Animals: Who Is Pulling The Leash? E and Dog Sex On Human-Dog Dyads When Walking On-Leash
Animals: Who Is Pulling The Leash? E and Dog Sex On Human-Dog Dyads When Walking On-Leash
Article
Who Is Pulling the Leash? Effects of Human Gender
and Dog Sex on Human–Dog Dyads When
Walking On-Leash
Hao-Yu Shih 1, * , Mandy B. A. Paterson 1,2 , Fillipe Georgiou 3 , Nancy A. Pachana 4 and
Clive J. C. Phillips 1
1 Centre for Animal Welfare and Ethics, University of Queensland, White House Building (8134),
Gatton Campus, Gatton, QLD 4343, Australia; [email protected] (M.B.A.P.);
[email protected] (C.J.C.P.)
2 Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4076, Australia
3 School of Mathematical and Physical Sciences, University of Newcastle, University Drive,
Callaghan, NSW 2308, Australia; [email protected]
4 School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia; [email protected]
* Correspondence: [email protected]
Received: 17 August 2020; Accepted: 14 October 2020; Published: 16 October 2020
Simple Summary: The gender of humans and the sex of dogs influence human–dog interactions.
This study investigated human–dog interactions when volunteers take shelter dogs for an on-leash
walk, using video recording and a canine leash tension meter. Male dogs tended to pull more frequently
and created higher leash tensions than female dogs. Dogs displayed more stress related behaviours
when interacting with men than women, with the signs including spending less time holding the
tail in a high position, and more frequent gazing and lip-licking behaviours. Finally, during the
walk, there was a greater pre-disposition in women to use verbal commands and language typically
associated with talking to babies, while men were more inclined to have physical contact with the
dogs. These results may be used to match shelter dogs with appropriate men and women volunteers
for dog walking exercise, and to improve potential socialisation of the dogs before rehoming.
Abstract: Previous studies have indicated that human gender and canine sex influences human–dog
interactions. However, the majority of studies have considered the interaction when dogs were
off-leash and the behavioural interactions when dogs are walked on a leash have not been addressed.
This study investigated human–dog interactions when shelter volunteers take shelter dogs for
an on-leash walk. Video records were made of 370 walks, involving 74 volunteers and 111 shelter
dogs, and a leash tension meter was used to determine the pull strength of dogs and walkers.
Human gender and canine sex had dyadic effects during the walk. Male dogs tended to pull more
frequently and created increased leash tensions. Dogs displayed more stress related behaviours when
interacting with men than women, with the signs being spending less time holding the tail in the
high position, and more frequent gazing and lip-licking behaviours. Finally, there was a greater
pre-disposition in women to use verbal commands, and language typically used with babies, while
men were more inclined to have physical contact with dogs. This study’s results may be used to
match shelter dogs with appropriate men and women volunteers for walking exercise of the dog, and
to improve potential dog socialisation efforts by shelters.
Keywords: gender; sex; on-leash walk; leash tension; behaviour; verbal cue; body gesture; human–dog
interaction; shelter
1. Introduction
Men and women interact with dogs in different ways, and dogs also respond to men and
women differently [1–3]. Women tend to talk more and speak in an excited, high-pitched voice when
interacting with dogs and they initiate talking to their dogs after a shorter latency [4]. In addition,
women owners are more inclined to offer more encouraging behaviours to dogs perceived to be
fearful [5]. These parental and caregiving behaviours are stronger and more developed in women
and have been argued to be related to the human–dog attachment [4]. Men are more likely to use
punishment-based rather than reward-based methods to train their dogs [6], and are more inclined
to physically restrain their dogs [7]. Dogs for their part are able to discriminate between the human
genders [8] and react differently toward men and women [2], in particular responding to men more
with defensive-aggression as evidenced by increased barking and gazing [2].
The sex of dogs also affects their behaviour. Compared to their female counterparts, male dogs are
bolder [9] and more likely to have behavioural issues, such as conspecific aggression, sexual problems
and straying [10]. Male dogs are overrepresented in the behaviour clinic population, with greater risks
of behaviour problems such as house soiling, coprophagia and destructive behaviour [11].
Previous research on human–dog interactions has mainly investigated behavioural interactions
while dogs were off-leash [12,13]. Mandatory dog leash laws have been implemented in many countries
around the world to protect wildlife [14], reduce disease transmission [15], prevent dog attacks and
dog involvement in road traffic accidents [16,17]. There is a growing emphasis on the importance
of loose leash heelwork, as a tense leash can be detrimental to the health of dogs by damaging their
trachea and having negative effects on their cornea and intraocular pressure [18]. However, research
about human–dog interactions when dogs are walked on a leash is limited.
