Aiou Assignment B.ed
Aiou Assignment B.ed
Aiou Assignment B.ed
Assignment No. 2
SPRING 2022
Question no.1
Emotional development is important for children even before going to
school. Discuss emotional characteristics of preschool children.
Emotional Development
Emotional development is a complex task that begins in infancy and continues into
adulthood. The first emotions that can be recognised in babies include joy, anger, sadness and
fear. As children’s sense of self develops, more complex emotions like shyness, surprise,
elation, embarrassment, shame, guilt, pride and empathy emerge. School aged children and
young people are still learning to identify emotions, to understand why they happen, and how
to manage them appropriately.
The rate of emotional development in children and young people can vary from person to person. Some
children may show a high level of emotional skill development while quite young, whereas others take
longer to develop the capacity to manage their emotions well into adolescence.
INSTANT GRATIFICATION
Toddlers and preschoolers are ruled by their emotions and don’t have much, if any,
impulse control. And if there is one thing they don’t like, it’s being delayed gratification.
This, combined with their struggle to separate feelings from actions, means they’ll want to
immediately express an emotion or gratify a desire. They will cry immediately when sad and
when they feel they want something they will try to take it right away.
Keep this in mind as you try your best to teach them appropriate ways to show what they’re
feeling as well as sharing and turn-taking skills.
Separation anxiety
Three-year-olds are generally less interested in playing with other children, and have
a greater capacity to be affected by separation anxiety; they’ll be more interested in staying
with their parents or primary caregivers. Generally, four-year-olds will have an easier time with
this and any separation anxiety will be more short-lived.
Though your 3-year-old is beginning to understand the emotions they are feeling, they
still have very little control over them. If they find something funny, they'll laugh hysterically.
If something makes them feel sad or angry, they'll burst into tears.
At this age, your preschooler still hasn't developed much impulse control. If they feel
something, they are likely to act on it. This may mean snatching a toy away from another child
if they want to play with it, or getting upset when they want a snack after being told they have
to wait until dinnertime. Delayed gratification means nothing to them -- they want it, and want
it now.
Three- and 4-year-old children may use hitting, biting, or pushing as a way to solve
conflicts. They simply don't understand the difference between appropriate and inappropriate
interactions yet. It's your job to teach your child that there are right and wrong ways to express
emotions and resolve problems with others.
As your child gets older, they'll begin to see a connection between emotional outbursts and negative
consequences. Throwing a tantrum may result in a "time out" or a favorite toy being taken away.
These consequences are helping your 4-year-old understand a tantrum isn't an acceptable way to
show emotion.
Your 4-year-old is also a budding comedian. They are starting to develop a sense of
humor, and love being silly and making people laugh. Don't be surprised if you hear them calling
their friend a "poo-poo head" and then laughing hysterically; 4-year-olds find potty talk highly
entertaining.
Around age 3, children begin to develop a vivid imagination. At this age, your preschooler
will begin to spend a great deal of time in a fantasy world of their own creation. Their dolls and
stuffed animals all have names and personalities. They may chat with imaginary friends. Parents
sometimes worry that imaginary friends are a sign of loneliness or isolation, but in fact they're just
the opposite. Children use this type of fantasy play to learn how to interact with real people. It's
practice for the "real world." At an age when your child has very little control over their own life,
their fantasy world is their own creation. They're in charge. Around the same time your
preschooler begins to talk to an imaginary friend, they may also develop a fear of the monster
living under their bed. These types of fears are common. They are also quite serious to them,
so don't make a joke out of it. The best thing you can do is reassure your child that they are
safe and nothing is going to hurt them.
As your child gets older, fantasy play will continue to be an important part of their life,
but they'll get better at understanding the difference between fantasy and reality. their fantasies
will get more elaborate and sophisticated, and don't be surprised if they sometimes involve
violence. Don't let games of shoot-'em-up bother you; it's totally normal for children to be
fascinated with weapons and violence at this age, and it's not a sign that they'll be violent when
they're older.
