thin rectangles to represent objects of
length. Children can use connecting cubes to make bars that they can separate into pieces.
Or they can use more traditional fraction models such as circular “pie” pieces (area), Cuise-
naire rods (length), or counters of different colors (sets).
As you create story problems and tasks for the children, remember the big ideas you
are trying to help them construct: (1) Fractions are numbers and (2) fractional parts are
equal shares or equal-sized portions of a whole or unit.
Solution 3:
Cut all the pancakes into fourths.
Activity 5 CUT THEM UP
Give each child one-fourth of each AGAIN!
pancake.
Each child gets six-fourths pancakes.
Pose a sharing story problem for which children
could partition the objects in multiple ways. For
example, “4 children share 10 brownies so that
each one will get the same amount. How much will each child get?” Tell children that their task
is to find at least two different ways to share the objects. Some children will use trial and error
while others will be more systematic in finding different ways to partition the objects. Have
children share the different ways they have found to partition the objects. Then challenge them
to determine if the different ways result in each child getting a different or equal amount of
brownie. Have children explain their reasoning.
The next activity uses the idea of a missing value to help children find equivalent frac-
tional amounts.
Pose a problem such as the following: “Suppose 6 children shared 8 cookies equally. If 3
more children arrive, how many cookies would you need if you want to give them the same
amount as the first 6 children?” Some children will share 8 cookies equally among the 6
children to determine how much each person will get in that situation and then use that
information to determine how much is needed for the additional 3 children. Others may rea-
son that because 3 is half of 6, they can halve the 8 cookies to see that 4 cookies are needed
for the additional 3 children. Make sure to have children share their reasoning for keeping
it fair!
Using a length model helps to emphasize that a fraction is a quantity and it also allows
children to compare fractions in terms of relative size. The following activity (adapted from
Bay-Williams & Martinie, 2003) is a fun way to use a real-world context to engage children
in thinking about fractions through a linear model.
290
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
e LLeerraannee
e
nglis
of the distance each child has already moved. Who is winning? Challenge children
r
rsrs fo
fo
to place the friends on a line to show where they are between the start and finish.
The game “Red Light–Green Light” may not be familiar to all children, especially ELLs.
Modeling the game with children in the class is a good way to build background and also
support children with disabilities.
291
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
Figure 10 Counting fractional parts. Here are some reasonable explanations for the numerator and
denominator.
• Numerator. This is the counting number. It tells how many
More than 1
or less than 1? shares or parts we have. It tells how many have been counted.
5 fourths
It tells how many parts we are talking about. It counts the
How much more to parts or shares.
3 fourths get to a whole? • Denominator. This tells what is being counted. It tells what
fractional part is being counted. If the denominator is a 4, it
10 fourths means we are counting fourths; if it is a 6, we are counting
sixths; and so on.
This formulation of the meanings of the numerator and de-
Wow! Ten-fourths: nominator may seem unusual to you. It is often said that the nu-
Did we count as far
as 2? Did we get to 3?
merator tells “how many” and the denominator tells “how many
parts it takes to make a whole.” This may be correct but can also be
misleading. For example, a 16 piece is often cut from a cake without
10 eighths making any slices in the remaining 56 of the cake. That the cake is
only in two pieces does not change the fact that the piece taken
That’s a lot of is 16 . Or if a pizza is cut in 12 pieces, two pieces still make 16 of the
eighths. Is ten-eighths pizza. In neither of these instances does the denominator tell how
as much as ten-fourths?
many pieces make a whole.
Is it as much as
five-fourths? The iterative concept described earlier is most clear when fo-
cusing on these two ideas about fraction symbols:
• The top number counts (numerator).
• The bottom number tells what is being counted (denominator).
The what of fractions are the fractional parts. They can be counted. Fraction symbols
are just a shorthand way of saying how many and what.
292
E x p a n d e d L e s s o n
293
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
294
Exploring Early Fraction Concepts
References
Bay‐Williams, J. M., & Martinie, S. L. (2003). through grade 8 mathematics: A quest for coherence.
Thinking rationally about number in the middle Reston, VA: Author.
school. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School,
Olive, J. (2002). Bridging the gap: Using interactive
8(6), 282–287.
computer tools to build fraction schemes. Teaching
Booth, J., & Siegler, R. (2008). Numerical Children Mathematics, 8(6), 356–61.
magnitude representations influence arithmetic
Post, T. R., Wachsmuth, I., Lesh, R. A., & Behr,
learning. Child Development, 79(4), 1016–1031.
M. J. (1985). Order and equivalence of rational
Cramer, K., & Henry, A. (2002). Using numbers: A cognitive analysis. Journal for Research
manipulative models to build number sense for in Mathematics Education, 16(1), 18–36.
addition of fractions. In B. Litwiller (Ed.), Making
sense of fractions, ratios, and proportions (pp. 41–48). Pothier, Y., & Sawada, D. (1990). Partitioning: An
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of approach to fractions. Arithmetic Teacher, 38(4),
Mathematics. 12–17.
Siebert, D., & Gaskin, N. (2006). Creating,
Empson, S. (2002). Organizing diversity in early
naming, and justifying fractions. Teaching Children
fraction thinking. In B. Litwiller (Ed.), Making
Mathematics, 12(8), 394–400.
sense of fractions, ratios, and proportions (pp. 29–40).
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Siegler, R. S., & Ramani, G. B. (2009). Playing
Mathematics. linear number board games—but not circular
ones—improves low‐income preschoolers’
Empson, S., & Levi, L. (2011). Extending children’s
numerical understanding. Journal of Educational
mathematics: Fractions and decimals. Portsmouth,
Psychology, 101, 545–560.
NH: Heinemann.
Siegler, R. S., Carpenter, T., Fennell, F., Geary, D.,
Fosnot, C. T., & Dolk, M. (2001). Young
Lewis, J., Okamoto, Y., Thompson, L., & Wray,
mathematicians at work: Constructing number
J. (2010). Developing effective fractions instruction
sense, addition, and subtraction. Portsmouth, NH:
for kindergarten through 8th grade: A practice guide
Heinemann.
(NCEE 2010‐4039). Retrieved from http://ies
Hodges, T. E., Cady, J., & Collins, R. L. (2008). .ed.gov/ncee/wwc/practiceguide.aspx?sid=15
Fraction representation: The not‐so‐common
Sowder, J. T., & Wearne, D. (2006). What do we
denominator among textbooks. Mathematics
know about eighth‐grade student achievement?
Teaching in the Middle School, 14(2), 78–84.
Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 11(6),
Hutchins, P. (1986). The doorbell rang. New York, 285–293.
NY: Greenwillow Books.
Tzur, R. (1999). An integrated study of children’s
Lamon, S. J. (2012). Teaching fractions and ratios construction of improper fractions and the
for understanding: Essential content knowledge and teacher’s role in promoting learning. Journal for
instructional strategies for teachers (3rd ed.). New Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 390–416.
York, NY: Routledge.
Wearne, D., & Kouba, V. L. (2000). Rational
Mack, N. K. (2001). Building on informal numbers. In E. A. Silver & P. A. Kenney (Eds.),
knowledge through instruction in a complex Results from the seventh mathematics assessment of the
content domain: Partitioning, units, and National Assessment of Educational Progress (pp. 163–
understanding multiplication of fractions. Journal 191). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of
for Research in Mathematics Education, 32, 267–295. Mathematics.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
(2006). Curriculum focal points for prekindergarten
295
This page intentionally left blank
Building Measurement
Concepts
From Chapter 15 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
297
Building Measurement Concepts
a unit to measure the length of an object. Second graders move to using standard measur-
ing tools, such as rulers, yardsticks, and meter sticks, and to using number lines to represent
whole numbers as lengths from zero. They also begin to explore the inverse relationship
between the size of the measuring unit and the measure (i.e., number of units used). First
graders begin to tell and write time in hours and half‐hours and second graders extend that
to 5‐minute intervals and the use of a.m. and p.m. Money is also included under the study of
measurement but does not occur until second grade.
Attribute: Weight
Units: objects that stretch the
spring in the scale
Attribute: Volume/Capacity
Units: cubes, balls, cups of water
Attribute: Length
Units: rods, toothpicks, straws, string
Attribute: Area
Units: index cards, squares of paper, tiles
298
Building Measurement Concepts
means that the attribute being measured is “filled” or “covered” or “matched” with a unit of
measure with the same attribute (as illustrated in Figure 1).
In summary, to measure something, one must perform three steps:
1. Decide on the attribute to be measured.
2. Select a unit that has that attribute.
3. Compare the units—by filling, covering, matching, or using some
other method—with the attribute of the object being measured. The
number of units required to match the object is the measure.
Measuring instruments such as rulers, meter sticks, scales, and clocks are devices that
make the filling, covering, or matching process easier. A ruler lines up the units of length
and numbers them. A clock lines up units of time and marks them off.
1—Making Children will understand the Make comparisons based on the attribute, for When it is clear that the
Comparisons attribute to be measured. example, longer/shorter, heavier/lighter. Use attribute is understood,
direct comparisons whenever possible. there is no further need for
comparison activities.
2—Using Children will understand how Use physical models of measuring units to fill, Begin with nonstandard
Models of filling, covering, matching, or cover, match, or make the desired comparison units. Progress to the direct
Measuring making other comparisons of an of the attribute with the unit. use of standard units when
Units attribute with measuring units appropriate and certainly
produces a number called a before using measuring
measure. tools.
3—Using Children will use common Make measuring instruments and use them in Without a careful
Measuring measuring tools with comparison with the actual unit models to see comparison with the
Instruments understanding and flexibility. how the measurement tool performs the same standard tools, much of the
function as the individual units. Be certain to value in student‐made tools
make direct comparisons between the student‐ can be lost.
made tools and the standard tools. Standard
measuring instruments, such as rulers, make the
filling, covering, or matching process easier.
299
Building Measurement Concepts
Making Comparisons
The first critical goal is for children to understand the attribute they are going to measure.
When children compare objects on the basis of some measurable attribute, that attribute
becomes the focus of the activity. For example, is the weight of one box more than, less than,
or about the same as the weight of another? No measurement is required, but some man-
ner of comparing one weight to the other must be devised.
The attribute of “weight” (heaviness) is inescapable.
Use and encourage precise language when helping children With a measure, such as length, you can sometimes
make comparisons. Avoid using the phrase “bigger than” and make a direct comparison by lining up one object against
instead use more specific language such as “longer than” or another. But often an indirect method using a third object
“is heavier.” is necessary. For example, if children wanted to compare
the height of a wastebasket to the distance around, they
Standards for would need to use something like a string to make that comparison. The string is the inter-
Mathematical Practice mediary, as it is impossible to directly compare these two lengths.
6 Attend to
◀
precision Using Physical Models of Measuring Units
The second goal is for children to understand how matching, filling, covering, or making
other comparisons of an attribute with measuring units produces a number called a measure.
For most attributes that are measured in elementary schools, it is possible to have physi-
cal models of units of measure. Time and temperature are exceptions. Units can be both
nonstandard (sometimes referred to as informal) units and standard units. For length, for
example, a drinking straw cut into an arbitrary length (nonstandard) and a 1‐foot‐long strip
of card stock (standard) might be used as units.
To help make the notion of units explicit and to aid children in understanding units, use as
many copies of the unit as are needed to fill or match the attribute measured (this can be thought
of as equal partitioning). The length of the room could be measured with giant footprints by
placing card stock copies of the footprint end to end, completely “covering” the length of the
room. Laying out copies of the same size unit and counting the units is called iteration (you may
recall that counting same‐size fractional parts is also called iteration). It is somewhat more dif-
ficult to use a single copy of a unit to complete this iteration. That means measuring a given
length (say, with a single footprint) by repeatedly moving it from position to position and keep-
ing track of where the last unit ended. Not only is this more difficult for younger children, but
it also obscures the meaning of the measurement—to see how many units will fill the length.
It is useful to measure the same object with units of different sizes to help children
understand that the size of the unit used is important. (This is an expectation for second
graders in the Common Core State Standards.) For each differently sized unit, estimate the
measure in advance and discuss the estimate afterward. Children should start to observe
that smaller units produce larger numeric measures, and vice versa. This is a difficult con-
cept for young children to understand but, with instruction, they can learn it (e.g., National
Research Council, 2009). Children construct this inverse relationship by estimating, then
experimenting, and finally reflecting on the measurements.
2009). Students on the same exam also experienced difficulty when the increments on a mea-
suring tool were not one unit. These results point to the difference between using a measur-
ing tool and understanding how it works.
If children build simple measuring instruments using unit models with which they are
familiar, it is more likely that they will understand how an instrument measures. A ruler is
a good example. If children line up individual physical units, such as paper clips or 1‐inch‐
long tiles, along a strip of card stock and mark them off, they can see that it is the spaces on
rulers and not the hash marks or numbers that are impor-
tant. It is essential that children discuss how measurement
If your children have been using individual paper clips as
with iterating individual units compares with measurement
units of measurement, ask them to create a chain of paper
using an instrument. Without this comparison and discus-
clips to be used as a ruler to make the transition from single
sion, children may not understand that these two methods
units to instruments more apparent.
are essentially the same.
failure to recognize and separate two types of objectives: (1) understanding the meaning and
technique of measuring a particular attribute and (2) learning about the standard units com-
monly used to measure that attribute.
Teaching standard units of measure can be organized
As you teach the standard units, make sure children,
around three broad goals:
especially ELLs, understand the terms (e.g., feet, yard) and
abbreviations (e.g., in, ft, cm). Include these on your math 1. Familiarity with the unit. Children should have
word wall. a basic idea of the size of commonly used units
and what they measure. Being able to estimate
a shelf as 5 feet long is as important as being able to measure the length of a shelf
accurately.
2. Ability to select an appropriate unit. Children should know both what is a reasonable
unit of measure in a given situation and the precision that is required. (Would
you measure your lawn to purchase grass seed with the same precision you
would use to measure a window to buy a pane of glass?) Children need practice
in selecting appropriate standard units and judging the level of precision.
3. Knowledge of relationships between units. Children should know the relationships that are
commonly used, such as those between inches, feet, and yards or minutes and hours.
Give children a physical model of a standard unit and have them search for objects that
have about the same measure as that one unit. For example, to develop familiarity with
the meter, give children a piece of rope 1 meter long and have them look for objects in
the classroom, outside on the playground, around the school building, or at home that
are about 1 meter in length. Have them make lists of things that are about 1 meter long
and separate lists for things that are a little shorter (or longer) or twice as long (or half
as long). In the case of lengths, be sure to include circular lengths. Later, children can try
to predict whether a given object is longer, shorter, or close to 1 meter. (Notice the use of
terms such as longer rather than more than. Remember to use precise language!)
For larger standard units, such as 1 mile or 1 kilometer, enlist parents to help children
find distances of that length. Suggest in a letter sent home that they check the distances
around the neighborhood, to the school or shopping center, or along other frequently trav-
eled paths. If possible, send home (or use in class) a 1‐meter or 1‐yard trundle wheel to
measure distances.
For the standard weights of gram, kilogram, ounce,
and pound, children can compare objects on a two‐pan
Meter trundle wheels measure off one meter with every ro-
balance with single copies of these units. It may be more
tation. Children may not understand that the circumference
effective to work with 10 grams or 5 ounces so you don’t
of the wheel is the same length as the flat meter stick. Help
have to use so many units to balance the pan. (Note that
children make the connection by having them match a rope
one slice of store‐bought sandwich bread usually weighs
or strip of paper that is 1 meter in length to a meter stick
about 1 ounce, so many objects will weigh much more than
and then to the wheel. You can also lay a meter stick down
1 ounce.) Children can be encouraged to bring in familiar
and roll the trundle wheel along the length of the stick.
objects from home to compare on the classroom scale.
302
Building Measurement Concepts
A second approach to develop unit familiarity is to begin with familiar items and use
their measures as references or benchmarks. A doorway is a bit more than 2 meters high. A
bag of flour is a good reference for 5 pounds. A paper clip weighs about a gram and is about
1 centimeter wide. A gallon of milk weighs a little less than 4 kilograms. The following ac-
tivity engages children in measuring familiar items in a variety of ways.
e Lerane
rE
rs
of ways familiar items can be measured. In this book, the author bridges between
fo
nonstandard units (e.g., dog biscuits, swabs, etc.) and standard units (inches, centimeters,
etc.) to measure her pet dog Penny. Have children use the idea of measuring Penny to
find something at home (or in class) to measure in as many ways as they can think of
using standard units. The measures should be rounded to whole numbers. Discuss in class
the familiar items chosen and their measures so that different ideas and benchmarks are
shared. Many of the units used in Measuring Penny are common in the United States but
not in other countries. Still, the book can be used to relate to how children from other
countries measure length and volume.
Children’s literature can also provide a way to engage children in developing familiarity
with various lengths. For example, the children’s book Actual Size ( Jenkins, 2011a) portrays
several different animals and insects (or a part of their body) in their actual size. From the
head of a 23‐foot‐long crocodile to the hand of a gorilla to the 212 ‐inch‐tall mouse lemur,
children can compare themselves and their classmates to these fascinating creatures while
learning about linear measurement.
It can be interesting to have children find length benchmarks on their bodies. Couple
the discussion of personal benchmarks with a discussion of growth because these bench-
marks will change as they grow! Have children compare their references with their parents
or an older sibling’s benchmarks to see how they differ in size. Point out that children will
likely need to adjust their benchmarks over time.
Measure various parts of your body. About how long is your foot, your stride, your hand
span (stretched and with fingers together), the width of your index finger or pinky finger,
your arm span (finger to finger and finger to nose), and the distance around your wrist
and around your waist? What is the height to your waist, shoulder, and head? Some of
these measures may prove to be useful benchmarks for single or multiples of standard
units. (The average child’s fingernail is about 1 cm in width and most people find a 10‐cm
length somewhere on their hands.)
303
Building Measurement Concepts
if you were determining how many 8‐foot molding strips to buy, the nearest foot would
probably be sufficient.
304
Building Measurement Concepts
Suppose you are measuring a length of ribbon with a ruler that only shows quarter inches;
our unit is a quarter of an inch. If the length of ribbon falls between 334 and 4 inches, we
would usually round to whichever number is closest to the length of ribbon. If the length
of ribbon is more than halfway towards the 4‐inch mark, we say it’s 4 inches long. However,
if the length of ribbon is less than halfway from 334 , we say it is closer to 334 inches. In either
case, we are within 18 of an inch or one‐half of the unit and are essentially ignoring the excess
or deficiency—and this constitutes our “error.” If we need more precision in our measure-
ment, we use smaller units to ensure that our measurement rounding or error is within a
range that we can accept.
305
Building Measurement Concepts
Today’s Estimate
3. Use subdivisions. This is a strategy similar to chunking, with the chunks imposed on
the object by the estimator. For example, if the wall length to be estimated has no useful
chunks, it can be mentally divided in half and then in fourths or even eighths by repeated
halving until a more manageable length is found. Length, volume, and area measurements
all lend themselves to this technique.
4. Iterate a unit mentally or physically. For length, area, and volume, it is sometimes easy
to mark off single units visually. You might use your hands or make marks or folds to keep
track as you go. If a child knows, for example, that her stride is about 12 meter long, she can
walk off a length and then add to get an estimate. Hand and finger widths are useful for
shorter measures.
306
Building Measurement Concepts
Select a single object such as a box, a pumpkin, a painting on the wall of the school, a
jar, or even the principal! Each day, select a different attribute or dimension to estimate.
For the pumpkin, for example, children can estimate its height, circumference, weight,
volume, and surface area.
Conduct estimation scavenger hunts. Give teams a list of either nonstandard or standard
measurements and have them find things that are close to having those measurements.
Do not allow children to use measuring instruments. A list might include the following
items:
• A length of 3 meters
• Something that is as long as your mathematics book
• Something that weighs more than a paper clip but less than your mathematics book
• Something that has the same area as the palm of your hand
In this chapter, we emphasize length, time, and money as these are the measurements
highlighted in the pre‐K–2 curriculum. We infuse ideas of teaching measurement and esti-
mation using activities as examples.
307
Building Measurement Concepts
Length
Length is usually the first attribute children learn to measure. Be aware, however, that length
measurement is not immediately understood by young children. Researchers (e.g., Clements
& Sarama, 2009; Curry, Mitchelmore, & Outhred, 2006) have identified some of the more
common misconceptions and difficulties children have when measuring length:
• Leaving gaps between units or overlapping units
• Using units that are not of equal size (e.g., measuring with paper clips of different sizes)
• Combining units of different sizes as if they were the same unit (e.g., combining 2 feet
and 4 inches as “6 long”)
• When using a ruler, beginning at “1” rather than “0” or measuring from the wrong end
of the ruler
• Counting the marks on a ruler or the “points” between heel‐to‐toe steps rather than the
spaces in between
• When comparing the length of two objects, comparing the objects at one end only
It is important to keep these misconceptions and difficulties in mind as you plan lessons and
observe your children during measurement activities.
Comparison Activities
At the pre‐K–K level, children should begin with direct comparisons of two or more lengths
and then move to indirect comparisons by the first grade (CCSSO, 2010; National Council
of Teachers of Mathematics, 2006).
Set up learning stations where children can explore which objects in a group are longer,
shorter, or about the same in length as a specified “target” object. Change the target ob-
ject and children may find that the shorter object is now longer than the target. A similar
task can involve putting a set of objects in order from shortest to longest.
Give pairs of children a strip of card stock, a stick, a length of rope, or some other object
with an obvious length dimension that will serve as a “target” unit. The task is for chil-
dren to find five things in the room that are shorter than, longer than, or about the same
length as their target unit. They can record what they find using pictures or words.
By making the target length a standard unit (e.g., a meter stick or a 1‐meter length of
rope), the activity can be repeated to provide familiarity with important standard units.
Throughout the school day, look for opportunities for children to compare lengths
directly. For example, which block tower is taller, which clay snake is shorter, which chair is
wider, and so on.
Indirect comparison, which means using another object to help make the measure, is
the focus in the next activity. Children who can use a third object to compare the lengths of
308
Building Measurement Concepts
two other objects must have a transitive understanding of measurement. For example, if the
length of a green pencil is shorter than the length of a blue pencil, and the length of the blue
pencil is shorter than the length of a red pencil, then the length of the green pencil must be
shorter than the length of the red pencil. Although transitivity is crucial to understanding
measurement, not all children in grades K–1 will be able to follow this argument (Curry,
Mitchelmore, & Outhred, 2006).
Challenge children to determine whether an object in the classroom or outside the class-
room (maybe in the library or cafeteria or on the playground) would fit through the
doorway of the classroom. For example, is the doorway wide enough for one of the
tables in the cafeteria to go through? Will one of the shelves in the library fit through
the doorway? What about the monkey bars on the playground? In pairs have children
brainstorm ways they can check and then discuss and try some of their suggestions.
Children should also compare lengths that are not in straight lines. One way to do this
is by using indirect comparisons. For example, children can wrap string around objects in
a search for things that are, say, as long around as the distance from the floor to their belly
button or as long as the distance around their head or waist.
Make some crooked or curvy paths on the floor (or outside) with masking tape
or chalk. The task is to determine which path is longest, next longest, and so on.
Children should suggest ways to measure the crooked paths so that they can be compared
easily. If you wish to offer a hint, provide pairs of children with a long piece of string (at
first make it longer than the path). Have children explain how they solved the problem.
For children with disabilities, you may need to tape the end of the string to the beginning
of the path and help them mark the final measurement on the string with a marker. Use
another string for the other path in the same way. Then compare the string lengths.
In “Crooked Paths” some children may argue that the path that “looks longer” is the
longer path, although a crooked and more compact path may actually be longer. You might
want to show an example of two paths on the floor (one crooked path and one straight path
that “looks longer” than the crooked path) and have children walk each path to see which
takes longer to walk.
309
Building Measurement Concepts
• The units must be without gaps—if not, part of the quantity is not being measured.
• The units must not overlap—otherwise, part of the quantity is measured more than
once.
Children can begin to measure length using a variety of nonstandard units, including
the following:
• Giant footprints. Cut out about 20 copies of a large footprint about 112 to 2 feet long on
poster board.
• Measuring ropes. Cut rope into lengths of 1 meter. These ropes can measure the perim-
eter and the circumference of objects such as the teacher’s desk, a tree trunk, or the class
pumpkin.
• Drinking straws. Straws provide large quantities of a useful unit as they are easily cut
into smaller units. They can also be linked together with a long string or slid onto a
length of pipe cleaner, creating an excellent bridge to a ruler or measuring tape.
• Short units. Flat toothpicks, linking cubes, and paper clips are all useful nonstandard
units for measuring shorter lengths. Cuisenaire rods are also useful because they are
easily placed end to end and are also metric (centimeters) and so make an excellent
bridge to a ruler. Paper clips can also be made into chains. Note that individual linking
cubes do not explicitly exhibit the attribute of length as well as Cuisenaire rods, tooth-
picks, paper clips, and straws.
Keep in mind the word of caution mentioned earlier about using a few well‐chosen
nonstandard units to avoid confusing children with a variety of units. Once children dem-
onstrate, in particular, that all the units used to measure a given length must be the same
length, then you may want to increase the variety of nonstandard units available.
310
Building Measurement Concepts
The following activity encourages children to develop their own approach to measuring
lengths.
Explain that you have just received an important request from the principal. She needs to
know exactly how tall each teacher in the building is. The children are to decide how to
measure the teachers and write a note to the principal explaining how tall their teacher
is and detailing the process that they used. If you wish to give a hint, ask if it might
be easier if they were to measure how long you are instead of how tall. You can ask,
“Would it help if I lie down?” Have children make marks at your feet and head and draw
a straight line between these marks.
Explain that the principal says they can use any nonstandard unit to measure with
(provide a few choices until you are confident children understand the necessity to use
equal‐length units). For each choice of unit, supply enough units to more than cover your
length. Put children in pairs and allow them to select one unit with which to measure.
Ask children to make an estimate and then have them use their unit to measure.
After children complete their measuring, follow up with questions such as “How did you
get your measurement?” “Did children who measured with the same unit get the same an-
swers? If not, why not?” “How could the principal make a line
that was as long as the teacher?” Focus on the value of carefully
lining up units end to end. Discuss what happens if you overlap
Figure 4
Record sheet for measuring with informal length units.
units, have gaps in the units, or don’t follow a straight line.
Repeat the basic task of “How Long Is the Teacher?”
with other measuring tasks, each time providing a choice of Name
units and the requirement that children explain their mea-
sures. It is always helpful if the same lengths (e.g., heights, Around your outline
distances around) are measured by several pairs of children Unit:
using the same unit so that possible errors can be discussed straw
and the measuring process refined. The following activity Estimate straws
adds an estimation component.
Measured straws
Make lists of things in the room to measure (see Figure 4). The teacher’s desk
Run a piece of masking tape along the dimension of ob- Unit: orange rod
jects to be measured. Include curves or other distances Estimate rods
Teacher’s desk
that are not straight lines. Have children make estimates Measured rods
before they measure. Young children will not be very
good at estimating distances at first so it is important
to provide them with some strategies for doing so (see Around math book
section on “Tips for Teaching Estimation”). For example, Math Unit: paper clip
have children make a row or chain of exactly 10 units to
help them with their estimates. They can first lay 10 units
Book
Estimate clips
against the object and then make their estimate. You can
Measured clips
also do this activity using standard units.
311
Building Measurement Concepts
The Common Core State Standards recommend that second graders consider the rela-
tionship between the size of a unit and the resulting measure. Children can find it a chal-
lenge to understand that larger units will create a smaller measure and vice versa. Engage
children in activities in which this issue is emphasized.
Have children measure a length with a specified unit. Then provide them with a different
unit that is either twice as long or half as long as the original unit.
Standards for
Mathematical Practice Their task is to predict the measure of the same length using the new unit. Children
should write down their predictions and discuss how they made their estimations. Then
2 Reason abstractly
◀ have them make the actual measurement. Cuisenaire rods are excellent for this activity,
and quantitatively
but other nonstandard units are useful as well.
• Provide a box with assorted units of different sizes. Cuisenaire rods would be suitable. Have children
use the materials in the box to measure a given length. Observe whether the child understands that
all units must be of similar size. If different lengths of units are used, ask the child to describe his or
her measurement.
• Ask children to draw a line or mark off a distance of a prescribed number of units. Observe whether
children know to align the units in a straight line without overlaps or gaps.
• Provide a length of string. Tell children that the string is 6 units long. How could they use the string
to make a length of 3 units? How could they make a length of 9 units? In this task, you are looking
to see if children can mentally subdivide the given length (string) based on an understanding of its
measure. That is, can children visualize that 6 units are matched to the string length and half of these
would be 3 units?
• Have children measure two different objects. Then ask how much longer the longer object is. Observe
whether children can use the measurements to answer or whether they need to make a third mea-
surement to find the difference.
312
Building Measurement Concepts
313
Building Measurement Concepts
Figure 6
Use an unmarked ruler and ask children to measure an object. In the example shown, the
correct length is 8 units. A child counting hash marks would respond with 9 units.
GeoGebra offers a couple of interactive worksheets in which children are asked to measure given
objects using a broken ruler. The worksheet at http://maine.edc.org/file.php/1/AssessmentResources
/BrokenRulers132_L.html allows you to change the ruler from a standard ruler to a standard ruler that
is broken along different intervals. There are nine objects children can measure, all of which are close
enough in length to approximate to whole units. The rulers on this worksheet are all divided into fourths.
Another worksheet uses a ruler that is divided into sixteenths and has children measuring various
parts of pets (http://maine.edc.org/file.php/1/AssessmentResources/BrokenRulers_MeasureMe32_L.html).
If children click on the hint button, a line is drawn along the path that should be measured.
Time
Time is different from most other attributes that are commonly measured in school because
it cannot be seen or felt and because it is more difficult for children to comprehend units of
time or how those units are matched against a given time period or duration.