Rein tension meters for horses have become popular in recent years among equestrians, enabling
researchers and trainers to monitor how a rider communicates with the horse by measuring the force
exerted on the reins by the human [19,20]. In this study, a similar concept was adopted for human–dog
interactions. A custom-made leash tension meter was used to capture the leash tension when a dog
was walked on a leash. Unlike the equine rein tension meter, our dog leash tension meter included an
accelerometer in the device to differentiate between human and dog pulling during the walk [21].
Behavioural observation is another validated and common approach for assessing human–dog
interactions [22,23]. Positions of their ears, tail and extremities, their facial expression, ongoing
behaviours and general body tension are all important indicators of the dog’s stress levels [24,25].
In humans, behavioural observation is also variable, with body gestures and verbal cues being
correlated with gender [4], personality [13] and training skills [26].
This study aimed to explore the role of human gender/dog sex in the behavioural interactions
between volunteers and shelter dogs while dogs are walked on a leash, using a canine leash tension
meter and video recorder. It was hypothesised first, that men volunteers walking male dogs would
show highest leash tension. The second hypothesis was that shelter dogs would be more defensive
and show more stress related behaviours when interacting with men volunteers [2], and thus would be
more likely to look at the men volunteers and show lip-licking. Additionally, we hypothesised that
women volunteers would talk to all shelter dogs, irrespective of sex, more frequently than do men
volunteers, and tend to speak with a high-pitched voice. Finally, it was hypothesised that women
volunteers would be more tolerant and satisfied with the interaction.
2.2. Subjects
2.2.1. Dogs
This study investigated 370 walks from July to early October 2019, involving 111 shelter dogs
and 74 volunteers. All participating dogs had to have been resident at the RSPCA, Queensland, for at
least one week, to enable them to become accustomed to the living and walking areas. Dogs’ walking
behaviour was categorized into four levels by RSPCA animal attendants who had been closely working
with those dogs. Levels were assigned according to the ease of walking the dogs, based on their
performance during the daily walk. Level 1 dogs walked on a loose leash most of the time. Level 2
dogs pulled the leash during the walk occasionally and had more undesirable behaviours than level 1
dogs. Level 3 dogs tended to pull the leash fiercely due to excitement or timidity. Level 3+ dogs had
severe behavioural issues, such as overt aggressiveness or fearfulness; however, they might or might
not pull the leash harder than level 3 dogs. Dogs with severe behavioural or medical issues that might
affect the observational study were excluded from the study because of safety and welfare concerns.
All included dogs had undergone an RSPCA behavioural assessment [27].
2.2.2. Volunteers
Volunteers were trained progressively in four stages, allowing them initially to walk dogs in
level 1 and in each stage learning to walk the more challenging dogs. Volunteers could only walk
level 1 dogs during their first month of volunteering; level 3+ volunteers were those who were most
experienced and had gone through a series of standardized training programs. Volunteers could only
walk dogs that had the same or lower level. Dogs were assigned to volunteers by experienced staff for
a daily walk based on the volunteer’s training level.
2.3. Measures
processed using MATLAB® (MATLAB® and Statistics Toolbox Release 2018b, The MathWorks, Inc.,
Natick, MA, USA) [21].
Table 1. Exit questionnaire for volunteers (n = 74) following walking dogs (n = 111) on a designated
route at Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals (RSPCA) Queensland, requiring them
to rate each sentence on a 5-point scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
Factor H Factor D
1. 0.537 −0.581
2. −0.556 0.443
3. 0.694 −0.325
4. −0.680 0.316
5. 0.739 -0.331
6. −0.559 0.419
7. -0.253 0.689
8. -0.353 0.518
9. 0.318 −0.644
10. −0.503 0.287
11. 0.631 -0.387
12. −0.276 0.456
13. 0.478 −0.475
Loadings in bold indicate the factor upon which each item was selected. Human satisfaction factor (Factor H,
Cronbach’s alpha = 0.88): 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 10, 11. Walker’s perception of dog factor (Factor D, Cronbach’s alpha = 0.83):
1, 7, 8, 9, 12, 13.
2.6. Ethograms
Canine behaviours (Table 3), human verbal cues (Table 4) and human body languages (Table 5)
were coded using ethograms developed by referring to previous research [13,24,30–33] and modified
during practice sessions. Behaviours were coded as ‘point events’ or ‘state events’. A point event
indicates the number of times the event was observed and a state event was defined as the duration of
the observed event.
Animals 2020, 10, 1894 6 of 19
Table 6. Generalized linear mixed model of dog sex and human gender effects on leash tension and pulling frequency. Mean (kg force) (µ) and standard deviation (SD)
of different dependent variables by gender/sex are provided, together with β, SE and p-values for significant or close to significant variables.
