Brain
Brain
This article is about the brains of all types of animals, including humans. For information specific to the human brain,
see Human brain. For other uses, see Brain (disambiguation).
The operations of individual brain cells are now understood in considerable detail but the way
they cooperate in ensembles of millions is yet to be solved.[2] Recent models in modern
neuroscience treat the brain as a biological computer, very different in mechanism from an
electronic computer, but similar in the sense that it acquires information from the surrounding
world, stores it, and processes it in a variety of ways.
This article compares the properties of brains across the entire range of animal species, with
the greatest attention to vertebrates. It deals with the human brain insofar as it shares the
properties of other brains. The ways in which the human brain differs from other brains are
covered in the human brain article. Several topics that might be covered here are instead
covered there because much more can be said about them in a human context. The most
important is brain disease and the effects of brain damage, that are covered in the human brain
article.
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 2 of 37
Contents
Anatomy
Cellular structure
Evolution
Generic bilaterian nervous system
Invertebrates
Vertebrates
Mammals
Primates
Development
Physiology
Neurotransmitters and receptors
Electrical activity
Metabolism
Functions
Perception
Motor control
Arousal
Homeostasis
Motivation
Research
History
See also
References
Further reading
External links
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 3 of 37
Anatomy
Cellular structure
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 4 of 37
centimeters in diameter, extending more than a kilometer.[8] These axons transmit signals in the
form of electrochemical pulses called action potentials, which last less than a thousandth of a
second and travel along the axon at speeds of 1–100 meters per second. Some neurons emit
action potentials constantly, at rates of 10–100 per second, usually in irregular patterns; other
neurons are quiet most of the time, but occasionally emit a burst of action potentials.[9]
Axons transmit signals to other neurons by means of specialized junctions called synapses. A
single axon may make as many as several thousand synaptic connections with other cells.[7]
When an action potential, traveling along an axon, arrives at a synapse, it causes a chemical
called a neurotransmitter to be released. The neurotransmitter binds to receptor molecules in
the membrane of the target cell.[7]
Most of the space in the brain is taken up by axons, which are often bundled together in what
are called nerve fiber tracts. A myelinated axon is wrapped in a fatty insulating sheath of myelin,
which serves to greatly increase the speed of signal propagation. (There are also unmyelinated
axons). Myelin is white, making parts of the brain filled exclusively with nerve fibers appear as
light-colored white matter, in contrast to the darker-colored grey matter that marks areas with
high densities of neuron cell bodies.[7]
Evolution
Main article: Evolution of the brain
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 5 of 37
cnidarians (which have a nervous system
consisting of a diffuse nerve net[13]), all living
multicellular animals are bilaterians, meaning
animals with a bilaterally symmetric body shape
Nervous system of a generic bilaterian animal, in the
(that is, left and right sides that are approximate form of a nerve cord with segmental enlargements,
[14] and a "brain" at the front
mirror images of each other). All bilaterians are
thought to have descended from a common
ancestor that appeared early in the Cambrian period, 485-540 million years ago, and it has
been hypothesized that this common ancestor had the shape of a simple tubeworm with a
segmented body.[14] At a schematic level, that basic worm-shape continues to be reflected in the
body and nervous system architecture of all modern bilaterians, including vertebrates.[15] The
fundamental bilateral body form is a tube with a hollow gut cavity running from the mouth to the
anus, and a nerve cord with an enlargement (a ganglion) for each body segment, with an
especially large ganglion at the front, called the brain. The brain is small and simple in some
species, such as nematode worms; in other species, including vertebrates, it is the most
