Lab Manual Phy252 Lab11
Lab Manual Phy252 Lab11
Color
11.1 Objectives
• Observe the transmission properties of the three additive primary
color filters and the three subtractive primary color filters.
• Observe and interpret the color sensations resulting from mixing ad-
ditive primary colors and mixing subtractive primary colors.
11.2 Introduction
Color is something we often take for granted (unless we are artists). Grass
is green, the sky is blue (well, maybe not in the winter). But how are these
colors formed? How can I mix two colors of paint and end up with a third
color? I can hear two different sounds at the same time, but why does
nothing look like two different colors at the same time?
Color is a complicated subject because it’s a combination of physics
(wavelengths, frequencies, atomic spectra, light, etc.), the physiology of
color as perceived by humans, and history. It’s somewhat muddled because
through the history of painting and early attempts by Goethe and Newton
to craft theories of color, some of the their language has stuck.
205
11. Color
11.4 Theory
How do we see color?
As you experienced in last week’s lab, white light is composed of light from
all wavelengths in the visible range (400–700 nm) but why do we “see”
different wavelengths as different colors? A picture (made using a beam of
electrons) of a human retina is shown in Fig. 11.1. There are two types of
cells in the retina: rods (shown in green) and cones (shown in blue). The
rod cells are sensitive to light and dark while our color vision is due to the
cones. There are 3 types of cones and the relative response to the different
cones is interpreted by your brain as light of a particular color. One kind
of cone is most sensitive to red; another is most sensitive to green; and
the third to blue. These three colors (red, green and blue) are called the
additive primaries, as they are typically combined together (positively)
to create other colors.
Each type of cone (or color receptor) is actually sensitive to a range of
wavelengths and therefore colors. For example the “blue” cone responds to
light with wavelengths between 400 to 550 nm, or colors ranging from violet
to indigo to blue. Fig. 11.2 shows which wavelengths of light each type of
cone is sensitive to and how sensitive they are. Notice that the human eye
has less sensitivity to “blue” than “red”. This is one reason that stop signs
are red and not blue. The exact color we see is dependent on how much
each type of cone is stimulated with respect to the others. For example,
when green and red cones are simultaneously stimulated, we can see orange
or yellow, depending on how much more intense the red is with respect
to the green. By mixing the three additive primary colors (R, G and B)
with different intensities, we can generate all possible colors. For example,
you can convince yourself that red and green make yellow by squinting at
Fig. 11.3.
In order for us to see anything at all, the light has to enter our eyes. The
light can come directly from the light source, or it can be reflected from an
1
http://hyperphysics.phy-astr.gsu.edu/hbase/hph.html
Figure 11.1: A picture of the human retina showing the rods (in green) and
cones (in blue).
Figure 11.2: A plot showing the sensitivity of the 3 types of cones (red,
green and blue) in a human eye.
Figure 11.3: Squint at this and you’ll see yellow, yet no actual yellow color
is in the picture. This is Ferris Bueller’s moment with M. Seurat...and how
comic books work.
object. The red light at the top of a traffic signal is red because the light
source is red. On the other hand, the red stop sign is red because it reflects
red light, and absorbs all other colors. These examples represent two kinds
of color mixing, additive and subtractive, respectively.
process: light from a source, like the sun, falls on the surface of an object
(or set of pigments) and is reflected. But not all wavelengths are reflected;
just some reach our eyes which we interpret as the color of that object as
in Fig. 11.4. The painter’s job is to mix the right pigments together in
order to cause other humans to “see” the color he or she desired. Said more
specifically, the painter chooses the pigments necessary in order to take out
the wrong colors from light that reflect from the surface, leaving the desired
color. So color is not an attribute of just the object: it’s a combination of
the reflection or absorption of light (atomic physics) and our interpretation
of that light through our highly-evolved eyeballs (physiology).
The subtractive primaries are a convention following a long history
of painting and printing. They are now usually Cyan, Magenta, and Yellow
(from this point on, we’ll call them C, M, and Y). Painters like them because
Figure 11.6: This is another way to make yellow: all colors fall on the
surface of the banana and are absorbed except the yellow.
Figure 11.7: You’ve seen this on your computer as well. It’s the color
selection appropriate for printing (subtractive).
you can get the truest black using these three colors (which you’ll do).2
Filters
A filter is a semi-transparent film which passes some wavelengths and not
others. Let’s deal with the Additive Primaries (R, B, G) and the Subtractive
Primaries (C, Y, M) only. Remember: White light is a mixture of all
of the colors, a controversial fact first worked out by Isaac Newton. So
if you pass white light through a filter that removes all wavelengths but R
then you’d call that a Red Filter. Likewise, for B and G. Filters are not
perfect: a red filter doesn’t pass only a single wavelength, but rather a band
of wavelengths which are reddish. You’ll measure these bands in this lab.
Let’s picture the effects of filters on the Additive and Subtractive Pri-
maries in Figs. 11.8–11.9. In each picture, white light is incident from the
left, passes through a filter, and only particular wavelengths of light emerge
on the right. The little spectrum window (on the far right) shows the effects
on the spectrum of visible light.