314
Building Measurement Concepts
Comparison Activities
Time can be thought of as the duration of an event from its beginning to its end. As with
other attributes, for children to adequately understand the attribute of time, they should
make comparisons of events that have different durations. If two events begin at the same
time, the shorter duration will end first and the other last longer. For example, which wind‐
up toy lasts longer? However, this form of comparison focuses on the ending of the duration
rather than the duration itself. In order to think of time as something that can be measured,
we need to focus on the duration itself.
Engaging tasks that address duration include the following:
• Stacking 10 blocks one at a time and then removing them one at a time
• Saying your full name
• Walking along a designated path
• Watering a plant
• Reading a page of a book
Children need to learn about seconds, minutes, and hours to develop some concept of how
long these units are. Point out to children the duration of short and long events during the Figure 7
school day. Have children time familiar events in their daily lives: brushing teeth, eating Approximate time with
dinner, riding to school, doing homework. Timing short events of 12 minute to 2 minutes one‐handed clocks.
at school and at home can be fun and useful. Children can work in pairs and use a timer,
such as a stopwatch, to time some of the tasks listed previously. For other examples, in
Just a Second author Steve Jenkins (2011b) shares some interesting events that occur in just 11 12 1
1 second, 1 minute, and 1 hour. 10 2
9 3
Reading Clocks 8 4
7 6 5
The common instrument for measuring time is the clock. However, learning to tell time
has little to do with time measurement and more to do with the skills of learning to read an
instrument. Clock reading can be a difficult skill to teach. Starting in first grade children are “About 7 o’clock”
usually taught to read clocks to the hour and then to the half hour. In second grade, they
learn to read to 5‐minute intervals (CCSSO, 2010). In the early stages of this sequence,
children are shown clocks set exactly to the hour or half hour. Thus, many children who can 11 12 1
read a clock at 7:00 or 2:30 are initially challenged by 6:58 or 2:33. 10 2
Digital clocks permit children to read times easily but do not relate times very well to 9 3
benchmark times. To know that a digital reading of 7:58 is nearly 8 o’clock, the child must 8 4
7 6 5
know that there are 60 minutes in an hour, that 58 is close to 60, and that 2 minutes is not
a very long time. These concepts are challenging for many first‐grade and second‐grade
children. The analog clock (with hands) shows “close to” times visually without the need for “A little bit
understanding big numbers or even how many minutes are in an hour. past 9 o’clock”
The following suggestions can help children understand and read analog clocks by fo-
cusing on the actions and functions of the minute and hour hands on a clock.
1. Begin with a one‐handed clock by breaking off the minute hand from a regular clock. 11 12 1
10 2
Use lots of approximate language: “It’s about 7 o’clock.” “It’s a little past 9 o’clock.”
9 3
“It’s halfway between 2 o’clock and 3 o’clock” (see Figure 7).
8 4
2. Discuss what happens to the big hand as the little hand goes from one hour to the 7 6 5
next. When the big hand is at 12, the hour hand is pointing exactly to a number. If the
hour hand is about halfway between numbers, about where would the minute hand
“Halfway between
be? If the hour hand is a little past or before an hour (10 to 15 minutes), about where
2 o’clock and 3 o’clock”
would the minute hand be?
315
Building Measurement Concepts
3. Use two real clocks, one with only an hour hand and one with two hands. Cover the
two‐handed clock. Periodically during the day, direct attention to the one‐handed
clock. Discuss the time in approximate language. Have children predict where the
minute hand should be. Uncover the other clock and check.
4. Teach time after the hour in 5‐minute intervals. After step 3 has begun, count by fives
going around the clock. Instead of predicting that the minute hand is “pointing at the
4,” transition to the language “it is about 20 minutes after the hour.” As skills develop,
suggest that children always look first at the little or hour hand to learn approximately
what time it is and then focus on the big or minute hand for precision.
5. Predict the reading on a digital clock when shown an analog clock, and vice versa; set
an analog clock when shown a digital clock.
6. Relate the time after the hour to the time before the next hour. This is helpful not
only for telling time but also for number sense.
The following activity assesses children’s ability to read a clock.
e Lerane
rE
rs
hour hand with a variety of placements that are appropriate for your grade (e.g.,
fo
on the hour, half past the hour, a quarter past the hour, a quarter until the
hour, and close to but not on the hour). For each clock face, the children’s
BLM
task is to write the digital time and draw the corresponding minute hand
on the clock. If you have ELLs, it is important to note that telling time is
done differently in different cultures. For example, in Spanish any time past 30 minutes is
stated as the next hour minus the time until that hour. For example, 10:45 is thought of
as 15 minutes before 11, or 11 minus a quarter. Be explicit that in English it can be said
either way—“10:45” or “a quarter till 11.”
The “Time— Analog and Digital Clocks” applet at the National Library of Virtual Manipulatives allows children
to change the time on an analog clock and see how it changes on the digital clock and vice versa (http://nlvm
.usu.edu/en/nav/grade_g_1.html). The “Time—Match Clocks” applet at the same website displays a time
on the analog clock and children have to match that time on the digital clock. Times are also shown on the
digital clock that children match using the analog clock.
After children learn how to read a clock, the following activity not only motivates them
to think about telling time, but it also helps them to consider the relationship between ana-
log clock reading and digital recording.
Give children a recording sheet with a set of clock faces (see Blackline Mas-
BLM ter 47). Secretly set a timer to go off at the hour, half hour, or minute.
When the bell rings, children should look up and record the time on the
clock face and in numerals on the recording sheet.
316
Building Measurement Concepts
Although intervals of time (elapsed time) are a third‐grade topic in the Common
Core State Standards, time provides a good context for adding and subtracting numbers.
“Ready for the Bell” provides opportunities for children to determine the time between
timer rings.
You can also pose story problems about time, keeping in mind the problem structures
for addition and subtraction. Consider the following examples:
• Sara started reading at 4:15 p.m. and stopped at 4:40 p.m. How long did Sara read?
(Change Unknown)
• Sara read today for 50 minutes. She stopped reading at 6:30 p.m. What time did Sara
start reading? (Start Unknown)
• Danny gets on the bus at 7:15 a.m. and arrives at school at 7:50 a.m. Callie gets on her
bus at 7:25 a.m. and arrives at school at 8:05 a.m. Who has a longer bus ride? (Compari-
son: Difference Unknown)
• It takes Frank 15 minutes to get to school on his bus. If Kiera gets on her bus at 7:20 a.m.
and rides the bus for 10 more minutes than Frank, what time does she arrive at school?
(Comparison: Larger Unknown)
You can modify the difficulty of these problems by changing the numbers (e.g., use time on
the hour or half hour to create easier problems, or use time in 5‐minute intervals to increase
the difficulty level). Start unknown and change unknown problems are also more difficult
than result unknown problems. Make sure to provide a variety of problems in which the
location of the unknown changes.
Money
Here is a list of the money ideas and skills typically required in the primary grades:
• Recognizing coins
• Identifying and using the value of coins
• Counting and comparing sets of coins
• Creating equivalent coin collections (same amounts, different coins)
• Selecting coins for a given amount
• Making change
• Solving word problems involving money starting in second grade (CCSSO, 2010)
317
Building Measurement Concepts
e Lerane
Explain to children that they will start skip counting by one number, and at your
rE
fo rs
signal they will shift to a count by a different number. Use any two of these num-
bers: 100, 50, 25, 10, 5, 1. Always start the skip count with the larger number. For
example, write the numbers 25 and 10 on the board. Point to the larger number
(25), and have children begin to skip count. After three or more counts, raise your hand
to indicate a pause in the counting. Then point to the smaller number (10). Children
continue the skip count from where they left off but now count by tens. Later, try three
numbers in descending order. If you have ELLs who are recent immigrants, invite them
to share coins from their country and see how they compare to our coins or use them for
more addition and subtraction experiences. Children with disabilities may need to use a
hundreds chart to help them with their skip counts.
Working with coins requires not only adding up the values but also first mentally giving
each coin a value. Engage children in using coins and developing addition concepts by hav-
ing them add a mixed collection of coins.
Using a projector, show a small collection of coins not arranged in any order. How many
coins to use will vary with the experiences of your children. Begin with only dimes and
pennies. Then add some nickels and eventually quarters. Give children time to identify
the coins and write down the corresponding numeric values of the coins (e.g., 5 for a
318
Building Measurement Concepts
nickel, 25 for a quarter). Have children share what they have written down to make sure
everyone has identified the correct amounts.
Now the children’s task is to add the numbers mentally. Do not suggest how they add
the numbers or in what order because there is almost always more than one good way to
do this. For example, rather than add from the largest values to the smallest—the typical
way coins are taught in books—it is also reasonable to use the 5s to make tens or other
methods. For this collection, note that it is easy to add 5 and 25, then 10, then 7 (the last
5 and two 1s). Discuss with children how they added the collection.
10 1 25
5 5 1
When discussing solutions to this last activity, be sure to value any approach that works.
However, pay special attention to those children who begin with the larger values and those
who put combinations together utilizing thinking with tens. There is no reason to require
children to add in any particular order, not with this activity or with coins.
As with time, you can also pose story problems about money using the problem struc-
tures for addition and subtraction. Consider the following examples:
• Alexis has some coins. Zig gives her 1 dime and 2 pennies. Now Alexis has 36¢. What
coins did Alexis have to begin with? (Joining: Start Unknown)
• Jay has 2 quarters and a dime. He needs 95¢ to buy a notebook. How much more money
does Jay need? (Part–Part–Whole: Part Unknown)
• Andy has $11.65. He gave Becky some money. Now Andy has $8.15. How much money
did Andy give Becky? (Separate: Change Unknown)
• Wendy has $2.67. That is $1.25 more than what Keith has. How much money does Keith
have? (Comparison: Smaller Unknown)
Again, changing the numbers and the location of the unknown will modify the level of
difficulty.
Making Change
When you pay for something at a store using cash, you may often give the clerk more
money than the purchase price and you expect to receive the correct change back. Know-
ing how to make change is important for the clerk but also for the customers—so they can
check to make sure they received the correct change!
You may have thought, I can start at 100, go back 10 to 90, then back 8 more to 82—so
the change would be 10 + 8 or 18 dollars. You could also start at 100, go back 20 to 80, up 2
to 82, so that’s 20 - 2 or 18. You may have thought to start at 82, go 8 more to 90 and then
10 more to 100—that’s 8 + 10 or 18 dollars.
319
Building Measurement Concepts
Write a target number on the board. This number should be the same as an amount of
money that might be given to a store clerk in a purchase, most likely 25, 50, 75, or 100.
To the left of this target, write a smaller starting number and an arrow. Here are some
examples:
13 S 25 56 S 75 29 S 50
In creating the amounts for this activity, think in terms of purchases. If the target is 75,
that means you gave the clerk 75¢. You would only do this for items costing more than
50¢. Similarly, for a target of 50, use numbers greater than 25. For a target of 100, any
smaller number would be appropriate because you may have given the clerk a dollar
bill.
Embed the numbers in story problems that describe making a purchase. Explain to
the children that the first number written represents the amount of the purchase and the
second number represents the amount of money given to the store clerk. The children’s
task is to find the difference (i.e., the change) using a strategy that they can explain. They
may need to write down intermediate results. Discuss the solution methods used by dif-
ferent children.
The next activity extends “How Much Is the Change?” and attempts to draw children’s
attention to the notion of using the fewest possible coins.
On the board, write start and target numbers as in “How Much Is the Change?” Then
write on the board the values of the coins: 25, 10, 5, 1. In this task, children must use
only the numbers (i.e., coins) in the list to create the difference. As they use a number,
they should write it down. Challenge children to try to use as few “coins” as possible
or, in other words, as many of the larger numbers as possible. For example, if the
target is 75 with a start of 58, they could write 1, 1, 10, 5. Have children discuss their
solutions.
320
Building Measurement Concepts
When your children are ready, “How Much Is the Change with Coins?” can be ex-
tended to values greater than a dollar (greater than targets of 100¢).
This sequence of suggested activities is not a surefire solution to the difficulties children
experience with money. It is designed to build on number and place‐value skills and con-
cepts that were developed without or before using coins.
Area
Area is the two‐dimensional space inside a region. As with other attributes, children must
first understand the attribute of area before measuring.
Comparison Activities
One purpose of early comparison activities with areas is to help children distinguish be-
tween size (or area) and shape, length, and other dimensions. A long, skinny rectangle may
have less area than a triangle with shorter sides. Also important to understand is the idea
that rearranging areas into different shapes does not affect the amount of area. These ideas
are especially difficult for young children to understand.
Direct comparison of two areas is frequently impossible except when the shapes
involved have some common dimension or property. For example, two rectangles with
the same width can be compared directly. Comparison of these special shapes, however,
fails to deal with the attribute of area. Instead, activities in which one area is rearranged
(conservation of area) are suggested. Cutting a shape into two parts and reassembling it
in a different shape can show that the before and after shapes have the same area, even
though they are different shapes. This idea is not at all obvious to children in grades
pre‐K–2.
321
Building Measurement Concepts
Draw two rectangles and a blob shape on a sheet of paper. Make it so that the three
areas are not the same but with no area that is clearly largest or smallest. The children’s
first task is to estimate which is the smallest and the largest of the three shapes. After
recording their estimate, they should use multiples of the same unit to fill in the shapes
(e.g., color tiles, round counters). Alternatively, they can trace or glue the same two‐
dimensional unit on the shapes. Children should explain what they discovered.
322
Building Measurement Concepts
Your objective in the beginning is to develop the idea that area is measured by covering
or tiling. Groups are likely to come up with different measures for the same region. Discuss
these differences with the children and point to the difficulties involved in making estimates
around the edges. Avoid the idea that there is one “right” approach.
Comparison Activities
Comparing the volumes of solid objects is very difficult. For primary grade children, it is
appropriate to focus on capacity. A simple method of comparing capacity is to fill one con-
tainer with something and then pour this amount into the comparison container.
Young children should have lots of experiences directly comparing the capacities of dif-
ferent containers such as cans, small boxes, and plastic containers. The following activity is
appropriate for pre‐K–2 children.
Provide a variety of containers with one marked as the “target.” The children’s task
is to sort the collection into those that hold more than, less than, or about the same
amount as the target container. Provide a recording sheet on which each container is
listed and a place to circle “holds more,” “holds less,” and “holds about the same.”
List the choices twice for each container—one to record an estimate and one for the
actual measure. Provide a filler (such as beans, rice, or Styrofoam peanuts), scoops,
and funnels. Working in pairs, have children measure and record results. Discuss what
children noticed in their estimating and measuring (e.g., rounder/fatter shapes seem
to hold more).
short, wide one because it is difficult for them to attend to two dimensions, the height and
width. By having them explore capacity using a variety of containers, you can challenge this
misconception.
You can also have children begin to use cups, pints, quarts, and gallon containers to help
them begin to get a sense of their sizes. Having them use, for example, a measuring cup to
fill a pint container or a pint container to fill a gallon container can help them begin to de-
velop a sense of the relative sizes of customary units of capacity.
Comparison Activities
Starting in kindergarten, children can begin exploring the concept of heavier and lighter.
The most conceptual way for children to compare the weights of two objects is to hold
one in each hand, extend their arms, and experience the relative downward pull on each—
effectively communicating to a pre‐K–1 child what “heavier” or “weighs more” means. This
personal experience can then be transferred to one of two basic types of scales—balances
and spring scales.
When introducing children to a balance, have them hold two different objects, one in
each hand, and estimate which of two objects is heavier. When they then place the objects in
the two pans of a balance, the pan that goes down can be understood to hold the heavier ob-
ject. Even a relatively simple balance will detect small differences. If two objects are placed
one at a time in a spring scale, the heavier object pulls the pan down farther. Both balances
and spring scales have real value in the classroom. (Technically, spring scales measure weight
and balance scales measure mass. Why?) With either scale, sorting and ordering tasks are
possible with very young children.
324
E x p a n d e d L e s s o n
Crooked Paths
325
Building Measurement Concepts
we tell which of these is longer?” Solicit ideas. Be sure • For children who need a greater challenge, ask them
the children hear the idea that the curve is longer than to make a row of blocks in a straight line that is just as
the line segment and that some children provide good long as the curvy path.
reasons. For example, say, “If you had to walk on these,
it would take longer to walk the curved path.”
After
Bring the class together to share and discuss the task:
• Remind the children of the task of comparing the two
paths. Have the children refer to their worksheets as
they talk about what they did.
• Ask, “How many thought the zigzag path was longer?”
Present the focus task to the class:
Count and record on the board next to a zigzag. “How
• Gather the children around one station. Say, “One path many thought the curvy path was longer?” Count
might be longer or they might be the same. Your task is and record. “How many thought they were about the
to decide.” same?” Count and record. Ask the children if the esti-
• Show the children the worksheets and explain how to mates they made were the same as the result that they
use them. Explain that they are to circle the path that figured out. Were they surprised? Why?
is longer or circle both paths if they think they are the • Select pairs to explain what they did. Get as many dif-
same. Then they are to draw a picture to show how ferent ideas and methods as possible.
they decided. Have the children work in pairs.
• If the children disagree about which path is longer,
• Ask the children which path they think is longer or have them explain their reasoning in a way that might
whether they think they are the same. Say, “Before you convince those who disagree. Give the children the op-
begin work, put an X on the picture of the path that portunity to change their minds, but ask, “What made
you think is longer. This is your estimate.” Have a few you change your minds?”
children share their estimates and their reasoning.
• Show the children that there is string for comparing
lengths, but they can use linked paper clips, blocks, or Assessment
whatever materials they want to help them decide.
Observe
Provide clear expectations: • Look for children who understand how length can exist
on a curved path (correctly compare or make appropri-
• Children will work with partners at the stations.
ate attempt). This can be a checklist item based on the
discussion or observations.
During
• For children who used units, did they use like‐sized
• Monitor station activity but do not interfere. Be sure units or make appropriate use of materials?
the children are completing worksheets to the best of
their abilities.
Ask
• If a pair seems unable to make a decision, ask, “If a
toy car was going to go along these paths, which path • Which path is longer? How do you know?
would it travel longer on?” or, “Could you use some • How can path A be longer if it takes less room to make
blocks from over in the block corner to help?” on the floor than path B?
326
Building Measurement Concepts
References
Blume, G., Galindo, E., & Walcott, C. (2007). 57–67). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers
Performance in measurement and geometry from of Mathematics.
the viewpoint of Principles and Standards for School
Kloosterman, P., Rutledge, Z., & Kenney, P.
Mathematics. In P. Kloosterman & F. Lester,
(2009). Exploring the results of the NAEP: 1980s
Jr. (Eds.), Results and interpretations of the 2003
to the present. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle
mathematics assessment of the National Assessment
School, 14(6), 357–365.
of Educational Progress (pp. 95–138). Reston, VA:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Leedy, L. (2000). Measuring Penny. New York, NY:
Henry Holt and Company.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2009). Learning and
teaching early math: The learning trajectories approach. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
New York, NY: Routledge. (2003). Navigating through measurement in
prekindergarten–grade 2. Reston, VA: Author.
Curry, M., Mitchelmore, M., & Outhred, L. (2006).
Development of children’s understanding of length, National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
area, and volume measurement principles. In J. (2006). Curriculum focal points for prekindergarten
Novotná, H. Moraová, M. Krátká, & N. Stehlíková through grade 8 mathematics: A quest for coherence.
(Eds.). Proceedings for 30th Conference of the Reston, VA: Author.
International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Education, Vol. 2, pp. 377–384.
(2011b, March). Position statement on the metric
Dietiker, L., Gonulates, F., Figueras, J., & Smith, system. Retrieved from www.nctm
J. P. (2010, April). Weak attention to unit iteration in .org/about/content.aspx?id=29000
U.S. elementary curriculum materials. Presentation at
Thompson, T. D., & Preston, R. V. (2004).
the annual conference of the American Educational
Measurement in the middle grades: Insights from
Research Association, Denver, CO.
NAEP and TIMSS. Mathematics Teaching in the
Jenkins, S. (2011a). Actual size. Boston, MA: Middle School, 9(9), 514–519.
Houghton Mifflin.
Towers, J., & Hunter, K. (2010). An ecological
Jenkins, S. (2011b). Just a second. New York, NY: reading of mathematical language in a grade
Houghton Mifflin. 3 classroom: A case of learning and teaching
measurement estimation. The Journal of
Joram, E. (2003). Benchmarks as tools for
Mathematical Behavior, 29, 25–40.
developing measurement sense. In D. H. Clements
(Ed.), Learning and teaching measurement (pp.
327
This page intentionally left blank
Developing Geometric
Reasoning and Concepts
BigIDEAS
1 What makes shapes alike and different can be determined by an array of geometric properties and defining
characteristics. For example, a square is like a rectangle because it has two pairs of parallel sides and four right angles; some
rectangles are different from squares because they do not have four equal sides.
2 Shapes can be described in terms of their location in a plane or in space. Initially children use the words of everyday
language such as above, below, next to, in front of, and beside to describe location and then later transition to simple
coordinate systems to describe locations more precisely.
3 Shapes can be moved in a plane or in space without changing the shape’s properties. These movements can be
described in terms of translations (slides), reflections (flips), and rotations (turns).
4 Visualization is “geometry done with the mind’s eye.” It involves being able to create and move mental images of
shapes, thinking about how they look from different viewpoints, and predicting the results of various transformations.
From Chapter 16 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
329
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
• In kindergarten, children are expected to identify and describe various two- and three-
dimensional shapes and describe their relative position using everyday language (e.g.,
above, below, beside). They should also draw and build shapes as well as compose larger
shapes from smaller ones.
• In first grade, children should be able to distinguish between a shape’s defining attributes
(e.g., number of sides, closed) and irrelevant attributes (e.g., color, size, orientation).
They also work on composing shapes to create new shapes and begin to decompose
shapes into smaller shapes.
• Second graders are expected to be able to recognize and draw shapes given specific
attributes (e.g., number of angles, equal length sides, and “square” angles). They con-
tinue to work on decomposing shapes into smaller shapes.
In order to best help children grow in their understanding of geometry, we need to under-
stand both aspects of geometry—reasoning and content.
330
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Geometric Content
For too long, geometry curricula in the United States emphasized the learning of terminol-
ogy. Geometry, in fact, involves a number of aspects that apply to all grade levels:
• Shapes and Properties includes a study of the properties of shapes in two and three
dimensions, as well as a study of the relationships built on properties.
• Transformation includes a study of translations, reflections, and rotations (slides, flips,
and turns), the study of symmetries, and the concept of similarity.
• Location refers to ways of specifying how objects are located in the plane or in space,
such as coordinate geometry.
• Visualization includes the recognition of shapes in the environment, developing rela-
tionships between two- and three-dimensional objects, and the ability to recognize,
draw, and think about objects from different viewpoints.
The chapter is organized around these four categories with experiences suggested for
each category. You will note that more attention is devoted to the topic shapes and proper-
ties as that category aligns with the emphasis of the Common Core State Standards for grades
pre-K–2. However, you should also note that experiences targeted toward each of the cat-
egories have potential to enhance children’s understanding in the other categories.
Pre-Recognition
The objects of thought at this level are specific visible or tactile objects.
Children at this level are unable to identify and distinguish between many common shapes.
They may notice only a subset of the visual characteristics of a shape, which results in an
inability to distinguish between some shapes. For example, they may be able to distinguish
between a circle and a square but not between a square and a triangle. Most children at this
level will know something about circles and squares. They tend to be less accurate identifying
triangles and rectangles. At this level children may identify any shape that has a prominent
331
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Figure 1 The van Hiele theory of geometric thought, modified to include the pre-recognition level.
Analysis of
deductive
Deductive systems
systems of
Relationships properties
among
4. Rigor
Properties of properties
shapes 3. Deduction
Classes of
shapes
2. Informal
Deduction
Shapes 1. Analysis
Visual or 0. Visualization
tactile objects
Pre-Recognition
“point” as a triangle and any four-sided shape with long parallel sides as a rectangle (Clem-
ents & Sarama, 2000a).
The products of thought at the pre-recognition level are shapes and what they “look like.”
Level 0: Visualization
The objects of thought at level 0 are shapes and what they “look like.”
Children at level 0 recognize and name figures based on the global, visual characteristics
of the figure—a gestalt-like approach to shape. For example, a square is defined by a child
at this level as a square “because it looks like a square.” Because appearance is dominant at
this level, appearances can overpower properties of a shape. For example, a rotated square
whose sides are all at a 45-degree angle to the vertical may now be called a diamond and no
longer a square according to children at this level. Children at level 0 will sort and classify
shapes based on their appearances—“I put these together because they are all pointy” (or
“fat,” or “look like a house,” or “are dented in,” and so on). With a focus on the appearances
of shapes, children are able to see how shapes are alike and different. As a result, children at
this level can create and begin to understand classifications of shapes.
The products of thought at level 0 are classes or groupings of shapes that seem to be “alike.”
332
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
and children begin to appreciate that a collection of shapes goes together because of
properties. If a shape belongs to a particular class such as cubes, it has the correspond-
ing properties of that class. “All cubes have six congruent faces, and each of those faces
is a square.” These properties are only implicit at level 0. Children operating at level 1
may be able to list all the properties of squares, rectangles, and parallelograms but not
see that these are subclasses of one another, that is, that all squares are rectangles and all
rectangles are parallelograms.
The products of thought at level 1 are the properties of shapes.
Level 3: Deduction
The objects of thought at level 3 are relationships among properties of geometric objects.
At level 3, students move from thinking about properties to reasoning and generating
proofs that are based on the properties. As this analysis of the informal arguments takes
place, the structure of the system, complete with axioms, definitions, theorems, corollaries,
and postulates, begins to develop and can be appreciated as the necessary means of estab-
lishing geometric truth. The student at this level is able to work with abstract statements
about geometric properties and make conclusions based more on logic than intuition. A
student operating at level 3 is not only aware that the diagonals of a rectangle bisect each
other (level 2) but also has an appreciation of the need to prove this from a series of deduc-
tive arguments.
The type of reasoning that characterizes a level 3 thinker is the same reasoning required
in high school geometry, where students build on a list of axioms and definitions to create
theorems. In a very global sense, high school geometry students are working to create a
complete geometric deductive system.
The products of thought at level 3 are deductive axiomatic systems for geometry.
Level 4: Rigor
The objects of thought at level 4 are deductive axiomatic systems for geometry.
At the highest level of the van Hiele hierarchy, the objects of thought are axiomatic systems
themselves, not just the deductions within a system. There is an appreciation of the distinctions
333
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
and relationships between different axiomatic systems. For example, spherical geometry is based
on lines drawn on a sphere rather than in a plane. This geometry has its own set of axioms and
theorems and is generally found at the level of college geometry courses.
We have given brief descriptions of all six levels to illustrate the scope of the van Hiele
theory. Most children at the pre-K–2 grades will be at the pre-recognition level or level 0
(visualization) with some children moving to level 1 (analysis). Consequently, a significant
number of the activities in this chapter address the first two levels.
334
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
• Challenge children with a wide range of geometric tasks. The use of physical materials, draw-
ings, and computer models at every level of geometric thought is a must. Activities
should also require and support children’s reflection about the ideas they are learning.
335
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
e Lerane
rE
point out the relevant attributes on the target shape (e.g., straight or curved
fo rs
sides, number of sides, right angles, and so on). Make sure children, especially
ELLs, understand the terms you use such as square corners and right turns. You
can compare a square corner or right angle to a capital “L” or the corner of a sheet of
paper or an index card. Use masking tape to create a large target shape on the commu-
nity rug so that children can walk around the shape. You can also have the children draw
the shape in the air. Now the task is for children to find one or two items in the class-
room, on the playground, in the library, or somewhere else at school that have this same
shape or attribute. If searching outside the classroom, you may want to take pictures of
the examples children find so that you can show the examples later. As children find their
examples, ask them how they know it is the target shape. Listen for how they describe
their example. What are they focusing on? What words are they using? For children with
disabilities, provide a cutout of the target shape that children can take with them as they
search for an example.
Have children search not just for triangles, circles, squares, and rectangles but also for
properties of shapes. A shape hunt will be much more successful if you let children look
for either one thing or for a specific list. Some examples children can search for are the
following:
• Right angles (“square corners”)
• Curved surfaces or curved lines
• Two or more shapes that can be put together to make another shape
• Circles inside each other (concentric)
• Shapes with “dents” (concave) or without “dents” (convex)
• Solids that are like a box (prism), a cylinder, a pyramid, a cone
• Five shapes that are alike in some way
336
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Create a variety of familiar shapes from the assorted two-dimensional shapes in the
BLM
Blackline Masters (20–26) using card stock in two colors. Create multiples of the same
shapes. Give each child a shape from the set. Select a shape that matches one of the
children’s shapes but in a different color. Ask the children to stand up if they think they
Standards for
have a shape that matches your shape. Have children (either those standing or others) Mathematical Practice
explain how they know the shapes match and to name the shape. Then have children try
7 Look for and
to find matches for their shapes with children sitting near them. Have children share their ◀
make use of structure
matches and encourage them to try to name the shapes.
The next activity introduces children to three-dimensional shapes and is adapted from
Clements and Sarama (2000b).
After identifying and talking about the relevant attributes of a three-dimensional shape,
have the children pretend they are inside of it. For example, after showing the children
an oatmeal box (cylinder), tell them to pretend they are inside of it. Tell them to close
their eyes and touch the walls of the cylinder. Ask them to describe the walls. Then have
them pretend to trace the top of the cylinder and then the bottom of the cylinder with
their finger. Ask, “What is the shape you traced?” The activity can be repeated with other
shapes, such as a prism, where they are feeling and trying to identify each face. Try to
find large boxes or tubes for your children to get in so they can actually feel the inside
and outside of the shapes.
Source: D. H. Clements and J. Samara, Young Mathematicians at Work: Constructing Algebra. Reprinted
with permission. Copyright 2010 by the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. All rights reserved.
Children at the pre-recognition level tend to ignore some of the attributes of shapes.
The next two activities help to draw their attention to shapes’ attributes as children consider
examples and nonexamples of shapes.
Show children an example of a two-dimensional shape that you’ve drawn on the board.