complex organ in the body.[3] Some types of worms, such as leeches, also have an enlarged
ganglion at the back end of the nerve cord, known as a "tail brain".[16]
There are a few types of existing bilaterians that lack a recognizable brain, including
echinoderms, tunicates, and acoelomorphs (a group of primitive flatworms). It has not been
definitively established whether the existence of these brainless species indicates that the
earliest bilaterians lacked a brain, or whether their ancestors evolved in a way that led to the
disappearance of a previously existing brain structure.[17]
Invertebrates
There are several invertebrate species whose brains have been studied intensively because
they have properties that make them convenient for experimental work:
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 6 of 37
◾ Fruit flies (Drosophila), because of the large array of techniques available for studying their
genetics, have been a natural subject for studying the role of genes in brain development.[21] In
spite of the large evolutionary distance between insects and mammals, many aspects of
Drosophila neurogenetics have been shown to be relevant to humans. The first biological clock
genes, for example, were identified by examining Drosophila mutants that showed disrupted
daily activity cycles.[22] A search in the genomes of vertebrates revealed a set of analogous
genes, which were found to play similar roles in the mouse biological clock—and therefore
almost certainly in the human biological clock as well.[23] Studies done on Drosophila, also show
that most neuropil regions of the brain are continuously reorganized throughout life in response
to specific living conditions.[24]
◾ The nematode worm Caenorhabditis elegans, like Drosophila, has been studied largely
because of its importance in genetics.[25] In the early 1970s, Sydney Brenner chose it as a model
organism for studying the way that genes control development. One of the advantages of
working with this worm is that the body plan is very stereotyped: the nervous system of the
hermaphrodite contains exactly 302 neurons, always in the same places, making identical
synaptic connections in every worm.[26] Brenner's team sliced worms into thousands of ultrathin
sections and photographed each one under an electron microscope, then visually matched
fibers from section to section, to map out every neuron and synapse in the entire body.[27] The
complete neuronal wiring diagram of C.elegans – its connectome was achieved.[28] Nothing
approaching this level of detail is available for any other organism, and the information gained
has enabled a multitude of studies that would otherwise have not been possible.[29]
◾ The sea slug Aplysia californica was chosen by Nobel Prize-winning neurophysiologist Eric
Kandel as a model for studying the cellular basis of learning and memory, because of the
simplicity and accessibility of its nervous system, and it has been examined in hundreds of
experiments.[30]
Vertebrates
The first vertebrates appeared over 500 million years ago (Mya),
during the Cambrian period, and may have resembled the modern
hagfish in form.[31] Sharks appeared about 450 Mya, amphibians
about 400 Mya, reptiles about 350 Mya, and mammals about
200 Mya. Each species has an equally long evolutionary history, but
the brains of modern hagfishes, lampreys, sharks, amphibians,
reptiles, and mammals show a gradient of size and complexity that
roughly follows the evolutionary sequence. All of these brains The brain of a shark
contain the same set of basic anatomical components, but many are
rudimentary in the hagfish, whereas in mammals the foremost part (the telencephalon) is greatly
elaborated and expanded.[32]
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 7 of 37
Brains are most simply compared in terms of their size. The relationship between brain size,
body size and other variables has been studied across a wide range of vertebrate species. As a
rule, brain size increases with body size, but not in a simple linear proportion. In general,
smaller animals tend to have larger brains, measured as a fraction of body size. For mammals,
the relationship between brain volume and body mass essentially follows a power law with an
exponent of about 0.75.[33] This formula describes the central tendency, but every family of
mammals departs from it to some degree, in a way that reflects in part the complexity of their
behavior. For example, primates have brains 5 to 10 times larger than the formula predicts.