Log10 NTmax Log10 NTmean Log10 DTmax Log10 DTmean Log10 DPF Log10 HTmax Log10 HTmean Log10 HPF
Women Women Women Women Women Women Women Women
µ 3.72, SD 2.01 µ 0.58, SD 0.26 µ 3.24, SD 1.81 µ 1.15, SD 0.5 µ 0.19, SD 0.14 µ 3.05, SD 1.72 µ 1.14, SD 0.49 µ 0.19, SD 0.13
Human Men Men Men Men Men Men Men Men
Gender 1
µ 3.68, SD 1.93 µ 0.59, SD 0.24 µ 3.29, SD 1.87 µ 1.16, SD 0.48 µ 0.18, SD 0.13 µ 2.97, SD 1.77 µ 1.14, SD 0.52 µ 0.17, SD 0.12
β -0.29
– – – – SE 0.09 – – –
p 0.0017
Female Female Female Female Female Female Female Female
µ 3.3, SD 1.72 µ 0.54, SD 0.22 µ 2.88, SD 1.58 µ 1.07, SD 0.39 µ 0.17, SD 0.13 µ 2.72, SD 1.5 µ 1.03, SD 0.38 µ 0.16, SD 0.1
Male Male Male Male Male Male Male Male
Dog
µ 4.12, SD 2.13 µ 0.63, SD 0.27 µ 3.64, SD 1.97 µ 1.25, SD 0.56 µ 0.21, SD 0.14 µ 3.35, SD 1.9 µ 1.24, SD 0.58 µ 0.21, SD 0.14
Sex 1
β 0.22 β 0.15 β 0.26 β 0.17 β 0.37 β 0.15 β 0.168 β 0.36
SE 0.071 SE 0.053 SE 0.078 SE 0.054 SE 0.12 SE 0.076 SE 0.055 SE 0.11
p 0.0031 p 0.0063 p 0.0011 p 0.0026 p 0.0032 p 0.051 p 0.0029 p 0.0018
Tension and pulling frequency were analysed after log10 transformation. NTmax: maximal net leash tension. NTmean: mean net leash tension. DTmax: maximal leash tension caused by
dog. DTmean: mean leash tension caused by dog. HTmax: maximal leash tension caused by handler. HTmean: mean leash tension caused by handler. DPF: dog pulling frequency. HPF:
handler pulling frequency. µ: mean (kg force). SD: standard deviation of µ. β: regression coefficient. SE: standard error of β. p: p-value of the model. –: Not included in the generalized
linear mixed model because the independent variable had high p-values in the bivariate regression model. 1. Women and female dogs were used as control.
Animals 2020, 10, 1894 10 of 19
Table 7. Generalized linear mixed model of dog sex and human gender effects on canine behaviour. Mean (µ) and standard deviation (SD) of different dependent
variables by gender/sex was provided.
Track (%) Tail High (%) Gaze (no./s) Lip-Lick (no./s) Eliminate-Mark (no./s) 3 Pant (%)
Women Women Women Women Women Women
µ 15.51, SD 11.62 µ 78.59, SD 29.85 µ 0.01, SD 0.02 µ 0.01, SD 0.01 µ 0.01, SD 0.01 µ 10.48, SD 11.07
Men Men Men Men Men Men
Human Gender 1 µ 14.64, SD 10.57 µ 76.82, SD 30.43 µ 0.02, SD 0.02 µ 0.01, SD 0.02 µ 0.01, SD 0.01 µ 10.36, SD 11.2
β −0.027 β −0.073 β 0.037 β 0.024
– –
SE 0.013 SE 0.032 SE 0.012 SE 0.011
p 0.042 p 0.023 p 0.0013 p 0.032
Female Female Female Female Female Female
µ 17.18, SD 12.58 µ 72.32, SD 33.04 µ 0.01, SD 0.02 µ 0.01, SD 0.02 µ < 0.01, SD < 0.01 µ 8.31, SD 10.4
Male Male Male Male Male Male
Dog Sex 1
µ 12.82, SD 9.16 µ 84.42, SD 24.84 µ 0.02, SD 0.02 µ 0.01, SD 0.01 µ 0.01, SD 0.01 µ 12.48, SD 11.38
β −0.041 β 0.038 β −0.0033 β 0.013 β −0.024
–
SE 0.02 SE 0.06 SE 0.016 SE 0.0049 SE 0.029
p 0.043 p 0.53 p 0.84 p 0.0073 p 0.41
Track (%): tracking time (s)/total walking time (s) × 100%. Tail high (%): tail high time (s)/total walking time (s) × 100%, analysed in power of 7. Gaze (no./s): Numbers of gazes / time when
the dog’s head was visible in the Gopro video (s), analysed in power of 0.4. Lip-lick (no./s): Numbers of lip-licks/time when the dog’s head was visible in the Gopro video (s), analysed in
power of 0.4. Eliminate-mark (no./s): Numbers of eliminate-marks/total walking time (s), analysed in power of 0.6. Pant (%): painting time (s)/time when the dog’s head was visible in the
Gopro video (s) × 100%, analysed in power of 0.5. µ: mean. SD: standard deviation of µ. β: regression coefficient. SE: standard error of β. p: p-value of the model. –: Not included in the
generalized linear mixed model because the independent variable had high p-values in the bivariate regression model. 1 Women and female dogs were used as control. Wagging tail,
shaking body and sniffing were not entered into the generalized linear mixed model because both independent variables, dog sexes and human genders, had high p-values in the bivariate
regression models.