Autonomy versus shame and doubt is the second stage of Erik Erikson's stages of
psychosocial development. This stage occurs between the ages of 18 months to approximately
3 years. According to Erikson, children at this stage are focused on developing a sense of
personal control over physical skills and a sense of independence.
Success in this stage will lead to the virtue of will. If children in this stage are encouraged and
supported in their increased independence, they become more confident and secure in their
own ability to survive in the world.
If children are criticized, overly controlled, or not given the opportunity to assert
themselves, they begin to feel inadequate in their ability to survive, and may then become
overly dependent upon others, lack self-esteem, and feel a sense of shame or doubt in their
abilities.
The child is developing physically and becoming more mobile, and discovering that he
or she has many skills and abilities, such as putting on clothes and shoes, playing with toys,
etc. Such skills illustrate the child's growing sense of independence and autonomy.
For example, during this stage children begin to assert their independence, by walking away
from their mother, picking which toy to play with, and making choices about what they like to
wear, to eat, etc.
Erikson states it is critical that parents allow their children to explore the limits of their
abilities within an encouraging environment which is tolerant of failure.
For example, rather than put on a child's clothes a supportive parent should have the patience
to allow the child to try until they succeed or ask for assistance.
So, the parents need to encourage the child to become more independent while at the same time
protecting the child so that constant failure is avoided.
A delicate balance is required from the parent. They must try not to do everything for the child,
but if the child fails at a particular task they must not criticize the child for failures and accidents
(particularly when toilet training).
The aim has to be “self control without a loss of self-esteem” (Gross, 1992).
The older your preschooler gets, the more they'll crave independence. It may sound like a
contradiction, but the best way to nurture your preschooler's independence and self-confidence is
to keep their life fairly structured. Give them choices, but don't give them endless choices. Let them
choose between two outfits to wear, or ask them if they want a turkey sandwich or macaroni and
cheese for lunch. When they ask to do something you know isn't a good idea, hold firm. Being
allowed choices within a structured framework will help to boost their self-confidence while at the
same time letting them know they are safe and secure.
References:
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/37811146_Emotional_Development
https://www.pubs.ext.vt.edu/content/dam/pubs_ext_vt_edu/350/350-053/350-053(FCE-134P).pdf
https://www.academia.edu/6652139/Emotional_Development
Questiono.2
What is the role of community in moral development of a child?
A child’s relationship environment begins in the family, but then extends to adults and
peers outside of the family who have important roles in their life. Educators and other education
and care staff are a significant part of many children’s relationship environment. Communities
that foster positive interactions and relationships between children, peers and adults strengthen
children’s outcomes.
When children have a sense of belonging and feel safe, secure and supported, they have
the confidence to play, explore and learn. A service that is strongly connected to the people
and place of its community is welcoming, inclusive, connected to the culture and context of
children’s families, while nurturing respectful and reciprocal relationships with children’s
families. Connection to community creates a responsive, safe and stable education and care
environment which, in turn, promotes children’s belonging and learning.
Children’s understanding of their self is developed through relationships and in the context of
their families and communities. ‘Relationships engage children in the human community in
ways that help them define who they are, what they can become, and how and why they are
important to other people’ (Center on the Developing Child, 2004, p. 1). Identity is a strong
foundation for children’s social and emotional development as well as their sense of agency.
Having everyday experiences and participating with the people and places of a community
enables children to observe, engage, understand and actively contribute to their expanding
world. This supports children to live interdependently with others, be decision-makers and have
influence. The ability to participate in different communities – a central element of citizenship
– helps young children to respond to diversity and become socially responsible.
Moral behaviour:
Moral behaviour means behaviour in conformity with the moral code of the social group. The
term ‘Moral’ comes from the Latin word ‘mores’ meaning manners, customs and folkways.
Moral behaviour not only conforms to social standards but also it is carried out voluntarily. It
is always a companied by a feeling of responsibility for one’s acts. It involves giving primary
consideration to the welfare of the group and considering personal gain or desires as having
secondary importance.
Moral concepts:
Moral concepts are the rules of behaviour to which the members of a culture become
accustomed and which determine the expected behaviour patterns of all group members.