Predators tend to have larger brains than their prey, relative to body size.[34]
The brains of vertebrates are made of very soft tissue.[7] Living brain tissue is pinkish on the
outside and mostly white on the inside, with subtle variations in color. Vertebrate brains are
surrounded by a system of connective tissue membranes called meninges that separate the
skull from the brain. Blood vessels enter the central nervous system through holes in the
meningeal layers. The cells in the blood vessel walls are joined tightly to one another, forming
the blood–brain barrier, which blocks the passage of many toxins and pathogens[35] (though at
the same time blocking antibodies and some drugs, thereby presenting special challenges in
treatment of diseases of the brain).[36]
Neuroanatomists usually divide the vertebrate brain into six main regions: the telencephalon
(cerebral hemispheres), diencephalon (thalamus and hypothalamus), mesencephalon
(midbrain), cerebellum, pons, and medulla oblongata. Each of these areas has a complex
internal structure. Some parts, such as the cerebral cortex and the cerebellar cortex, consist of
layers that are folded or convoluted to fit within the available space. Other parts, such as the
thalamus and hypothalamus, consist of clusters of many small nuclei. Thousands of
distinguishable areas can be identified within the vertebrate brain based on fine distinctions of
neural structure, chemistry, and connectivity.[7]
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 8 of 37
Although the same basic components are present in all vertebrate brains, some branches of
vertebrate evolution have led to substantial distortions of brain geometry, especially in the
forebrain area. The brain of a shark shows the basic components in a straightforward way, but
in teleost fishes (the great majority of existing fish species), the forebrain has become "everted",
like a sock turned inside out. In birds, there are also major changes in forebrain structure.[37]
These distortions can make it difficult to match brain components from one species with those
of another species.[38]
◾ The thalamus is a collection of nuclei with diverse functions: some are involved in relaying
information to and from the cerebral hemispheres, while others are involved in motivation. The
subthalamic area (zona incerta) seems to contain action-generating systems for several types
of "consummatory" behaviors such as eating, drinking, defecation, and copulation.[41]
◾ The cerebellum modulates the outputs of other brain systems, whether motor related or
thought related, to make them certain and precise. Removal of the cerebellum does not prevent
an animal from doing anything in particular, but it makes actions hesitant and clumsy. This
precision is not built-in, but learned by trial and error. The muscle coordination learned while
riding a bicycle is an example of a type of neural plasticity that may take place largely within the
cerebellum.[7] 10% of the brain's total volume consists of the cerebellum and 50% of all neurons
are held within its structure.[42]
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 9 of 37
◾ The optic tectum allows actions to be directed toward points in space, most commonly in
response to visual input. In mammals it is usually referred to as the superior colliculus, and its
best-studied function is to direct eye movements. It also directs reaching movements and other
object-directed actions. It receives strong visual inputs, but also inputs from other senses that
are useful in directing actions, such as auditory input in owls and input from the thermosensitive
pit organs in snakes. In some primitive fishes, such as lampreys, this region is the largest part of
the brain.[43] The superior colliculus is part of the midbrain.
◾ The pallium is a layer of gray matter that lies on the surface of the forebrain and is the most
complex and most recent evolutionary development of the brain as an organ.[44] In reptiles and
mammals, it is called the cerebral cortex. Multiple functions involve the pallium, including smell
and spatial memory. In mammals, where it becomes so large as to dominate the brain, it takes
over functions from many other brain areas. In many mammals, the cerebral cortex consists of
folded bulges called gyri that create deep furrows or fissures called sulci. The folds increase the
surface area of the cortex and therefore increase the amount of gray matter and the amount of
information that can be stored and processed.[45]
◾ The hippocampus, strictly speaking, is found only in mammals. However, the area it
derives from, the medial pallium, has counterparts in all vertebrates. There is evidence that this
part of the brain is involved in complex events such as spatial memory and navigation in fishes,
birds, reptiles, and mammals.[46]
◾ The basal ganglia are a group of interconnected structures in the forebrain. The primary
function of the basal ganglia appears to be action selection: they send inhibitory signals to all
parts of the brain that can generate motor behaviors, and in the right circumstances can release
the inhibition, so that the action-generating systems are able to execute their actions. Reward
and punishment exert their most important neural effects by altering connections within the
basal ganglia.[47]
◾ The olfactory bulb is a special structure that processes olfactory sensory signals and
sends its output to the olfactory part of the pallium. It is a major brain component in many
vertebrates, but is greatly reduced in humans and other primates (whose senses are dominated
by information acquired by sight rather than smell).[48]
Mammals
The most obvious difference between the brains of mammals and other vertebrates is in terms
of size. On average, a mammal has a brain roughly twice as large as that of a bird of the same
body size, and ten times as large as that of a reptile of the same body size.[49]