Animals 2020, 10, 1894 11 of 19
Table 8. Generalized linear mixed model of dog sex and human gender (independent variables) effects on human verbal cues (dependent variables). Mean (µ) and
standard deviation (SD) of different dependent variables by gender/sex was provided. All verbal cues were analysed as frequencies (numbers of events/total walking
time).
Total Verbal Cues (no./s) 1 Attention Seeking (no./s) 2 Communication (no./s) 2 Negative Verbal Cue (no./s) 2 Praise (no./s) 1 High-Pitched Voice (no./s) 1 Command (no./s) 1
Women Women Women Women Women Women Women
µ 0.09, SD 0.07 µ 0.02, SD 0.02 µ 0.01, SD 0.01 µ < 0.01, SD 0.01 µ 0.02, SD 0.02 µ 0.02, SD 0.02 µ 0.03, SD 0.03
Men Men Men Men Men Men Men
Human Gender 3 µ 0.07, SD 0.06 µ 0.02, SD 0.02 µ < 0.01, SD < 0.01 µ < 0.01, SD < 0.01 µ 0.02, SD 0.03 µ < 0.01, SD 0.01 µ 0.03, SD 0.02
β −0.034 β −0.031 β −0.003 β −0.011 β 0.005 β −0.062 β −0.032
SE 0.017 SE 0.017 SE 0.012 SE 0.0081 SE 0.011 SE 0.011 SE 0.011
p 0.041 p 0.076 p 0.8 p 0.17 p 0.65 p < 0.001 p 0.0056
Female Female Female Female Female Female Female
µ 0.08, SD 0.06 µ 0.02, SD 0.02 µ 0.01, SD 0.01 µ < 0.01, SD < 0.01 µ 0.02, SD 0.02 µ 0.01, SD 0.02 µ 0.03, SD 0.03
Male Male Male Male Male Male Male
Dog Sex 3
µ 0.08, SD 0.07 µ 0.02, SD 0.02 µ < 0.01, SD 0.01 µ < 0.01, SD < 0.01 µ 0.02, SD 0.03 µ 0.01, SD 0.02 µ 0.03, SD 0.03
β −0.021
– – – – – – SE 0.012
p 0.085
1.Analysed after transformation to the power of 0.5. 2. Analysed after transformation to the power of 0.4. 3 Women and female dogs were used as control. µ: mean. SD: standard deviation
of µ. β: regression coefficient. SE: standard error of β. p: p-value of the model –: Not included in the generalized linear mixed model because the independent variable had high p-values in
the bivariate regression model.
Animals 2020, 10, 1894 12 of 19
Table 9. Generalized linear mixed model of dog sex and human gender (independent variables) effects on human body language (dependent variables). Mean (µ) and
standard deviation (SD) of different dependent variables by gender/sex was provided. All body languages were analysed as frequencies (numbers of events/total
walking time).
Total Body Language (no./s) 1 Treating Dog with Food (no./s) Physical Contacts (no./s) 1
Women Women Women
µ 0.01, SD 0.02 µ < 0.01, SD < 0.01 µ < 0.01, SD 0.01
Men Men Men
Human Gender 2 µ 0.01, SD 0.02 µ < 0.01, SD 0.01 µ < 0.01, SD 0.01
β 0.06 β 0.001 β 0.067
SE 0.023 SE 0.00063 SE 0.019
p 0.0089 p 0.11 p 0.0007
Female Female Female
µ 0.01, SD 0.02 µ < 0.01, SD 0.01 µ < 0.01, SD 0.01
Dog Sex 2 Male Male Male
µ 0.01, SD 0.02 µ < 0.01, SD <0.01 µ < 0.01, SD 0.01
– – –
1.Analysed after transformation to the power of 0.3. Women and female dogs were used as control. µ: mean. SD: standard deviation of µ. β: regression coefficient. SE: standard error of
2.
β. p: p-value of the model. –: Not included in the generalized linear mixed model because the independent variable had high p-values in the bivariate regression model. Body gestures and
asking the dog to sit were not entered into the generalized linear mixed model because both independent variables, dog sexes and human genders, had high p-values in the bivariate
regression models.
Animals 2020, 10, 1894 13 of 19
This is the second report of a larger research project that explores the behavioural interaction
between shelter dogs and volunteers during on-leash walks (see also [21]). This article reports on
the effects of dog sex and human gender on the human–dog interactions during on-leash walks with
respect to both human and dog behaviour. Other variables (e.g., human and canine demographics,
personality and canine behavioural assessment) will be reported in future publications. Relationships
between volunteers’ training levels and the leash tension/pulling frequency were reported in our
previous article [21].
