Healthy Steps As A Moderator
Healthy Steps As A Moderator
This document has been sent to you from APA PsycNET, APA's delivery platform for
its 6 premier databases: PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, PsycBOOKS, PsycEXTRA,
PsycTESTS, and PsycTHERAPY
Briggs, R. D., Silver, E. J., Krug, L. M., Mason, Z. S., Schrag, R. D. A., Chinitz, S., &
Racine, A. D. (2014). Healthy Steps as a moderator: The impact of maternal trauma on
child social-emotional development. Clinical Practice in Pediatric Psychology, 2(2),
166–175. https://doi.org/10.1037/cpp0000060
Abstract
KEYWORDS:
Lanzi, 2009; Cohen, Hien, & Batchelder, 2008). For example, mothers with a history of
trauma may have difficulty interpreting their children’s emotional states and responding
in an affectively appropriate manner to their children’s bids for interaction (Gusella, Muir,
& Tronick, 1988; Schechter, Kaminer, Grienenberger, & Amat, 2003; Schechter et al.,
2005). These parents may also employ more negative parenting practices, including
higher rates of severe physical punishment, abuse potential, psychological aggression,
and neglectful behaviors (Banyard et al., 2003; Bert et al., 2009; Cohen et al., 2008). In
this way, an intergenerational cycle continues in which traumatized caregivers struggle
to apply adequate parenting techniques, thereby potentially exposing their own children
to trauma as well.
One pathway by which this cycle affects young children’s socioemotional growth is by
exacerbating the impact of their exposure to toxic stress, which refers to the protracted
activation of a child’s stress response, in the absence of an effective, supportive caregiver
(Garner et al., 2012; Shonkoff, Boyce, & McEwen, 2009). In other words, appropriate
and responsive caregiving is a protective factor against the potentially toxic effects of
stress, but parents with their own traumatic histories may not be able to provide this type
of environment. Thus, their children may be particularly vulnerable to toxic stress and
thereby to changes in brain architecture that put them at risk for poor social-emotional
development.
The American Academy of Pediatrics recently issued a policy statement (Garner et al.,
2012) detailing the effects of toxic stress on the developing child, and calling for
pediatricians to respond. This call to action suggested that the medical home was an ideal
setting for interventions to interrupt the intergenerational cycle of trauma, as it is
universally accessed and nonstigmatized (American Academy of Pediatrics, Medical
Home Initiatives for Children with Special Needs Project Advisory Committee, 2002).
However, the medical and psychological community remains undecided about the most
effective way to identify and support children at risk.
Based on current evidence, we believe that the children most vulnerable to the effects of
toxic stress may be those whose caregivers have been exposed to childhood trauma
themselves. Thus, a reliable measure of adverse childhood experiences applied to adult
caregivers could serve as a useful proxy instrument to identify at-risk children. In
particular, we hypothesize that caregivers’ childhood experiences of trauma would
predict difficulties in their children’s social-emotional development. Moreover, if we
accept the initial hypothesis that a parent’s own traumatic childhood is a valid way to
identify young children at risk at the earliest possible moment, then we should devise
appropriate interventions to interrupt the intergenerational cycle of trauma. Our second
hypothesis, therefore, is that parent-focused interventions are a way to address this
strategic objective.
One such program that may be effective in this regard is Healthy Steps (HS), an
intervention that places early childhood development specialists in primary care
pediatrics to comanage the care of young children alongside the medical provider, with
an enhanced focus on development and behavior (Lawrence, Magee, & Bernard, 2001;
Minkovitz, Strobino, Hughart, Scharfstein, & Guyer, 2001). Although not manualized,
the intervention, which has been implemented at numerous sites across the country, is
standardized in its goal to increase caregivers’ understanding of development and
behavior using a range of tools and strategies (see http://www.healthysteps.org). Within
4
this basic framework, however, there is room for each site to develop the program in such
a way as to maximize its effectiveness for the particular population that it serves.
Although HS has historically focused primarily on developmental and procedural
outcomes (Johnston, Huebner, Anderson, Tyll, & Thompson, 2006; Minkovitz et al.,
2001), we have implemented an HS program at our institution that focuses significant
attention on caregiver trauma and child social-emotional development. Over several years
we have enrolled and followed a cohort of at-risk families. As part of the evaluation of
our HS program, we collected data on parents’ childhood experiences of trauma and also
screened for problems in the child’s socioemotional functioning at various intervals up to
36 months of age. We also obtained the same measures from a demographically similar
group of families at another site that did not offer HS. This has provided us with the
opportunity to examine the following hypotheses: (a) that there would be a positive
relationship between caregiver report of childhood trauma and problems in their
children’s social-emotional development, and (b) that the HS intervention would serve as
a moderator of this relationship, that is, that the association between presence of caregiver
trauma and increased socioemotional problems in their children would be reduced or
eliminated in families in the HS program.