Ask them for the name of the shape and ask them to tell why it is that shape. Draw an-
other shape that is somehow different from the first shape—it could be another example
of the shape or a nonexample that looks like the target shape and that children could
mistake for the shape. For example, if the target shape is a triangle, display triangles that
are “skinny” or oriented with the vertex pointing down. Or display a three-sided shape
whose sides are not straight. You can also provide counterexamples such as a “triangle”
musical instrument that has an open side. Have children identify and discuss the differ-
ences. Summarize by highlighting the relevant features of the target shape.
Clements and Sarama (2009) recommend pairing examples and nonexamples that look
alike so as to focus attention on a shape’s critical attributes. The next activity provides this
kind of experience.
337
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
338
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
equilateral and not always shown with the vertex at the top. Shapes should have curved
sides, straight sides, and combinations of these.
• Each child randomly selects a shape. In turn, the children tell one or two
things they find interesting about their shape.
• Children each randomly select two shapes and try to find something that is
alike about their two shapes and something that is different.
• The group selects one target shape at random and places it in the center of
the workspace. Their task is to find all other shapes that are like the target
shape according to the same rule. For example, if they say, “This shape is like
the target shape because it has a curved side and a straight side,” then all
other shapes that they put in the collection must have these properties. Do a
second sort with the same target shape but use a different property.
• Do a “secret sort.” You (or one of the children) create a small collection of
about five shapes that fit a secret rule. Leave other shapes that belong in
your set in the pile. Children try to find additional pieces that belong to the
set and/or guess the secret rule.
Figure 4 illustrates a few of the many possible ways a set might be sorted.
Most of these activities can and should be done with three-dimensional shapes as
well.
These activities can elicit a wide variety of ideas as children examine the shapes.
They may start describing the shapes with ideas such as “curvy” or “looks like a
tree” rather than typical geometric properties. But as children notice more sophis-
ticated properties you can attach appropriate names to them. For example, some
children may notice that some shapes have corners “like a
square” (explain that those are also called right angles) or
that “these shapes are the same on both sides” (symmetrical).
In any sorting activity, the children—not the teacher—
What makes Activity 7 a level 0 activity is that chil-
should decide how to sort. This allows the children to do the
dren are operating on the shapes that they see in front of
activity using ideas they own and understand. By listening to
them and are beginning to see similarities and differences
the kinds of attributes that they use in their sorting, you will
in shapes. By forming groups of shapes, they begin to imag-
be able to tell which properties they know and use and how
ine shapes that are not in the collection that belong to the
they think about shapes.
classes or groups they identify.
339
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
340
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
The next activity helps draw children’s attention away from appearance to relevant
attributes.
Activity 9 FEELING IT
You will need two sets of shapes, one that you will display and one that you will use
to select a secret shape from. On one day, focus on two-dimensional shapes. On an-
other day, use three-dimensional shapes. Give children time to explore the displayed
shapes, asking them to run their fingers along the sides and around the corners (for
two-dimensional shapes). Secretly select one of the shapes from your set and place
it in a box or a bag. Use a box with holes cut into the sides or a bag that has elastic
around the top—or you could use a long sock. The idea is for children to be able to
get their hands into the box, bag, or sock without being able to peek! Have the chil-
dren feel the secret shape and try to guess what shape it is. You can have one child
feel the shape and describe it to others so that they can decide which of the displayed
shapes it matches.
341
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Cut squares or rectangles made from card stock into smaller pieces.
Children can use these to recompose the squares or rectangles.
Children can use pattern blocks to compose and decompose different shapes.
Easy
Medium
Full-sized
outlines
Fit in the
tangram pieces.
Hardest
Outlines are
Each of these to scale but
shapes can be made much smaller.
Dog using all seven pieces.
342
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
“Patch Tool” at NCTM’s Illuminations site provides a computerized environment where children can
compose shapes using different pattern blocks (http://illuminations.nctm.org/ActivityDetail.aspx?ID=27).
It also provides five outlines of different designs where children have to decide which shapes are used
to complete the designs. The National Library of Virtual Manipulatives has a tangram applet with a set
of fourteen puzzle figures that can be made using all seven of the pieces (http://nlvm.usu.edu/en/nav
/category_g_1_t_3.html). The e-version of tangrams has the advantage of motivation and the fact that you
must be much more deliberate in arranging the shapes.
The geoboard is one of the best devices for creating Figure 8 Shapes on geoboards BLM
two-dimensional shapes. Here are just two of many pos- (see Blackline Masters 28–29).
sible activities appropriate for level 0.
Teach children from the very beginning to copy their geoboard designs on geoboard
paper (use Blackline Masters 28–29). To help children transfer their designs to paper, sug- BLM
gest that they first mark the dots on the paper for the corners of their shape by identifying
the location of the pegs (e.g., “second row, first peg”). With the corners identified, it is much
343
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Figure 9
Decomposing and composing shapes (see Blackline Masters 28–29). BLM
Three triangles that are Four triangles What is the fewest Fill with three
all the same number of triangles rectangles that are
that will fit this? all the same
One version is to use multiple shapes to create
a larger shape. Alternatively, start with a shape
and cut into smaller shapes.
easier for them to draw lines between corners to make the shapes. These drawings can be
placed in groups for classification and discussion, made into booklets illustrating a new idea
that is being discussed, or sent home to families.
The NCTM e-Standards provides electronic geoboard activities entitled “Investigating the Concept of a
Triangle and Properties of Polygons” (www.nctm.org/standards/content.aspx?menu_id=1155&id=26867).
Children can select and delete bands and select and delete vertices as they create triangles and other
polygons. The geoboard applet from the National Library of Virtual Manipulatives (http://nlvm.usu.edu
/en/nav/category_g_1_t_3.html) is essentially the same kind of set up with some additional features.
“Shape Tools” at NCTM’s Illuminations site allows children to decompose pattern blocks and then compose
them into different shapes (http://illuminations.nctm.org/ActivityDetail.aspx?ID=35). Children can use
“Shape Cutter” at the same site to create and decompose a wide variety of shapes and then compose
them into new shapes (http://illuminations.nctm.org/ActivityDetail.aspx?ID=72).
344
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Building Shapes
Standards for
Building activities at level 0 help children pay attention to the properties of shapes. For Mathematical Practice
building two-dimensional models, use an activity like Activity 6, “Build It,” but do not pro-
vide drawings as models for children. Challenge them to create two or more examples of a 3 Construct
viable arguments
target shape and explain why they are that shape. See whether others in the class agree with ◀
and critique the
their explanations. reasoning of others
Building three-dimensional shapes is a little more difficult
than building two-dimensional shapes. A variety of commercial
materials permit fairly creative construction of geometric sol- Figure 10 Start with a shape and then draw a
ids (e.g., Polydron and the Zome System). The following are segment to divide the shape into two new shapes.
three approaches to making handmade models.
• Plastic coffee stirrers with modeling clay or pipe cleaners. Plastic
stirrers can be cut into different lengths and used to build
the edges of the three-dimensional shape. To connect cor-
ners, use small balls of clay or insert pipe cleaners cut into Begin with: 3 sides Begin with: 4 sides
2-inch lengths into the ends of the stirrers. Two new shapes: 7 sides Two new shapes: 7 sides
Figure 11
Large skeletal structures and special shapes can be built with tightly rolled newspaper.
345
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
many bridges, in the long booms of construction cranes, in gates, and in the structural parts
of buildings. Discuss why this may be so.
Shape Description
Symmetrical, nonsymmetrical Shapes may have one or more lines of symmetry and may or
may not have rotational symmetry. These concepts will require
more detailed investigation.
Triangles
Classified by sides
Equilateral All sides are congruent.
Isosceles At least two sides are congruent.
Scalene No two sides are congruent.
Classified by angles
Right Has a right angle.
Acute All angles are smaller than a right angle.
Obtuse One angle is larger than a right angle.
Convex Quadrilaterals
346
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Triangles
Convex Quadrilaterals
Kite
Isosceles
*Some definitions of trapezoid specify only one pair of parallel sides, in which case parallelograms
would not be trapezoids. The University of Chicago School Mathematics Project (UCSMP) uses
the “at least one pair” definition, meaning that parallelograms and rectangles are trapezoids.
347
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
this type of subrelationship. They may quite correctly list all the properties of a square, a
rhombus, and a rectangle and still might identify a square as a “nonrhombus” or a “non-
rectangle.” You can suggest to children to consider how one of their classmates can belong
to more than one club or team. A square is an example of a quadrilateral that belongs to
two other clubs.
Important and interesting shapes and relationships also exist in three dimensions. Table 2
describes classifications of solids. Figure 13 shows examples of cylinders and prisms. Note
that prisms are defined here as a special category of cylinder—a cylinder with a polygon for
a base (Zwillinger, 2011). Figure 14 shows a comparable grouping of cones and pyramids.
Shape Description
Sphere and “egglike” shapes Shapes with no edges and no vertices (corners).
Shapes with edges but no vertices (e.g., a flying saucer).
Shapes with vertices but no edges (e.g., a football).
Polyhedron Shapes made of all faces (a face is a flat surface of a solid). If all surfaces are faces, all the edges will
be straight lines.
Some combination of faces and rounded surfaces (cylinders are examples, but this is not a definition
of a cylinder).
Shapes with curved surfaces.
Shapes with and without edges and with and without vertices.
Faces can be parallel. Parallel faces lie in places that never intersect.
Cylinders
Cylinder Two congruent, parallel faces called bases. Lines joining corresponding points on the two bases are
always parallel. These parallel lines are called elements of the cylinder.
Right cylinder A cylinder with elements perpendicular to the bases. A cylinder that is not a right cylinder is an oblique
cylinder.
Prism A cylinder with polygons for bases. All prisms are special cases of cylinders (Zwillinger, 2011).
Cones
Cone A solid with exactly one face and a vertex that is not on the face. Straight lines (elements) can be
drawn from any point on the edge of the base to the vertex. The base may be any shape at all. The
vertex need not be directly over the base.
Pyramid Cone with a polygon for a base. All faces joining the vertex are triangles. Pyramids are named by the
shape of the base: triangular pyramid, square pyramid, octagonal pyramid, and so on. All pyramids are
special cases of cones (Zwillinger, 2011).
348
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Special Cylinders
Cylinders have two parallel faces, and parallel lines join corresponding points on these faces.
If the parallel faces are polygons, the cylinder can be called a prism.
Cones
Special cones—pyramids
Not cones
Cones—not pyramids
Cones and cones with a polygon base (pyramids) all have straight-line
elements joining every point of the base with the vertex. (Yes, a
pyramid is just a special type of cone.)
Many textbooks limit the definition of cylinders to circular cylinders. These books do
not have special names for other cylinders. Under that definition, the prism is not a special
case of a cylinder. This points to the fact that definitions are conventions, and not all con-
ventions are universally agreed upon.
349
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
If you keep track of solutions to the challenges in the last activity, there is an added
possibility of creating classes of shapes possessing certain properties that may result in defi-
nitions of new classes of shapes. The activity can also include impossible tasks, such as a
four-sided shape with exactly three right angles. Also, a triangle with three congruent sides
(equilateral) is not possible on a geoboard.
Use a projector to show a collection of shapes that have one or more properties in
common and another collection of shapes that do not have this commonality, such
as in Figure 15. For your first collection be certain that you have allowed for all possible vari-
Standards for
Mathematical Practice ables. In Figure 15, for example, a square is included in the set of rhombi. Similarly, choose
nonexamples to be as close to the positive examples as is necessary to help with an accurate
1 Make sense definition. The third or mixed set should also include those nonexamples with which chil-
of problems and ◀ dren are most likely to be confused. Children should justify their choices. Note that the use
persevere in
solving them of examples and nonexamples is particularly helpful for children with disabilities.
350
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
e Lerane
rE
fo
group will need a set of the Assorted Triangles
rs
e Lerane
fo rs
351
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
has to apply to all of the shapes on their sheet. They will need tools such as index cards
(to check right angles, to compare side lengths) and tracing paper (for identifying angle
congruence). Encourage children to say the words “at least” when describing how many
of something; for example, “rectangles have at least two sides that are equal” because
squares—included in the rectangles—have four. Groups then share their lists with the
class and eventually a class list for each category of shape will be developed.
Standards for This last activity may take two or three days. Share lists beginning with parallelograms,
Mathematical Practice
then rhombi, then rectangles, and finally squares. Have one group present its list. Then oth-
3 Construct ers who worked on the same shape should add to or subtract from it. The class must agree
◀
viable arguments with everything that is put on the list. As new relationships come up in this presentation-and-
and critique the discussion period, you can introduce proper terminology. For example, if an angle is larger
reasoning of others
than a right angle, then it is called obtuse. Other terms such as acute, parallel, and so on can be
clarified as you help children write their descriptions.
Compare the last activity, “Property Lists for Quadrilaterals,” with Activity 7,
“Shape Sorts.” In “Shape Sorts,” the objects of children’s thought are the very shapes
that are in front of them. The results of that activity are collections or classifications
of shapes. For young children, these classifications will be nonstandard groupings that
make sense to the children—fat, tall, pointy, looks like houses, and so on. Soon they
will also construct standard groupings that have standard names—squares, triangles,
rectangles, prisms, and so on—as you supply the appropriate names as the collections
are developed.
In “Property Lists for Quadrilaterals,” the children only see a small collection of shapes at
the top of their paper. However, the object of their thinking is the entire class of shapes for which
these few are representative. In “Property Lists for Quadrilaterals” the object of thinking is the
very type of thing (a class of shapes) that was the product of thinking in “Shape Sorts” at level 0.
352
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Clements and Sarama (2009) recommend having children use computer programs and
applets to explore slides, flips, and turns of objects because children have to purposefully
select the screen tools to perform the geometric motion, which makes the motions more
explicit to children. Some applets for exploring transformations are listed in the next Tech-
nology Note.
e Lerane
rE
Flag and then copy the mirror image on the backs of these copies (see Figure 18).
fo rs
You want the back image to match the front image when held to the light. Cut
off the excess paper to leave a square. Alternatively, you can make the same de-
sign on squares of card stock using a marker. Give all children a two-sided Mo-
BLM tion Flag. It might be helpful, especially for children with disabilities, to give
them two Motion Flags, one to leave at the original position and the other to
manipulate.
Demonstrate each of the possible motions. A slide is simply a slide. The figure does not
rotate or turn over. Demonstrate turns. (With young children, use only clockwise turns for
this activity.) Similarly, demonstrate a horizontal flip (top
goes to bottom) and a vertical flip (left goes to right). For
all children, especially ELLs, it is important that these dem-
onstrations include explicit practice with the terms and that
Figure 18
Motion Flag is used to show slides, flips, BLM
visuals are posted for reference. Practice by having every-
and turns (see Blackline Masters 42–43).
one start with their Motion Flag in the same orientation. As
you announce one of the moves, children slide, flip, or turn
their Motion Flag accordingly.
Then display two Motion Flags side by side in any ori-
entation. The task is to decide what motion or combina-
tion of motions will get the flag on the left to match the
flag on the right. Children use their own flag to work out
a solution. Test the solutions that children offer. If both
flags are in the same position, call that a slide.
353
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Once children understand how to use the Motion Flag, they can work in pairs. They
begin with their Motion Flags in the same position. One child then changes his or her
Motion Flag and challenges the other child to say what motion is required to make the
two Motion Flags match. The solution is then tested and the roles reversed.
At first, children will be confused when they can’t get their Motion Flag into the new
position with one move. This causes an excellent problem. Don’t be too quick to sug-
gest that it may take two moves. If flips across each of the two diagonals are added to the
motions along with vertical and horizontal flips, the Motion Flag can assume any new
position in exactly one move.
Line Symmetry
Transformations for level 1 thinkers should involve the initial development of line sym-
metry. If a shape can be folded on a line so that the two halves match exactly, then it is said
to have line symmetry (or mirror symmetry). Notice that the fold line is actually a line
of reflection—the portion of the shape on one side of the line is reflected onto the other
side—demonstrating a connection between line symmetry and transformations (flips).
One way to introduce line symmetry to children is to show examples and nonexamples
using an “all-of-these” or “none-of-these” approach as in Figure 15. As another possibility,
consider the following example that creates a line of symmetry: Fold a sheet of paper in half.
Start somewhere along the fold and cut out a shape of your choosing. Open the paper and
describe what you notice.
A third way to introduce line symmetry is to have children create designs with mirror
images, as in the following activity.
Children need a plain sheet of paper with a straight line through the middle. Using no
more than six to eight pattern blocks, they make a design completely on one side of the
line that touches the line in some way. When the one side is finished, children try to make
the mirror image of their design on the other side of the line. After it is built, they use a
mirror (or a Mira, a red plastic image reflector) to check their work. With the mirror sit-
ting perpendicular on the line, they should see exactly the same image as they see when
they lift the mirror.
Children often refer to the ideas of line and rotational symmetry when working with
pattern blocks and other designs. They may observe, “When I turn the (equilateral) triangle
a little, it fits back on top of itself” or “I can fold my butterfly drawing and it matches!” Capi-
talize on these opportunities to discuss the idea of symmetry so your children have a strong
foundation for more formal study that will come later.
354
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Prepare some simple maps of your playground area that identify some big landmarks
and different paths. Give different groups of children different maps. Children will use
their map to move from one location to another to find a hidden treasure. On each map,
highlight the starting point and the ending point and draw a path along the route you
want children to take.
After children have some experience using simple maps, having them create their own
maps will help them notice landmarks and how those landmarks are positioned as they are
challenged to identify a path that connects these landmarks.
The maps young children create can vary greatly due to individual children’s drawing
skills. You do not want children to get bogged down in providing a lot of detail in represent-
ing landmarks. Encourage them to use simple drawings or provide cutout pictures of the
landmarks that they can glue to their maps so they can attend more to the positions of the
landmarks in relation to each other and the path.
355
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Next, children can begin to use simple coordinates to describe positions. By ex-
tending the grid in “Hidden Positions” to a 6 by 6 grid, the need arises for a system to
BLM label positions. Use a simple coordinate grid like the one shown in Figure 20 (see Black-
line Master 44). Explain how to use two numbers to designate an intersection point on
the grid. The first number tells how far to move to the right. The second number tells
how far to move up. Initially use the words along with the numbers: “right 3 and up 0.”
Be sure to include 0 in your introduction. Select a point on the grid and have children decide
Figure 20 what two numbers name that point. If your point is at (2,4) and children incorrectly say “four,
A simple coordinate
two,” then simply indicate where the point is that they named.
grid. The X is at (3,2)
The next activity explores the notion of different paths on a grid.
and the O is at (1,3).
Use the grid to play
“Three in a Row” (like
“Tic-Tac-Toe”). Put
marks on intersections, Activity 22 PATHS
not spaces.
On a sheet of 2-cm grid paper (see Blackline Master 30), mark two dif-
4 BLM ferent points A and B as shown in Figure 21. Using a projector or floor
tiles, demonstrate how to describe a path from A to B. For the points
3 in the figure, one path is “up 5 and right 6.” Another path might be
“right 2, up 2, right 2, up 3, right 2.” Count the length of each path. As long as
2 you always move toward the target point (in this case either right or up), the path
lengths will always be the same. Here they are 11 units long. Children draw three
1 paths on their papers from A to B using different-colored crayons. For each path
they write directions that describe their paths. They should check the lengths of
0 each path. Ask the children, for example, “What is the greatest number of turns
0 1 2 3 4 that you can make in your path? What is the smallest number? Where would A and
Coordinate Grid B have to be in order to get there with no turns?”
356
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Figure 21
Coloring in different paths
on a grid. What is the
NCTM’s e-Example 4.3 (found at www.nctm.org/standards/content.aspx?menu_id=1155&id=26868) is fewest number of turns
similar to “Paths” but offers some additional challenges. Children move a ladybug by listing directions needed to get from A to
B? The most?
that will hide the ladybug beneath a leaf. When the directions are complete, the ladybug is set in motion
to follow them. The ladybug can also be directed to draw shapes such as a rectangle or to travel through
B
mazes. Creating directions and predicting their outcome will help children develop ideas about navigation
and location as they hone their visualization skills.
If you add a coordinate system to the grid in “Paths,” children can describe their paths
with coordinates: For example, one of the paths in Figure 21 can be described as follows: A
Use a half-sheet of paper that will easily fit on a projector. Fold it in half. Children make a
sketch of the paper when it is opened, showing a line for the fold. With the paper folded,
cut notches in one or two sides and/or cut off one or two corners. You can also use a
paper punch to make a hole or two. While still folded, place the paper on the overhead
showing the notches and holes. The folded edge should be to the left (see Figure 22). The
task is for children to draw the notches and holes that they think will appear when you
open the paper.
357
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Step 1: Step 2:
Fold paper Cut notches
Fold
Step 3: Step 4:
Show on projector Children draw their predictions
The activity “Notches and Holes” can be modified for use with children who are at any
level of geometric thought. To introduce this activity, begin with only one fold and only one
cut. Adding additional cuts and additional folds increases the challenge. Having to predict
the result helps build visualization skills and is also very motivating!
Draw some simple sketches of figures that will be easy for children to reproduce.
Some examples are shown in Figure 23. Display one of the sketches for about
5 seconds. Then have children attempt to reproduce it on their own. Show the same
358
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
figure again for a few seconds and allow children to modify their drawings. Repeat Standards for
with additional figures. Mathematical Practice
Ask children to share how they thought about the figure or to describe in words 7 Look for and
what helped them remember what they saw. As children learn to verbally describe what ◀
make use of structure
they see, their visual memory will improve. Have children with disabilities identify the
displayed figure from a set of figures that look alike.
Figure 23
Visualization skills can also be developed through the map-building activities that are Examples of designs to
used to help children learn about location. Consider the following task that you can pose just use in the “Quick
after your class returns to the classroom from another location in the Images” activity.
building:
Imagine that we are walking back to our classroom from the gym.
What are some of the landmarks that we pass along the way?
After children have had time to visualize the path between the gym
and their classroom, make a list of the landmarks that children thought
of and take it with the class the next time you walk that path.
Activity 25 PENTOMINOES
A pentomino is a shape formed by joining five squares as if cut from a square grid. Each Standards for
Mathematical Practice
square must have at least one side in common with another. Provide children with five
square tiles and a sheet of square grid paper for recording. Challenge them to see how 1 Make sense
many different pentomino shapes they can find. Shapes that are flips or turns of other ◀ of problems and
persevere in
shapes are not considered different. Do not tell children how many pentomino shapes
solving them
there are. Good discussions will come from deciding if some shapes are really different
and if all shapes have been found.
Once children have decided that there are just 12 pentominoes (see Figure 24), glue the
grids with the children’s pentominoes onto card stock and let them cut out the 12 shapes.
Following is a variety of additional activities that can be done with pentominoes.
359
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
Figure 24 It is also fun to explore the number of shapes that can be made
from six equilateral triangles or from four 45-degree right triangles
There are 12 different pentomino shapes.
(halves of squares) (see Figure 25). With the right triangles, sides that
touch must be the same length. How many of each of these “ominoes”
do you think there are?
Another aspect of visualization for young children is to be able
to think about solid shapes in terms of their faces or sides. For these
activities you will need to make “face cards” by tracing around the
different faces of a three-dimensional shape, making either all faces
on one card or a set of separate cards with one face per card (see
Figure 26).
There are two versions of the task: Given a face card, find the corresponding solid, or
given a solid, find the face card. With a collection of single-face cards, children can select
the cards that go with a particular solid. For another variation, stack all of the single-face
cards for one solid face down. Turn them up one at a time as clues to finding the solid.
Visualization involves creating and manipulating mental images. You can use activities
in the other geometry categories (e.g., shapes and properties, transformation, location) to
help children improve their visualization skills simply by asking them to recall and describe
a hidden shape, describe how a shape would look from a different viewpoint, and predict
what will happen before they manipulate (e.g., decompose, compose, flip, slide, turn) shapes.
Look for those opportunities as you plan lessons and as you interact with your children.
Figure 25 Figure 26
Finding all possible shapes. Matching face cards with solid shapes.
Shape Sorts
Content and Task Decisions • Record a list of the ideas children share to describe
their shapes and draw a sketch next to it as a reminder.
Grade Level: K–2
• To introduce the names of common shapes or impor- • Make sure the rule for a given target shape is written
tant properties (when and if the opportunity arises down near the target shape so that children with dis-
within the activity) abilities can refer to it during the session.
361
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
362
Developing Geometric Reasoning and Concepts
References
Clements, D. H., & Battista, M. T. (1992). National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Geometry and spatial reasoning. In D. A. Grouws (2000). Principles and standards for school
(Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.
and learning (pp. 420–464). Old Tappan, NJ:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Macmillan.
(2006). Curriculum focal points for prekindergarten
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J., (2000a). Young through grade 8 mathematics: A quest for coherence.
children’s ideas about geometric shapes. Teaching Reston, VA: Author.
Children Mathematics, 6(8), 482–488.
Sowder, J. T., & Wearne, D. (2006). What do we
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2000b). The know about eighth‐grade student achievement?
earliest geometry. Teaching Children Mathematics, Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 11(6),
7(2), 82–86. 285–293.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2009). Learning and Zwillinger, D. (Ed.) (2011). Standard mathematical
teaching early math: The learning trajectories approach. tables and formulae (32nd ed.). Boca Raton, FL:
New York, NY: Routledge. CRC Press.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J., (2000a). Young
children’s ideas about geometric shapes. Teaching
Children Mathematics, 6(8), 482–488.
363
This page intentionally left blank
Helping Children Use Data
From Chapter 17 of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
365
Helping Children Use Data
using picture and bar graphs. Second graders extend their work to generating and repre-
senting measurement data on line plots. The focus of the learning at these and every grade
level should be on ways to use, present, and interpret data in the context of real questions.
Statistics and mathematics are two different fields; however, statistical questions are often
asked in assessments with questions that are mathematical in nature rather than statistical.
The harm in this is that children are not focusing on statistical reasoning, as shown by the
following exemplars from Scheaffer (2006).
2. Table 1 gives the times each girl has recorded for seven trials of the 100‐meter
dash this year. Only one girl may compete in the upcoming track meet. Which
girl would you select for the meet and why?
So, which of these problems involves statistical reasoning? Both? Neither? As explained
by Schaeffer, only the second one is statistical in nature. The first requires computing with
multiplication—mathematical thinking, not statistical thinking. The second question is sta-
tistical in nature because the situation requires analysis—graphs or averages might be used
to determine a solution. The mathematics here is basic; the focus is on statistics. Also notice
the context is central to responding to the second question, which is an indication that it is
statistical reasoning.
Runner Race
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Suzie 15.2 14.8 15.0 14.7 14.3 14.5 14.5
Tanisha 15.8 15.7 15.4 15.0 14.8 14.6 14.5
Dara 15.6 15.5 14.8 15.1 14.5 14.7 14.5
366
Helping Children Use Data
Just as learning addition involves much more Figure 1 Process of doing statistics.
than the procedure for combining, doing statistics is
much more than the process of creating a bar graph Step 1: Formulate Questions
or line plot. To meaningfully engage children in learn- Clarify the problem at hand.
ing and doing statistics, they should be involved in Formulate one (or more) questions that can be answered with data.
the full process, from asking and defining questions
Step 2: Collect Data
to collecting and analyzing data to interpreting data. Design a plan to collect appropriate data.
This chapter is organized around this process, which Employ the plan to collect the data.
is presented in Figure 1.
Step 3: Analyze Data
Select appropriate graphical and numerical methods.
Use these methods to analyze the data.
Formulating Questions
Step 4: Interpret Results
The first goal in the Data Analysis and Probability Interpret the analysis.
standard of Principles and Standards for School Math- Relate the interpretation to the original question.
ematics says that children should “formulate questions
Source: Franklin, C. A., Kader, G., Mewborn, D., Moreno, J., Peck, R., Perry, M., &
that can be addressed with data and collect, organize, Scheaffer, R. (2005). Guidelines for Assessment and Instruction in Statistics Ed-
and display relevant data to answer them” (National ucation: A Pre‐K‐12 Curriculum Framework. Alexandria, VA: American Statis-
Council of Teachers of Mathematics, 2000, p. 48). tical Association. Reprinted with permission. Copyright 2005 by the American
Statistical Association. All rights reserved.
Notice that data collection should be for a purpose—
to answer a question—just as in the real world. Then the analysis of data should add infor-
mation about some aspect of our world. This process of gathering data to answer questions
and make informed decisions is what political pollsters, advertising agencies, market re-
searchers, census takers, wildlife managers, and hosts of others do.
Whether the question is teacher initiated or student initiated, children should engage
in conversations about how well‐defined the question is. For example, if the teacher asks,
“How many brothers and sisters do you have?” there may be a need to discuss half siblings.
If children want to know how many shoes each classmate owns, questions may arise as to
whether they should count bedroom slippers and flip‐flops.
When children formulate the questions they want to ask, the data they gather be-
come more and more meaningful. How they organize the data and the techniques for
analyzing them have a purpose. For example, one class of children might gather data
concerning which cafeteria foods are most often thrown in the garbage. As a result of
these efforts, certain items could be removed from the regular menu. The experience can
illustrate to children the power of organized data, and it can help them get food that they
like better!
Often the need to gather data will come from the class naturally in the course of
discussion or from questions arising in other content areas. Science, of course, is full of
measurements and thus abounds in data analysis possibilities. Social studies is also full of op-
portunities to pose questions requiring data collection. The next few sections suggest some
ideas related to these content areas as well as additional ideas.
367
Helping Children Use Data
• Numbers: Number of pets, siblings, or letters in name; hours watching TV, hours of
sleep, or hours spent on the computer
• Measures: Height, arm span, length of foot, long‐jump distance, time to run around
the swing set on the playground, minutes spent traveling to school, daily temperature,
shadow length
368
Helping Children Use Data
Comparisons
Another type of progression from the questions children ask about themselves and their class-
mates is to consider if they as a class are alike or different from other groups. Do other sec-
ond‐grade children spend the same amount of time watching
TV or like the same foods as they do? How much taller are
children in the next grade or two grades ahead of them?