A-moral stage:
This stage lasts from birth to two years. The new born baby has no idea of good or bad,
or right and wrong. He is moved solely by his urges and wants. He is unaware of the effects
his act has on others.
Self-centered stage:
This stage usually goes up to six years. Some individuals may remain fixated at this
stage even much later. At this stage the person learns to distinguish between self-interest and
group interest but he is not prepared to sacrifice his own interest. A self-centred act is done
more with a view to satisfy one’s impulses or desires than to disregard a moral rule.
• Regular physical activity promotes growth and development and has multiple
benefits for physical, mental, and psychosocial health that undoubtedly contribute to
learning.
• Specifically, physical activity reduces the risk for heart disease, diabetes mellitus,
osteoporosis, high blood pressure, obesity, and metabolic syndrome; improves
various other aspects of health and fitness, including aerobic capacity, muscle and
bone strength, flexibility, insulin sensitivity, and lipid profiles; and reduces stress,
anxiety, and depression.
• Physical activity can improve mental health by decreasing and preventing conditions
such as anxiety and depression, as well as improving mood and other aspects of well-
being.
• These attributes in turn are important determinants of current and future participation
in physical activity. Sedentary behaviors such as sitting and television viewing
contribute to health risks both because of and independently of their impact on
physical activity.
• Health-related behaviors and disease risk factors track from childhood toadulthood,
indicating that early and ongoing opportunities for physical activity are needed for
maximum health benefit.
• Frequent bouts of physical activity throughout the day yield short-term benefits for
mental and cognitive health while also providing opportunities to practice skills and
building confidence that promotes ongoing engagement in physical activity.
• Distinct types of physical activity address unique health concerns and contribute in
distinct ways to children's health, suggesting that a varied regimen including aerobic
and resistance exercise, structured andunstructured opportunities, and both longer
sessions and shorter bouts will likely confer the greatest benefit.
However, children and young people across the OECD are not engaging enough in the
behaviors they need to be healthy. Between 2000 to 2016, PISA data show that children and
young people were less likely to reach the minimum recommended daily physical activity
levels (>60 minutes of moderate to vigorous activity daily). They were also less likely to get
enough quality sleep, and more likely to be overweight and obese and have poor dietary
habits (including increasing overconsumption of soft drinks, sweets, salty snacks and fast
food).
These trends are extremely concerning. Unhealthy lifestyle behaviors are associated
with higher rates of cardiovascular diseases and type II diabetes, and while historically
considered to be diseases of adulthood, these are now evident in children as young as two
years old.
References:
https://www.cdc.gov/healthyschools/physicalactivity/facts.htm#:~:text=Students%20who%20are%20phys
ically%20active,%2C%20on%2Dtask%20behavior).&text=Higher%20physical%20activity%20and%20
physical,concentration%2C%20memory)%20among%20students.
https://ihtusa.com/why-physical-education-is-necessary-for-every-student/
https://studentwellbeinghub.edu.au/students/secondary/year-10-year-12/topics/physical-and-mental-
health-for-learning/
Question no.3
Language needs of community to be developed. Discuss.
Need of community to Developed
A speech community is a group of people who share rules for conducting and
interpreting at least one variety of a language or dialect. The term can be applied to a
neighborhood, a city, a region or a nation. We all belong to at least one speech community. The
earliest speech community we belong to is the one we share with our primary caregivers
(usually our parents) and is the basis for some of the most intimate and long term relationships
we form across our life. The rules and norms of this speech community show up in a dialect
referred to as the vernacular, the most basic variety or dialect of language we command. Our
vernacular speech is least susceptible to monitoring and least likely to change across our
lifetime.
Most of us were immersed in language from our first awareness of the world around
us. Since infants can hear the sound of their mother’s voice and the noises and interactions in
her environment in the womb, we probably hear our first sounds before we take our first breath.