3. Results
3.1. Demographics
This study involved 111 shelter dogs and 74 human participants. For dogs, there were 58 (52.3%,
n = 58/111) females and 53 (47.7%, n = 53/111) males, all gonadectomised. For human participants,
there were 47 (63.5%, n = 47/74) women, 26 (35.1%, n = 26/74) men and 1 (1.4%, n = 1/74) person
self-nominated as the third gender. Since there was only one third gender participant, to avoid potential
bias, this person was excluded from all analysis regarding gender. Personality scores across five
personality traits were not significantly different between men and women (Table 10).
Table 10. Comparison of mean personality scores (µ) and stand deviation (SD) between men and
women volunteers across five personality traits (Two sample t-test).
sex did not influence the handler’s verbal expression and body language. Finally, the items and the
factor loadings obtained for the exit questionnaire are presented in Table 2. Results show that human
gender and canine sex had no significant effects on the human experience of the walk.
4. Discussion
This study investigated the role of human gender and dog sex in the behavioural interactions
when dogs are walked on a leash by different handlers. The first hypothesis was that a dog creates
a tighter leash during the walk if it is male or it is walked by a man. We also hypothesised that dogs are
more likely to show defensive and stress-related signs, such as staring and lip-licking if they are walked
by men. Finally, it was hypothesised that women tend to speak to the dogs more often, commonly
accompanied with a higher-pitched, baby-directed vocal communication and would be more tolerant
and satisfied with the interaction.
4.5. Limitations
A limitation of our study was that the 370 interactions were not completely independent because
the same volunteers and dogs participated in more than one walk, in order to create a larger sample
size of walks. In addition, dogs were not randomly matched with participants, but were assigned
to participants based on both the level assigned by shelter staff to dogs and participants. However,
considering human safety, animal welfare and the operation of the shelter, the present results suggest
potential ways to maximise the benefits and enjoyment of the walk for both dogs and people, by paying
some attention to both the experience and gender of volunteers. Finally, a substantial limitation of this
research was that the only coder of the data was familiar with the hypotheses of the study and was
aware of handlers’ gender when coding videos. To prevent the potential bias, future studies should
involve independent video coders naive to the study hypotheses.
5. Conclusions
This is the first study investigating the human–dog interactions during an on-leash walk focusing
on shelter dogs and volunteers and the first time a leash tension meter was used for canine science.
Results showed that male dogs generally caused higher leash tension and pulling frequency when
walked on a leash. Dogs showed more stress-related signs when interacting with men; stress signs
included shorter tail-high periods, more frequently looking toward men and lip-licking behaviour.
Animals 2020, 10, 1894 16 of 19
Finally, there was a greater pre-disposition in women to use language as a relational tool, while men
were more inclined to have physical contact with dogs. It is noted that the numeric differences
(expressed in % or frequency) in both canine and human behaviours were small so the results should
be interpreted with caution. However, the average walking time recorded in this study was only
around 5 min. In reality, the entire time for each walk at the RSPCA would be 15–30 min so such
differences (in time or counts of a behaviour) could be larger. This study may be useful in improving
shelter procedures or decision-making processes about walking partners for dogs. For instance, for the
safety concern, incoming male dogs without a previous behavioural history could be recommended
to be initially walked by more experienced volunteers while the shelter gathers information on the
dog’s behaviour, rather than less experienced volunteers who might have difficulties handling strong
and sudden dog pulling. Compared to men, women may be better candidates to interact with fearful
and stressed dogs. Finally, when interacting with dogs, especially timid individuals, men should be
reminded to be more aware of any physical contacts that may escalate the stress-related behaviour of
the dog.
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, H.-Y.S., C.J.C.P., M.B.A.P. and N.A.P.; methodology, H.-Y.S., C.J.C.P.,
F.G., M.B.A.P. and N.A.P.; software, F.G. and H.-Y.S.; validation, H.-Y.S., F.G. and C.J.C.P.; formal analysis, H.-Y.S.,
F.G. and C.J.C.P.; investigation, H.-Y.S.; resources, M.B.A.P. and C.J.C.P.; data curation, H.-Y.S., F.G. and C.J.C.P.;
writing—original draft preparation, H.-Y.S. and F.G.; writing—review and editing, H.-Y.S., C.J.C.P., M.B.A.P. and
N.A.P.; visualization, H.-Y.S. and C.J.C.P.; supervision, C.J.C.P., M.B.A.P. and N.A.P.; project administration, H.-Y.S.,
C.J.C.P. and M.B.A.P.; funding acquisition, C.J.C.P. and H.-Y.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published
version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research was funded by the Higher Degree Research Fund of University of Queensland and the
RSPCA Fund donated by philanthropists.
Acknowledgments: We thank RSPCA, QLD for allowing us to conduct this research at the shelter. We also
appreciate all dogs, volunteers and all RSPCA staff for playing important roles in this study. Finally, we acknowledge
Solomon Woldeyohannes for the statistical advice.