Comparisons can also be made between your own class If your children have “buddies” in the upper grades, they can
and selected groups of adults to which the children have ac- collect data from their buddies.
cess, such as parents or faculty.
To further expand your children’s perspective, you might explore ways that your class
can compare themselves or their data with similar classes in other places in the state, other
states, or perhaps even in a foreign country. This can open up not just a source of interesting
data but also a way for your children to see beyond their own localities.
Data Collection
There are two main types of data—categorical and numerical. Categorical data refer to in-
formation about things that can be grouped by labels such as favorite after‐school activity,
colors of cars in the school parking lot, and the most popular name to give the class guinea
pig. Categorical data are ordered arbitrarily—in other words, the bars in a bar graph could
be put in any arrangement.
Numerical data, on the other hand, are ordered numeri-
cally, such as on a number line, and could include fractions
Note that the word data is plural—hence, we say “data are.”
or decimals. This kind of data includes how many miles to
The singular form of the word is datum.
school, the temperature in your town over a one‐week period,
or the weight of the children’s backpacks.
Collecting Data
Gathering data is not easy for children, especially young children. A teacher asked her first
graders to gather data on “Are you 6?” After hearing the request, 18 eager children began asking
others in the class if they were 6 and tallying yes or no responses. Eventually they realized that
they had no idea whom they had asked more than once or whom they had not asked at all. This
provided an excellent entry into a discussion about how statisticians gather data. Have your
children brainstorm ways to gather the needed data in an organized manner. One kindergarten
teacher asked her children to think of an organized way they could gather data from their class-
mates about their favorite flavors of ice cream. These kindergartners decided a class list would
help them keep track of who had been asked and what their response had been (Cook, 2008).
Data can also be collected through observation. This creates a shared context for chil-
dren in that they all will be a part of observing phenomena. For example, set up a bird feeder
outside the classroom window and collect data at different times during the day to either
count the number or type of birds. Children can also collect observational data on field trips
(Mokros & Wright, 2009) and at evening or weekend activities with their families.
369
Helping Children Use Data
is also an excellent and engaging resource. Children can tally words in a repeating verse
like “Hickory, Dickory, Dock” (Niezgoda & Moyer‐Packenham, 2005). Similarly, books like
Goodnight Moon (Brown, 2005) and Green Eggs and Ham (Seuss, 1960) have many repeated
words or phrases.
Children may be interested in facts about another country as a result of a social studies
unit or a country in the news. Olympic records in various events over the years or data re-
lated to environmental issues are other examples of topics around which children’s questions
may be formulated. For these and hundreds of other questions, data can be found on the
Web. Following are a few websites with a lot of interesting data.
• State Data Map (http://illuminations.nctm.org/ActivityDetail.aspx?ID=151) is a source
that displays state data on population, land area, political representation, gasoline use,
and more.
• Olympic Records (www.olympic.org/medallists-results) provides information about
medalists in every Olympic event since 1896.
• Internet Movie Database (www.imdb.com) offers information about movies of all
genres.
• Google Earth (http://earth.google.com) allows you to take a virtual trip to explore cities
and regions in any place in the world.
Data Analysis
Data analysis begins with organizing the data in a meaningful way, such as sorting or graph-
ing, with the resulting visual providing a kind of summary for the data. The data are then
analyzed with a focus on how the information answers the question that provided the pur-
pose for collecting the data.
Classification
Initially young children tend to not use categories to represent data and instead simply
put the data in one list (Clements & Sarama, 2009). For example, if children were asked to
represent the class data about how children arrived at school, some children may simply
list each child and their response. Classification involves making decisions about how to
categorize things, a basic activity that is fundamental to data analysis. In order to formulate
questions and decide how to represent data that have been gathered, decisions must be made
about how things might be categorized. For example, children might group farm animals
by number of legs; by type of product they provide; by those that work, provide food, or
are pets; by size or color; by the type of food they eat; and so on. Each of these groupings
is based on a different attribute of the animals. Those decisions about how things might be
categorized need to become an emphasis in the early grades.
According to the Curriculum Focal Points (National Council of Teachers of Mathematics,
2006) and the Common Core State Standards (CCSSO, 2010), classification by attributes is a
topic for pre‐K–K children. Attribute activities are explicitly designed to develop flexible
reasoning about the characteristics of data.
370
Helping Children Use Data
Figure 2
A teacher‐made attribute set. Woozle cards can be duplicated on card stock, quickly colored in two colors, laminated,
and cut into individual cards (see Blackline Master 49).
Woozle
Cards
(Blackline Masters)
and gender are attributes of children. Each attribute has a number of different values: for
example, blond, brown, or red (for the attribute of hair color); tall or short (for height);
male or female (for gender). A teacher‐made attribute set is displayed in Figure 2 (see also
Blackline Master 49). BLM
Commercially available attribute blocks are sets of 60 plastic attribute pieces with
each piece having four attributes: color (red, yellow, blue), shape (circle, triangle, rectangle,
square, hexagon), size (big, little), and thickness (thick, thin). The specific values, number of
values, or number of attributes that a set may have is not important.
Initially, do attribute activities by sitting in a large circle on the floor where all children
can see and have access to the materials to be sorted. The following activity is a simple Venn
diagram activity that pre‐K–K children will enjoy.
371
Helping Children Use Data
Figure 3 A three‐loop activity with attribute pieces. As shown in Figure 3, the labels need not be re-
stricted to single attributes. If a piece does not fit in
s any region, it is placed outside of all of the loops.
cate e
Indi triangl As children progress, introduce labels for nega-
NO T
Da tive attributes such as “not red” or “not small.” Also
gr rk important is the eventual use of and and or con-
ay
nectives to form two‐value rules such as “red and
square” and “big or happy.” This use of and, or,
and not significantly widens children’s classification
schemes.
An engaging and challenging activity is to infer
how things have been classified when the loops are
Smile drawn not labeled. The following activities require children
on card to make and test conjectures about how things are
being classified.
e Lerane
Mathematical Practice
rE
rs
on an attribute such as “blue jeans” or “stripes on clothing” but do not tell your
fo
1 Make sense rule to the class. Silently look at one child at a time and move the child to the left or right
of problems and ◀ according to this secret rule. After a number of children have been sorted, have the next
persevere in child come up and ask children to predict in which group he or she belongs.
solving them
Before the rule is articulated, continue the activity for a while so that others in the
class will have an opportunity to determine the rule. The same activity can be done with
virtually any materials that can be sorted, such as children’s shoes, shells, or buttons.
Encourage ELLs to use their native language and English to describe the rule.
e Lerane
rE
rs
two of the cards face down, one on each loop. Ask children to select an attribute
fo
piece for you to place. For ELLs and children with disabilities, provide a list of the
labels with pictures and/or translations for each as a reference. Begin to sort pieces
according to the hidden rules. As you sort, have children try to determine what the labels
are for each of the loops. Let children who think they have guessed the labels try to place
a piece in the proper loop but avoid having them guess the labels aloud. Children who
think they know the labels can also be asked to “play teacher” and respond to the
guesses of the others. Point out that one way to test an idea about the labels is to select
a piece that you think might go in a particular section. Wait to turn the label cards up
until most children have figured out the rule.
372
Helping Children Use Data
Figure 4 Guess the rule that was used to sort these bugs.
Backyard Bugs
Fly Don't fly
to sort by relevant attributes and have children guess the sorting rule. Following are
additional ideas that can be used for classification activities related to social studies and
science.
For social studies, children could sort and eventually graph data about:
• Places in the United States and outside the United States
• Country or state of origin of classmates
• Past, present, and future events
• Goods (e.g., bread, milk, apples, pants, socks, shoes) and services (e.g., waiting tables at
a restaurant, mowing yards, repairing cars)
• Continents and oceans
For science, children might classify and graph data using the following attributes:
• Sensory descriptors for items such as sweet/sour/bitter/salty, rough/smooth, hard/soft,
cold/warm, loud/soft, high/low, and bright/dull
• Relative size or weight of a collection of objects (big/little, large/small, heavy/light,
wide/thin, long/short)
• Position of objects found in the classroom (over/under, in/out, above/below, left/right)
• Speed (fast/slow) of different animals (e.g., turtles, ants, snakes, cheetahs) or transporta-
tion (e.g., car, bicycle, airplane, walking)
• Materials or objects that float or sink in water
• Weather observations (e.g., sunny, cloudy, raining, snowing)
• Materials in the recycling bin (plastics, aluminum, glass, paper)
• Substances that will or will not dissolve in water
373
Helping Children Use Data
• After gathering data, do children use categories to sort the data or do they focus on the particulars (e.g.,
list each child in the class and their favorite ice cream)?
• Are children able to place items into categories once they have been identified? Do they use valid rea-
sons for placing items in a category?
• Do children contribute to ideas for classification schemes?
• Do children understand that different classification schemes will result in a different organization of
the items being sorted? For example, children’s names may be sorted by number of letters, and they
can also be sorted by which part of the alphabet they begin with—first half or second half. These two
schemes would result in different classifications.
• Do children correctly use an overlapping category—items that belong to two different groups at the
same time?
• Can children correctly use the logical connectors and and or and the adjective not when creating
classifications?
Graphical Representations
A graph provides a visual image of the data that cannot be captured in other forms. The loops
used with the attribute materials provide a first form of graphical representation. The class can
“graph” data about themselves by placing information in loops with labels. A graph of “Our
Pets” might consist of a picture of each child’s pet or favorite stuffed animal (in lieu of a pet)
that can be affixed to a wall display showing how the pets were classified. Different classifica-
tions would produce different graphs. For example, the graphs could show pets by the number
of legs; by fur, feathers, or scales; by how long they have been with the family; and so on.
How data are organized should be directly related to the question that caused you to
collect the data in the first place. For example, suppose that children want to know how
many pockets they have on their clothing (Russell, 2006). Data collection involves each
child in the room counting his or her pockets.
374
Helping Children Use Data
A bar graph made with one bar for every child will certainly tell how many pock-
ets each child has. However, is it the best way to answer the question? If the data
were categorized by number of pockets, then a graph showing the number of children
with no pockets, one pocket, two pockets, three pockets, and so on will easily show
which number of pockets is most common and how the number of pockets varies across
the class.
Children should be involved in deciding how they want to represent their data. How-
ever, for children with little experience with the various methods of picturing data, you will
need to introduce options. Graph the data using these options and facilitate a discussion
with the class to decide which method might be best and why.
Once children have made the display, they should discuss the meanings of various parts Standards for
of the representation. Analyzing data that are numerical (number of pockets) versus cat- Mathematical Practice
egorical (color of socks) is an added challenge for children as they struggle to make sense of
6 Attend to
the graphs. If, for example, the graph has seven stickers above the five, children may think ◀ precision
that five people have seven pockets or seven people have five pockets. Children should be
challenged to determine how this issue can be remedied.
Often young children have difficulty distinguishing between the data and the larger
event from which the data were collected. They can think of the data as triggers or
“pointers” to the event, not as abstracting specific information about the event (Konold,
Higgins, Russell, & Khalil, 2004). For example, instead of recognizing that more children
in the class have two pockets than five pockets, they may describe how they can put their Standards for
Mathematical Practice
lunch money in their front pocket or that they counted their pockets by twos. Focusing
on the meaning of the symbols used in a graphical representation of the data as well as 1 Make sense
having children make comparisons between different groups (e.g., How many pockets ◀ of problems and
do fifth graders have?) can help make the intent of the graph more salient for children persevere in
solving them
(Russell, 2006).
The goal is to help children see that graphs tell information and that different types
of representations tell different things about the same data. The value of having children
actually construct their own graphs is not so much that they learn the techniques as that
they are personally invested in the data and that they learn how a graph conveys infor-
mation. Once a graph is constructed, the most important activity is discussing what it
tells the people who see it, especially those who were not involved in making the graph.
Discussions about graphs of real data that the children have themselves been involved in
gathering will help them analyze and interpret other graphs that they see in books and
newspapers and on TV.
What we should not do is only concentrate on the details of graph construction.
Your objectives should focus on the issues of analysis and communication, which are
much more important than the technique! In the real world, technology will take care of
details.
Children should construct graphs or charts by hand and with technology. First, en-
courage them to make graphs that make sense to them and that they feel communicate the
information they wish to convey. The intent is to get the children involved in accurately
communicating a message about their data.
375
Helping Children Use Data
Considering the complexities of graphs and the results of various research studies, Friel,
Curcio, and Bright (2001) recommend the following progression for the introduction of
different kinds of graphs in grades pre‐K–2:
• Object graphs
• Picture graphs
Figure 5 • Line plots
A graph mat for • Bar graphs
displaying real objects
These types of graphs help children focus on the count or frequency for each value
can be made from a
sheet of plastic such as
along the vertical scale. They also progress from graphs where each piece of data is evident to
a shower curtain liner. graphs where the individual data values may seem to disappear—at least from the perspective
of a child. At first glance, this progression does not follow the one recom-
mended in the Common Core State Standards (CCSSO, 2010) and the Cur-
riculum Focal Points (National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, 2006)
because line plots appear after bar graphs in both of these documents. We
address this issue in the sections on bar graphs and line plots.
Object Graphs
An object graph uses the actual objects being graphed. Examples include
types of shoes, favorite fruit, and shapes of buttons. Figure 5 shows a graph
mat that can be used on the floor to display the objects in an organized man-
ner. The mat can be made from a sheet of plastic about 8 to 10 feet long.
Make 5 or 6 columns with 12 to 15 squares in each column. Children place
each object in a square so that comparisons and counts are easily made. No
numeric scale is necessary. Notice that an object graph is a small step from
sorting. If real objects are sorted into groups, those groups can be lined up
for comparison—an object graph!
Picture Graphs
Picture graphs (also called pictographs) move up a level of abstraction by using a drawing or pic-
ture of some sort that represents what is being graphed. The picture can represent one piece of
data or it can represent a designated quantity. For example, a picture of a book can be assigned to
mean five books in a graph of how many books were read each
day of the week. When children view the graph and see four
books for Monday, they skip count to determine that twenty
Pictographs are a great way to connect to skip counting and
books were read on Monday.
early experiences with multiplication.
Children can make their own drawings for pictographs,
but this can often become time consuming and tedious. There
are various ways to make pictographs easier to create and thereby keep the focus on the mean-
ing of the graph rather than the creation of it. You can duplicate student‐made drawings. You
can use clip art and create a full page of images that can be colored (optional) and cut out to suit
particular needs. If you have a die‐cut machine, you can use available shapes to help you pick a
topic for a graph. For example, if your die‐cut has various vehicles (cars, trains, bus, bike, boat,
etc.), children can use those shapes to indicate each of the vehicles they have ridden in this year.
Stickers can also be a quick way to make a pictograph.
Bar Graphs
After object and picture graphs, bar graphs are among the first ways used to group and pres-
ent data with children in grades pre‐K–2. The following activity can be used to introduce
children to the idea of using bar graphs to organize data.
376
Helping Children Use Data
Each pair of children needs a set of about 12 information cards that have two attributes
written on them. (See Figure 6a for examples of cards.) Tell the children that these cards
describe children from another class and their task is to sort and organize the cards so that
they can find out some things about this class. Emphasize that they need to focus on the
class and not on individual children. After pairs have organized their cards in some way,
Standards for
have the children walk around to look at the different arrangements. (See Figure 6b for Mathematical Practice
possible arrangements.) You may want to draw the different arrangements on the board
2 Reason abstractly
for children to see while the class discusses the different ways. First ask, “What can you tell ◀ and quantitatively
about the class when the cards are arranged this way or that way?”
Ask “how many” type questions, such as “Which arrangement helps us see how
many children like chocolate ice cream?” or “Can we tell how many more boys than girls
are in the class with this arrangement?” or “Which arrangement helps you see which ice
cream flavor is most liked?” You can also have pairs of children gather and organize their
cards as you ask each question so that the answer to the question is more apparent. Have
children justify how their arrangement helps to answer a given question.
Initially bar graphs should be made with each bar consisting of countable parts so that
the individual pieces of data do not seem to disappear. The information cards in the previous
activity, “Organizing Cards,” served this purpose. Having children use something to repre-
sent the pieces of data or the things being counted helps transition from object and picture
graphs to bar graphs. An easy idea is to use sticky notes to represent the individual pieces
of data. These can be posted directly to the board or to a chart and rearranged if needed.
To help children keep their notes in line you may wish to use a gridded chart similar to the
graph mat described in the section on object graphs.
Girl
Boy
Superman
Chocolate
girls boys
girls boys
girls boys
girls boys
377
Helping Children Use Data
After identifying a question, make a quick graph of class data by following these steps:
1. Decide as a class which groups of data will make up the different bars. It is good to have
two to six different bars in a graph.
2. Have children look at the choices and decide where they will place their sticky note.
Have them write their name on the note.
3. In small groups, have children place their entry on the graph.
Figure 7 illustrates two additional quick ways to gather information (step 2 of the process of
doing statistics) and analyze it (step 3). A class of 25 to 30 children can make a graph in less than
10 minutes, leaving ample time to use the graph for questions and observations (step 4).
The next transition is to begin using rectangular bars as seen in traditional bar graphs,
but have children shade each bar with alternating colors to keep individual pieces of data vis-
ible. Friel, Curcio, and Bright (2001) also recommend using grid lines to help children read the
counts or frequencies and label the bars with numerical values.
Figure 7 Some ideas for quick graphs that can be used again and again.
Bus 417
Bus 206
Bus 63
Walk
Children in Family
Only 2 in 3 in More
you family family than 3
378
Helping Children Use Data
The Bar Chart applet at http://nlvm.usu.edu/en/nav/grade_g_1.html allows children to create bar graphs of 3
to 12 columns and 5 to 20 rows of data. Each piece of data is its own block in the bar so that individual data
are visual. A numerical scale is shown along the vertical scale to help read the frequency for each category.
The following activity uses traditional bar graphs in which the individual pieces of data
(inches or centimeters) disappear and children focus on each bar as a whole.
This activity involves children collecting data over time about the amount of rainfall (or
snowfall) right outside their classroom. Install a rain (or snow) gauge outside the classroom
in a location where it can be easily accessed. After a heavy rain (or snow) storm, send a pair
of children outside with a piece of cash register tape. Their task is to cut the tape as long
as the height of the rain on the scale. Then they should mark the tape with the date of the
storm. Place the strips chronologically on a base line labeled with the month and day. These
strips form a bar graph that can be added to and analyzed over the year. Have children to
round to the nearest 12 or 14 inch. For example, if it rained about 1 inch on October 5, about
1 1
4 inch on October 10, and about 2 inch on October 23, there would be 3 bars with the indi-
1 1
cated heights (1 inch, 4 inch, 2 inch), each labeled with the corresponding date. If the storm
occurred over the weekend, the children can problem solve how they want to record that
observation (one strip of paper for the whole weekend if it is one storm, cut the piece in
half if there are two storms, or consult the news for approximate rainfalls). The important
components are combining and comparing the data—how does the storm total in October
compare to the total in April? Note that this paper strip to bar graph approach can also be
used with monitoring plant growth as well as other similar activities.
Bar graphs can clearly show the category that is the larg-
est and the one that is the smallest, as well as tell exactly how
large each category is. They can also be a source for creating put‐
Figure 8
together, take‐apart, and comparison word problems. Using bar graphs to create story problems.
Doll
$12
Activity 6 STORY BARS
379
Helping Children Use Data
Activity 7 STAND BY ME
380
Helping Children Use Data
Classmates
Classmates
people wear sneakers than any other kind of
shoe) and also provide opportunities to make
inferences that are not directly observable in the
graph (e.g., kids in this class do not like to wear
leather shoes).
When asked about graphs, young children
tend to initially focus on individual pieces of
data in a data display and not on the data as a
Red
Yellow
Green
Blue
Baseball
Football
Orange
Soccer
Basketball
Swimming
whole (Clements & Sarama, 2009). So when
asked to describe something about a graph, they
may state, “There I am! I ride the bus to school.”
However, much more can be said about a set of
data than about the individual pieces of data.
The idea is to help children consider the data set as a whole. One way to accomplish this is to
try to focus their attention on the shape of data: a sense of how the data are spread or grouped.
In grades pre‐K–2 this can be discussed informally by looking at almost any graph.
Whenever there are two or more graphs of the same type, it is worth talking with chil-
dren about the overall look of the graphs—how they are alike and how they are different.
Consider, for example, the two graphs in Figure 10, showing data that have been gathered
from a class of 25 children. In Our Favorite Spring Colors graph, the children are almost
distributed equally though there is a most favored color. In Our Favorite Sports graph, two
sports received most of the votes from the class and the remainder of the class is somewhat
spread among the other three.
When data are depicted on a number line, such as on a line plot, the idea of data as
being spread out or grouped together also takes on a numeric meaning. For example, in a
measure of the heights of boys and girls in inches, we might notice that the girls’ heights
are spread over a wider range than the boys’ heights. The boys’ heights may cluster more
around a particular height.
As discussed earlier in this chapter, sometimes questions focus on mathematical ideas
rather than statistical ideas. Although it is helpful to ask mathematical questions, it is essential
to ask questions that are statistical in nature. That means the questions focus on the context of
the situation, seeing what can be learned or inferred from the data. During the interpretation
phase, children might even want to create a different data display to get a different look at the
data or gather data from a different class to see how their results compare.
Different researchers have recommended questions that focus on statistical thinking
(Franklin et al., 2005; Friel, O’Conner, & Mamer, 2006; Russell, 2006; Shaughnessy, 2006).
Here are some ideas from their lists to get you started on having meaningful discussions about
interpreting data: Standards for
Mathematical Practice
• What do the numbers tell us about our class (or another class)?
3 Reason abstractly
• If we asked another class the same questions, how would that data look? What if we ◀
and quantitatively
asked a larger group, how would the data look?
381
Helping Children Use Data
382
E x p a n d e d L e s s o n
Content and Task Decisions • Model the process of gathering data using children
from the class. You can model “how to” and “how not
Grade Level: K–2 to” as a way to help all the children (particularly ELLs)
comprehend the process.
Mathematics Goals
• In selecting how the data will be displayed, show il-
• To determine a question that can be answered through lustrations of different options and have them labeled
gathering data with the appropriate terminology.
• To determine how to gather the data
• To use data to create graphs For Children with Disabilities
• To use graphs to answer questions concerning a • Provide a table with the possible answers for children
population with disabilities if they are collecting data from another
class. This will support them in organizing the tallies.
Grade Level Guide • When the children are creating a bar graph, have them
make an X in the grid prior to coloring. This will help
NCTM Common them focus on the quantity to ensure accuracy.
Curriculum Focal Points Core State Standards
• When analyzing the data, pose some questions that
Children in kindergarten First graders organize, repre- will help the children compare data points in a think-
collect data and use counting sent, and interpret data. They aloud mode. Some children with disabilities may need
to answer questions. By first are able to ask and answer to hear the way you compare data to help them draw
grade, children are represent- questions by comparing data. conclusions and answer the question.
ing data in picture graphs By second grade, children are
and bar graphs. able to draw picture graphs
and bar graphs (using a
Materials
single unit scale) to represent • Large pieces of paper, such as chart paper
data. They are also able • Grid paper, such as a 1‐cm grid (see Black- BLM
to use the information in line Master 31)
a bar graph to answer
• Chart grid paper (optional)
problems.
• Markers/crayons
• Connecting cubes or other manipulatives for graphing
Consider Your Children’s Needs data
This lesson assumes that the children have had experi- • Computers (visit http://nlvm.usu.edu for free graph‐
ences with several different types of graphs, including making programs)
Venn diagrams and some form of bar graph, and that they
are able to use tallies or numbers for gathering data. You
may also want to allow the children to do this phase of Lesson
the lesson using appropriate graphing programs on the This lesson may require several days to complete. It em-
computer. Involve the children in selecting questions that ploys the four‐step process of doing statistics: (1) What
are of interest to them and are culturally relevant. question do we want to answer? (2) How will we collect
the data? (3) What type of analysis (e.g., a graph) should
For English Language Learners we use to view our data? (4) What do the data tell us
• Include written prompts and pair‐share for the discus- about our question? In this lesson, the process is split into
sions at the start of this lesson as a way to increase the two tasks with the fourth phase addressed in the After
participation of ELLs. portion of the second task.
383
Helping Children Use Data
384
Helping Children Use Data
• Ask the children whether they can answer other ques- • Are the children able to actually create the graph?
tions using the data. • Do the children know how to interpret the data in the
• Finally, ask the children what new questions might be graph? Can they make conjectures?
asked, having learned what they did from this question.
Ask
Assessment • What do we need to consider when planning a survey
or question?
Observe • What do you need to do to gather data correctly?
• Are the children using a table, checklist, or tally to • How did you decide on the type of graph to make?
gather their data? Do they have a process in place to • How did you create your graph?
ensure they gather data from each person only once?
• What does (indicate specific data point) mean on the
• Are the children considering appropriate types of graph?
graphs? Do they understand how the type of data lim-
• What do the data tell us about the question posed?
its what makes sense for a graph?
385
Helping Children Use Data
References
Brown, M. (2005). Goodnight moon. New York, NY: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Harper Collins. (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics.
Reston, VA: Author.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2009). Learning and
teaching early math: The learning trajectories approach. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
New York, NY: Routledge. (2006). Curriculum focal points for prekindergarten
through grade 8 mathematics: A quest for coherence.
Cook, C. D. (2008). I scream, you scream: Data
Reston, VA: Author.
analysis with kindergartners. Teaching Children
Mathematics, 14(9), 538–540. Niezgoda, D. A., & Moyer‐Packenham, P. S.
(2005). Hickory, dickory, dock: Navigating through
Franklin, C. A., & Mewborn, D. S. (2008). Statistics
data analysis. Teaching Children Mathematics, 11(6),
in the elementary grades: Exploring distribution of
292–300.
data. Teaching Children Mathematics, 15(1), 10–16.
Russell, S. J. (2006). What does it mean that “5 has
Franklin, C. A., Kader, G., Mewborn, D., Moreno,
a lot”? From the world to data and back. In G. F.
J., Peck, R., Perry, M., & Scheaffer, R. (2005).
Burrill & P. C. Elliott (Eds.), Thinking and reasoning
Guidelines for assessment and instruction in statistics
about data and chance: 68th NCTM yearbook (pp.
education (GAISE) report. Alexandria, VA: American
17–30). Reston VA: National Council of Teachers of
Statistical Association.
Mathematics.
Friel, S. N., Curcio, F. R., & Bright, G. W.
Seuss, Dr. (1960). Green eggs and ham. New York,
(2001). Making sense of graphs: Critical factors
NY: Random House.
influencing comprehension and instructional
implications. Journal for Research in Mathematics Shaughnessy, J. M. (2006). Research on students’
Education, 32, 124–158. understanding of some big concepts in statistics.
In G. F. Burrill & P. C. Elliott (Eds.), Thinking
Friel, S. N., O’Conner, W., & Mamer, J. D. (2006).
and reasoning about data and chance: 68th NCTM
More than “meanmedianmode” and a bar graph:
yearbook (pp. 77–98). Reston, VA: National Council
What’s needed to have a statistical conversation?
of Teachers of Mathematics.
In G. F. Burrill & P. C. Elliott (Eds.), Thinking
and reasoning about data and chance: 68th NCTM Shaughnessy, J. M. (2007). Research on statistics
yearbook (pp. 117–138). Reston, VA: National learning and reasoning. In F. Lester, Jr. (Ed.),
Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching
and learning (pp. 957–1010). Reston, VA: National
Konold, C., Higgins, T., Russell, S., & Khalil, K.
Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
(2004). Data seen through different lenses. Amherst,
MA: University of Massachusetts. Retrieved from
www.srri.umass.edu/publications/konold‐2004dst
Mokros, J., & Wright, T. (2009). Zoos, aquariums,
and expanding students’ data literacy. Teaching
Children Mathematics, 15(9), 524–530.
386
Appendix
The Standards for Mathematical Practice describe varieties of expertise that math-
ematics educators at all levels should seek to develop in their students. These prac-
tices rest on important “processes and proficiencies” with longstanding importance in
mathematics education. The first of these are the NCTM process standards of problem
solving, reasoning and proof, communication, representation, and connections. The
second are the strands of mathematical proficiency specified in the National Research
Council’s report Adding It Up: adaptive reasoning, strategic competence, conceptual
understanding (comprehension of mathematical concepts, operations and relations),
procedural fluency (skill in carrying out procedures flexibly, accurately, efficiently and
appropriately), and productive disposition (habitual inclination to see mathematics
as sensible, useful, and worthwhile, coupled with a belief in diligence and one’s own
efficacy).
Source: © Copyright 2010. National Governors Association Center for Best Practices and Council of
Chief State School Officers. All rights reserved.
From Appendix A of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
387
Appendix: Common Core State Standards: Standards for Mathematical Practice
388
Appendix: Common Core State Standards: Standards for Mathematical Practice
6 Attend to precision.
Mathematically proficient students try to communicate precisely to others. They try to use
clear definitions in discussion with others and in their own reasoning. They state the mean-
ing of the symbols they choose, including using the equal sign consistently and appropriately.
They are careful about specifying units of measure, and labeling axes to clarify the correspon-
dence with quantities in a problem. They calculate accurately and efficiently, express numeri-
cal answers with a degree of precision appropriate for the problem context. In the elementary
grades, students give carefully formulated explanations to each other. By the time they reach
high school they have learned to examine claims and make explicit use of definitions.
389
Appendix: Common Core State Standards: Standards for Mathematical Practice
they have a repeating decimal. By paying attention to the calculation of slope as they re-
peatedly check whether points are on the line through (1, 2) with slope 3, middle school
students might abstract the equation ( y - 2)/(x - 1) = 3. Noticing the regularity in the way
terms cancel when expanding (x - 1)(x + 1), (x - 1)(x 2 + x + 1), and (x - 1)(x 3 + x 2 + x + 1)
might lead them to the general formula for the sum of a geometric series. As they work to
solve a problem, mathematically proficient students maintain oversight of the process, while
attending to the details. They continually evaluate the reasonableness of their intermediate
results.