Fairly early in our development, we target in our babbling those sounds that form the phonology
of our language or dialect. In interaction with us, our mother adjusts her speech to reflect the
phonology, morphology, semantic and syntactic relationships that we are learning. Indeed, our
vernacular speech forms the very basis of all future linguistic interaction and development.
Across our lifetime we will participate in, construct, engage in, and possibly abandon many
speech communities. No other will be as primary.
When we enter school we bring more than the pronunciation patterns, lexicon, syntactic
structures, semantic and interpretive frameworks of the language variation or dialect we speak.
We have begun to learn to whom we should say what and when. Furthermore, we have learned
rules of conversation and linguistic interaction. We have learned to identify whose turn it is to
speak, how to get the floor ourselves, and when a person’s turn is over. All of these linguistic
skills support us in our first steps toward the development of literacy. When the patterns of the
speech communities we join at school are not that much different from the discourse patterns
of the speech community (or communities) we participate in with our parents, literary
development is more natural and easier.
When the linguistic heritage we bring to school contrasts sharply with the norms of the
speech community of the school, it creates difficulties not just for speaking but for
participating. If our linguistic heritage is viewed as problematic, divergent, or substandard, we
may think of ourselves as problems. We may feel shame for who we are and the community
we come from. If how we speak, gain access to participation, interpret behavior, or respond
politely is misunderstood by the school as laziness, recalcitrance, disrespectfulness, or
stupidity, our entire educational future and our ability to achieve our intellectual potential may
be called into question.
Linguistic heritage that is suspect usually comes from those who either speak a different
language or use dialects judged to be non-standard. John Ogbu points out that just because
people speak a different language or dialect does not mean they will not do well in learning a
new language and in achieving success in a new culture. But in the United States as well as
other countries in the world, some groups do better in this process than others. Some point to
cultural patterns to account for differences in successful participation.
This term refers to the linguistic resources which learners need in order successfully to cope
with the forms of communication in which they are going to be involved in the short or medium
term. These needs (and hence these communication situations) are identified as part of a
specific process which consists of gathering together the information required to assess what
uses will actually be made of the language learnt and thereby determine what types of content
should be taught on a priority basis.
This process necessarily is the starting point for the development of language
programmes intended for learners like adults who are not covered by school education. It is
particularly relevant for adult migrants who have to cope in a pressing manner, from the
moment of their arrival and on a daily basis, with exchanges in a language of which they have
limited or no knowledge. It must lead to the development of tailor-made courses, which are the
only means of meeting the expectations of the relevant groups. However, it should not be
reduced to a technique for specialists, as the needs cannot be defined without input from those
concerned or indeed on their behalf.
Still more recently, since about 1950, there has been an increasing movement toward
the laboratory study of the ways in which patterns of development themselves change as age
changes. This recent work has been not so much concerned with the effects of age itself as with
the development in children of certain functional relationships between experience and
performance that have been demonstrated in human adults and have been found lacking in most
infrahuman species. The emphasis is on the application of laboratory controls and experimental
manipulations to the study of cognitive development. The aim is to control the stimulus
conditions under which behavior is observed and to explain why intellect develops, as well as
describing how and when it develops.
Such an approach does not obviate the need for study of the child’s understanding as it
changes with age. Rather, it relies on developmental descriptions of intellectual processes and
products for clues as to when a certain level of understanding or specific intellectual
accomplishment is likely to be achieved, and what repertoire of cognitive processes constitutes
the means available for such an accomplishment at that age. Even the correlation of processes
with products over ages, however, leaves the detailed cause-effect analysis still to be
performed.
2. Understanding the language of your community makes users feel like they’re on the
inside.
Everyone likes to feel important or in the know. Once again, knowledge of a community
language helps people feel like they are an integral part of the group, fostering a stronger sense
of member or customer loyalty.
3. The words that we use to refer to people change how we treat those people.
Using the right words will help to reinforce the community objective, while the wrong words
will obscure the purpose of the community.