Conflicts of Interest: Mandy B.A. Paterson is employed as the principal scientist by RSPCA, QLD. None of the
authors receive any interest or financial support from people or organizations who can bias the research.
References
1. Kotrschal, K.; Schoberl, I.; Bauer, B.; Thibeaut, A.-M.; Wedl, M. Dyadic relationships and operational
performance of male and female owners and their male dogs. Behav. Process. 2009, 81, 383–391. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
2. Wells, D.L.; Hepper, P.G. Male and female dogs respond differently to men and women. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.
1999, 61, 341–349. [CrossRef]
3. McGuire, B.; Fry, K.; Orantes, D.; Underkofler, L.; Parry, S. Sex of Walker Influences Scent-marking Behavior
of Shelter Dogs. Animals 2020, 10, 632. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
4. Prato-Previde, E.; Fallani, G.; Valsecchi, P. Gender Differences in Owners Interacting with Pet Dogs:
An Observational Study. Ethology 2006, 112, 64–73. [CrossRef]
5. Pirrone, F.; Pierantoni, L.; Mazzola, S.M.; Vigo, D.; Albertini, M. Owner and animal factors predict the
incidence of, and owner reaction toward, problematic behaviors in companion dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2015,
10, 295–301. [CrossRef]
6. Blackwell, E.J.; Bolster, C.; Richards, G.; Loftus, B.A.; Casey, R.A. The use of electronic collars for training
domestic dogs: Estimated prevalence, reasons and risk factors for use, and owner perceived success as
compared to other training methods. BMC Vet. Res. 2012, 8, 93. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
7. Aliabadi, I.; Wedl, M.; Schoberl, I.; Bauer, B.; Kotrschal, K. Effects of gender on performance in human-dog
dyads in an agility parcours. In Proceedings of the 2010 Canine Science Forum, Vienna, Austria, 25–28
July 2010.
8. Ratcliffe, V.F.; McComb, K.; Reby, D. Cross-modal discrimination of human gender by domestic dogs.
Anim. Behav. 2014, 91, 127–135. [CrossRef]
9. Starling, M.J.; Branson, N.; Thomson, P.C.; McGreevy, P.D. Age, sex and reproductive status affect boldness
in dogs. Vet. J. 2013, 197, 868–872. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Animals 2020, 10, 1894 17 of 19
10. Wells, D.L.; Hepper, P.G. Prevalence of behaviour problems reported by owners of dogs purchased from an
animal rescue shelter. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2000, 69, 55–65. [CrossRef]
11. Col, R.; Day, C.; Phillips, C.J.C. An epidemiological analysis of dog behavior problems presented to
an Australian behavior clinic, with associated risk factors. J. Vet. Behav. 2016, 15, 1–11. [CrossRef]
12. Rooney, N.J.; Bradshaw, J.W.S.; Robinson, I.H. A comparison of dog–dog and dog–human play behaviour.
Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2000, 66, 235–248. [CrossRef]
13. Kis, A.; Turcsán, B.; Miklósi, Á; Gácsi, M. The effect of the owner’s personality on the behaviour of owner-dog
dyads. Interact. Stud. 2012, 13, 373–385. [CrossRef]
14. Bowes, M.; Keller, P.; Rollins, R.; Gifford, R. The Effect of Ambivalence on On-Leash Dog Walking Compliance
Behavior in Parks and Protected Areas. J. Park Recreat. Adm. 2017, 35, 81–93. [CrossRef]
15. Day, M.J.; Breitschwerdt, E.; Cleaveland, S.; Karkare, U.; Khanna, C.; Kirpensteijn, J.; Kuiken, T.; Lappin, M.R.;
McQuiston, J.; Mumford, E.; et al. Surveillance of Zoonotic Infectious Disease Transmitted by Small
Companion Animals. Emerg. Infect. Dis. 2012, 18, e1. [CrossRef]
16. Thompson, P.G. The public health impact of dog attacks in a major Australian city. Med. J. Aust. 1997,
167, 129–132. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
17. Klainbart, S.; Bibring, U.; Strich, D.; Chai, O.; Bdolah-Abram, T.; Aroch, I.; Kelmer, E. Retrospective evaluation
of 140 dogs involved in road traffic accidents. Vet. Rec. 2018, 182, 196. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
18. Pauli, A.M.; Bentley, E.; Diehl, K.A.; Miller, P.E. Effects of the Application of Neck Pressure by a Collar or
Harness on Intraocular Pressure in Dogs. J. Am. Anim. Hosp. Assoc. 2006, 42, 207–211. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
19. Hawson, L.A.; Salvin, H.E.; McLean, A.N.; McGreevy, P.D. Riders’ application of rein tension for walk-to-halt
transitions on a model horse. J. Vet. Behav. 2014, 9, 164–168. [CrossRef]
20. Warren-Smith, A.K.; Curtis, R.A.; Greetham, L.; McGreevy, P.D. Rein contact between horse and handler
duringspecific equitation movements. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 108, 157–169. [CrossRef]
21. Shih, H.-Y.; Georgiou, F.; Curtis, R.A.; Paterson, M.B.A.; Phillips, C.J.C. Behavioural evaluation of a leash
tension meter which measures pull direction and force during human-dog on-leash walks. Animals 2020,
10, 1382. [CrossRef]
22. Protopopova, A.; David, C.; Wynne, L. Adopter-dog interactions at the shelter: Behavioral and contextual
predictors of adoption. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 157, 109–116. [CrossRef]
23. Foyer, P.; Svedberg, A.-M.; Nilsson, E.; Wilsson, E.; Faresjö, A.; Jensen, P. Behavior and cortisol responses of
dogs evaluated in a standardized temperament test for military working dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2016, 11, 7–12.