390
Appendix
1. Students use numbers, including written numerals, to represent quantities and to solve
quantitative problems, such as counting objects in a set; counting out a given number of
objects; comparing sets or numerals; and modeling simple joining and separating situations
with sets of objects, or eventually with equations such as 5 + 2 = 7 and 7 − 2 = 5.
(Kindergarten students should see addition and subtraction equations, and student writing of
equations in kindergarten is encouraged, but it is not required.) Students choose, combine, and
apply effective strategies for answering quantitative questions, including quickly recognizing
the cardinalities of small sets of objects, counting and producing sets of given sizes, counting
the number of objects in combined sets, or counting the number of objects that remain in a
set after some are taken away.
2. Students describe their physical world using geometric ideas (e.g., shape, orientation,
spatial relations) and vocabulary. They identify, name, and describe basic two‐dimensional
shapes, such as squares, triangles, circles, rectangles, and hexagons, presented in a variety of
ways (e.g., with different sizes and orientations), as well as three‐dimensional shapes such as
cubes, cones, cylinders, and spheres. They use basic shapes and spatial reasoning to model
objects in their environment and to construct more complex shapes.
Grade K Overview
Counting and Cardinality
• Know number names and the count sequence.
• Count to tell the number of objects.
• Compare numbers.
Source: © Copyright 2010. National Governors Association Center for Best Practices and Council of
Chief State School Officers. All rights reserved.
From Appendix B of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
391
Appendix: Common Core State Standards: Grades K–2 Critical Content Areas and Overviews
Geometry
• Identify and describe shapes.
• Analyze, compare, create, and compose shapes.
1. Students develop strategies for adding and subtracting whole numbers based on their
prior work with small numbers. They use a variety of models, including discrete objects and
length‐based models (e.g., cubes connected to form lengths), to model add‐to, take‐from,
put‐together, take‐apart, and compare situations to develop meaning for the operations
of addition and subtraction, and to develop strategies to solve arithmetic problems with
these operations. Students understand connections between counting and addition and
subtraction (e.g., adding two is the same as counting on two). They use properties of addition
to add whole numbers and to create and use increasingly sophisticated strategies based on
these properties (e.g., “making tens”) to solve addition and subtraction problems within
20. By comparing a variety of solution strategies, children build their understanding of the
relationship between addition and subtraction.
2. Students develop, discuss, and use efficient, accurate, and generalizable methods to
add within 100 and subtract multiples of 10. They compare whole numbers (at least
to 100) to develop understanding of and solve problems involving their relative sizes.
They think of whole numbers between 10 and 100 in terms of tens and ones (especially
recognizing the numbers 11 to 19 as composed of a ten and some ones). Through activities
that build number sense, they understand the order of the counting numbers and their
relative magnitudes.
392
Appendix: Common Core State Standards: Grades K–2 Critical Content Areas and Overviews
4. Students compose and decompose plane or solid figures (e.g., put two triangles together
to make a quadrilateral) and build understanding of part‐whole relationships as well as
the properties of the original and composite shapes. As they combine shapes, they recognize
them from different perspectives and orientations, describe their geometric attributes, and
determine how they are alike and different, to develop the background for measurement and
for initial understandings of properties such as congruence and symmetry.
Grade 1 Overview
Operations and Algebraic Thinking
• Represent and solve problems involving addition and subtraction.
• Understand and apply properties of operations and the relationship between addition
and subtraction.
• Add and subtract within 20.
• Work with addition and subtraction equations.
Geometry
• Reason with shapes and their attributes.
1
Students should apply the principle of transitivity of measurement to make indirect comparisons, but
they need not use this technical term.
393
Appendix: Common Core State Standards: Grades K–2 Critical Content Areas and Overviews
1. Students extend their understanding of the base‐ten system. This includes ideas
of counting in fives, tens, and multiples of hundreds, tens, and ones, as well as number
relationships involving these units, including comparing. Students understand multi‐digit
numbers (up to 1000) written in base‐ten notation, recognizing that the digits in each place
represent amounts of thousands, hundreds, tens, or ones (e.g., 853 is 8 hundreds + 5 tens +
3 ones).
2. Students use their understanding of addition to develop fluency with addition and
subtraction within 100. They solve problems within 1000 by applying their understanding
of models for addition and subtraction, and they develop, discuss, and use efficient, accurate,
and generalizable methods to compute sums and differences of whole numbers in base‐ten
notation, using their understanding of place value and the properties of operations. They
select and accurately apply methods that are appropriate for the context and the numbers
involved to mentally calculate sums and differences for numbers with only tens or only
hundreds.
3. Students recognize the need for standard units of measure (centimeter and inch) and
they use rulers and other measurement tools with the understanding that linear measure
involves an iteration of units. They recognize that the smaller the unit, the more iterations
they need to cover a given length.
4. Students describe and analyze shapes by examining their sides and angles. Students
investigate, describe, and reason about decomposing and combining shapes to make other
shapes. Through building, drawing, and analyzing two‐ and three‐dimensional shapes,
students develop a foundation for understanding area, volume, congruence, similarity, and
symmetry in later grades.
Grade 2 Overview
Operations and Algebraic Thinking
• Represent and solve problems involving addition and subtraction.
• Add and subtract within 20.
• Work with equal groups of objects to gain foundations for multiplication.
Geometry
• Reason with shapes and their attributes.
394
Appendix
From Appendix C of Teaching Student‐Centered Mathematics, Developmentally Appropriate Instruction for Grades Pre‐K–2, Second Edition.
John A. Van de Walle, LouAnn H. Lovin, Karen S. Karp, Jennifer M. Bay‐Williams. Copyright © 2014 by Pearson Education, Inc.
All rights reserved.
395
Appendix: A Guide to the Blackline Masters
Workmats such as the ten‐frame mat and the place‐value mat are best if not laminated.
Lamination makes the mats slippery so that counters slide around or off.
With materials that require cutting into smaller pieces, we suggest that you laminate
the card stock before you cut out the pieces. This will preserve the materials for several
years and save valuable time in the future. Here are some additional, specific instructions for
certain masters.
• Dot Cards (BLMs 3–8): Make each set of six pages a different color. Otherwise, it is
difficult to tell to which set a stray card belongs.
• Little Ten‐Frames (BLMs 17 and 18): Make the full ten‐frames on one color of card
stock and the less‐than‐ten sheet on another. One set consists of the ten cards of each
type, cut from a strip of ten on the master.
• Assorted Shapes (BLMs 20–26): Make each set of seven pages a different color.
Otherwise, it is very difficult to tell to which set a stray shape belongs.
396
Appendix: A Guide to the Blackline Masters
BLM 9
BLM 7 BLM 8 Blank Hundreds
Dot Cards (e) Dot Cards (f) Chart (10 × 10 Square)
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30
31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40
41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50
51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70
71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80
81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90
91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100
1. Make two copies of this page. Cut out the grid from each copy.
2. Overlap the two grids, and tape onto a blank sheet
to form a 20-by-25-cm grid with 4 complete
hundreds squares and 2 rows of 5 tens each.
3. Use this as a master to make copies on card stock.
397
Appendix: A Guide to the Blackline Masters
398
Appendix: A Guide to the Blackline Masters
399
Appendix: A Guide to the Blackline Masters
Parallelograms Rhombuses
E J
F
C L
B
G
N
Properties of angles: Properties of angles:
M K
BLM 38 BLM 39
BLM 37 Property Lists for Property Lists for
Assorted Triangles Quadrilaterals (Parallelograms) Quadrilaterals (Rhombuses)
Rectangles Squares
400
Appendix: A Guide to the Blackline Masters
Name
Object Object
Estimate Estimate
4 Actual Actual
Object Object
2 Estimate Estimate
Actual Actual
0 number number
0 1 2 3 4
Name
Name
Name
12 12
11 1 11 1
Collection #1
10 2 10 2
Danielle has 72 baseball cards in her collection. She has already put 35 of them into
the plastic holders. How many more cards need to be placed in plastic holders? 9 3 9 3
8 4 8 4
7 5 7 5
6 6
Circle the longer path. If they are the same, circle both.
Collection #2 11
12
1 11
12
1 How we decided: (Draw pictures)
Timothy collects Matchbox cars and has them on two shelves in his bedroom. He has 10 2 10 2
24 Matchbox cars in all. On the top shelf he has put 16 of the cars. How many will be
placed on the bottom shelf? 9 3 9 3
8 4 8 4
7 5 7 5
6 6
Collection #3
12 12
Danielle and Timothy both like to collect state quarters. When they last visited, Danielle 11 1 11 1
had 32 quarters and Timothy had 24 quarters. How many more does Danielle have than 10 2 10 2
Timothy?
9 3 9 3
8 4 8 4
7 5 7 5
6 6
401
This page intentionally left blank
Index
Page references followed by "f" indicate illustrated problem structures, 143, 156, 317, 319 139, 153, 162, 166, 168, 174, 184, 188-189,
figures or photographs; followed by "t" indicates a properties of, 87, 225, 228, 245, 263-264, 388, 192, 196, 204, 211, 219, 222-223, 235, 246,
table. 392-394 252, 260, 274, 282, 286, 294-295, 307, 310,
Addition and subtraction, 29, 48-49, 66, 91, 99-100, 312, 314, 318, 326-327, 330, 335, 340, 362,
102-103, 106-107, 121, 128, 130, 138, 367, 374, 380, 385-386, 390
A 143-146, 148, 150, 152-153, 157, 162, Assessment:, 48
Abbreviations, 302 171-172, 174, 182, 184, 186, 191-193, alternative, 86, 139
Abilities, 32, 49, 93, 274, 326, 388 196-197, 215-216, 219, 223, 225-226, 228, community, 335
Abstract symbols, 3 230, 232, 240, 244-245, 247, 255, 264, 273, components of, 211
Abstraction, 88, 376 317-319, 391-394, 395, 398 continuous, 380
Academic achievement, 68, 186 standard algorithm for subtraction, 103 culturally responsive, 77
academic language, 68, 71, 73 Additions, 251 curriculum materials, 327
development, 68, 71 Adjustment, 234 decision making, 53, 86
importance, 68 Adjustments, 59 definitions, 162, 362
in classroom discourse, 73 Administrators, 29, 109-111 demand for, 60
Academic performance, 111 educational, 111 descriptive, 362
Academies, 2, 12, 95, 140, 193, 247 school, 29, 109-111 differentiated instruction, 37
ACCESS, 18, 36-37, 49, 63, 70-71, 77, 81, 87-88, 92, Adults, 107, 114, 143, 146, 198, 227, 280, 369, 384 direct, 10, 166, 174
106, 219, 246, 255, 274, 338, 361, 369, 371 Advertisements, 368 ecological, 327
Accommodation, 4-5, 81, 117 Advertising, 365, 367 family, 153, 188-189, 192, 282, 374
Accommodations, 70, 75-76, 81-82, 94 advice, 395 formal, 330
accommodations, 70, 75-76, 81-82, 94 Advocacy, 111 formative, 16, 47-48, 51, 53, 60, 70, 75, 77, 86,
Accountability, 45, 101 Affect, 8-9, 37, 143, 171, 274, 275, 321, 365 115, 120, 130, 132, 134, 153, 162, 166,
of teachers, 45 Age, 67, 91, 111, 114, 140, 169, 223, 279, 334, 384 174, 184, 188-189, 196, 204, 211, 219,
Accuracy, 175, 307, 383 mental, 91 235, 252, 260, 282, 286, 307, 310, 312,
Achieve, 34, 102, 104 Agreement, 97, 202, 291 314, 318, 340, 374, 380
Achievement, 60, 67-68, 81-83, 97, 104-105, 111, 186, AIDS, 2, 85, 152 fractions and, 295
295, 363, 390 Algebra, 29, 92, 94-95, 99, 104, 191, 249-250, 252, gathering data, 367, 374
academic, 68, 82, 111, 186 254, 262, 273, 275, 337, 355, 389 group, 37, 48, 51, 53, 55, 115, 139, 184, 196, 204,
in mathematics, 60, 68, 104, 111, 295, 390 Algebraic reasoning, 100, 249-272, 274-275 222, 246, 318, 327, 335, 340, 362
influences on, 111 generalization from arithmetic, 250 guidelines for, 367, 386
Acquisition, 247 patterns in, 268-272 HELPING, 75, 120, 174, 184, 188-189, 192, 282,
ACT, 1, 16, 19, 27, 62, 102, 104, 146, 148-149, 238, Algorithms, 63-65, 103, 121, 148, 193, 225-227, 367, 374, 380, 385-386
275, 279, 283, 294 229-231, 238, 240, 244-245, 247 in geometry, 330
Acting out, 177, 256 cultural differences in, 231 in grades, 223, 282
actions, 26-27, 52, 63, 148-149, 279, 315, 357, 388 Alternative assessments, 86 informal, 295, 330
Activities, 6, 10, 14, 16, 27, 35, 41, 45, 58, 83, 85-87, Alternative strategies, 101, 226 integrated, 295
91-92, 99-100, 102, 115-119, 121-131, 133, Alternatives, 89 interpretation of, 76
135-136, 139, 153, 161, 176-177, 186, Ambiguity, 18 math, 85, 327, 386
188-190, 199-205, 207-211, 214-215, 219, American Educational Research Association, 12, 223, mathematics, 10, 16, 47, 53, 55, 58, 60, 70, 75-77,
226, 235, 239, 253, 256, 265, 267, 269, 287, 327 86, 166, 174, 188-189, 223, 282, 295,
299, 301, 304-308, 310, 312, 315, 318, 321, Analysis, 51, 55, 92, 95, 149, 164, 295, 332-335, 346, 307, 318, 327, 330, 335, 367, 386, 390
323-324, 334-339, 341-346, 350, 352-353, 365-367, 370, 374-375, 383, 386 mathematics instruction, 390
355-360, 369-373, 379, 392 story, 55, 149, 164, 374 measurement principles, 327
bridging, 219 Anecdotal notes, 48 methods, 10, 16, 50, 53, 58-59, 86, 139, 153, 166,
developmental, 118, 334 Angles, 71, 329-330, 332-333, 336, 338-339, 346, 174, 196, 235, 246, 326, 367, 374
initiating, 14 350-352, 362, 394, 400 methods of, 196, 374
instructional, 6, 10, 83, 86, 118, 189, 200, 304, 392 units of, 394 misunderstandings, 47, 58-59, 168
learning, 6, 10, 14, 16, 27, 35, 41, 45, 58, 83, animals, 120, 136, 152, 183-184, 278, 285, 303, 368, monitoring, 86, 189
85-87, 91-92, 99-100, 102, 115, 118, 122, 370, 373-374 monitoring and, 86
131, 186, 189-190, 200, 215, 253, 267, anxiety, 6, 95, 107, 110, 173, 187, 189-190 need for, 10
287, 308, 315, 335-336, 352, 355, 357 Application, 73, 94, 114, 176, 304 objective, 50, 85, 162
ongoing, 139, 307 Applications, 36, 41, 60, 93, 106, 172 observation, 48, 50, 58, 166, 312
planning, 35, 83, 85-87, 91-92 Appreciation, 333 plan for, 48, 59, 189
space for, 209 Apprehension, 98 planning and, 76
varying, 41, 91, 116, 130 Approaches, 7-11, 19-21, 27, 34, 47-48, 50, 53, 56, principles, 47, 86, 223, 327, 330, 335, 367, 386
Adaptation, 35-36, 51, 54, 241 65-66, 76, 82-83, 86, 88-89, 93, 101, 103, problem, 10, 16, 37, 47-48, 50-51, 53, 55, 59, 70,
Adaptations, 94 172, 175, 198, 215, 219, 228, 230, 232, 252, 75-77, 85, 120, 130, 153, 166, 168, 189,
Adaptive reasoning, 2, 387 256, 294, 306, 345, 387 192, 204, 219, 223, 235, 246, 260, 274,
Addition, 3-4, 6, 8-10, 15, 19-23, 26-27, 29, 33, 36-38, brief, 48, 175 282, 294, 318, 367, 390
43-44, 48-50, 53-54, 58, 64, 66-68, 71, 76, Appropriate language, 73 procedures, 51, 390
85, 87, 91, 99-103, 106-107, 121, 123, 128, Area, 54, 66, 68, 83, 88, 91-92, 100, 117, 138, 250, purpose of, 70
130, 138, 140, 142-146, 148, 150-157, 159, 278, 280-281, 286, 288-289, 293, 297-298, purposes, 53, 166, 189
161-162, 164, 166-169, 171-174, 176-179, 301, 304, 306-307, 321-323, 327, 330, 336, reasons for, 58, 374
181-187, 191-193, 196-197, 199, 214-219, 355, 368, 370-371, 374, 394 rubric for, 58
223, 225-226, 228, 230-232, 234-238, Arguments, 2-3, 125, 136, 143, 163-164, 175-176, rubrics for, 58
240-241, 243-245, 247, 250-252, 254-255, 218, 230, 232, 264, 285-286, 333, 345, 352, scoring, 55
258, 263-264, 273-274, 282, 295, 317-319, 388 service, 223
322, 367, 388, 391-394, 395, 398 examining, 143 social studies, 367
comparison models, 153 Array Multiplication, 395, 397 software, 85-86
contextual problems, 36, 142-143, 155 Art, 66, 85, 297, 330, 376 student achievement, 295, 390
implied, 76, 143 defining, 330 summative, 47-48
invented strategies for, 219, 232, 236-238, 245 music, 297 technology, 189, 380, 390
number line, 3, 6, 8, 43, 67, 91, 148, 151-152, 162, standards for, 330 terms in, 70
177, 214, 217, 219, 234-235, 245, 247, Assessing, 47-60, 61-78, 79-95, 130, 231 transition, 86, 211, 235
273 Assessment, 10, 16, 37, 47-48, 50-51, 53, 55, 58-60, work samples, 58
of fractions, 282, 295 70, 75-77, 85-86, 115, 120, 130, 132, 134, Assessments, 34, 47-48, 53-54, 58-59, 75, 82-83,
403
85-86, 88-89, 104, 109, 366, 390 recorded, 384 293-294, 330-331, 336, 391
classroom, 34, 82, 104, 109 talking, 76 fraction, 277, 280, 289, 294
comprehensive, 83 Borrowing, 240 Citizenship, 365
quality, 82 Boys, 377, 381 Clarifying, 3, 11, 21, 258
Assets, 94 Brain, 4-5, 46, 186 Clarity, 85, 351
Assignments, 56, 106 research, 4 Class discussions, 41, 58, 65, 218, 374
Assimilation, 4-5 Bridging, 219, 240, 295 Class list, 352, 369
Assistance, 41, 82, 88-89, 94, 105 Budgets, 107 Classification, 339, 344, 347, 365, 370-374
cards, 41 Buildings, 346 Classroom, 4, 6, 8-11, 14, 16, 19-22, 24-29, 32-35, 37,
Association, 1, 12, 46, 78, 94-95, 99, 103, 223, 327, Bulletin boards, 322 39-40, 45-46, 50-51, 63-68, 70, 73, 77-78,
366-367, 386, 387, 391 81-82, 84, 87, 92-94, 98, 102, 104, 106,
Association for Supervision and Curriculum C 109-111, 116-117, 119, 133-134, 140, 149,
Development, 12 Calculator use, 190 152, 173, 177, 202, 220, 228, 231-232, 249,
Associative property, 155, 225 Calculators, 102, 210, 272-274 256, 275, 278, 299, 302, 307, 309, 324, 327,
in addition, 155 California, 77, 95 335-336, 338, 343, 355, 359, 369, 373,
assumptions, 388-389 Capacities, 323 379-380
conclusions, 388 Capacity, 2, 298, 301, 323-324 arrangements, 82
atmosphere, 231 Cardinality, 17, 38, 53, 115, 121, 391 conference, 327
Attending, 32-33, 37, 201, 388, 390 Cards, 35, 41-43, 49, 71, 84, 100, 116, 118-120, 124, displays, 93
Attention, 1, 4, 6, 15, 18, 21, 23-24, 32, 34, 49, 62, 69, 126, 129-130, 132-134, 137-139, 146, 172, environment in, 232
71-72, 81, 83, 85, 94, 125, 128, 136, 139, 178-181, 187-192, 200-201, 210-214, 216, language in, 70, 110, 327
160, 162, 164, 176, 179, 184, 204, 210, 212, 235-237, 239-240, 249, 254-255, 258, 262, organizing, 35, 81, 92, 228
222, 228, 234, 241-242, 250-252, 267, 280, 266, 269, 271, 298, 304, 322, 343, 352, 360, classroom discourse, 21, 27, 66, 73
286, 305, 316, 319-320, 327, 331, 334, 337, 371-372, 377, 395-397, 400-401 Classroom environment, 37, 65, 67-68, 232
341, 345-346, 350, 355, 381, 390 Career, 104 time, 67-68
and learning, 34 coaching, 104 Classrooms, 6-10, 15, 68, 73, 77, 82, 93, 109, 147,
negative, 23 Categories, 47-48, 56-57, 68, 91, 99, 105, 172, 331, 212, 278, 334
positive, 350 340, 346, 348, 351, 360-361, 370, 374 for grades, 278
student, 1, 21, 24, 81, 139, 334, 390 Categorization, 144, 265 regular, 93, 109
theories, 4 Census, 365, 367 special, 93, 212, 334
Attitudes, 10, 104 Centers, 37, 40-41 Cleanup, 91
teacher, 10, 104 computer, 40 CLEAR, 3, 27, 34, 58, 68, 71-72, 76, 82, 85, 97, 126,
Attraction, 34 Cerebral palsy, 89 128, 139, 154, 162, 165, 168, 192, 205, 210,
Attribute materials, 370, 374 Chains, 205, 310 222, 245, 264, 269, 273-274, 291-292, 294,
Audio, 41, 54, 106 Chalk, 309 299, 304, 326, 329, 362, 371, 384, 389
online, 106 Change, 3-4, 26, 38, 40, 42-43, 45, 80, 103, 115, Climate, 11
Austin, 111, 193 125-127, 129, 144-145, 147, 149, 152, 178, Clip art, 85, 376
Authenticity, 34 182, 205-206, 210-214, 216, 222, 226, 229, Clips, 111, 205, 221, 301, 308, 310-311, 324, 326
AUTHOR, 12, 78, 140, 193, 223, 247, 275, 295, 303, 231, 233-234, 237, 250-252, 254, 256, 261, Clock reading, 315-316
315, 327, 363, 386 272, 274-275, 281, 286, 292, 303, 308, 314, Clothing, 368, 372, 374
Authors, 81 316-317, 319-321, 326, 335, 352, 362, 379, Clues, 52, 87, 153, 211, 360
Autism, 89 387 Clustering, 382
Autonomy, 29 continuing, 272 Clusters, 186, 188
Average, 104, 303, 305, 365-366 facilitating, 40 Coaching, 10, 98, 104, 232
Averages, 366 planned, 43 Codes, 49
Avoiding, 6, 177, 190 problems with, 145, 147, 149, 182, 226, 237, 379 Cognates, 70, 77
Awareness, 34, 47, 361 stages of, 211 Cognition, 50, 275
Changes, 26, 36, 40, 81, 98, 109, 208, 211, 225, 252, Cognitive demand, 18, 21, 33, 39
B 267, 270-271, 273-274, 316-317, 335, 352, Cognitive dissonance, 51
Back, 4, 11, 27-28, 48, 51, 59, 66, 82, 84, 98, 101, 354 Coherence, 140, 193, 275, 295, 327, 363, 386
103, 109, 115-116, 120-121, 123, 129, 145, Charting, 86 COIN, 55, 201, 317-318, 321
152, 168, 183, 211, 219, 230, 232-234, Charts, 86, 133, 214, 218, 241-242, 375, 395, 398 Collaboration, 44, 84
237-239, 246, 252, 255, 263, 269, 286, data, 86, 375 small groups, 44, 84
319-320, 322, 353-354, 359, 379-380, 386, feedback, 86 understanding, 44
389-390 Checklist, 35, 49-50, 59, 246, 252, 307, 314, 326, 385 Collecting, 117, 220, 365, 367, 369-370, 379, 383
Background, 4, 35, 37, 41, 63, 69, 72, 74, 78, 232, Checklists, 49, 90 color, 71, 85, 99, 126-127, 131, 136, 201, 253,
291, 332, 393 Chicago, 95, 347 267-268, 270, 280-282, 287, 313, 322, 330,
Background knowledge, 4, 72 Child development, 295 335, 337, 361, 367, 370-371, 375, 381, 396,
Balance, 65, 90, 256-258, 273, 302, 324 curriculum, 295 400-401
Bar graphs, 136, 366, 376-381, 383-384 Child needs, 148, 186, 362 Colors, 15, 122-123, 126-128, 150-152, 269, 289, 313,
BASIC, 1, 8-9, 15, 34, 49, 83, 88, 90-91, 99-104, 114, Children, 1-12, 14-29, 32-45, 47-60, 61-78, 79-95, 337, 369, 371, 378, 380-381, 395
119-121, 127, 130-131, 142-143, 153-154, 97-111, 114-139, 142-143, 145-169, Com, 152, 186, 233, 251, 370, 395
163, 165, 171-193, 208, 234, 237, 250-251, 171-193, 195-223, 225-247, 249-275, Commitment, 97
254-255, 263, 302, 304, 311, 324, 352, 277-291, 293-295, 297-327, 329-348, Communication, 2, 14-15, 23, 36, 57, 68, 97-98, 108,
365-366, 370, 391 350-363, 365-386, 392, 395 183, 186, 375, 387