References:
https://www.fraserhealth.ca/health-topics-a-to-z/children-and-youth/need of
community#.Ys2ha3XMIps
https://www.encyclopedia.com/social-sciences/applied-and-social-sciences-
magazines/community-development
https://childdevelopmentinfo.com/child-development/Role of development and
community-in-children-and-teens/#gs.5kq8g1
Question no.4
Discuss general characteristics of learning.
Types of Learning
Motor Learning:
Our day to day activities like walking, running, driving, etc, must be learnt for ensuring
a good life. These activities to a great extent involve muscular coordination.
Verbal Learning:
It is related with the language which we use to communicate and various other forms
of verbal communication such as symbols, words, languages, sounds, figures and signs.
Concept Learning:
This form of learning is associated with higher order cognitive processes like
intelligence, thinking, reasoning, etc, which we learn right from our childhood. Concept
learning involves the processes of abstraction and generalization, which is very useful for
identifying or recognizing things.
Discrimination Learning:
Learning which distinguishes between various stimuli with its appropriate and different
responses is regarded as discrimination stimuli.
IndepePreschoolers with healthy independence will follow predictable daily routines and activities at
school and at home, start identifying a favorite friend and ask that friend to play, independently play
with toys and materials at home, school, or an outdoor playground, and complete many self-care tasks,
such as getting dressed, going to the bathroom, eating snacks, feeding themselves, or getting ready for
bed. Independent preschoolers will also tell caregivers about their day and learn and use new vocabulary
daily.
Moral development focuses on the emergence, change, and understanding of morality from
infancy through adulthood. Morality develops across a lifetime and is influenced by an individual's
experiences and their behavior when faced with moral issues through different periods' physical and
cognitive development. In short, morality concerns an individual's growing sense of what is right and
wrong; it is for this reason that young children have different moral judgments and character than that
of a grown adult. Morality in itself is often a synonym for "rightness" or "goodness". It refers to a certain
code of conduct that is derived from one's culture, religion or personal philosophy that guides one's
actions, behaviors and thoughts. This term is related to psychology. There are other types of
development such as social development, physical development and cognitive development.
Notions of morality development have been developed over centuries, the earliest came from
philosophers like Confucius, Aristotle, and Rousseau, who all took a more humanist perspective and
focused on the development of the conscience and sense of virtue. In the modern day, empirical research
has explored morality through a moral psychology lens by theorists like Sigmund Freud and its relation
to cognitive development by theorists like Jean Piaget, Lawrence Kohlberg, B. F. Skinner, Carol
Gilligan and Judith Smetana.
The interest in morality spans many disciplines (e.g., philosophy, economics, biology,
and political science) and specializations within psychology (e.g., social, cognitive, and cultural). In
order to investigate how individuals understand morality, it is essential to consider their beliefs,
emotions, attitudes, and behaviors that contribute to their moral understanding. Additionally,
researchers in the field of moral development consider the role of peers and parents in facilitating moral
development, the role of conscience and values, socialization and cultural influences, empathy and
altruism, and positive development, in order to understand what factors impact morality of an individual
more completely.
Learning of Principles: Learning which is based on principles helps in managing the work
most effectively. Principles based learning explains the relationship between various concepts.
Attitude Learning: Attitude shapes our behaviour to a very great extent, as our positive or
negative behaviour is based on our attitudinal predisposition.
Communication and social skills are good to teach at an early age as most of these skills are
needed throughout life. You can do this in a fun way.
For example, teaching new simple words like mat, cat, rat, cat, bat, fat for very young
children and enacting each one of them as you say them. You can even show some pictures
related to those words to make it more interesting.
For social skills, teach them to smile, maintain eye contact, and say a greeting (it hardly takes
around three seconds to do these three things for anyone).
When children are thrown a challenge, they most certainly try to complete it. Tell them that
they can never do this particular work or plead them not to do a particular work. (Play way/
Using negative psychology).In most cases, students would end up doing exactly that and as a
result, the primary task gets completed. This could also have an adverse effect, at times.