[CrossRef]
24. Palestrini, C.; Minero, M.; Cannas, S.; Rossi, E.; Frank, D. Video analysis of dogs with separation-related
behaviors. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 124, 61–67. [CrossRef]
25. Siniscalchi, M.; Lusito, R.; Vallortigara, G.; Quaranta, A. Seeing Left- or Right-Asymmetric Tail Wagging
Produces Different Emotional Responses in Dogs. Curr. Biol. 2013, 23, 2279–2282. [CrossRef]
26. Alexandera, M.B.; Frienda, T.; Haug, L. Obedience training effects on search dog performance. Appl. Anim.
Behav. Sci. 2011, 132, 152–159. [CrossRef]
27. Clay, L.; Paterson, M.; Bennett, P.; Perry, G.; Phillips, C. Early Recognition of Behaviour Problems in Shelter
Dogs by Monitoring them in their Kennels after Admission to a Shelter. Animals 2019, 9, 875. [CrossRef]
28. Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals South Australia. The Best-Ever Walking Harness
for Your Dog (and the Must-Avoid Collars and Leads). Available online: https://www.rspcasa.org.au/best-
walking-harness-dogs/ (accessed on 13 August 2020).
29. McCrae, R.R.; Costa, P.T., Jr. Brief Versions of the NEO-PI-3. J. Individ. Differ. 2007, 28, 116–128. [CrossRef]
30. Grainger, J.; Wills, A.P.; Montrose, V.T. The behavioral effects of walking on a collar and harness in domestic
dogs (Canis familiaris). J. Vet. Behav. 2016, 14, 60–64. [CrossRef]
31. McGowan, R.T.S.; Bolte, C.; Barnett, H.R.; Perez-Camargo, G.; François, M. Can you spare 15 min? The
measurable positive impact of a 15-min petting session on shelter dog well-being. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.
2018, 203, 42–54. [CrossRef]
32. Beerda, B.; Schilder, M.B.H.; van Hooff, J.A.R.A.M.; de Vries, H.W.; Mol, J.A. Behavioural, saliva cortisol
and heart rate responses to different types of stimuli in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1998, 58, 365–381.
[CrossRef]
Animals 2020, 10, 1894 18 of 19
33. Cimarelli, G.; Turcsán, B.; Bánlaki, Z.; Range, F.; Virányi, Z. Dog Owners’ Interaction Styles: Their Components
and Associations with Reactions of Pet Dogs to a Social Threat. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1–14. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
34. Friard, O.; Gamba, M. BORIS: A free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding
and live observations. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2016, 7, 1325–1330. [CrossRef]
35. R Core Team. A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing:
Vienna, Austria, 2019.
36. Lumley, T. Leaps: Regression Subset Selection; Thomas Lumley based on Fortran code by Alan Miller; CRAN,
2020; Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/leaps/leaps.pdf (accessed on 16 October 2020).
37. Venables, W.N.; Ripley, B.D. Modern Applied Statistics with S, 4th ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2002.
38. Fox, J.; Weisberg, S. An {R} Companion to Applied Regression, 3rd ed.; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2019.
39. Fox, J.; Weisberg, S.; Price, B. CarData: Companion to Applied Regression Data Sets; CRAN, 2020; Available
online: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/carData/carData.pdf (accessed on 16 October 2020).
40. Bates, D.; Maechler, M. Matrix: Sparse and Dense Matrix Classes and Methods; CRAN, 2019; Available online:
https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/Matrix/Matrix.pdf (accessed on 16 October 2020).
41. Fox, J. Polycor: Polychoric and Polyserial Correlations; CRAN, 2019; Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/
web/packages/polycor/polycor.pdf (accessed on 16 October 2020).
42. Wickham, H. The Split-Apply-Combine Strategy for Data Analysis. J. Stat. Softw. 2011, 40, 1–29. [CrossRef]
43. Revelle, W. Psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research; Northwestern University:
Evanston, IL, USA, 2020.