Size, however, is not the only difference: there are also substantial differences in shape. The
hindbrain and midbrain of mammals are generally similar to those of other vertebrates, but
dramatic differences appear in the forebrain, which is greatly enlarged and also altered in
structure.[50] The cerebral cortex is the part of the brain that most strongly distinguishes
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 10 of 37
mammals. In non-mammalian vertebrates, the surface of the cerebrum is lined with a
comparatively simple three-layered structure called the pallium. In mammals, the pallium
evolves into a complex six-layered structure called neocortex or isocortex.[51] Several areas at
the edge of the neocortex, including the hippocampus and amygdala, are also much more
extensively developed in mammals than in other vertebrates.[50]
The elaboration of the cerebral cortex carries with it changes to other brain areas. The superior
colliculus, which plays a major role in visual control of behavior in most vertebrates, shrinks to a
small size in mammals, and many of its functions are taken over by visual areas of the cerebral
cortex.[49] The cerebellum of mammals contains a large portion (the neocerebellum) dedicated to
supporting the cerebral cortex, which has no counterpart in other vertebrates.[52]
Primates
Encephalization Quotient
Species EQ[53]
Human 7.4–7.8
Chimpanzee 2.2–2.5
Elephant 1.13–2.36[55]
Dog 1.2
Horse 0.9
Rat 0.4
The brains of humans and other primates contain the same structures as the brains of other
mammals, but are generally larger in proportion to body size.[56] The encephalization quotient
(EQ) is used to compare brain sizes across species. It takes into account the nonlinearity of the
brain-to-body relationship.[53] Humans have an average EQ in the 7-to-8 range, while most other
primates have an EQ in the 2-to-3 range. Dolphins have values higher than those of primates
other than humans,[54] but nearly all other mammals have EQ values that are substantially lower.
Most of the enlargement of the primate brain comes from a massive expansion of the cerebral
cortex, especially the prefrontal cortex and the parts of the cortex involved in vision.[57] The
visual processing network of primates includes at least 30 distinguishable brain areas, with a
complex web of interconnections. It has been estimated that visual processing areas occupy
more than half of the total surface of the primate neocortex.[58] The prefrontal cortex carries out
functions that include planning, working memory, motivation, attention, and executive control. It
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 11 of 37
takes up a much larger proportion of the brain for primates than for other species, and an
especially large fraction of the human brain.[59]
Development
Main article: Neural development
For vertebrates, the early stages of neural development are similar across all species.[60] As the
embryo transforms from a round blob of cells into a wormlike structure, a narrow strip of
ectoderm running along the midline of the back is induced to become the neural plate, the
precursor of the nervous system. The neural plate folds inward to form the neural groove, and
then the lips that line the groove merge to enclose the neural tube, a hollow cord of cells with a
fluid-filled ventricle at the center. At the front end, the ventricles and cord swell to form three
vesicles that are the precursors of the forebrain, midbrain, and hindbrain. At the next stage, the
forebrain splits into two vesicles called the telencephalon (which will contain the cerebral cortex,
basal ganglia, and related structures) and the diencephalon (which will contain the thalamus
and hypothalamus). At about the same time, the hindbrain splits into the metencephalon (which
will contain the cerebellum and pons) and the myelencephalon (which will contain the medulla
oblongata). Each of these areas contains proliferative zones where neurons and glial cells are
generated; the resulting cells then migrate, sometimes for long distances, to their final positions.
[60]
Once a neuron is in place, it extends dendrites and an axon into the area around it. Axons,
because they commonly extend a great distance from the cell body and need to reach specific
targets, grow in a particularly complex way. The tip of a growing axon consists of a blob of
protoplasm called a growth cone, studded with chemical receptors. These receptors sense the
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 12 of 37
local environment, causing the growth cone to be attracted or repelled by various cellular
elements, and thus to be pulled in a particular direction at each point along its path. The result
of this pathfinding process is that the growth cone navigates through the brain until it reaches its
destination area, where other chemical cues cause it to begin generating synapses.
Considering the entire brain, thousands of genes create products that influence axonal
pathfinding.[60]
The synaptic network that finally emerges is only partly determined by genes, though. In many
parts of the brain, axons initially "overgrow", and then are "pruned" by mechanisms that depend
on neural activity.[60] In the projection from the eye to the midbrain, for example, the structure in
the adult contains a very precise mapping, connecting each point on the surface of the retina to
a corresponding point in a midbrain layer. In the first stages of development, each axon from the
retina is guided to the right general vicinity in the midbrain by chemical cues, but then branches
very profusely and makes initial contact with a wide swath of midbrain neurons. The retina,
before birth, contains special mechanisms that cause it to generate waves of activity that
originate spontaneously at a random point and then propagate slowly across the retinal layer.
