Session 2: Outline
Session 2: Outline
Session 2: Outline
Outline
1. Review Homework (5 min.)
2. The Pervasiveness and Normalcy of Experiential Control (10 min.)
3. The Futility and Costs of Experiential Control (20 min.)
Chocolate Cake Exercise
Polygraph Metaphor
Falling in Love Metaphor
4. Willingness as an Alternative to Experiential Control (20 min.)
Two Scales Metaphor
Carrying Your Depression Exercise
5. Assignment of Homework (5 min.)
Agenda
1. Review Homework
Start off this session by reviewing with your clients what additional forms of expe-
riential control they may have noticed during the week by looking over their Mood
Regulation Diary. Increasing client awareness of the pervasiveness of emotional control
is more important than the completion of the diary per se, and, if it was not filled out,
still ask you clients what additional ways of digging or pulling on the rope they may
have noticed themselves using. As illustrated in chapter 4 with the client who identified
reading as a preferred and long-standing experiential control strategy, it is not unusual
for clients to uncover methods of mood regulation to which they had previously been
oblivious. Ask your clients to consider two basic questions about whatever is identified:
We’ve now had a chance to look at some of the different ways that you try to
regulate your mood and control how you feel emotionally. We can compare
it to digging [or tugging on a rope], but for right now let’s just refer to it as
emotional control—the tendency we all have to deliberately run away from
unpleasant feelings and thoughts and to run toward ones we like.
We all make conscious, deliberate efforts and create plans that we follow in
order to control things we don’t like. Let’s take a broader look at how those work.
For example, suppose you didn’t like the way the furniture and other furnishings
in this room are arranged. Could you do anything about it? If I offered you a
thousand dollars to rearrange the furniture in this room, could you do it? What
if instead of furniture, what you try to rearrange is how you think and feel
about something? For instance, what if you take your thoughts and feelings about
[mention some personal event from the client’s life story, linked to depression]
and try to arrange them so they’re no longer depressing to you?
How well does that work? Could you rearrange them for a thousand dollars?
Here’s what I’ d like you to consider—the possibility that the control agenda
that works so well in the world outside of ourselves, like the world of furniture,
doesn’t work when we try to apply it to the world inside of ourselves, to the world
of our own thoughts and feelings. What if it’s the case that the operative rule
about the outside world is “ if you don’t like something, just change it,” but the
operative rule about our own thoughts and feelings is “ if you’re not willing to
have them, you’ve got them”?
I’m not asking you to believe me—maybe it seems to you that it shouldn’t
be this way, and that it’s not fair—but instead what does your own experience
tell you about the way it is?
So it’s no wonder you’ve been digging, and, as you say, you haven’t known what
else to do. Quite likely most, if not all of us—including myself—have done
and would do exactly as you have done. We all have our own holes. The only
advantage I have so I can help you with yours is one of perspective—that is,
whatever hole I may be in is not the same hole as the one you’re in.
Chocolate Cake Exercise. To illustrate the futility of thought suppression, ask your
clients to deliberately “not think” of a particular object. The original exercise presented
by Hayes et al. (1999, pp. 124–125) used chocolate cake (and thus the origin of its name),
although other objects can be used. Hayes and Smith (2005, pp. 24–25), for example,
used a yellow jeep in their version of the exercise, and I personally prefer jelly donuts in
presenting it.
If you’re willing to do so, I’ d like to have you participate in a little exercise with
me. Whatever you do right now, don’t think of jelly donuts! You can think of
anything as long as it’s not a jelly donut. You know the kind of jelly donuts that
are soft and sweet smelling so that when put them into your mouth and bite into
them, the jelly squirts out into your mouth and is all sticky and sweet-tasting.
Don’t think about them! You can think of anything else, but whatever you do,
don’t think about jelly donuts.
Afterward, underscore the similarity between the exercise and spontaneous attempts
by your clients to suppress unwanted thoughts.
Instead of jelly donuts, suppose it’s important that you don’t think about how
you may have messed up your life. And so you tell yourself not to think about
mistakes you may have made. How well has that worked for you?
Polygraph Metaphor. Extend the futility of the experiential control agenda to efforts
to avoid unwanted emotions by presenting the Polygraph Metaphor (Hayes et al., 1999,
pp. 123–124).
Client: You might as well just shoot me right now and get it over with.
Therapist: So you couldn’t stay relaxed under those circumstances. Could anyone?
Therapist: So anyone in that situation—you, me, the average guy on the street—
couldn’t stay relaxed. Anxiety itself has now become something to be
anxious about, and if you’re not willing to have it, you’ve got it. So you’re
now anxious about being anxious. Let’s see how this might work with
depression. You’ve told me when you’re depressed, you’ve tried to cheer
yourself up. How has that worked?
Therapist: What’s more depressing than trying not to feel depressed and failing?
Now suppose instead of not feeling anxious, your task is one of not
moving the furniture in this room and I told you I would shoot you if
you did? What would happen?
Falling in Love Metaphor. Because experiential control in depression can also take the
form of clients deliberately trying to induce certain desirable emotional states, such as
“feeling happy” or “good about myself,” offer the following metaphor as well:
Therapist: Sometimes the way we try to control emotions we don’t like is to create
and hold on to their opposites. So, for example, instead of trying to avoid
feeling unhappy and depressed, we try to find and capture happiness. Let’s
take a closer look at how this works. Suppose I have a million dollars, and
I tell you that what you need to do to earn it is quite simple. We will leave
the office here, and all you have to do is fall madly in love with the first
stranger you see, regardless of their gender, age, or physical appearance.
Could you do it?
Client: That I was just saying I was madly in love with them.
Therapist: So doesn’t it seem to be the case that deliberately trying to create certain
emotions doesn’t work any better than trying to get rid of certain feelings?
Don’t take my word for it. Look at your own experience. Now notice
what would happen if I said, “Okay, you don’t have to actually fall madly
in love with the first stranger you see, but you have to run up to them
and profess your passionate love for them.” Could you do that? Would
you do that?
Therapist: So you can’t control how you feel, but you can control how you move
your mouth and feet.
Two Scales Metaphor. This metaphor was originally presented by Hayes (1987, pp.
352–353) and subsequently further developed by Hayes et al. (1999, pp. 133–134) as
a means of suggesting to clients an alternative to the experiential control agenda. It is
modified here slightly to be more specific to depression.
5. Assignment of Homework
Also discussed in chapter 6 was the Thoughts on Cards Exercise. For homework,
provide your clients with a deck of index cards on which they are asked to write down
(one per card) unwanted, depressing thoughts that they encounter during the week.
Instruct your clients to note on the back of each card any ways they attempted to control
the thought or emotions related to it. Model the assignment by selecting a negative auto-
matic thought expressed by the client in session. Use the completed cards to begin some
defusion exercises at the beginning of the next session.