Method
Design
In the intervention practice, HS was offered to all first-time parents by the pediatrician;
if parents accepted, families were enrolled before children turned 2 months old (either
prenatally during the last trimester to ensure a viable pregnancy, or within the first 2
5
months of life), and continued in the program until age 3. Throughout the course of the
intervention, they were screened regularly regarding child social-emotional development.
In the comparison practice, families needed to meet the same enrollment criteria (first-
time parents, children less than two months old). They were recruited by a trained
research interviewer in the waiting room, and were asked to complete the same screening
measures, during the same-age well-child visits as HS mothers. All enrolled participants
provided written informed consent, and the study was approved by the Institutional
Review Board of the Montefiore Medical Center.
Intervention
The HS program was located at a large urban pediatric practice within a federally
qualified health center, and was staffed by one licensed child psychologist and one
licensed clinical social worker as Healthy Steps Specialists (HSSs). The specialists had a
combined 8 years of experience as HSSs. The pediatric practice serves as a continuity
clinic for a large children’s hospital, and annually provides more than 23,000 pediatric
visits to over 11,000 distinct patients, ages 0 to 21. More than 80% of patients are either
Hispanic or African American, and more than two thirds of patients have Medicaid or
other state-sponsored insurance programs. As stated previously, the comparison practice
was equivalent to the intervention practice on a range of variables.
As a standard of HS care, families received comanaged visits at the newborn, 2-, 6-, 12-,
18-, 24-, 30-, and 36-month health care maintenance visits. Those families enrolled
prenatally also received a prenatal HS visit. In addition, families received contacts in the
form of additional office visits, a baby-and-me group, home visits and other nonclinic
visits, and clinically based phone calls.
Families received services until their children were 3 years old, at which time they
“graduated” from the program. In addition to the standard core elements, HS also offered
home visits as needed (because of, e.g., safety concerns or desire to forge a stronger
alliance with the family) and baby-and-me support and education groups. These groups
6
were 1 hr in length, offered 1 day per week, and focused on parenting education, social
support, and structured play time.
Measures
The FPS, administered at enrollment, detects the presence of caregiver risk factors that
may impact aspects of child development. The questionnaire includes sections on family
medical history, substance use, depression, and social support. The section “When you
were a child” contains eight items that measure a respondent’s history of trauma and
abuse. Positive responses to any of the first four relevant questions, not including the
question regarding parental use of time-outs, are considered a positive screen, which may
predispose parents to disciplinary practices that may be abusive or too permissive. As per
the scoring criteria, the four relevant questions are
This section of the FPS has a sensitivity of 92% to 95% and specificity of 87% to 92%
(Kemper & Kelleher, 1996).
Analyses
General linear model (GLM) and logistic regression analyses were used to determine the
relationship between maternal childhood trauma as measured on the FPS and maternal
report of child’s social-emotional development on the ASQ:SE at 36 months, adjusting
for site of enrollment (HS vs. comparison group [CG]) and for baseline parental
characteristics associated with childhood trauma that might affect child’s functioning
7
(age, educational level, race/ethnicity, having been depressed in the prior 12 months;
Hypothesis 1). Analyses also tested for a moderating effect of HS on the relationship
between maternal childhood trauma and child ASQ:SE score by determining whether
there was a significant interaction between participating in HS and presence of caregiver
childhood trauma (Hypothesis 2).
Results
For both the HS intervention and the CG, approximately two thirds of caregivers asked
to participate agreed to do so; we originally enrolled 208 children in HS and 116 in the
CG (total N = 324). Power analysis determined these numbers would be sufficient to
detect differences in their ASQ:SE scores following the intervention. However, at the 36-
month assessment, we had experienced significant attrition, such that 85 HS children were
assessed (41%) and 39 CG children were assessed (34%) on the ASQ:SE (see Figure 1).
Figure 1. Initial enrollment and admission. The color version of this figure appears in
the online article only.