Basic skills, 99 bilingual, 68, 70, 73, 77-78, 111 good, 108, 183, 186
Behavior, 169, 327 constructivist approach, 10 language development, 68
Behaviors, 84 focus on, 17-18, 25, 37-39, 41, 43, 48-49, 53, language development and, 68
Benchmarks, 56, 121, 126, 297, 303, 305-306, 327 65-66, 68, 71, 84-86, 88, 93, 99-100, 103, parents, 97-98, 108
Benefits, 52, 101, 229, 301 109, 120, 127, 129, 143, 148, 151, 154, selection of, 23
Best practice, 95 156, 161, 164, 174, 176-177, 180-181, Communication skills, 183
Best practices, 1, 77, 103, 111, 387, 391 185-186, 189, 205, 207, 209, 215, 228, Communications, 104-105
Big ideas, 34-36, 49, 65-66, 84, 86, 289 235, 241, 256, 274, 278, 281-282, 293, Community, 3, 9, 11, 14, 27-28, 64-65, 67, 93, 97-111,
Bilingual, 68, 70, 73, 77-78, 111 302, 304, 306, 311, 315-317, 321, 323, 335-336, 368, 388
Bilingual education, 68, 78 332-333, 335-336, 341, 350, 370, and principals, 97-111
Bills, 318, 321 374-377, 379, 381, 383, 392 groups, 27-28, 64, 99, 101, 106, 335
Birthday, 15, 67, 76, 148-149 Children with disabilities, 41, 81, 83-90, 119-120, 129, Community of learners, 9, 27-28
Blocks, 8, 25-26, 55, 90, 106, 128, 135, 146, 159, 173, 138, 167, 175-177, 180, 183, 186-187, 191, Comparison, 92, 137, 144-145, 147, 153-154, 182,
197, 200-201, 204, 208-211, 229, 243-244, 203, 208-209, 211, 213, 215, 217, 221, 236, 297-301, 308, 315, 317, 319, 321, 323-324,
267-270, 280-281, 287, 303, 315, 322-326, 239, 241, 245, 256, 258-259, 268, 273, 286, 376, 379, 384
341-344, 354, 356, 371 291, 304, 309, 318, 325, 336, 340, 350, 353, Comparisons, 117, 119, 136, 190, 220, 256, 282, 288,
Body language, 52 356, 359, 361, 371-372, 383 290, 299-300, 302, 304, 308-309, 315, 325,
Bookmark, 50 Children with special needs, 22, 88-89, 293 369, 375-376, 384, 393
Books, 36, 73, 76-77, 106, 117, 129, 152, 161, 196, Chip, 49 Compensation, 233, 235, 255
269, 275, 295, 305, 319, 322, 349, 369-370, Chips, 41, 150, 201, 203, 241, 322 Competence, 2, 387
374-376, 384 Choice, 9, 27, 35-36, 38, 45, 65, 68, 88, 100, 108, mathematical, 2, 387
aids in, 152 183, 219, 240, 244, 311, 365 strategic, 2, 387
multicultural, 77 Chunking, 240, 305-306 Competency, 225
picture, 305, 374, 376 Circles, 38-39, 55, 58, 66, 166, 277, 280, 289, Competing, 4, 85, 108
404
Competition, 189-190 Context, 3, 15-17, 20, 24, 26, 34, 36, 66-67, 69, 71-73, trends, 104, 365
Complexity, 35, 41-43, 91, 93 76, 82, 102-103, 129, 131, 138-139, 143, Curriculum development, 12, 46, 78
Components, 45, 53, 66, 87, 98, 105, 108, 145, 199, 150, 152-154, 158, 161, 164, 175, 215, 226, Curriculum Focal Points for Prekindergarten Through
210-211, 250, 379 236, 247, 251, 255, 262, 273, 286-290, 293, Grade 8 Mathematics, 140, 193, 275, 295,
Composition, 26 304, 317, 325, 353, 366, 369, 381, 387-389, 327, 363, 386
Comprehensible input, 72-73, 75 394
Comprehension, 2, 21, 149, 245, 386, 387 Contextual problems, 36, 142-143, 149, 155 D
knowledge and, 2 Conventions, 23, 149, 207, 231, 304, 317, 349 Daily instruction, 5
Computation, 3, 27, 38, 85, 102, 104, 107, 114, 124, Conversations, 88, 109, 367 Daily living, 227
131, 148, 150, 153, 163-164, 185, 190, beginning, 88 Data, 1, 16, 24, 35-36, 47-48, 50, 53, 67, 82, 86, 92,
195-196, 212, 214-215, 219, 225-244, Cooperative groups, 48, 73, 101 103, 136, 220-221, 297, 330, 365-386,
246-247, 249-250, 254, 258, 260, 263 Cooperative learning, 10, 101 387-389, 392-394
instruction, 38, 131, 195-196, 215, 225, 229, 240, approaches, 10, 101 Data:, 381
249, 254 Coordination, 199 Data analysis, 92, 365, 367, 370, 374, 386
computation instruction, 196 Copyright, 1-2, 13, 18-20, 31, 47, 61, 79-80, 97, 99, Data analysis and probability standard, 367
Computational estimation, 195 103, 113, 141, 171-172, 174, 195, 215, 225, probability, 367
Computational skills, 19, 102, 225 249, 277, 297, 329, 337, 365, 367, 387, 391, Data collection, 82, 367, 369, 374, 384
for addition and subtraction, 102 395 data sources, 369
Computer programs, 353 Costs, 164, 262 Data points, 383
Computers, 102, 383 Council of Chief State School Officers, 1, 29, 45, 60, Databases, 369
Computing, 121, 148, 163-165, 225-227, 239, 249, 94, 103, 111, 140, 169, 193, 387, 391 Dating, 49, 254
255, 259, 366 Counting, 5-6, 8, 19-20, 33, 38, 40-41, 48-51, 67, Decimals, 121, 295, 369
Concept, 1, 5, 10, 16-17, 22-25, 36-37, 40-41, 45, 50, 90-91, 114-123, 125, 127, 129-131, 133, number sense, 121, 295
53-54, 57-58, 60, 65, 68-69, 71, 75, 85, 139, 142-143, 146, 148, 150-151, 155, 157, Decision making, 53, 86, 293
88-89, 114, 118-119, 121, 125, 131-132, 135, 160, 163, 171-179, 181, 183-186, 188, 192, shared, 293
142, 151, 167, 195-199, 204, 206, 209-210, 196-199, 202-204, 207-209, 211-212, Deck, The, 255
215, 219, 237, 240, 250, 266-268, 280, 286, 217-219, 221-222, 227-228, 232, 234-235, Deduction, 332-333
292, 299-301, 315, 318, 322-324, 331, 334, 237-241, 245-246, 252-253, 271-272, 274, Definition, 150, 265-266, 346-351
344, 351 277, 279-280, 288, 291-292, 299-300, Definitions, 18, 27-28, 87, 145, 162, 265, 333,
Concept development, 125, 142, 204, 351 308-309, 314, 317-318, 320, 350, 374, 376, 346-347, 349-351, 362, 388-389
Concept maps, 69 383, 391-394 Democracy, 77
Concepts, 2, 4-8, 14-15, 17-18, 23-25, 36, 40, 45, 48, counting on, 40, 48, 50, 118-119, 122-123, 129, demonstrations, 73, 87, 353
51, 53-54, 65, 69, 71, 73, 82-91, 95, 100, 139, 146, 148, 174, 176-177, 211, 218, Denominators, 291
103-104, 110, 113-137, 139-140, 153, 158, 232, 234, 245, 392 density, 85
163, 167, 169, 172, 185, 193, 195-223, skip counting, 33, 90, 157, 160, 184, 217, 246, 253, Dependency, 88
228-229, 241, 247, 249-250, 252-253, 271-272, 318, 376 Depth, 35, 59, 91, 93-94, 100, 298
277-292, 294-295, 297-324, 326-327, Counting all, 174, 177, 222 Derived facts, 146, 157, 166, 174
329-360, 362-363, 365, 386, 387, 389-390, Counting on, 40, 48, 50, 118-119, 122-123, 129, 139, Description, 53, 332, 346, 348
393 146, 148, 174, 176-177, 211, 218, 232, 234, Descriptions, 103, 330, 334-335, 352, 355, 387
circles, 277, 280, 289, 294, 330-331, 336 245, 392 Descriptors, 335, 373
guides, 8, 86 Counting skills, 121 Design, 10, 12, 20, 41, 50, 69, 75, 85, 118, 128, 151,
introducing, 4, 73, 200, 205, 221, 282, 291, 320, Courses, 334 173, 176, 269, 341, 353-355, 359, 366-367,
324, 340 Courtesy, 2 388
teaching of, 83 Creating, 5-6, 25, 29, 41, 68, 77, 86, 119, 122, 128, Designs, 93, 128, 341, 343, 353-354, 359
Conceptual knowledge, 63 131, 158, 178, 180, 205, 214, 232, 270, 295, Development, 2-3, 6-7, 9, 12, 18, 20, 27, 29, 32-33,
lack of, 63 310, 317, 320, 343, 350, 357, 360, 362, 367, 36, 46, 48, 55, 68, 70-71, 78, 87, 94,
Conceptual understanding, 1-2, 4, 49, 52, 85, 90, 94, 374, 379, 383, 388 108-109, 111, 114, 122, 124-125, 127, 130,
102, 190, 201, 226, 297, 387 Critical period, 195 135-136, 142, 148, 153, 155, 163, 172-173,
Conclusions, 75, 98, 266, 333, 380, 383-384, 388, 390 Critiquing, 58 175-176, 189, 193, 195-197, 202, 204,
Conferences, 48, 98, 107, 153 Cubes, 27, 42-43, 48, 85, 87, 89, 117, 127-129, 135, 211-212, 214, 223, 226-231, 275, 282, 295,
families, 98, 107, 153 138-139, 151, 154, 200, 202-204, 209, 297, 301, 304, 318, 327, 329-330, 351, 354,
parent, 48, 98 221-222, 228, 256, 265-270, 289, 293, 298, 390
Confidence, 16, 23, 39, 44-45, 58, 73, 98, 104, 107, 301, 310, 323-324, 330, 333-334, 379, 383, Development of children, 223, 327
123, 149, 171, 173, 240 391-392 Developmental approach, 101
Conflict, 5 Cue, 41, 67 Developmental disabilities, 193
Confusion, 5, 51-52, 58, 85, 232-233, 307 Cues, 82, 85, 119 Developmental perspective, 223, 334
congruence, 352, 393-394 visual, 82, 85 Devices, 67, 299, 343
Conjectures, 21, 103, 232, 263-264, 312, 372, 385, Cuisenaire rods, 280-281, 287-289, 310, 312, 322 Diagnosis, 54
387-388 Cultural, 4, 62-67, 69-70, 199, 231 Diagnostic assessment, 60
justifying, 263 Cultural differences, 231 Diagnostic interview, 50-52, 75, 184, 204, 219, 310,
Connections, 2-6, 8-10, 14-15, 18, 21, 23, 25, 27-28, Cultural diversity, 64 335, 340
36, 40-41, 56, 65-66, 72, 85, 88, 91, Cultural practices, 62, 67 Diagrams, 2-4, 16, 24, 69, 103, 150, 310, 383,
100-102, 104, 106, 111, 121, 142, 190-191, Culturally responsive instruction, 64-65 387-388
199, 210-211, 220, 222, 240, 278, 283, 387, Culturally responsive teaching, 77 Venn, 383
392 Culture, 10, 37, 62-65, 67, 72, 77, 111 Dialogue, 11, 22, 89, 101, 264
Consequence, 19, 254 high, 62-63, 65 Dictionaries, 71
Consequences, 389 meaning of, 72 Differences, 8, 32, 39-40, 63, 80-81, 109, 115, 137,
Conservation, 321 Curiosity, 232 146, 228-229, 231, 235, 267, 323-324, 331,
Consideration, 64, 72, 105, 303 Curriculum, 12, 44, 46, 60, 78, 82, 84, 89, 91-95, 337, 339, 361, 380, 382, 394
Consistency, 85 98-100, 102, 104, 108-109, 111, 114, 138, Differentiated classroom, 37, 46, 78
Constructivism, 4, 6, 10, 29 140, 158, 163, 167, 191, 193, 221, 225, 245, Differentiated instruction, 37, 46
and teaching, 29 249-250, 272-273, 275, 277, 293, 295, 297, Differentiation, 32-37, 44, 46, 65
Constructivist approach, 10 304, 307, 325, 327, 329-330, 361, 363, 365, Digit Correspondence Task, 211
Constructivist theory, 81 367, 370, 376, 383, 386, 390 Diligence, 2, 387
Constructs, 5, 127 adaptations, 94 Dimensions, 321-322, 324, 331, 335, 348
Content, 2, 9, 15-16, 28, 34-37, 40-41, 45, 49, 53, aligned, 109 Direct instruction, 10-11, 100
56-57, 64-66, 68-70, 72, 75, 77, 89, 91-93, basic skills, 99 Directions, 9, 16, 25, 32, 41, 81, 83-85, 155, 168, 211,
97, 99, 101, 103, 105-106, 109, 133, 136, challenges to, 272 221, 258, 356-357, 400
138, 167, 191, 221, 245, 273, 286, 293, 295, compacting, 92, 95 Directives, 72, 89, 329
297, 325, 327, 329-331, 334-336, 344, 357, differentiated, 46, 78 Disabilities, 41, 81, 83-92, 94-95, 105, 117, 119-120,
361, 367-368, 372, 374, 383, 389-390, explicit, 84, 89, 100 122, 129, 138, 167-168, 172, 175-177, 180,
391-394 implementing, 91, 98, 108-109 183, 186-187, 191, 193, 201, 203, 208-209,
expectations, 45, 65, 72, 75, 245, 273, 390 kinds of, 94, 104, 250, 376 211, 213, 215, 217, 221, 236, 239, 241, 245,
knowledge, 2, 9, 28, 34, 36, 53, 64-65, 69, 72, 77, manipulatives, 12, 82, 84, 98-99, 167, 245, 293, 256, 258-259, 268, 273, 286, 291, 304, 309,
91, 93, 101, 136, 295, 361, 389 383 318, 325, 336, 340, 350, 353, 356, 359, 361,
meaningful, 15, 34, 66, 69, 106, 297, 330, 367 options for, 82, 91, 93 371-372, 383
Content knowledge, 295 parallel, 89, 329-330 intellectual, 89, 186
Content objectives, 49 relationships and, 249, 272 Disability, 81-82, 95
Content standards, 390 structured, 44 Discipline, 9, 16, 390
405
Discipline issues, 16 English Language Learners, 21, 63, 68, 74, 77, 81, 94, Falls, 193, 246, 270, 280, 291, 305
Discipline problems, 16 116, 138, 149, 167, 191, 197, 221, 231, 245, Families, 84, 97-111, 114, 153, 187-189, 191-192,
Discourse, 14, 16, 21, 27, 29, 66, 71, 73 273, 293, 325, 361, 383 230-232, 250, 344, 367, 369
practice, 14, 16, 29, 73 bilingual education, 68 children with disabilities, 84, 187, 191
Discussion, 2, 7, 9-11, 21-22, 26, 28, 37-38, 40, 44, English language learners (ELLs), 21, 63, 149, 197, involving, 107-108, 111, 153
53, 55, 58, 66-68, 70, 73, 75, 83-84, 100, 231 needs, 108-109, 191
105-106, 109, 125-126, 132, 134, 136, 143, English learners, 73, 77-78 step, 109, 188, 367
149, 153, 155, 161, 166, 175, 177, 182, 186, Enrichment, 81, 91-94 Family, 63, 67, 98, 100-101, 105, 107-108, 111,
202, 206, 212, 217-218, 240-241, 243, 252, Enrichment activities, 92 144-145, 153, 177, 187-189, 191-192, 231,
255, 259, 262-263, 265, 267-268, 270, Enthusiasm, 16 282, 365, 374, 378
285-286, 294, 301, 303, 312, 324, 326, 335, Environment, 34-35, 37, 41, 65, 67-68, 81, 85, 104, PET, 374
338, 344, 367, 369, 375, 381, 384, 389 136, 232, 317, 331, 343, 355, 391 Family history, 67
guided, 10, 301 arranging, 343 Family members, 67, 177, 189, 231
Discussion groups, 106 home, 67, 104, 136 Family support, 111
Discussions, 6-7, 11, 21-22, 25-27, 29, 40-41, 58, 65, Equal opportunity, 62, 80 schooling, 111
73, 77, 90, 126, 177, 209, 218, 232, 249, Equal sign, 90, 146, 150, 249, 254-256, 258-260, 273, fear, 267
282, 294, 334-335, 359, 368, 374-375, 381, 275, 389 Feedback, 10, 14-15, 48, 51, 57-58, 86, 108, 189, 321
383 relational thinking and, 260 facilitative, 10
conceptual, 90 Equality, 17, 249, 275, 282 immediate, 189
issue, 375 Equations, 53, 66, 88, 100, 103, 128, 146, 150-151, feelings, 267
Disequilibrium, 5, 11, 29 153-154, 157-158, 161-162, 165, 168, 178, Field trips, 369
Dispositions, 101 185, 233, 245-246, 254-255, 258-259, File, 126, 128-129, 314, 340
Distracting, 52 261-262, 273, 275, 387, 391, 393 Files, 395
Distraction, 106 Equity, 62-63, 77, 80-81, 94, 140 Findings, 4, 44, 70
Distribution, 366, 386 gender equity, 77 fire, 368
District of Columbia, 1 Equity principle, 62, 80 First grade, 57, 133, 164-165, 195, 202, 207, 212-213,
Diversity, 19, 45, 62, 64, 77-78, 111, 193, 295 Equivalent groupings, 197-199 220-221, 273, 277, 282, 297, 308, 315, 330,
pairing, 45 Erlbaum, Lawrence, 12, 29, 77, 275 383
Division, 58, 63, 67, 142, 155-161, 167, 169, 229, 278, Error, 51, 88, 147, 157, 168, 227, 246, 267, 285, 290, Fish, 148, 182
293 301, 305, 380 Flash cards, 100, 126, 172, 179, 181, 189-192
measurement division, 156-157 error analysis, 51 Flexibility:, 12, 193
partition division, 156-157, 160 Error correction, 88 Flexible grouping, 44-45, 84
Domain, 138, 221, 250, 273, 295 Errors, 11, 51, 57, 90, 215, 229, 231, 233, 241-242, Floors, 17
Dominoes, 123-124 244, 297, 311, 389 Flow, 48, 52
Dot Cards, 119-120, 124, 129, 395-397 computational, 229, 233, 244 Fluency, 2, 49, 51-52, 83, 94, 101, 148, 155, 163,
Doubling, 122 Estimation, 107, 135, 195, 204-205, 214, 221, 230, 171-172, 183, 190-191, 193, 226, 245, 304,
Doubt, 15 297, 304-307, 311, 327, 389 387, 393-394
Drawing, 20, 22-23, 26, 34, 36-37, 41, 55, 73, 75, 77, tips for teaching, 306, 311 calculating, 51
106, 143, 151, 166, 280, 283-284, 286-287, Estimation skills, 306 FOCUS, 1, 8-9, 17-18, 25, 32, 37-41, 43, 48-49, 53,
294, 322, 338, 350, 354-355, 376, 389, 394 Ethnic, 64 65-66, 68-69, 71-72, 76, 84-86, 88, 93,
Duration, 314-315 Ethnic groups, 64 99-100, 103, 109, 120, 125, 127, 129, 136,
Dynamic geometry software, 389 Evaluation, 27, 47, 53, 77, 83, 94, 294 139, 143, 148, 151, 154, 156, 161, 164, 168,
Events, 34, 65, 67-68, 91, 107-108, 148, 315, 370, 174, 176-177, 180-181, 185-186, 189, 191,
E 373 205, 207, 209, 215, 222, 228, 231, 235, 241,
Early childhood, 114, 140, 169, 193, 278, 329, 334 Evidence, 6-7, 14, 32, 34, 47-48, 52, 57-58, 70, 83, 250, 252, 256, 267, 273-274, 278, 281-282,
Earth, 324, 370 88, 99, 104, 110-111, 114, 117, 172-173, 293-294, 300-302, 304, 306, 308, 311,
Education, 1, 12, 13, 29, 31-32, 44, 47, 60, 61-62, 68, 182, 229, 275, 280, 312, 340 315-317, 321, 323, 326, 332-337, 341, 350,
77-78, 79-80, 83-84, 88-89, 94-95, 97, 108, Excellence and equity, 140 359, 361-362, 366, 370, 374-377, 379,
111, 113, 141, 146, 171, 174, 193, 195, 225, Exceptional, 94 381-383, 390, 391-393
247, 249, 275, 277-278, 295, 297, 327, 329, Exceptions, 207, 300 Folders, 153, 340
365, 367, 386, 387, 391, 395 Exemplars, 264, 269, 366 work, 153
at home, 62, 84, 111 Exercise, 116, 215, 360 Food, 91, 93, 367-368, 370, 384
benchmarks for, 297 Exercises, 2, 51, 57, 116, 153, 161, 206, 242 Formal thinking, 330
for teachers, 108, 295 Expectation gaps, 81 Formative assessment, 16, 47-48, 51, 53, 60, 70, 75,
prevention and, 83, 94 Expectations, 27, 45, 62, 65, 71-72, 74-75, 80-82, 84, 77, 86, 115, 120, 130, 132, 134, 153, 162,
supports, 44, 47, 84, 89, 329 116, 139, 168, 192, 222, 225, 228, 245, 273, 166, 174, 184, 188-189, 196, 204, 211, 219,
Education evaluation, 83, 94 294, 324, 326, 362, 384, 390 235, 252, 260, 282, 286, 307, 310, 312, 314,
Education for gifted, 94 Experience, 3-4, 16, 34, 36-37, 41, 55, 63, 88, 98-101, 318, 340, 374, 380
equity in, 94 103, 105, 109, 127, 131, 143, 145, 159, 201, Formative assessments, 48, 75, 82
Educational research, 12, 60, 77, 111, 193, 223, 327 228, 244-245, 254, 264, 267, 275, 294, 302, Forms, 54, 88, 103, 146, 210, 374, 387
basic, 193 321, 324, 337-338, 355, 367, 375, 380 Formula, 390
Educators, 97, 102, 163, 173, 331, 387 experiences, 5, 27, 41, 63, 65, 69, 73-74, 93-94, 97, Formulas, 18, 388
Effective instruction, 82 100-102, 105, 107, 114-115, 131, 139, Forum, 98, 106
Effectiveness, 45, 51, 57, 98, 388 146-147, 149, 155, 162, 174, 179, 186, 188, Forward, 28, 47, 91, 115-116, 131, 152, 268, 294
Efficiency, 175, 230 190, 199, 206, 221, 231-232, 249, 258-259, Foundations, 100, 163, 167, 293, 392, 394
Effort, 1, 4, 15, 18, 56-58, 81, 89, 108, 115, 119, 202, 261, 277-278, 282, 289, 299, 301, 304, 318, Fowler, 111
204, 251 320-321, 323, 330-331, 334-336, 352, Fraction Bars, 280
Eighth grade, 99, 334 361-362, 372, 376, 383-384 Fractions, 29, 121, 158, 277-283, 285, 287-293, 295,
Elementary grades, 60, 275, 330, 386, 389 in school, 94, 97, 107, 320 369
Elementary school, 12, 47, 95, 98, 110, 225, 254, 275 Experiments, 93, 368 and decimals, 295
Elementary schools, 300 Expert, 88-89 improper, 291, 295
Elementary students, 388-389 Explanation, 2, 25, 54, 57, 63-64, 87, 119, 143, 259, Frames, 90, 124-126, 128, 130, 132, 139, 178-180,
Emotions, 104, 111 265, 294, 307 188-189, 191-192, 201, 203, 209-210, 216,
Empty number line, 233-234, 245-247 Extensions, 16, 27, 69, 88, 100, 195, 240 219, 234, 236, 239, 395-398
Encouragement, 89, 228 Extracurricular activities, 35 France, 64
Energy, 185 eyes, 152, 268, 337 Free time, 93
Engagement, 9, 20, 34-36, 44, 108, 111 Freedom, 66, 78
in learning, 9 F Frequency, 101, 193, 376, 379
English, 21, 36, 62-63, 66, 68, 70, 72-78, 81, 84, 94, Face value, 211 Fun, 16, 107, 116, 126, 128, 162, 186, 193, 271, 290,
106, 108, 115-116, 138, 149, 167, 180, 191, FACES, 66, 316, 333, 341, 348-350, 360, 395, 401 315, 360
197, 207, 221, 231, 245, 273, 293, 316, 325, Facilitating, 25, 34, 40, 71, 279 Functioning, 146, 335
361, 372, 383 Facilities, 108 Functions, 55, 249-250, 267, 315, 389
Middle, 94, 273 Factors, 95, 158, 162, 232, 386 Funds of knowledge, 63, 77
nonstandard, 74, 221, 325 Facts, 1, 15, 18, 49, 83, 90-91, 95, 99-103, 114, 121,
Standard, 36, 74, 207, 231, 245 130-131, 146, 153-155, 157, 163, 165-166, G
English language, 21, 63, 68, 70, 74, 76-77, 81, 84, 171-193, 234, 237, 245, 247, 250-251, 254, Galen, 173, 193, 229, 233, 247
94, 116, 138, 149, 167, 191, 197, 221, 231, 262-263, 370 Games, 36, 71, 92, 102, 122, 186, 188-189, 193, 295
245, 273, 293, 325, 361, 383 Fading, 88, 226, 228 Gasoline, 370
correct, 21, 116, 167 Failure, 57, 302, 318 Gender, 62, 77, 371
406
General education, 44 Height, 298, 300, 303, 305-307, 324, 368, 370-371, targeted, 48, 51, 83, 88, 105
Generalizability, 252 379, 381 tiered, 37, 41, 43-45, 82, 95
Generalization, 186, 249-251, 264, 269 Helping, 2-3, 12, 14, 25, 27, 66, 73, 75, 95, 102, 106, unit, 32, 34, 47, 77, 98, 131, 195-196, 277,
Generalizations, 87, 249-250, 252, 261, 366 108, 111, 120-121, 123-124, 131, 135, 142, 279-280, 297, 299-301
from arithmetic, 250 152, 154-155, 171-190, 192-193, 198-199, Instructional activities, 83, 200
Generic rubrics, 56 207, 212, 240, 244, 247, 249, 256, 258, 262, Instructional design, 10
Genres, 370 282, 291, 297, 300, 355, 365-382, 384-386 Instructional gaps, 81
Geoboard, 281, 343-344, 350, 395, 399 Hierarchy, 331, 333 Instructional objectives, 6
Geometric concepts, 335 Highly qualified, 84 Instructional strategies, 81, 83-84, 88, 295
Geometry, 67, 91-92, 297, 327, 329-331, 333-334, Hints, 28, 69, 242 Integration, 193, 197-199, 211
355, 357, 360, 363, 388-389, 392-394 History, 67 Intellectual disabilities, 89, 186
spatial sense, 91, 330 Holes, 341, 357-358 moderate, 89
Gestalt, 332 Home, 62-63, 67, 71, 73, 76, 84, 98, 101, 104-108, severe, 89
Giant journal, 55-56 111, 136, 220, 302-303, 306, 315, 344, 365, Intensity, 83, 297
Gifted programs, 93 384 Interaction, 9, 11, 81, 98, 101, 186
Gifted students, 93-95 Home visits, 98 Interactions, 21, 107, 232
Giftedness, 91 Homework, 29, 97-99, 104-106, 108, 111, 315 Interference, 29
Girls, 3, 12, 81, 377, 381 sharing, 98, 105 Internalization, 193
Glossaries, 106 Homework assignments, 106 Internet, 94, 106, 369-370
Goals, 9-10, 15, 19, 34, 41, 69-71, 85-86, 89, 97-98, homework completion, 104 Interpretation, 76, 366-367, 381
103, 109, 138, 163, 167, 190-191, 221, 245, Homonyms, 69 Interpreting, 23, 35-36, 91, 365, 367, 381
273, 293, 302, 325, 330, 335, 361, 383, 387 Hope, 110, 173, 198 Intervention, 44, 82-83, 88, 94
chart, 86, 191, 221, 245, 273, 383 Humor, 105 Interventions, 76, 82-84, 86, 94-95
lesson, 9, 15, 34, 41, 69-71, 85-86, 109, 138, 167, Hundreds chart, 3, 6-8, 22, 40, 43, 87, 133-134, 191, Interviews, 48, 50-51, 75-77, 83-84, 184, 211, 312
190-191, 221, 245, 273, 293, 325, 361, 212-214, 218-219, 221, 234, 245-246, intrinsic motivation, 305
383 252-253, 318, 395, 397 Introduction, 40, 100, 158, 288, 352, 356, 376
modifying, 10, 34 Intuition, 121, 330, 333
Google, 370 I Intuitive, 91, 115, 121-122, 135, 231, 240, 333, 346
Google Earth, 370 Id, 40, 120, 125, 133, 258, 267, 321, 327, 343-344, Invented strategies, 63, 103, 215, 219, 225, 227-232,
Government, 247, 368-369 350, 353, 357, 370 236-240, 244-245, 272
Governors, 1, 103, 387, 391 IDEAL, 99, 281 for addition, 230, 232, 236, 240, 244-245
Grades, 1, 12, 13, 29, 31, 45, 47-48, 53-55, 60, 61, Ideas, 2-11, 14-16, 18, 20-28, 32-38, 40-41, 43, 47, for multiplication, 272
66-67, 71, 77-78, 79, 93-95, 97, 99-100, 102, 49, 52-56, 65-67, 70, 73-75, 81, 83-84, 86, for subtraction, 103, 238, 240, 244
113, 135-136, 141-142, 145, 147-148, 88, 90-91, 93, 97, 99, 101-102, 104, 106, models to support, 232
155-156, 158, 162, 164, 171-172, 191, 193, 109, 114-115, 117, 120-123, 127-128, 132, Issues, 16, 65, 90, 103, 106, 109, 135, 149, 166, 231,
195, 212, 223, 225-226, 247, 249-250, 134-137, 142-143, 153, 158, 161-163, 240, 370, 375
254-257, 261, 267, 269, 274-275, 277-279, 166-169, 171, 173, 175, 179, 183, 189, Items, 67, 85, 92, 118, 158, 160, 196, 198, 203,
281-282, 284, 293, 297, 301, 304, 309, 317, 196-199, 202, 206, 212, 217, 219-220, 205-206, 256, 268, 282, 284, 303, 306-307,
321, 327, 329-331, 334-335, 341, 351, 361, 228-229, 232-233, 243, 245-246, 249, 251, 313, 320, 336, 367, 373-374
365, 369-370, 376, 381-382, 386, 387-389, 254-256, 263, 265-266, 273-274, 279, 285,
391-394, 395 287, 289, 291-294, 303, 307, 317, 321-322, J
Graph, 23-24, 90, 92, 103, 136-137, 367, 369, 326, 330-331, 334-335, 338-339, 351-352, Japan, 158
373-386, 387 354, 357, 361-363, 367, 373-375, 378, 381, Join problems, 145, 149