However, teachers must strategize this in their own way
References:
https://www.fraserhealth.ca/health-topics-a-to-z/children-and-youth/Characteristies of
learning#.Ys2ha3XMIps
https://www.encyclopedia.com/social-sciences/applied-and-social-sciences-
magazines/Role
https://childdevelopmentinfo.com/child-development/role of learning -in-children-and-
teens/#gs.5kq8g1
Question no.5
How is individual difference measured?
Individual Difference
All animals learn, remember and integrate information in order to reach decisions and
behave appropriately, but how, why and when these cognitive abilities evolve remains
uncertain. One reason for this uncertainty is that research in animal cognition has frequently
ignored individual differences and this precludes our understanding of how natural selection
sifts individual differences leading to evolutionary changes. Instead, the study of cognition in
animals has traditionally taken one of three (non-exclusive) forms. First, particular model
species (e.g. pigeons or rats) have been used to investigate the mechanisms underpinning
specific cognitive processes.
This has typically used laboratory paradigms involving prolonged training of batches
of individuals to complete tasks aimed to elucidate fundamental learning principles. Second,
the comparative approach tests species or populations with the same, or purportedly similar,
tasks to understand when in evolutionary history particular cognitive processes may have
emerged and what ecological or social conditions may facilitate these processes. The abilities
of a sample of individuals within a population or species are pooled and considered
representative of the whole grouping. Finally, within a species, the abilities of particular, often
highly enculturated, ‘genius’ individuals are explored in great detail in order to establish the
presence of, or limits to, particular cognitive capacities. From these instances of presence or
absence, broader patterns of evolution may be suggested and inferences drawn about the
adaptive benefits of possessing such cognitive abilities for the species, based on its ecology
and social behaviour. While progress has been made in understanding both fine-scale cognitive
mechanisms and broad-scale evolutionary patterns using these methods, it is hard to understand
the evolution of cognitive abilities through natural selection when we ignore inter- and intra-
individual variation in cognitive abilities.
While individual differences have been central to human psychology since the early
20th century research on non-human animals has, until recently, tended to ignore the variation
amongst individuals. Over the last decade, there has been a growing focus on intraspecific
variation in non-human animals. This is perhaps influenced by (i) the behavioural ecology
approach originating in the 1980s, which explicitly considered natural selection on individual
phenotypes; (ii) the more recent studies of animal personality, which emphasize individual
differences across correlated suites of behaviours (iii) the development of statistical (mixed)
modelling techniques that permit explicit consideration of individual differences; and (iv) the
development of technology permitting fine-scale tracking of individuals’ movements and
interactions. This special issue draws together recent theoretical and empirical developments
in the emerging field of individual variation in cognition.
As cognitive abilities cannot be directly observed, they must be inferred through careful
experimentation. Measuring individual cognitive variation poses particular logistical and
analytical challenges because it requires repeated testing of known individuals under
standardized conditions, in a way that allows for noise caused by differences in, for example,
motivation, attention and prior experience to be identified, quantified and/or removed
(experimentally or statistically). Papers in the first section of this special issue explore these
methodological aspects further.
By measuring the cognitive abilities of individuals, we can address otherwise
intractable questions regarding the mechanisms, development and evolution of cognition. For
instance, how is the ontogeny of cognitive phenotypes shaped by the physical or social
environment? What is the relationship between personality and cognitive performance? Does
an individual’s ability to solve cognitive problems influence its ability to survive and
reproduce? This special issue examines both the causes and consequences of individual
variation in cognitive abilities.
One means of addressing (although not eliminating) confounds is to assess individual
consistency through repeated testing. If we measure an individual once, and it makes more
errors in remembering a rewarded location than a conspecific, this could be because it has
‘worse’ spatial memory or because it happened to be distracted. However, if we measure
individuals repeatedly and we find that some individuals perform consistently better than
others, this may be indicative of a stable phenotypic trait that selection can act upon. It is,
therefore, vital that individuals are tested repeatedly to determine genuine among-individual
phenotypic variation. However, certain confounds may also be consistent over time and so
these would also consistently confound performance. This is an issue that has received a great
deal of attention in the literature on animal personality, yet is only just beginning to be
recognized in studies of animal cognition.