44. Kassambara, A. Ggpubr: ’ggplot2’ Based Publication Ready Plots; CRAN, 2020; Available online: https:
//cran.r-project.org/web/packages/ggplot2/ggplot2.pdf (accessed on 16 October 2020).
45. Pinheiro, J.; Bates, D.; DebRoy, S.; Sarkar, D.; R Core Team. Nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models;
R Core Team: Vienna, Austria, 2020.
46. Zuur, A.F.; Ieno, E.N.; Elphick, C.S. A protocol for data exploration to avoid common statistical problems.
Methods Ecol. Evol. 2010, 1, 3–14. [CrossRef]
47. Maarschalkerweerd, R.J.; Endenburg, N.; Kirpensteijn, J.; Knol, B.W. Influence of orchiectomy on canine
behaviour. Vet. Rec. 1997, 140. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
48. Hart, B.L.; Eckstein, A.R. The role of gonadal hormones in the occurrence of objectionable behaviours in
dogs and cats. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1997, 52, 331–344. [CrossRef]
49. Warnes, C. Five myths commonly associated with neutering in dogs. Vet. Nurs. 2014, 5, 502–508. [CrossRef]
50. Kubinyi, E.; Turcsán, B.; Miklósi, Á. Dog and owner demographic characteristics and dog personality trait
associations. Behav. Process. 2009, 81, 392–401. [CrossRef]
51. Starling, M.J.; Branson, N.; Thomson, P.C.; McGreevy, P.D. “Boldness” in the domestic dog differs among
breeds and breed groups. Behav. Process. 2013, 97, 53–62. [CrossRef]
52. Hart, B.L.; Hart, L.A. Breed and gender differences in dog behavior. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution,
Behavior and Interactions with People, 2nd ed.; Serpell, J., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK,
2016; pp. 119–132.
53. Perez-Gomez, J.; Rodriguez, G.V.; Ara, I.; Olmedillas, H.; Chavarren, J.; González-Henriquez, J.J.; Dorado, C.;
Calbet, J.A.L. Role of muscle mass on sprint performance: Gender differences? Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2008,
102, 685–694. [CrossRef]
54. Yong, M.H.; Ruffman, T. Domestic dogs match human male voices to faces, but not for females. Behaviour
2015, 152, 1585–1600. [CrossRef]
55. Leaver, S.D.A.; Reimchen, T.E. Behavioural responses of Canis familiaris to different tail lengths of
a remotely-controlled life-size dog replica. Behaviour 2008, 145, 377–390. [CrossRef]
56. Tami, G.; Gallagher, A. Description of the behaviour of domestic dog (Canis familiaris) by experienced and
inexperienced people. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 120, 159–169. [CrossRef]
57. Jakovcevic, A.; Mustaca, A.; Bentosela, M. Do more sociable dogs gaze longer to the human face than less
sociable ones? Behav. Process. 2012, 90, 217–222. [CrossRef]
58. Bentosela, M.; Wynne, C.D.L.; D’Orazio, M.; Elgier, A.; Udell, M.A.R. Sociability and gazing toward humans
in dogs and wolves: Simple behaviors with broad implications. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2016, 105, 68–75.
[CrossRef]
Animals 2020, 10, 1894 19 of 19
59. Roth, L.S.V.; Jensen, P. Assessing companion dog behavior in a social setting. J. Vet. Behav. 2015, 10, 315–323.
[CrossRef]
60. Buttner, A.P.; Thompson, B.; Strasser, R.; Santo, J. Evidence for a synchronization of hormonal states between
humans and dogs during competition. Physiol. Behav. 2015, 147, 54–62. [CrossRef]
61. Jones, A.C.; Josephs, R.A. Interspecies hormonal interactions between man and the domestic dog (Canis
familiaris). Horm. Behav. 2006, 50, 393–400. [CrossRef]
62. Herzog, H.A. Gender Differences in Human–Animal Interactions: A Review. Anthrozoös 2007, 20, 7–21.
[CrossRef]
63. Wells, D.L.; Hepper, P.G. Directional tracking in the domestic dog, Canis familiaris. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.
2003, 84, 297–305. [CrossRef]
64. Řezáč, P.; Viziová, P.; Dobešová, M.; Havlíček, Z.; Pospíšilová, D. Factors affecting dog–dog interactions on
walks with their owners. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 134, 170–176. [CrossRef]
65. Firnkes, A.; Bartels, A.; Bidoli, E.; Erhard, M. Appeasement signals used by dogs during dog-human
communication. J. Vet. Behav. 2017, 19, 35–44. [CrossRef]
66. Weisberg, Y.J.; DeYoung, C.G.; Hirsh, J.B. Gender differences in personality across the ten aspects of the Big
Five. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 178. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional
affiliations.
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Reproduced with permission of copyright owner. Further reproduction
prohibited without permission.