Graph paper, 90 384, 391, 394 Joint, 345
Graphic organizer, 69, 82, 102 sequence of, 34, 321 Judging, 27, 164, 302
Graphic organizers, 36, 84, 86 identity, 264 Judgment, 57, 215
Graphical representations, 374 Illustration, 5, 54-55, 128, 186 Justice, 62
Graphs, 2, 91, 103, 136, 220, 365-366, 374-381, Illustrations, 76, 85, 90, 105, 186, 265, 351, 383
383-386, 387-389 Immersion, 186
Grass, 302 K
Immigrant students, 78 Key words, 71, 85, 163-164
gravity, 324 Immigration, 111
Grid, The, 49, 356-357, 383-384, 397 Kim, 21, 29
Implementation, 111 Kindergarten, 38, 56, 71, 78, 109, 116, 119, 124, 130,
Grids, 86, 281, 356-357, 359, 397 Importance, 3, 8-9, 68, 97-98, 101-103, 107, 114, 209,
Group discussion, 44 132-133, 138, 155, 164, 166, 172, 184,
279, 387 195-196, 207, 212-213, 250, 254, 267, 273,
Groupable models, 200-201 Improper fractions, 295
Grouping, 5, 37, 44-45, 73, 84, 120, 131, 155, 191, 295, 324-325, 330, 338, 355, 362, 369, 383,
Income, 295 391
196-199, 201-202, 204-205, 219-220, 228, independent practice, 87
348, 392 Kindergarten students, 78, 325, 391
Independent study, 92 Knowledge, 2, 4-6, 9, 18, 25, 27-28, 34, 36, 43, 53, 58,
by ability, 44 Individual differences, 80-81
flexible, 44-45, 84, 196 63-65, 69, 72, 77, 81-85, 87-88, 91, 93, 95,
Influence, 23, 148, 163, 234, 284, 295 101, 115, 122, 124, 128, 131, 135-136, 173,
Groups, 5-6, 20-22, 27-28, 34-35, 41, 43-45, 48, 55, Information, 1, 3-5, 10, 16, 20, 25, 33-36, 47-48,
64, 68, 70, 73, 75, 81-82, 84, 87, 91, 99, 175, 178, 182-183, 188, 193, 197, 204, 215,
51-52, 54-55, 59, 63, 67, 77, 80, 83-85, 231, 234, 275, 277, 279, 283, 295, 302, 307,
101, 106, 122-123, 125, 131, 135, 142, 90-91, 98-99, 103, 106, 111, 140, 153, 164,
155-157, 160-161, 167, 171, 178, 184, 361-362, 389
166, 169, 177, 212, 223, 246, 257, 265, 275, prior, 4-5, 9, 18, 27-28, 43, 65, 69, 72, 82, 84-85,
186-187, 192, 195-200, 202-207, 210-211, 290, 304, 307, 365, 367, 369-370, 374-375,
213-214, 221-222, 257, 266, 268, 271, 286, 101, 115, 361
377-378, 380-381, 383-384, 387 prior knowledge, 4-5, 9, 18, 27-28, 43, 65, 72, 82,
288, 293, 304, 323, 325, 335, 339, 344, Information age, 111, 140, 169, 223, 275
351-352, 355, 362, 369, 374-376, 378, 384, 84-85, 101
Initiative, 97 topic, 6, 93
394 In/out, 373
Groups:, 222 vertical, 115
Inquiry, 10, 87, 91, 119 Knowledge of mathematics, 81
focal, 84, 99, 167, 221, 293, 325, 376 Instincts, 243
Growing pattern, 269-270 Institute of Education Sciences, 94, 278
Growth, 11, 47-48, 59, 83, 86, 92-93, 303, 379 Institute of Education Sciences (IES), 278 L
Guessing, 162 Instruction, 1, 5, 10-11, 13, 16, 18, 29, 31-46, 47-48, Labels, 369, 371-372, 374, 384
Guidance, 89, 105, 173, 227 50-52, 58-60, 61, 64-66, 68, 71, 73, 75-77, Landmark numbers, 246
guided practice, 10 79, 81-84, 86-89, 91-92, 94-95, 97-98, 100, Language, 21, 26, 36, 43-44, 52, 55-56, 59, 62-65,
Guidelines, 55, 367, 386 105, 113, 131, 135, 141, 143, 171-173, 68-78, 81, 84-85, 94, 100, 102, 105, 108,
Guidelines for Assessment and Instruction in Statistics 195-196, 215, 225, 229, 240, 249, 254, 275, 110, 116, 123, 138, 147, 149, 161, 167, 176,
Education (GAISE), 386 277-281, 283, 295, 297, 299-301, 329, 334, 180, 191, 195, 197-200, 206-207, 209-211,
Guides, 8, 86 346, 365, 367, 382, 386, 387, 390, 391, 395 221, 231, 245, 259, 261, 263, 266, 273, 282,
and gender, 77 288, 291, 293, 300, 302, 305, 307, 315-316,
H balanced, 390 325, 327, 329-330, 334, 340, 355, 361-362,
Handling, 160 differentiating, 31-46, 65, 95 372, 383
Handouts, 188, 351 indirect, 300 body, 52
Hard copy, 252 individualized, 33, 89 clear, 68, 71-72, 76, 85, 210, 245, 273, 291, 329,
Hardware, 324 learning strategies, 94 362
Harvard University, 12, 94 metacognitive, 89 delay, 231
Health, 365 strategy, 38-40, 50, 66, 73, 75, 81, 84, 86-89, 95, difference, 26, 68, 71-72, 74, 100, 105, 197, 207,
heart, 4, 34, 104, 109, 264 105, 173, 229, 240, 254 231, 261
407
receptive, 73 Library, 106, 243-244, 267, 309, 316, 336, 343-344 227, 238, 240, 249, 251, 254, 258, 260-262,
written, 36, 63, 69-70, 76, 84-85, 102, 105, 116, life experiences, 149, 186 273, 275, 278, 287, 293, 298, 300-302, 313,
138, 161, 197, 199, 207, 209-210, 261, life skills, 89, 91, 101 325, 347, 351, 375-376, 381, 387-389,
291, 361, 383 Lighting, 37 392-393
Language and culture, 62, 72, 77 Limitations, 89, 172, 389 of words, 85
and assessment, 77 Limits, 385 search for, 103, 302, 387
Language development, 68, 70 Line graphs, 380 Meanings, 3, 71, 85, 90, 140, 141-166, 168-169, 173,
use, 68, 70 Line symmetry, 354, 362 226, 250, 278, 291-292, 375
Language experience approach, 55 Linear measurement, 279, 303, 309, 392 Measurement, 6, 39, 67, 72, 89, 92, 107, 135,
Language instruction, 77 Lines, 5, 89, 110, 150-151, 156, 159, 207, 214, 221, 156-158, 161, 167, 221, 278-279, 281, 293,
Language learning, 77 233, 298-299, 309, 311, 325, 330, 334, 336, 297-327, 355, 366, 392-394
Language skills, 68, 73 344, 346, 348-350, 362, 378 attribute, 297-302, 304, 307-308, 310, 315, 321,
Languages, 62-63, 68, 70, 73, 77, 115, 199 Liquids, 323, 368 324-325
Large group, 53 Listening, 4, 7, 10, 33-34, 48, 55, 64, 69, 73, 87, 134, composite, 393
Latinos, 111 204, 213, 237, 339, 374 meaning and process of, 298
Leaders, 98 to understand, 7 models for fractions, 281
Leadership, 46, 77, 99, 111 Literacy, 106, 135, 247, 365, 386 of area, 297, 321-322
Leading questions, 52 Literature, 65, 68, 303, 369 of mass, 324
Leads, 32, 82, 164, 229, 269, 271 Local government, 368 of time, 92, 135, 279, 297, 299, 314-315, 317
Learners, 4, 6, 9-10, 16, 19, 21, 23, 27-28, 34-35, logical reasoning, 56 proficiency, 92
37-38, 42, 45-46, 47-48, 55, 63-64, 68, Longitudinal study, 247 terms, 167, 221, 279, 293, 301-302, 306-307, 320,
73-74, 77-78, 80-82, 85-89, 91-94, 98, 116, Lunchtime, 93 323-325, 355, 392
135, 138, 149, 167, 187, 189, 191, 197, 221, variables, 366
230-231, 245, 273-274, 293-294, 325, 361, M Measurements, 6, 22, 74, 91, 220, 280, 297, 300-301,
383 Magnitude, 295 304-307, 312, 322, 367
active, 48 Main ideas, 27-28, 47, 84 Measures, 67, 74, 82, 135-136, 151, 221, 230, 279,
Learning, 2, 4-6, 9-12, 14, 16, 19-21, 23, 25, 27-29, Management, 85 297-298, 300-301, 303-307, 311, 314,
34-38, 40-41, 43-45, 47-60, 63, 65-66, Manipulative materials, 88 323-324, 367-368, 384
68-70, 73-74, 77, 80-89, 91-92, 94-95, Manipulatives, 2-3, 8, 12, 16, 18-19, 24-26, 36-37, Media, 365
97-108, 111, 115, 118, 122, 131, 134, 140, 53-56, 69, 73, 82, 84, 87, 98-99, 106, 152, advertising, 365
143, 148, 155, 157, 167-169, 172-174, 178, 166-167, 183, 201, 227, 243-246, 262, 265, Median, 365
180, 182, 184, 186, 189-190, 193, 200, 215, 267-268, 270, 283, 287, 293-294, 316, Meetings, 98
223, 231, 234, 247, 251, 253, 267, 275, 343-344, 383 class, 98
279-280, 287, 295, 302-303, 308, 315, 325, Maps, 69, 355, 369 Memories, 97
327, 329, 331, 335-336, 352, 355, 357, 363, Marking, 214 Memorization, 18, 100, 103, 171-174, 186, 189-190,
366-367, 386, 389, 391 Massachusetts, 386 193
Learning:, 60, 94, 275 Mastery, 54, 60, 101-102, 130, 163, 171-174, 181-182, Memory, 35, 67, 85, 90, 174-175, 186, 191, 228, 359
and problem solving, 29, 102 184, 186, 190, 192-193 constructed, 228
connected, 5, 9, 52, 66, 91, 104-105, 200 Mastery of basic facts, 130, 163, 186 Memory aids, 85
contexts for, 35 Matching, 55, 89, 115, 120, 123-124, 131, 156, 200, Mental models, 234
distance, 59, 303, 325, 355 208, 210, 299-301, 360, 384 Messages, 68, 102, 111
enjoyable, 189 Materials, 3-4, 8, 25, 35-37, 51-52, 54, 84-85, 88, 109, Metric system, 304, 327
events, 34, 65, 68, 91, 107-108, 148, 315 117, 122-123, 127, 131-132, 138-139, 143, Mexico, 64, 74
in small groups, 21, 27-28, 41, 70, 84, 178 161, 167, 191, 196-197, 201-202, 204-213, Middle grades, 94-95, 195, 277, 327, 388
mastery, 54, 60, 101-102, 172-174, 182, 184, 186, 221, 227-228, 241, 243, 245-246, 265, Middle school, 12, 29, 45, 80, 94-95, 99, 111, 254,
190, 193 267-270, 273-274, 293, 312, 325-327, 335, 275, 277, 295, 300, 327, 363, 380, 390
readiness, 34-37, 41, 43-45 339, 341-342, 345, 361, 370-374, 383-384, Midpoint, 34
scaffold, 74 395-397 Milk, 158, 220, 303, 368, 373
strengths and weaknesses, 82, 84 adaptation of, 51, 54 Minority students, 77
to learn, 4, 6, 10-11, 14, 20-21, 34-35, 44, 65-66, for children with special needs, 293 Misconceptions, 20, 27-28, 47, 49, 53-54, 56, 189,
68, 74, 77, 80, 89, 91, 104, 108, 148, manipulative materials, 88 254, 294, 308
172-173, 189, 315, 367 Math standards, 145, 156 Mnemonics, 84-85
Learning activities, 86 Mathematical knowledge, 2, 25, 87, 389 Mode, 383
Learning center, 40, 134 Mathematical power, 94 Modeling, 8, 14, 21, 29, 86, 88, 146-147, 153, 157,
Learning disabilities, 83-86, 94-95, 105, 172, 193 Mathematical processes, 15, 48, 53 166, 174, 210, 227-228, 230, 242, 250-251,
instructional strategies for, 83 Mathematical understanding, 1, 10, 16, 18, 24, 45, 75 283, 291, 345, 391
Learning disability, 82, 95 Mathematics, 1-12, 13-29, 31-35, 39-40, 44-45, 47, Models, 7, 10, 14, 23, 25, 29, 43, 49, 52, 57-58, 65,
Learning environment, 34-35, 37 49, 53, 55, 58, 60, 61-73, 75-78, 79-84, 86, 67, 69, 73-74, 77, 82, 84-88, 95, 124, 129,
Learning environment:, 37 88-89, 91-95, 97-111, 113-114, 127, 135, 138, 142, 150-154, 159, 161, 164, 178,
Learning environments, 45, 102 138, 140, 141, 146, 149, 154, 163, 166-167, 195-196, 199-201, 205, 207-211, 213, 218,
Learning experiences, 101 169, 171-174, 177, 179, 185, 188-191, 193, 226-228, 232, 234-235, 240-243, 245-246,
Learning goals, 9, 19, 34, 41, 69-70, 86, 190 195, 209, 215, 220-221, 223, 225-226, 280-282, 286-289, 295, 299-301, 309,
Learning objectives, 36, 69, 91 230-232, 241, 245, 247, 249-250, 254, 262, 322-324, 335, 340, 345-346, 389, 392, 394
Learning opportunities, 51, 68, 100, 190 264, 267, 271-273, 275, 277, 282, 291, 293, to support invented strategies, 232
Learning outcomes, 34 295, 297, 304, 307-308, 318, 325, 327, Modification, 81-82
Learning process, 6, 34, 47, 87, 89 329-330, 335, 337, 347, 355, 357-358, 361, Money, 55, 67, 89, 91, 148-149, 164, 201, 234, 298,
Learning stations, 308, 325 363, 365-368, 370, 376, 383, 386, 387-388, 307, 317-321, 375, 379, 394
Learning Strategies, 9, 94 390, 391-393, 395 counting sets of coins, 318
Learning targets, 53-54 alternative assessments, 86 Monitoring, 18, 82-83, 86, 90, 189, 379
Learning theories, 9 center, 1, 6, 40, 60, 62, 77-78, 94-95, 103, 108, progress, 82-83, 86
Lecture, 4, 10 387, 391 Monitoring assessments, 82
Lenses, 386 lessons, 8-9, 27, 29, 32-35, 44, 69, 76, 83-84, 105, Monitors, 84
Lesson planning, 35, 249 111, 247, 308 motion, 72, 353-354, 357, 395, 400-401
Lessons, 8-9, 27, 29, 32-35, 41, 43-44, 48, 54, 69, 76, virtual manipulatives, 26, 106, 267 Motivation, 34-35, 38, 45, 305, 343
83-84, 105, 111, 123, 143, 176, 247, 285, Mathematics instruction, 1, 18, 32-33, 45, 64-65, 68, intrinsic, 305
301, 308, 317, 360 71, 73, 83, 135, 390 Mouse, 201, 269, 275, 303
scripted, 54 cooperative groups, 73 Movement, 41, 89, 116, 152, 273, 374
structuring, 54 diverse learners, 64 Movies, 370
Letters, 86, 93, 98, 101, 106, 108, 214, 261, 368, 374 Mathematics performance, 78 Multimedia, 73
to parents, 106 matter, 10, 28, 74, 104, 107, 115, 153-155, 214, 226, Multiple disabilities, 89
Level, 18, 20, 23, 32, 35, 38-40, 42, 44, 56-58, 60, 231, 243, 263-265, 275, 313, 324, 390 Multiple entry points, 9, 19
62-63, 69, 82-84, 89, 91-94, 97, 99, 102, Mean, 3, 7, 25, 27, 52, 69, 74, 76-77, 81, 93, 101, 109, Multiplication, 1, 67, 95, 142, 155-162, 164, 167-168,
104, 109, 111, 116, 118, 120, 123, 128, 131-132, 135, 158, 163, 168, 177, 225, 228, 171, 184-185, 193, 200, 263, 272, 295, 366,
131-134, 136, 138, 143, 146, 152, 157-158, 258, 265, 267, 275, 277, 286, 313, 325, 365, 376, 394, 395, 397
163, 167, 185, 191, 196, 213, 217, 221, 376, 384-386 Multiplication and division, 155-159, 161
225-226, 243, 245, 250, 256, 273, 279, 293, Meaning, 4-6, 9, 14, 16, 24-25, 36, 44, 63, 65, 69, Multiplicative situations, 167
300-302, 304, 308, 317, 319, 324-325, 72-74, 76-77, 83, 85, 87-88, 90, 101, 103, Multitasking, 34
331-341, 343, 345-347, 350, 352-356, 131, 142, 149-150, 152-153, 156, 161, Music, 35, 93, 297
358-359, 361, 365-367, 376, 383 163-164, 169, 177, 191, 199, 202, 209, 211,
408
Objectives, 6, 36, 49-50, 58, 69-71, 82, 91, 163, Pictures, 43, 55-56, 58, 69, 71, 73-75, 85, 88, 100,
N 301-302, 330, 375 103, 106, 109, 119-120, 123, 143, 149, 152,
Names for numbers, 198-199, 202, 207 Observation, 35, 48-50, 58, 125, 166, 262, 266, 312, 160-161, 166, 206, 266, 268, 273, 284, 286,
National Academy of Sciences, 2 351, 369, 379 289, 294, 308, 321, 336, 353, 355, 372, 378,
National Assessment of Educational Progress, 295, focused, 49 387, 401
327, 330 Observations, 48, 50, 59, 130, 256, 262, 265, 326, Place value, 6, 8, 49, 51, 72, 75, 92-93, 99, 107, 121,
National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP), 333, 335, 361, 373, 378 131, 133, 155, 195-197, 199, 201, 204, 209,
330 oceans, 373 211, 215, 221, 223, 225-226, 228-229,
National Association of Secondary School Principals, Odd and even relationships, 265 244-245, 252, 318, 392-394
99 open questions, 37, 39-41 Place values, 199, 201, 318
National Center for Education Statistics, 62, 77 openness, 232 Placement, 168, 374
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, 1-2, 12, Opinions, 98 Planning, 11, 34-35, 37, 48, 52, 56, 61-78, 79-95, 159,
18, 29, 40, 45, 47, 62, 77-78, 80, 83, 94-95, Oral language, 206 231, 249, 385
106, 111, 114, 140, 169, 172, 174, 191, 193, Order property, 154, 162 for assessment, 75
215, 223, 226, 247, 249, 275, 293, 295, 297, Org, 29, 40, 45, 60, 94, 99, 103-104, 106, 111, 120, learning activities, 86
304, 308, 327, 329-330, 337, 358, 363, 365, 125-126, 128-129, 133, 140, 169, 193, 258, plants, 374
367, 370, 376, 386 267, 286, 314, 321, 327, 343-344, 350, 353, Play, 10, 27, 40, 42, 102, 119-120, 123-125, 131, 152,
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM), 357, 370 160, 169, 199, 232, 255, 271, 356, 372
106 Organization, 68, 119, 374 blocks, 356
National Governors Association, 1, 103, 387, 391 Organizers, 36, 84, 86 Plays, 68, 108, 121, 125, 198-199
National Research Council, 1-2, 12, 81, 95, 115, 135, study, 86 Plot, 367, 380-381
140, 175, 193, 226, 247, 300, 329, 387 Organizing, 35-36, 81, 86, 92, 160, 186, 195, 228, Plots, 366, 376, 380
Native language, 36, 68, 70, 72, 75, 108, 138, 180, 295, 370, 375, 377, 383-384 Pockets, 133, 202, 374-375
207, 293, 325, 372 Orientation, 229, 330, 332, 335, 352-353, 391, 400 Podcasts, 106
Nature, 10, 18, 44-45, 103, 106, 173, 279, 306, 330, Outcomes, 34, 51, 80-82, 93, 111, 258 Pointing, 74, 91, 106, 118, 172, 263, 315-317, 337,
366, 381, 384 Outlines, 81, 174, 342-344 356, 362
NCES, 77, 247 Overlap, 6, 299, 310-311, 347, 397 Policy, 60, 102
needs, 18, 22-23, 33, 37, 41, 43, 45, 58, 65, 68-69, Polyhedron, 348
72-74, 76, 81-83, 86, 88-89, 91, 95, 108-109, P Population, 89, 365, 370, 383
125, 138, 148, 160, 164, 167, 186, 191, 210, Pacing, 186 Portfolio, 37
221, 245, 273, 293, 311, 319, 325, 361-362, Painting, 307 Position statements, 68
376-377, 383 Palm, 130, 307 Posters, 84, 384
Negative numbers, 23, 64 PANs, 257, 324 Pot, 270
Net, 26, 60 Paradigm, 15 Potential, 11, 16, 18-19, 45, 51, 74, 85, 95, 100, 148,
Neuroscience, 46 Parallel lines, 330, 348-350, 362 201-202, 240, 331, 380, 390
New Math, 109 Parent conferences, 48 Poverty, 108
New York, 29, 60, 77, 95, 111, 140, 169, 193, 223, Parental Involvement, 97 power, 12, 26, 45, 64-65, 68, 78, 94, 102, 297, 317,
275, 295, 327, 363, 386 Parents, 29, 58, 91, 97-111, 136, 153, 302-303, 369 331, 340, 367
News, 111, 220, 370, 379 communication with, 97, 108 balance of, 65
Newsletters, 98, 106 involvement, 97, 111 Power Solids, 340
Newspapers, 304, 365, 369, 375 Parking lot, 100, 147, 369 Practice, 2-4, 10, 14, 16, 18, 22, 25, 29, 36, 40, 57-58,
Noise, 35, 37 Participants, 21, 100 64, 70, 73, 77, 82, 86-88, 94-95, 100,
Nonproportional models, 201 Participation, 21, 72, 105, 187, 383 102-103, 105-106, 108-109, 111, 115, 122,
Norm, 230 Partitioning, 54, 58, 66, 99, 168, 251, 277-279, 282, 125-127, 130-132, 136, 138, 143-144, 146,
Norms, 29 284-287, 289-290, 293-295, 300 149-150, 152, 154-159, 162-164, 166, 173,
Notebooks, 106 Partnerships, 65 175-180, 184, 188-189, 191-193, 197,
Notes, 48-49, 72, 84, 108, 172, 299, 366, 377, 380 Path, 40-41, 52, 89, 92, 309, 313-315, 325-326, 200-202, 206, 208-209, 213, 216, 218-219,
anecdotal, 48 355-356, 359, 401 231-235, 237, 241, 251-252, 255-256, 259,
sticky, 377, 380 Patience, 122 261-262, 264, 267-268, 272, 275, 278,
Novice, 89 Pattern recognition, 122 282-283, 285-286, 289, 291, 295, 300,
Number and operations, 92, 100-101, 167, 169, 191, Patterned set recognition, 122 302-303, 305, 312, 337-338, 345, 350,
392-394 Patterning, 267 352-353, 355, 359, 371-372, 375, 377, 381,
Number line, 3, 6-8, 43, 67, 90-91, 115-116, 148, Patterns, 4, 36, 54, 56, 66, 100, 102, 116, 121-123, 387-390
151-152, 162, 177, 214, 217, 219, 233-235, 130, 133-134, 172, 190, 195, 207, 212-214, Preassessments, 34
245-247, 273, 280, 313, 369, 380-381 217, 221, 249-253, 261, 267-273, 275, 343, Predicting, 56, 316, 329, 357
Number lines, 150-151, 156, 159, 214, 221, 298 353 Prediction, 86
Number patterns, 133, 212, 221, 271, 273 growing, 249, 267, 269-271 Pregrouped models, 200
Number relationships, 6, 9, 19, 114-115, 119, 121, number, 4, 100, 102, 116, 121-123, 130, 133-134, Prekindergarten, 140, 193, 223, 275, 295, 325, 327,
127, 130, 136, 171, 173, 176, 178, 188, 213, 172, 195, 207, 212-214, 217, 221, 363, 386
221, 244, 252, 259, 271-272, 392, 394 249-253, 261, 269, 271-273, 343 Prerequisite knowledge, 182, 361
Number sense, 3, 19-20, 29, 90-91, 102-104, 113-137, repeating, 249, 267-269, 271 Preschoolers, 295
139-140, 155, 169, 172, 177-178, 182, 195, Paying attention, 21, 160, 234, 355, 390 Presentation, 60, 70, 327, 352, 384
204, 214, 226-227, 229, 295, 316, 320, 392 Peak, 94-95 prevention, 82-83, 94-95
decimal, 195 Peck, 367, 386 Pride, 65, 105, 110
Numbers, 4-9, 15, 17-19, 22-23, 26-27, 38-40, 42-43, Pedagogy, 12, 60, 99-100 Primary language, 68, 77
49-51, 53-54, 58, 64, 66, 72, 85, 89-91, Pencils, 117, 228 Principals, 97-111
101-102, 104, 107, 114-122, 124-128, Pentominoes, 359 Print, 369, 395
130-139, 143, 145-146, 148, 150-155, 158, Perception, 16 Printing, 247
160, 162-166, 171, 174, 178-179, 185, Performance, 10, 47-49, 51, 53, 55-59, 78, 82, 88, 95, Prior experiences, 115
187-188, 190-193, 195-202, 204-205, 207, 105, 111, 193, 226, 247, 314, 327 Probability, 92, 148, 182, 367
209-221, 225-238, 240, 244-246, 250-253, level of, 56, 82 Problem solving, 2, 13-29, 32, 36, 45, 48, 65, 69, 75,
255, 257-258, 260-261, 263-266, 269, Performance assessment, 314 85, 87, 90-91, 102, 111, 142, 153-154, 223,
271-274, 277-280, 283-285, 288-289, 291, Performance indicators, 55, 57 360, 387
294-295, 298-299, 301, 303, 305, 313-315, Period, 28, 48, 91, 104, 121, 195, 228, 289, 307, 314, teaching through, 14-15, 20
317-320, 355-356, 365-366, 368, 380-383, 352, 369 Problem-solving, 14, 16, 27, 102
389, 391-392, 394 Perseverance, 103 Procedural fluency, 2, 52, 101, 387
Numeration, 93, 121, 195, 201, 215, 221, 223 Personal characteristics, 80 Procedural knowledge, 193, 197, 215
Numeration concepts, 223 Personal experience, 324 Procedures, 1-2, 5-6, 8-9, 17-18, 51, 63, 65, 78, 90,
Numerators, 291 Personnel, 368 101-102, 226, 230, 387, 390
Numerical representations, 85 Pets, 35-36, 67, 136, 314, 367-368, 370, 374, 384 Processing, 89
Phi Delta Kappan, 60, 111 task, 89
Philadelphia, 60 Product, 34-35, 37, 41, 45, 56, 65, 142, 156, 162, 268,
O 352, 370
Object, 25, 39, 115, 117-118, 136, 204, 221-222, 256, Philosophy, 15
Photographs, 67 Productive disposition, 2, 34, 387
277, 279, 284, 286, 297-300, 302, 306-312, Products, 37, 53, 86, 99, 193, 247, 251, 332-334
314, 324-325, 336, 352, 355, 365, 376-377, Picture, 17, 19, 33-34, 48, 50, 52-54, 57, 70-72, 86-87,
91, 100, 102, 107, 119, 162, 166, 168, 180, Professional development, 18, 108-109, 111, 390
380, 393, 401 Profiles, 34-35, 41, 44-45
Objective, 8-9, 36, 38, 43-44, 50, 57, 85, 162, 190, 210, 294, 305, 307, 326, 365-366, 374,
376-377, 380, 383 Programs, 93, 95, 98, 186, 232, 353, 383
198, 208, 323 Progress monitoring, 83
Picture dictionary, 70
409
Project, 4, 37, 91, 111, 178, 216, 237, 244, 258, 275, 151, 155, 161-164, 166, 168, 171, 173-178, risks, 3, 232, 365
293, 347, 384 181-184, 186, 188-190, 192, 206, 218-219, Role-play, 27
Projection, 133, 139, 168, 201, 221 232-234, 249-272, 274-275, 285-286, 288, Roles, 45, 65, 89, 101, 180, 241, 354, 356
Projects, 93, 136 290, 305, 312, 326, 329-363, 366, 370, 374, Roots, 70
Prompts, 22, 55, 66, 82-83, 87-88, 90, 102, 105, 152, 386, 387-389, 391-392 Rotational symmetry, 346, 354, 362
217, 382-383 Reasoning and proof, 2, 387 Rotations, 329, 331, 352
dependency on, 88 Recall, 9, 39, 97, 171, 173, 175, 185, 188-191, 245, Rounding, 305
Property, 4, 15, 154-155, 162, 174, 184, 225, 252, 300, 360 Routines, 29, 90-91
263-265, 321, 339-340, 351-352, 362, 389, Receiving, 137, 167, 321 Rubber band, 56, 200, 324
395, 400 Receptive language, 73 Rubric, 49, 55-59
Proportional reasoning, 388 Recognition, 33, 86, 122-123, 197, 331-332, 334-337, Rubrics, 55-56, 58
Psychologists, 173 355, 358 generic rubrics, 56
Psychology, 94-95, 295, 327 Recommendations, 21, 27, 105, 189 performance indicators, 55
Publications, 94, 369, 386 Recording, 35, 48, 52, 59, 63, 67, 69, 102, 135, 143, Rulers, 297-299, 301, 312-314, 325, 394
Publishing, 111, 140, 169, 223, 275 190-192, 199, 203-204, 206, 221-222, Rules, 9, 11, 16, 18, 24, 32, 90, 104, 155, 162, 185,
Puzzles, 341-342, 352 230-231, 233, 235, 241-245, 270, 316, 241, 271, 273, 333, 362, 372
322-323, 325, 344, 357, 359, 395, 398-399 Rush, 73, 231
Q Records, 7, 87, 184, 228, 241, 271, 370
Quality, 15, 17, 44, 82, 84, 91, 97 Recycling, 154, 368, 373 S
Quantitative reasoning, 388 Reference, 51, 72, 90, 130, 263, 293, 303, 305, 353, Samples, 58-59
Quantities, 3, 54, 91, 115, 120, 122, 130, 133, 372 SAT, 55, 104, 116
135-136, 138, 142, 145, 147-149, 153, 156, Referral, 83 Scaffold, 74
160-161, 183, 196-199, 208, 220-221, 251, Reflecting, 15, 19, 29, 32, 201, 300, 357 Scaffolding, 41, 82
255, 259, 261-263, 277-280, 284-285, 288, break, 201 Scale, 52, 55-57, 59, 93, 256, 258, 298, 302, 324, 342,
293-294, 310, 317, 388-389, 391 Reflective thinking, 9, 122 376, 379-380, 383
Quantity, 5, 67, 85, 107, 114, 124, 127, 137, 142, Register, 214, 313, 379-380 Scales, 93, 299, 324, 374
145-147, 158, 168, 182, 196-198, 201-202, Regrouping, 8, 38, 197, 215, 219, 225, 235, 240-242 Schema, 5, 60