In addition, when we looked at the 124 families included in the present analyses, we found
baseline differences between the HS and CG groups (see Table 2). HS mothers were more
likely to have completed education beyond high school (p < .001), and they were, on
average, approximately two years older than their CG counterparts (p = .03). HS mothers
were also twice as likely to report experiences of childhood trauma (55% vs. 26%; p =
.003; see Figure 2).
Figure 2. Prevalence of childhood trauma in mothers. The color version of this figure
appears in the online article only.
Hypothesis 1
In the GLM analysis that adjusted for baseline differences, the overall mean score on the
36-month ASQ:SE was 55.9. As we had hypothesized, children of mothers reporting
childhood trauma had significantly higher (worse) ASQ:SE mean scores than children of
mothers who did not report childhood trauma (75.9 vs. 35.9; p < .0001), with their average
mean score falling well above the scale’s risk cutoff of 59 (see Figure 3).
Figure 3. Full sample mean Ages and Stages Questionnaires: Social-Emotional scores.
The color version of this figure appears in the online article only.
In addition, 24% of the children overall (26% of HS, 21% of CG, n.s.) scored above the
risk cutoff. The logistic regression analysis showed that, with baseline differences
controlled, children of mothers with childhood trauma were 9 times as likely as children
whose mothers did not report childhood trauma to have a score above the risk cutoff (OR
= 9.08; p < .0001).
Hypothesis 2
article only.
Discussion
A common refrain of our HSSs is, “If a baby feels safe, a baby will explore. And if a baby
explores, a baby will learn.” The association between responsive, serve-and-return
caregiving environments that can buffer the effects of toxic stress and subsequent child
exploration and learning is of great importance in our efforts to capitalize on the plasticity
of the developing brain. It may allow us the possibility to prospectively address a range
of important child outcomes, including school readiness, relationships, and the ability to
manage their environment effectively.
It is important that we did not assess parenting behavior per se; nevertheless, research
informs our assumption that healthy child social-emotional development stems from
reliable, consistent, and empathic parenting behavior. We hypothesize that HS moderated
the relationship between caregiver childhood trauma and impaired child social-emotional
development by increasing these positive parenting behaviors that were perhaps more
likely to be lacking in those mothers. Moreover, as mothers receive a package of many
different services as part of the HS intervention, and these are delivered through multiple
modalities and focus on a range of issues, we cannot be certain exactly which aspects of
the program were responsible for measured differences between groups. Further
understanding of this pathway represents an important area for future study.
There are important limitations of this study. First, our attrition rate, from enrollment at
birth to age 3, was substantial, as we lost 59% of the intervention group and 66% of the
comparison group. The primary reason for attrition, as per our participants, was their
choice to move, either out of the area completely or to a provider at a practice closer to
their home. The intervention site practice also houses the high-risk obstetrics and
gynecology practice for the borough, and many families begin their pediatric care there
but then move to a more geographically convenient practice. Neither group received any
financial remuneration for their participation in the assessments. Although these attrition
rates are high, they are not unusual in a largely Hispanic research cohort. In their study
of a largely Hispanic population in San Francisco, Marin and Marin (1991) reported a
45% attrition rate at 12 months follow-up; similarly, with a Hispanic sample in Texas, the
attrition rate at 36 months follow-up was 65%.
Second, and related to attrition, is our small sample size, which may limit generalizability
of results. Additionally, our intervention was not blinded, so that the participants in the
intervention knew they were receiving specified services. This feature of our quasi-
experimental design is not unique but rather common to care systems with limited
resources that are able to offer certain services to some patients but not to all. In fact, it
was the natural outcome of this apportionment of HS services that provided a built-in
comparison group with which to compare the outcomes of HS participants.
direction in which HS parents have fewer concerns (lower scores); we would expect that
some mothers might report greater concerns about their child’s socioemotional
functioning because of this education.
Although the HSSs did observe the child during the program period, their ratings could
not be used to corroborate parental reports because they also would not have been
independent from ASQ:SE scores and knowledge of maternal childhood trauma. Pediatric
records would also have primarily relied on maternal report. A superior model of
independent observation and coding, such as that used in Perrin, Sheldrick, McMenamy,
Henson, and Carter (2014), may be of use. However, it is worth noting that Perrin and
colleagues reported that videotaped observations within pediatric practice were
technologically challenging. Further research in this area would benefit from prospective
measures of trauma and independent ratings of parenting practices and child social-
emotional development. Future investigations of our own data will also include maternal
depression screening results. We know that maternal depression can impact both parents’
perceptions of their children and children’s’ social-emotional development. Although we
do screen for, and attempt to intervene with, maternal depression as part of HS, we do not
do so at 36 months, thus making it impossible to include in this preliminary model.