These waves are useful because they cause neighboring neurons to be active at the same time;
that is, they produce a neural activity pattern that contains information about the spatial
arrangement of the neurons. This information is exploited in the midbrain by a mechanism that
causes synapses to weaken, and eventually vanish, if activity in an axon is not followed by
activity of the target cell. The result of this sophisticated process is a gradual tuning and
tightening of the map, leaving it finally in its precise adult form.[61]
Similar things happen in other brain areas: an initial synaptic matrix is generated as a result of
genetically determined chemical guidance, but then gradually refined by activity-dependent
mechanisms, partly driven by internal dynamics, partly by external sensory inputs. In some
cases, as with the retina-midbrain system, activity patterns depend on mechanisms that operate
only in the developing brain, and apparently exist solely to guide development.[61]
In humans and many other mammals, new neurons are created mainly before birth, and the
infant brain contains substantially more neurons than the adult brain.[60] There are, however, a
few areas where new neurons continue to be generated throughout life. The two areas for which
adult neurogenesis is well established are the olfactory bulb, which is involved in the sense of
smell, and the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus, where there is evidence that the new neurons
play a role in storing newly acquired memories. With these exceptions, however, the set of
neurons that is present in early childhood is the set that is present for life. Glial cells are
different: as with most types of cells in the body, they are generated throughout the lifespan.[62]
There has long been debate about whether the qualities of mind, personality, and intelligence
can be attributed to heredity or to upbringing—this is the nature and nurture controversy.[63]
Although many details remain to be settled, neuroscience research has clearly shown that both
factors are important. Genes determine the general form of the brain, and genes determine how
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 13 of 37
the brain reacts to experience. Experience, however, is required to refine the matrix of synaptic
connections, which in its developed form contains far more information than the genome does.
In some respects, all that matters is the presence or absence of experience during critical
periods of development.[64] In other respects, the quantity and quality of experience are
important; for example, there is substantial evidence that animals raised in enriched
environments have thicker cerebral cortices, indicating a higher density of synaptic connections,
than animals whose levels of stimulation are restricted.[65]
Physiology
The functions of the brain depend on the ability of neurons to transmit electrochemical signals to
other cells, and their ability to respond appropriately to electrochemical signals received from
other cells. The electrical properties of neurons are controlled by a wide variety of biochemical
and metabolic processes, most notably the interactions between neurotransmitters and
receptors that take place at synapses.[7]
Neurotransmitters are chemicals that are released at synapses when an action potential
activates them—neurotransmitters attach themselves to receptor molecules on the membrane
of the synapse's target cell, and thereby alter the electrical or chemical properties of the
receptor molecules. With few exceptions, each neuron in the brain releases the same chemical
neurotransmitter, or combination of neurotransmitters, at all the synaptic connections it makes
with other neurons; this rule is known as Dale's principle.[7] Thus, a neuron can be characterized
by the neurotransmitters that it releases. The great majority of psychoactive drugs exert their
effects by altering specific neurotransmitter systems. This applies to drugs such as
cannabinoids, nicotine, heroin, cocaine, alcohol, fluoxetine, chlorpromazine, and many others.[66]
The two neurotransmitters that are used most widely in the vertebrate brain are glutamate,
which almost always exerts excitatory effects on target neurons, and gamma-aminobutyric acid
(GABA), which is almost always inhibitory. Neurons using these transmitters can be found in
nearly every part of the brain.[67] Because of their ubiquity, drugs that act on glutamate or GABA
tend to have broad and powerful effects. Some general anesthetics act by reducing the effects