Measures of repeatability provide a way of assessing individual differences by
quantifying among- and within-individual variation. These modest estimates may be due in
part to carry-over effects of learning and memory. For example, if all individuals learn over
repeated attempts at the same task, then this can reduce among-individual variance and thus
repeatability. Another approach is to present individuals with tasks that differ in their physical
characteristics but are designed to measure the same cognitive trait by having the same causal
contingencies. Most studies have conducted only single repeats of tests; it remains to be
determined how adding further repeat variants of a test increases the reliability of the measure
of cognitive ability.
“Individual differences are found in all psychological characteristics physical mental
abilities, knowledge, habit, personality and character traits.”
“The psychology of individual differences is largely the study of group differences. This study
classifies individuals by age, traits, sex, race, social class and so on, and observes the
differences within and between those groups. Physical, mental, social and cultural differences
etc. are being studied, under individual differences.” – John P.De Ceeceo
Perhaps the first task of every teacher in a class should be to know and study individual
differences among his pupils. Individual differences in bodily appearance and physique, habits
and skills, interests and temperaments, abilities and attainments have already been recognised.
a) Reducing noise
(b) Repeatability
Developing different tasks that measure the same cognitive ability is more difficult than it
appears. For instance, in this issue Völter et al. explicitly examine individual performance in
two tasks that are widely assumed to measure inhibitory control (or self-control, i.e. the ability
to inhibit pre-potent behavioural responses): the detour-reaching task and the A-not-B task. A
recent high-profile study found that across species, average performance on the two tasks was
positively correlated and showed a strong, positive correlation with average brain size. This
was interpreted as suggesting that increases in brain size across evolutionary time underlie the
evolution of increased self-control. To determine whether the tasks genuinely measured the
same cognitive trait, Völter et al. re-analysed these and other datasets at the individual level,
consistently finding no correlation between individual performance on tasks measuring
inhibitory control. This suggests that (i) correlations across species do not necessarily imply
that the same pattern holds within species and (ii) that, contrary to common assumption, these
two tasks do not necessarily measure the same ability . Völter et al. advocate triangulating
across batteries of tasks by measuring individuals' patterns of mistakes as a marker of the limits
of their abilities in a particular cognitive domain . It remains unclear whether the lack of
correlation between performances in tasks deemed a priori to test the same cognitive process
is due to differential demands on cognitive processes or differential effects on attention, or non-
cognitive factors such as motivation.
A related issue is whether individuals are also consistent in their performance across different
cognitive domains. As Dubois et al. highlight in this issue, human psychometric studies
consistently show strong positive correlations in individuals' performance on a disparate range
of tasks (e.g. verbal comprehension, reasoning, working memory). These results suggest that
common information processing mechanisms may underlie performance across different
cognitive domains, often referred to as general intelligence or ‘g’ . In contrast, the animal
cognition literature has tended to favour a modular approach, emphasizing specific cognitive
adaptations to specific ecological problems (e.g. spatial memory as an adaptation to the
challenges of food cache recovery . However, in recent years, researchers have also begun to
turn their attention to the potential for cross-domain individual consistency in non-human
animals . Although this consistency in non-human animals is also sometimes referred to as ‘g’,
it is important to note that animal psychometric tests often incorporate very different types of
tasks, methodologies and statistical approaches to human psychometrics. Indeed, it has been
suggested that in humans, test batteries primarily test reasoning or rule extraction while those
used with animals primarily focus on associative learning and memory . Thus, while there are
superficial similarities between human general intelligence and animal ‘general cognitive
performance’ , these may not necessarily reflect the same underlying cognitive architecture.
Interpretation is made harder because the likelihood of extracting a single component indicative
of a general ability is highly susceptible to the exact set of tasks that are included in the test
battery .
Conclusion
References:
https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/10.1098/rstb.2017.0280
https://www.fraserhealth.ca/health-topics-a-to-z/children-and-youth/individual-
development measured-in-children#.Ys2ha3XMIps
https://www.encyclopedia.com/social-sciences/applied-and-social-sciences-
magazines/individual