Conclusion
Although this much is generally agreed upon, significant challenges remain regarding the
design of effective identification and intervention programs. HS, a national program
13
begun in 1996, shows promise in this area. Our experience suggests that provision of a
HS program, with particular focus on caregiver childhood trauma and child social-
emotional development, may be an appropriate and effective intervention within the
pediatric setting. In our setting, the HS program costs approximately $100 per family, per
year, also suggesting that it is a cost-effective approach to a significant challenge.
References
• Alim, T. N., Graves, E., Mellman, T. A., Aigbogun, N., Gray, E., Lawson, W.,
& Charney, D. S. (2006). Trauma exposure, posttraumatic stress disorder and
depression in an African-American primary care population. Journal of the
National Medical Association, 98, 1630–1636.
• Banyard, V. L., Williams, L. M., & Siegel, J. A. (2003). The impact of complex
trauma and depression on parenting: An exploration of mediating risk and
protective factors. Child Maltreatment, 8, 334–349.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1077559503257106
• Bert, S. C., Guner, B. M., & Lanzi, R. G. (2009). The influence of maternal
history of abuse on parenting knowledge and behavior. Family Relations: An
Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 58, 176–187.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3729.2008.00545.x
• Cohen, L. R., Hien, D. A., & Batchelder, S. (2008). The impact of cumulative
maternal trauma and diagnosis on parenting behavior. Child Maltreatment, 13,
27–38. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077559507310045
• Gusella, J. L., Muir, D., & Tronick, E. Z. (1988). The effect of manipulating
maternal behavior during an interaction on three- and six-month-olds’ affect and
attention. Child Development, 59, 1111–1124. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130278
• Johnston, B. D., Huebner, C. E., Anderson, M. L., Tyll, L. T., & Thompson, R.
S. (2006). Healthy Steps in an integrated delivery system: Child and parent
outcomes at 30 months. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 160,
793–800. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.160.8.793
• Lawrence, P. R., Magee, T., & Bernard, A. (2001). Reshaping primary care: The
Healthy Steps Initiative. Journal of Pediatrics Health Care, 15, 58–62.
• Marin, G., & Marin, B. (1991). Research with Hispanic populations. Newbury,
Park, CA: Sage.
• Minkovitz, C., Strobino, D., Hughart, N., Scharfstein, D., & Guyer, B. (2001).
Early effects of the Healthy Steps for Young Children Program. Archives of
Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 155, 470–479.
https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.155.4.470
• Perrin, E. C., Sheldrick, R. C., McMenamy, J. M., Henson, B. S., & Carter, A. S.
(2014). Improving parenting skills for families of young children in pediatric
settings: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of the American Medical
Association Pediatrics, 168, 16–24.
https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.2919
15
• Schechter, D. S., Coots, T., Zeanah, C. H., Coates, S. W., Grienenberger, J. F.,
Davies, M., . . . Myers, M. M. (2005). Maternal mental representations of the
child in an inner-city clinical sample: Violence-related posttraumatic stress and
reflective functioning. Attachment & Human Development, 7, 313–331.
https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500246011
• Schechter, D. S., Kaminer, T., Grienenberger, J. F., & Amat, J. (2003). Fits and
starts: A mother-infant case-study involving intergenerational violent trauma
and pseudoseizures across three generations. Infant Mental Health Journal, 24,
510–528. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.10070
• Shonkoff, J. P., & Bales, S. N. (2011). Science does not speak for itself:
Translating child development research for the public and its policymakers.
Child Development, 82, 17–32. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
8624.2010.01538.x
• Squires, J., Bricker, D., & Twombly, E. (2002). Ages and Stages Questionnaire:
Social-emotional (ASQ:SE). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Support for this research was provided for by the Altman Foundation and the Price
Family Foundation.Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Rahil
D. Briggs, Children’s Hospital at Montefiore, Department of Pediatrics, Albert Einstein
College of Medicine, 1 Fordham Plaza, Suite 1100, Bronx, NY 10458
Email: [email protected]
Received December 17, 2013
Revision received April 1, 2014
Accepted April 3, 2014
--
Juana R. Pinzás G., PhD
Profesora Principal
Dpto. de Psicología
16