of glutamate; most tranquilizers exert their sedative effects by enhancing the effects of GABA.[68]
There are dozens of other chemical neurotransmitters that are used in more limited areas of the
brain, often areas dedicated to a particular function. Serotonin, for example—the primary target
of antidepressant drugs and many dietary aids—comes exclusively from a small brainstem area
called the Raphe nuclei.[69] Norepinephrine, which is involved in arousal, comes exclusively from
a nearby small area called the locus coeruleus.[70] Other neurotransmitters such as acetylcholine
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 14 of 37
and dopamine have multiple sources in the brain, but are not as ubiquitously distributed as
glutamate and GABA.[71]
Electrical activity
Metabolism
All vertebrates have a blood–brain barrier that allows metabolism inside the brain to operate
differently from metabolism in other parts of the body. Glial cells play a major role in brain
metabolism by controlling the chemical composition of the fluid that surrounds neurons,
including levels of ions and nutrients.[74]
Brain tissue consumes a large amount of energy in proportion to its volume, so large brains
place severe metabolic demands on animals. The need to limit body weight in order, for
example, to fly, has apparently led to selection for a reduction of brain size in some species,
such as bats.[75] Most of the brain's energy consumption goes into sustaining the electric charge
(membrane potential) of neurons.[74] Most vertebrate species devote between 2% and 8% of
basal metabolism to the brain. In primates, however, the percentage is much higher—in
humans it rises to 20–25%.[76] The energy consumption of the brain does not vary greatly over
time, but active regions of the cerebral cortex consume somewhat more energy than inactive
regions; this forms the basis for the functional brain imaging methods PET, fMRI,[77] and NIRS.[78]
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 15 of 37
The brain typically gets most of its energy from oxygen-dependent metabolism of glucose (i.e.,
blood sugar),[74] but ketones provide a major alternative source, together with contributions from
medium chain fatty acids (caprylic[79] and heptanoic[80] acids), lactate,[81] acetate,[82] and possibly
amino acids.[83]
Functions
The function of the brain is to provide coherent control over the actions of an animal. A
centralized brain allows groups of muscles to be co-activated in complex patterns; it also allows
stimuli impinging on one part of the body to evoke responses in other parts, and it can prevent
different parts of the body from acting at cross-purposes to each other.[85]
Perception
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 16 of 37
sensory modality. This primary sensory nucleus sends information to higher-order sensory
areas that are dedicated to the same modality. Eventually, via a way-station in the thalamus, the
signals are sent to the cerebral cortex, where they are processed to extract the relevant
features, and integrated with signals coming from other sensory systems.[7]
Motor control
Motor systems are areas of the brain that are involved in initiating body movements, that is, in
activating muscles. Except for the muscles that control the eye, which are driven by nuclei in the
midbrain, all the voluntary muscles in the body are directly innervated by motor neurons in the
spinal cord and hindbrain.[7] Spinal motor neurons are controlled both by neural circuits intrinsic
to the spinal cord, and by inputs that descend from the brain. The intrinsic spinal circuits
implement many reflex responses, and contain pattern generators for rhythmic movements such
as walking or swimming. The descending connections from the brain allow for more
sophisticated control.[7]
The brain contains several motor areas that project directly to the spinal cord. At the lowest
level are motor areas in the medulla and pons, which control stereotyped movements such as
walking, breathing, or swallowing. At a higher level are areas in the midbrain, such as the red
nucleus, which is responsible for coordinating movements of the arms and legs. At a higher
level yet is the primary motor cortex, a strip of tissue located at the posterior edge of the frontal
lobe. The primary motor cortex sends projections to the subcortical motor areas, but also sends
a massive projection directly to the spinal cord, through the pyramidal tract. This direct
corticospinal projection allows for precise voluntary control of the fine details of movements.