204-206, 209, 255, 262-263, 277-280, 286, Regularity, 4, 54, 103, 132, 155, 162, 177-178, 206, Schemas, 4, 10
289-290, 309-310, 376, 383, 388 251-252, 267, 387, 389-390 Schemes, 118, 243, 295, 372, 374
Question mark, 39-40, 147, 187 Reinforcement, 105, 186, 189, 201 School, 1, 12, 26, 29, 35, 38, 45, 47, 60, 62-63, 65, 73,
Questioning, 11, 19, 29, 50, 87, 105, 107, 232, 241, RELATE, 10, 15, 22, 26, 83-84, 87-89, 120, 124, 214, 78, 80-81, 92-95, 97-99, 101-104, 107-111,
260, 294 222, 279, 285, 303, 315-316, 367, 394 114-115, 117, 121, 131, 133, 140, 148, 155,
for understanding, 11, 29 Relational understanding, 5-6, 8-12, 14, 18, 23-24, 32, 169, 193, 215, 220, 223, 225-226, 229, 247,
Questionnaires, 35 34, 45, 199 254, 275, 277, 295, 297, 300, 302, 307-308,
Question(s), 384 Relationship, 8, 25, 97, 103-104, 122, 125, 127, 314-315, 317, 320, 327, 329-330, 333-336,
Questions, 1, 3, 7-8, 10-11, 15-16, 21, 27, 36-42, 130-131, 138, 147, 150, 153-154, 182-183, 347, 357, 363, 367-370, 380-381, 386,
45-46, 50, 52, 54, 65, 68-70, 72, 83-85, 87, 198-199, 228, 245, 249-250, 253-254, 260, 387-390, 391
90, 98-102, 105-107, 109-110, 114, 116, 119, 264, 269-270, 273, 278, 280, 283, 286-288, School day, 308, 315
125, 133, 135-137, 164, 166, 192, 206, 217, 298, 300, 304, 312, 316, 334, 392-393 School district, 47
232, 245, 249-250, 252-253, 255, 270, 274, Relationships, 3, 5-6, 8-10, 18-19, 25, 54, 56, 64, 66, School leaders, 98
279, 291, 294, 303, 311, 313, 340, 365-370, 88, 92, 103, 114-115, 119, 121-124, 126-128, School lunch, 92, 117
377-378, 381-385, 388, 391 130-133, 135-138, 142-144, 147, 149, 157, School mathematics, 12, 47, 62, 65, 78, 80, 94-95,
closed, 10 171, 173, 176, 178, 188, 193, 200-201, 97-98, 104, 110-111, 193, 215, 223, 226,
easy, 274, 369, 377, 384 212-214, 217, 221, 244, 249, 252, 255, 247, 275, 327, 329-330, 335, 347, 357, 363,
encouraging, 11, 21, 217, 245 258-260, 262, 265, 271-273, 293, 302, 304, 367, 386, 390
formulating, 365, 367 330-334, 346, 348, 352, 387-388, 392-394 NCTM principles, 80, 98, 215, 329, 335
generating, 10, 101, 250, 366 Remembering, 83, 120 Schooling, 77, 97-98, 111
harder, 68 Reminders, 85 Schools, 47, 62, 82, 94, 108, 298, 300, 365
ideas for, 114, 279, 378 Repeating patterns, 267-269, 271 Science, 36, 92, 95, 104, 247, 297, 367-368, 373, 384
leading, 52, 250 Reports, 1, 16 new, 36, 95, 384
leads, 164 Representation, 2-3, 15, 23-25, 36, 40, 48, 54, 90, Sciences, 2, 94, 278
on tests, 84 124, 201, 206, 209, 218, 246, 252, 264, 273, Scope, 334
probing, 50 291, 294-295, 314, 365, 370, 374-375, 380, Scores, 47, 57, 357
purposeful, 16, 36, 366 387-388 Scoring, 55, 57
scaffolding, 41 Representations, 2-3, 8, 14, 23-26, 28-29, 36, 54, 56, rubric, 55, 57
what if, 16, 27, 274, 313, 381 58, 73, 84-86, 88-90, 120, 127-128, 130-131, Search, 85, 103, 107, 202, 253, 302, 309, 336, 387
190, 197, 201, 205-206, 217, 246, 249, 261, Searches, 216
266, 287, 294-295, 365, 374-375 Seating, 37
R of mathematical ideas, 24
Race, 62, 366 Second Step, 48, 270
reproduction, 18, 78, 223 Secondary school, 99
Rand Corporation, 275 Research, 1-4, 12, 18, 21, 29, 32, 47, 51, 60, 70, 73,
Random numbers, 125 Section, 27, 55, 68, 81, 109, 125, 149, 201, 207, 215,
75, 77-78, 81-82, 86, 92, 94-95, 102, 219, 238, 246, 254, 311, 313, 357, 372, 377
Random selection, 134 104-105, 108-109, 111, 115, 118, 130, 135,
Range, 6, 9, 16, 32, 69, 82, 94, 116-117, 130, 147, Self, 18, 22, 38, 53, 57-58, 70, 75, 77, 84, 86-88,
140, 143, 146, 169, 174-176, 189, 193, 223, 90-91, 105, 122, 152, 189, 368
164, 226, 229, 305-306, 335, 342, 381 226, 228-229, 247, 254, 275, 284, 295,
Rates, 186 self-monitoring, 18
300-301, 305, 314, 327, 329, 331, 363, 376, Self-regulation, 18
Rational numbers, 295 386, 387
ratios, 295 Selling, 131
findings, 4, 70 Sense of self, 22
Reaching, 71, 135, 181, 334 theory and, 60
Readiness, 34-37, 39, 41, 43-45, 126, 162 Sentences, 68, 72, 74, 142, 149-150, 258-261, 263,
Research in education, 60 384
Reading, 7, 32, 41, 57, 67, 69, 73, 76, 81, 95, 99, 102, Resilience, 81
106-107, 124, 126-127, 135, 166, 176, 182, Separation, 130
Resources, 40, 73, 98, 104-106, 267, 331, 369, Sequence, 34, 85-88, 114-116, 118, 133-134, 146,
226, 245, 247, 253, 262, 270, 283, 291, 389-390
315-317, 319, 327, 366, 374 151, 195, 213, 227, 269, 272, 299, 315, 321,
Respect, 11, 38, 62, 390 391, 393
aloud, 102 Responding, 27, 32, 45, 50-51, 54, 77-78, 193, 231,
choral, 73 Setting, 9, 21, 37, 44, 56, 100, 110, 174
366 Severe disabilities, 89, 91-92
difficulties, 32, 95, 166 Response, 27, 44-45, 52, 54, 56, 58, 73, 75-76, 81-82,
ELLs, 69, 73, 76, 176, 245, 291, 316 Shadow, 368
86, 94-95, 109, 111, 171, 187, 208, 215, 258, Shapes, 43-44, 55-56, 67, 81-82, 86-87, 91-92, 107,
partner, 41, 124 286, 369-370
pointed, 135 109, 128, 139, 256-258, 267-269, 277, 280,
Response to Intervention (RTI), 44, 82, 94 289, 293, 321-323, 329-348, 350-353,
to students, 95 Responses, 15, 20, 27, 29, 56-59, 63, 74, 83-84, 87,
wide, 127, 135 356-363, 372, 374, 376, 381, 389, 391-394,
100, 110, 139, 177, 205, 208, 211, 217, 254, 395-396, 398-399
Reading comprehension, 245 260, 284, 288, 322, 368-369, 384
Reading skills, 81 cubes, 43, 87, 128, 139, 256, 267-269, 289, 293,
Responsive teaching, 46, 77-78 323, 330, 333-334, 391-392
Real numbers, 220, 389 Retention, 24, 189
Reasonableness, 49, 85, 215, 390 sorting and classifying, 339, 350
Retrieval, 174, 186 two-dimensional, 293, 337, 339-341, 343, 345-347,
Reasoning, 2-4, 7, 10, 14-15, 17-18, 21-23, 25, 27, 36, Revision, 5, 388
39, 41, 44, 48, 53-54, 56, 66, 68, 70, 87-89, 350
Rigidity, 345 Shared responsibility, 45, 101
93, 100-103, 121, 125, 132, 136, 143, 146, Rigor, 332-333 Sharing, 16, 22, 28, 35, 53, 57, 59, 64, 74, 88, 98,
410
102, 105, 107, 142, 155-156, 158, 160-161, linking, 200, 202, 209 Support, 3, 6, 14, 20, 22, 25-28, 32, 36, 40-41, 44-45,
168, 243, 259, 266, 278-279, 282-290, Standards-based, 18, 98 48, 55, 59, 62, 64, 69-71, 73-75, 78, 80-90,
293-294 State and local, 368 94, 97, 100-102, 105-106, 108, 111, 118-119,
Siblings, 35, 67, 231, 367-368 State standards, 1-2, 15, 29, 36, 45, 60, 65, 73, 84, 122, 135, 138, 148-149, 172-173, 175,
Significance, 4, 6, 15, 125, 389 89, 94, 97-100, 103-104, 109, 111, 114, 133, 177-178, 183, 186, 189, 203, 211, 226, 228,
statistical, 389 138, 140, 145, 148, 155-157, 159, 164, 167, 232-234, 239, 245, 255, 261, 267-268, 272,
Signifying, 254 169, 171-172, 185-186, 191, 193, 195, 221, 275, 291, 304, 325, 330, 335, 340, 346, 383
Signs, 48, 91, 107, 254, 273, 304 226, 228, 240, 245, 250, 261, 267, 273, 277, Support system, 44, 82, 86
Silence, 22 279, 286, 293, 297, 300, 304, 312, 317, Supporting, 22, 63, 75, 95, 97, 105, 168, 175, 182,
Simulations, 93 324-325, 329-331, 341, 361, 365, 370, 376, 186, 231, 329
Singing, 267 383, 387-390, 391-394 Surveys, 34-35
Size, 26, 39, 44, 71, 85, 123, 126, 135, 142, 148, 156, States, 4, 48, 59, 63-64, 74, 80, 82-84, 158, 167, 184, Susceptible, 227
161, 164, 184, 195, 202, 229, 239-240, 265, 232, 251, 297, 303, 331, 369, 373 Sweden, 60
277, 280-282, 286, 288, 290, 298, 300-303, Statistics, 62, 77, 365-367, 378, 381, 383, 386 Switch, 70, 139, 180, 356
305, 308, 310, 312, 321, 323, 327, 330, 332, Sticky notes, 377, 380 Symbolic representation, 264
335, 343, 346, 352, 356, 370-371, 373, 395 Stop, 7, 11, 15-16, 20, 28, 38-39, 43, 45, 57, 59, 71, Symbolism, 23, 149, 151, 157-158, 161, 167-168, 217,
Skill development, 87 74-75, 77, 89, 94, 99, 110-111, 124, 126, 249-250, 268, 282, 291
Skills, 1, 4, 15, 19, 32, 34, 36, 41, 47-49, 53, 68, 71, 131, 139, 145, 147, 160, 175, 182-183, 192, equal sign, 249
73, 81, 83-85, 88-92, 99-103, 105, 110, 114, 197, 215-216, 226, 230, 236, 239, 242, Symbols, 2-3, 18, 23-24, 26-28, 63, 66, 84-85, 88, 90,
121, 124, 153, 163, 174, 183, 193, 217, 223, 252-254, 257, 260, 262, 266, 270, 283-284, 102, 149, 155, 197-198, 209-210, 231, 243,
225-226, 267, 277, 299, 306, 315-317, 321, 291, 305, 319, 354, 358, 366, 375 249-252, 254, 263, 283, 291-292, 375, 378,
330, 355, 357-361 Storage, 138, 172 388-389
attending, 32 Stories, 26, 65-67, 73, 76, 104, 107, 110, 167-168, Symmetry, 330, 338, 341, 346, 350, 352, 354, 362,
practicing, 73 178, 220, 245, 293, 368 393-394
receiving, 321 interest in, 104, 220 line, 354, 362
speaking, 73 Storms, 379 Synthesis, 60, 111
thinking and reasoning, 48 Story Problems, 29, 65, 68, 76, 102, 104, 121, 130, System, 4, 44, 48, 59, 66, 72, 82-84, 86, 93, 121, 148,
Skip counting, 33, 90, 157, 160, 184, 217, 246, 253, 143, 149-150, 153-155, 157-158, 162-164, 197, 200-201, 209, 212, 221, 226, 229, 234,
271-272, 318, 376 167-168, 175-176, 178, 180, 182-185, 232, 249-250, 255, 263, 265, 271, 304, 317, 327,
Sleep, 368 234, 237, 261-262, 278-279, 282-285, 289, 333, 345, 355-357, 389, 394
Slides, 329, 331, 352-353 293, 317, 319-320, 379 Systems, 82, 93, 95, 304, 329, 332-334
Slope, 390 Strands, 2, 89, 172, 191, 250, 297, 329, 387
Small group, 35-36, 45, 87, 115, 139, 246, 362 Strategic competence, 2, 387 T
Small groups, 21-22, 27-28, 41, 44, 70, 75, 82, 84, Strategies, 3, 7-10, 14-17, 19-21, 27-28, 36-40, 44-46, Tables, 54, 69, 101, 103, 278, 309, 363, 373, 387-388
122-123, 125, 161, 178, 192, 271, 374, 378 55-56, 63-65, 68-69, 73-76, 78, 81-84, Tabs, 134
Snacks, 19-20, 117, 160-161 86-90, 93-94, 97-103, 106, 108, 119, 131, Talking, 11, 34, 58, 76, 89, 291-292, 332, 337, 381
Social studies, 297, 367-368, 370, 373 138, 140, 146, 149, 153, 155, 157, 160-161, Tangrams, 92, 341, 343
Sociocultural theories, 10 166, 171-178, 181, 183-184, 186, 188-192, Target strategy, 175
Software, 85-86, 186, 389 196, 215-216, 219, 223, 225-247, 263, 272, Targeted instruction, 48, 51, 83
Software programs, 186 284-286, 290, 293-295, 305-306, 311, 320, Tasks, 1-3, 6, 14-21, 23, 25-29, 34, 36-41, 43-44,
Solutions, 2-3, 6, 10, 14-16, 21-22, 27-29, 37, 45-46, 391-392 47-48, 51, 53-55, 57-58, 62, 72, 75, 82,
58, 65, 101-102, 106, 109, 111, 155, 165, Strategy instruction, 84, 86-89, 95, 173 84-85, 87, 89, 91-92, 98-103, 107-110, 115,
211, 216, 244, 250-251, 278, 282, 284-285, Stress, 285 118, 123-124, 126, 131, 136, 166, 173, 209,
289-290, 293-294, 319-320, 350, 353, 389 Strips, 127, 129, 151, 200, 210, 268-269, 280-281, 211, 252-253, 258, 260, 280, 282-284, 289,
Sorting, 56, 93, 324, 335, 339-340, 350, 362, 370, 288-289, 299, 304, 313, 379 294, 307, 311-312, 315, 321-322, 324, 335,
373, 376 Structure, 4, 20, 26-27, 34, 36, 39, 41, 43-44, 52-53, 350, 383
Sound, 33, 55, 72, 109, 182, 191, 202, 232, 389 56, 76, 82, 85, 87, 115, 122, 126, 131, 138, Teacher, 2, 6-12, 14-17, 19-22, 25, 27, 32-33, 35, 39,
Sounds, 287 144-146, 148-149, 156, 162-163, 166, 173, 42, 44-45, 47-48, 51-52, 54-57, 63, 65-68,
Space, 37, 41, 56, 59, 92, 116, 119, 125, 155, 192, 184, 197, 200, 204, 219, 226, 234, 249, 252, 72-75, 80, 82, 84-88, 93, 98-99, 101,
209, 242, 261-262, 304, 321, 329-331, 255, 263-265, 267-269, 283, 289, 305, 333, 104-110, 115, 117, 125-126, 128, 131, 136,
355-356, 391 337, 359, 389 139-140, 143, 161, 191, 202, 221, 223, 225,
Space in the classroom, 116 Student achievement, 67, 104-105, 111, 295, 363, 390 231-232, 251, 263-264, 293, 295, 310-311,
Spatial relations, 391 Student involvement, 45 338-339, 343, 367, 369, 371-372
Spatial relationships, 92, 121, 330 Student outcomes, 111 Teacher observation, 35
Spatial sense, 91, 330 Student performance, 59 Teachers, 1-3, 6, 9-12, 15-16, 18, 20, 23, 25, 29, 32,
Speaking, 64, 69-70, 73, 259 Student strategies, 84 36, 40, 45, 47-48, 51, 58-59, 62, 65-68, 70,
Special education, 44, 83-84, 94 Student success, 41 72, 75, 77-78, 80-81, 83-84, 86, 89, 91-92,
Special education services, 83 Students, 3, 6, 8, 29, 32, 34, 47, 62, 73, 77-78, 80, 82, 94-95, 97-98, 101-102, 104, 106-108,
Special education teacher, 44, 84 86, 88, 93-95, 97, 102-104, 106, 111, 143, 110-111, 114, 117-118, 121, 140, 143, 163,
Special needs, 22, 86, 88-89, 95, 293 181, 183, 193, 215, 238, 247, 254, 256, 275, 169, 172-174, 176, 191, 193, 215, 223, 226,
speed, 103, 123, 187, 189, 373 277, 301, 304, 325, 330, 333-334, 336, 365, 229, 232, 247, 249, 254, 267, 275, 277, 293,
Splitting, 19, 183, 233, 235, 277, 279 378, 386, 387-390, 391-394 295, 297, 304, 308, 311, 327, 329-330, 334,
Sports, 36, 136, 367-369, 381 distinctions, 333 337, 358, 363, 365, 367, 370, 376, 386
Stages, 84, 136, 171, 211, 229, 315 exceptional, 94 educators, 97, 102, 163, 173
Stakeholders, 110-111 Students with disabilities, 94 ESL, 66
collaborating with, 110-111 Students with special needs, 95 head, 102, 104, 311
Standard units, 74, 107, 299-305, 308, 311, 322-323, gifted students, 95 highly qualified, 84
393-394 Studies, 51, 77, 97, 143, 172, 228, 247, 297, 301, Teaching, 1-12, 13-29, 31-32, 45-46, 47, 50, 55, 57,
choosing appropriate, 303 366-368, 370, 373, 376 60, 61-78, 79-95, 97, 99-101, 105, 108-111,
familiarity with, 74, 302-303, 305, 308 D, 77, 247, 366-367 113, 132, 135, 140, 141, 143, 164, 169,
important, 300-302, 304-305, 308, 311 G, 77, 97, 172, 247, 297, 301, 367-368, 373 171-174, 176, 189, 193, 195, 199, 207, 216,
Standardized tests, 99 Study guides, 86 223, 225, 230-231, 240, 247, 249, 254, 275,
Standards, 1-2, 4, 10, 12, 15, 18, 29, 36, 40, 45, 47, Style, 123, 378 277, 285, 295, 297, 302, 306-307, 311, 327,
58, 60, 62, 65-66, 68-69, 73, 77-78, 80, 82, Subdivisions, 306, 335 329, 334, 363, 365, 386, 387, 391, 395
84, 89, 94, 97-100, 102-104, 108-109, 111, Subitizing, 115, 120, 122-123 Teaching:, 46, 77
114-115, 122, 125-127, 130-133, 136, 138, Subtraction, 3, 6, 17, 19-20, 26, 29, 37-38, 48-49, 51, Teaching strategies, 64
140, 143-146, 148-150, 152, 154-159, 63-64, 66, 68, 76, 91, 99-103, 106-107, 121, Teams, 98, 307
162-164, 167, 169, 171-173, 175-179, 128, 130, 138, 140, 142-146, 148-150, Techniques, 29, 45, 60, 105, 143, 148, 153, 163-164,
184-186, 188, 191, 193, 195, 197, 200-202, 152-157, 162, 167-169, 171-172, 174, 197, 306, 367, 375, 384
206, 208-209, 213, 215, 218, 221, 223, 226, 181-184, 186, 188, 191-193, 196-197, Technology, 26, 40, 92, 102, 189, 353, 375, 380,
228, 231-235, 237, 240-241, 245, 247, 215-216, 218-219, 223, 225-226, 228, 389-390
250-252, 255-256, 259, 261-262, 264, 230-232, 234, 238-241, 243-245, 247, 251, computers, 102
267-268, 272-273, 275, 277-279, 282-283, 255, 258, 263-264, 273-274, 295, 317-319, temperature, 92, 300, 368-369
285-286, 289, 291, 293, 297, 300, 303-305, 391-394, 395, 398 Ten-frames, 203, 210, 397-398
312, 317, 324-325, 327, 329-331, 335, as think-addition, 153 Tens method, 202
337-338, 341, 344-345, 350, 352, 355, Suggestions, 23, 28, 52, 58, 68, 84, 106, 108, 149, Terminology, 4, 23, 27-28, 68, 71, 87, 156, 162, 199,
357-359, 361, 363, 365, 367, 370-372, 202, 214, 235, 307, 309, 315, 322, 374 214, 266, 331, 351-352, 362, 383
375-377, 381, 383, 386, 387-390, 391-394 Summative assessment, 47-48 Tessellation, 359
content in, 68, 136, 330, 390 Supervision, 12, 46, 78 Test, 24, 37, 47, 75, 78, 84, 189, 251, 267, 269, 312,
411
335, 353, 372 Truth, 225, 275, 333, 388 292-294, 298, 303, 305, 314, 319, 338, 379,
testing, 52, 60 Turns, 40, 67, 74, 87, 107, 117-118, 124, 216, 307, 381, 384, 391-394
Tests, 53, 84, 99, 172, 189, 232 329, 331, 336, 346, 352-353, 356-357, 359, Whole-class discussion, 22, 38, 294
select, 53 362, 384 Windows, 306, 362
Text, 11, 36, 99, 191, 334 Tutorials, 106 Word order, 149
Textbooks, 98, 109, 124, 163-164, 172-173, 200, 205, Tutors, 89 Word prediction, 86
209-210, 238, 254, 267, 280, 295, 349 Two-digit numbers, 221 word problems, 14, 48, 81, 130, 148, 163-164,
adapted, 109, 254 166-167, 177, 317, 379-380
The Parent, 110 U Word Wall, 87, 208, 302, 338, 351
Theme, 66 Underground Railroad, 66, 78 Word walls, 71, 90
theories, 4, 9-10 Understanding, 1-12, 14-21, 23-25, 27-29, 32, 34-37, Words, 7, 10, 16-18, 21-22, 24, 28, 43-44, 50, 52-55,
Theory, 4, 6, 9-10, 60, 81, 88, 94, 111, 331-332, 334, 39-41, 44-45, 47, 49-55, 57-58, 66, 70-71, 58, 68-73, 75, 85, 87, 106, 115-116, 120,
336 73, 75-76, 81, 85-87, 89-90, 93-94, 97, 123, 131, 138, 143, 149, 163-165, 185,
Think, 1-2, 4, 7-8, 11, 14-17, 19-20, 22-24, 27-28, 99-104, 114-115, 117, 119-121, 131-134, 196-198, 202-204, 207-208, 210, 221, 235,
34-36, 38-39, 41-43, 45, 49, 51-52, 54, 57, 137, 142-143, 145, 150-151, 162-163, 167, 245, 251-252, 255, 259, 263-266, 273, 282,
62, 64, 66, 68, 70, 73-75, 81, 84-86, 89, 100, 176, 184, 190, 195-197, 199-201, 210-215, 286-287, 289, 291, 293-294, 297, 305, 308,
106-107, 110-111, 114, 124, 127, 131, 219, 221-223, 225-226, 228-229, 231, 240, 320, 329, 336, 352, 356, 359, 369-370, 374
134-136, 139, 143, 145-150, 153, 155, 244-245, 247, 252-254, 256, 259-260, 263, base, 7, 54, 85, 196-198, 202, 204, 207-208, 210,
157-158, 163-167, 171, 173, 178, 182-183, 272, 274-275, 277-278, 280, 282-285, 287, 221, 235, 263
187, 191-192, 195, 197-198, 201-202, 204, 289, 291, 293-295, 297, 299-302, 304, 307, categorization of, 265
209-210, 212-216, 222, 225, 229-238, 240, 309, 312, 314-315, 317, 323, 327, 330-331, classes of, 336
249-252, 254, 256, 261, 264-266, 271, 335, 346, 365, 386, 387, 389-390, 392-394 Work, 3, 6, 8, 15, 19-20, 26-29, 32, 35-41, 43-45,
273-274, 278, 280, 282, 284, 287, 291, Undoing, 252 50-51, 53, 55, 57-59, 69-70, 77, 81, 83, 86,
293-294, 303-307, 312, 315-317, 319-320, Unit iteration, 327 89, 91, 93, 98-102, 105, 107, 110, 115-116,
323, 325-326, 330-332, 337-339, 353, 355, UNIT test, 47 119-120, 123, 127, 130-131, 134, 140, 153,
357, 359-362, 369, 372, 375, 384, 392 United States, 63-64, 74, 158, 297, 303, 331, 373 155-156, 160-165, 167, 169, 172-175, 180,
Think-aloud, 293 Units, 3, 6, 56, 74, 101, 107, 131, 136, 151-152, 160, 183-184, 186, 188-190, 192, 195-197, 202,
Thinking, 7, 9-11, 15, 18, 21-24, 26-29, 34, 36, 38-39, 195-196, 199-201, 204, 221-222, 235, 204, 206-207, 211, 214, 216-218, 222,
42, 44, 47-48, 50-54, 56, 59, 63, 66-67, 69, 279-280, 295, 297-315, 322-326, 356, 225-226, 228, 230-231, 234-236, 239-242,
75-76, 84-89, 93-94, 101-102, 105-106, 108, 388-389, 392-394 244-246, 249, 251-253, 260-263, 265, 268,
111, 117, 122, 124, 130, 138, 142, 150, 152, University of Wisconsin, 60 273-275, 277, 284-286, 293-295, 297, 302,
158, 162, 166, 173, 184, 187, 191-193, 196, U.S. Department of Education, 78, 94 304, 315, 321-322, 324, 326, 330, 333, 337,
208, 219-221, 223, 228, 230, 232-234, 239, 339, 351, 353-354, 362, 366, 368, 370, 384,
245-246, 249-250, 256, 259-261, 263, 265, 390, 392-394
267, 273-275, 290, 293-295, 319, 325, V Workforce, 97, 225
329-331, 333-335, 340, 346-347, 352, 357, Vacations, 67 Working memory, 90, 175
359, 366, 381, 384, 386, 392-394 Validity, 9 Working through, 18
algebraic, 67, 138, 249-250, 256, 259-261, 263, Values, 27, 51, 75-76, 103, 177, 191, 199, 201, 245, Workplace, 92, 102, 304, 388
265, 267, 273-275, 392-394 253, 256-257, 260-261, 263, 279, 288, Worksheets, 188, 213, 267, 314, 326
possibility, 48, 52, 56, 130, 267 317-321, 371, 376, 378, 380-381 Writers, 55
Thompson, 71, 76-77, 295, 297, 327 Variables, 249, 254, 257-259, 261-263, 350, 366 Writing, 2-3, 26, 34, 37, 49, 51, 53-55, 57, 59, 69,
Thousands chart, 214, 218 as quantities that vary, 261-263 71-73, 84, 90, 93, 99, 102, 104, 108, 110,
Three-phase lesson format, 26 measurement, 366 115, 127, 146, 153, 196, 199, 209, 217, 254,
Tiered instruction, 45 Venn diagram, 371-372, 384 262-264, 291, 294, 388, 391
Tiered lessons, 41, 43-44 Venn diagrams, 383 form of, 37, 127, 146, 264
Time, 3-5, 10, 16, 20-22, 24-28, 32-34, 40-42, 47-48, Verbal skills, 361 kid, 59
51-53, 67-68, 70, 72-73, 76-77, 85-86, Vermont, 47 numeral, 196
88-93, 98, 100-102, 104, 106, 108-109, Verse, 370 purpose for, 57
114-117, 121-123, 125-129, 131, 135-138, Video, 48, 106, 367 right, 115, 199, 209
146, 148, 150-151, 154-155, 157, 160-161, Videos, 106 structures in, 72
166-168, 173, 177, 179, 181, 184-186, 189, Virtual manipulatives, 26, 106, 243-244, 267, 343-344 to solve, 3, 26, 51, 59, 72, 102, 104, 146, 153, 254,
196, 206-207, 209-210, 215-218, 221, Virtue, 197 262, 294, 388, 391
225-227, 229-231, 233, 237, 241, 261, 263, Vision, 105, 108 Writing prompts, 55
266, 269, 277, 279, 282, 289, 291, 297-300, Visual cues, 85 Written records, 228
303, 307, 311, 314-319, 321, 324, 330, 341, Visual displays, 85, 93
356, 359-360, 366-369, 372, 374, 376, Visual information, 304
Visual learners, 85 Y
378-379, 381, 389-390, 391-394, 396 Young children, 6, 14, 18, 26, 56, 102, 106, 115, 119,
elapsed, 317 Visual memory, 359
Visualization, 125, 329, 331-332, 334-335, 338, 122, 135, 143, 149, 155, 169, 193, 223, 240,
engaged, 10, 16, 40, 98, 161, 186, 279 250, 265, 267, 272, 278-282, 284-286, 288,
to think, 20, 22, 24, 34, 51-52, 100, 127, 131, 135, 357-360
Visualizing, 121 300, 305, 308, 311, 321, 323-324, 331, 334,
146, 148, 150, 167, 209-210, 225, 233, 339, 352-353, 355, 360, 363, 367, 369-370,
237, 315-316, 330, 359-360, 369 Vocabulary, 27, 44, 68-69, 71-72, 75-76, 82, 84-85, 87,
102, 149, 277, 282, 293, 361-362, 391 375, 381
units, 3, 101, 131, 136, 151, 160, 196, 221, 279, Ysseldyke, J., 95
297-300, 303, 307, 311, 314-315, 324, ideas and, 27
356, 389, 392-394 Vocabulary Development, 71
Title, 102 Vocabulary instruction, 71 Z
Tone, 37, 52 Voice, 52, 85-86, 125 Zero, 4, 51, 151-152, 154-155, 162, 177-178, 185,
Tone of voice, 52 Voice recognition, 86 209-211, 242-243, 250, 252, 263-264, 298
Tools, 3-4, 14, 18, 23-25, 42-43, 46, 49, 53-54, 56, 59, Volume, 67, 92, 298, 301, 303, 306-307, 321, 323, Zone of proximal development, 6
73, 78, 86-88, 91, 105-106, 150, 159, 178, 327, 394
201, 212, 233, 252, 254, 263, 295, 297-300, volunteering, 97
303, 327, 352-353, 388-389, 394 Volunteers, 103
for teaching, 86, 91
Topics, 34-35, 40-41, 47, 49, 65, 91, 93, 99, 104, 106, W
121, 167, 189, 250, 368, 370, 391 Wait time, 52, 70, 72
touch, 4, 85, 115, 337, 360 Walls, 71, 90, 337
Touching, 128, 338 wants, 15, 75, 156-158, 167, 178
Toys, 114, 262, 379 Washington, 2, 12, 78, 94-95, 140, 193, 247
Traits, 34 Wealth, 106
Transfer, 228, 343 Websites, 26, 71, 98, 106, 186, 267, 368, 370
Transformation, 331, 352-354, 360 Weight, 135, 273, 297-298, 300-301, 304-305, 307,
Transitions, 85, 225 321, 324, 366, 369, 373
Transitivity, 309, 393 of matter, 324
Translation, 54-55, 75, 78, 138, 352 Whole, 17, 21-22, 37-38, 41, 45, 51, 54, 64-66, 76, 86,
Travel, 59, 326, 357, 380 94, 102, 110, 116, 121-122, 125-130,
Triangles, 40, 43-44, 53, 56, 86, 128, 322, 330-331, 138-139, 142, 144-145, 147-148, 150-154,
334-338, 340, 344-348, 351-352, 360, 371, 156, 158-159, 161, 169, 174, 178, 182,
391, 393, 395, 400 191-193, 195-220, 222-223, 225-247, 259,
area of, 330 261, 271, 273-274, 277-285, 287-289,
412