Fig. 11.8 shows what happens when white light is passed through filters
that are the additive primary colors (R, G, B). Notice that if you pass white
light through a red filter only “red” (or the reddish wavelengths of) light is
transmitted. In addition it might not be too surprising that if you pass red
light through a red filter, all you get is red light out. But what happens
if you pass red light through a blue filter? The blue filter only transmits
light with wavelengths around 400-500 nm, but “red” light has wavelengths
around 600-700 nm. Does any of the red light make it through a blue filter?
The Subtractive Primary colors (C, M, Y) can also be fashioned into
filters, but here something different happens because both the physics and
our perception is different. Fig. 11.9 shows the results when white light
enters from the left and passes through:
. . . a cyan filter — to produce cyan colored light. It does that by remov-
ing the longest wavelengths (reddish) leaving the rest, which is represented
in the top picture as green and blue arrows. (Look at the color wheel:
the cyan filter removes its complementary color on the opposite side of the
wheel, i.e. red.)
2
Sometimes you see printers using CMYK, where K stands for “key”, which is not
black, per se, but a printer-specific designation for the black “key” plate that prints the
detail and is, in fact, black.
Figure 11.8: White light is incident on red (top), green (middle), or blue
(bottom) filters.
• The table shows that each subtractive primary (C, M, Y) absorbs one
additive primary color (R, G or B), as indicated by the color that
is shaded. For example, cyan absorbs red but transmits (or reflects)
green and blue.
• You can get one additive primary color by combining two subtractive
primary colors. For example, a mixture of magenta and yellow would
absorb both green and blue, allowing only red to be seen.
• You can add two additive primary colors together to get one of the
subtractive primary colors. For example, if you want to make the
color cyan you can add green and blue together. This effect is how
you see yellow in Fig. 11.3, notice that the bottom row in the table
tells you that red plus green will make yellow.
Table 11.1: For a given subtractive primary color that subtracts from
white light, this table shows which colors are absorbed and which are re-
flected/transmitted. Colors with a gray background and in bold are ab-
sorbed.
11.6 Equipment
• Digital spectrometer from the previous lab, but with an absorptome-
ter with integrated sampling system that creates light internally and
passes it through insertable filters for digitization (Ocean Optics USB650
Red Tide, see Fig. 11.10)
• One empty cuvette3 for calibration (the clear tube in Fig. 11.10(a))
3
A “cuvette” is a glass or plastic holder designed to hold objects destined for spec-
troscopic analysis. It’s pronounced “coo-vette” as in “corvette.”
11.7 Procedure
Setup
1. Start the data-collection program Logger Pro and choose New from
the File menu.
2. Make sure the power supply for the fan is plugged into the part of the
digital spectrometer that is labeled “USB-ISS-UV-VIS”.
a) Place the empty cuvette in the square hole in the top of the
spectrometer (see Fig. 11.10); make sure to align the cuvette so
that the frosted sides are parallel with the long edge of the whole
device (this ensures that the clear sides are facing the light source
of the spectrometer).
1. Insert the cuvette with the desired filter into the sample holder, press
the “Collect” button in Logger Pro, and observe the spectrum that
results. If the spectrum is not visible or is cut off at the top of the
graph, right click on the y-axis label Absorbance, select Autoscale
Autoscale.
2. Write down your observations for each filter in the table under Ques-
tion 1. Describe which colors are transmitted and record the wave-
length ranges of the resulting absorption spectra. Note that you may
see a continuous band of colors (the “main” band), then a gap, and a
narrower range of colors.
3. Print out the curves from the R, G and B filters all on 1 graph.
You can do this by hitting “collect” when starting the next filter and
choosing the option to keep the previous result in the dialog box.
4. Label each curve by going to Insert Text Annotation. A text
box will appear and you can grab the end of the line with the mouse
and point it at the curve that you’ve referenced.
For this part of the lab you will turn in 2 graphs taken with the spec-
trometer: one for the additive primaries (R, G, and B) and one for the
subtractive primaries (C, M, and Y). Make sure to label all of the curves.
11.8 Questions
1. Record your observations below:
green
blue
cyan
magenta
yellow
3. How well do the R, G and B filters match the colors you expected?
Would you say that the filters are “ideal”?
red + blue
green + blue
5. How well did your predictions agree with your results? Explain any
differences.
blue + yellow
cyan + magenta
cyan + yellow
magenta + yellow
7. Why does “red + blue” give different results in this part of the ex-
periment compared to the part with the projector box? Explain what
caused the results for red + blue to be different in each case (that is,
explain how each case, additive vs. subtractive, works).
9. For blue + yellow, did the color you observe match your prediction?
If it didn’t, then why not? What wavelengths of light must the filters
let through?
10. Check this with the spectrometer by using the cuvette that has two
filters (a blue and a yellow) in it. Did you accurately estimate which
wavelengths the filters would let through? If not, which wavelengths
did the filters let through? Print out the graph from the spectrometer.
11. Give a practical example from everyday life of additive and subtractive
color mixing. (You must give a different example than the stop sign
and stop light from the write-up.)