Other motor-related brain areas exert secondary effects by projecting to the primary motor
areas. Among the most important secondary areas are the premotor cortex, basal ganglia, and
cerebellum.[7]
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 17 of 37
Major areas involved in controlling movement
Area Location Function
Motor cortex Frontal lobe Direct cortical activation of spinal motor circuits
Supplementary motor
Frontal lobe Sequences movements into temporal patterns[89]
area
In addition to all of the above, the brain and spinal cord contain extensive circuitry to control the
autonomic nervous system, which works by secreting hormones and by modulating the
"smooth" muscles of the gut.[7]
Arousal
See also: Sleep
Many animals alternate between sleeping and waking in a daily cycle. Arousal and alertness are
also modulated on a finer time scale by a network of brain areas.[7]
A key component of the arousal system is the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), a tiny part of the
hypothalamus located directly above the point at which the optic nerves from the two eyes
cross. The SCN contains the body's central biological clock. Neurons there show activity levels
that rise and fall with a period of about 24 hours, circadian rhythms: these activity fluctuations
are driven by rhythmic changes in expression of a set of "clock genes". The SCN continues to
keep time even if it is excised from the brain and placed in a dish of warm nutrient solution, but
it ordinarily receives input from the optic nerves, through the retinohypothalamic tract (RHT),
that allows daily light-dark cycles to calibrate the clock.[91]
The SCN projects to a set of areas in the hypothalamus, brainstem, and midbrain that are
involved in implementing sleep-wake cycles. An important component of the system is the
reticular formation, a group of neuron-clusters scattered diffusely through the core of the lower
brain. Reticular neurons send signals to the thalamus, which in turn sends activity-level-
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 18 of 37
controlling signals to every part of the cortex. Damage to the reticular formation can produce a
permanent state of coma.[7]
Sleep involves great changes in brain activity.[7] Until the 1950s it was generally believed that
the brain essentially shuts off during sleep,[92] but this is now known to be far from true; activity
continues, but patterns become very different. There are two types of sleep: REM sleep (with
dreaming) and NREM (non-REM, usually without dreaming) sleep, which repeat in slightly
varying patterns throughout a sleep episode. Three broad types of distinct brain activity patterns
can be measured: REM, light NREM and deep NREM. During deep NREM sleep, also called
slow wave sleep, activity in the cortex takes the form of large synchronized waves, whereas in
the waking state it is noisy and desynchronized. Levels of the neurotransmitters norepinephrine
and serotonin drop during slow wave sleep, and fall almost to zero during REM sleep; levels of
acetylcholine show the reverse pattern.[7]
Homeostasis
In vertebrates, the part of the brain that plays the greatest role is the hypothalamus, a small
region at the base of the forebrain whose size does not reflect its complexity or the importance
of its function.[93] The hypothalamus is a collection of small nuclei, most of which are involved in
basic biological functions. Some of these functions relate to arousal or to social interactions
such as sexuality, aggression, or maternal behaviors; but many of them relate to homeostasis.
Several hypothalamic nuclei receive input from sensors located in the lining of blood vessels,
conveying information about temperature, sodium level, glucose level, blood oxygen level, and
other parameters. These hypothalamic nuclei send output signals to motor areas that can
generate actions to rectify deficiencies. Some of the outputs also go to the pituitary gland, a tiny
gland attached to the brain directly underneath the hypothalamus. The pituitary gland secretes
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 19 of 37
hormones into the bloodstream, where they circulate throughout the body and induce changes
in cellular activity.[95]
Motivation
Most organisms studied to date utilize a reward–punishment mechanism: for instance, worms
and insects can alter their behavior to seek food sources or to avoid dangers.[98] In vertebrates,
the reward-punishment system is implemented by a specific set of brain structures, at the heart
of which lie the basal ganglia, a set of interconnected areas at the base of the forebrain.[47] The
basal ganglia are the central site at which decisions are made: the basal ganglia exert a
sustained inhibitory control over most of the motor systems in the brain; when this inhibition is
released, a motor system is permitted to execute the action it is programmed to carry out.
Rewards and punishments function by altering the relationship between the inputs that the
basal ganglia receive and the decision-signals that are emitted. The reward mechanism is better
understood than the punishment mechanism, because its role in drug abuse has caused it to be
studied very intensively. Research has shown that the neurotransmitter dopamine plays a
central role: addictive drugs such as cocaine, amphetamine, and nicotine either cause
dopamine levels to rise or cause the effects of dopamine inside the brain to be enhanced.[99]
Almost all animals are capable of modifying their behavior as a result of experience—even the
most primitive types of worms. Because behavior is driven by brain activity, changes in behavior
must somehow correspond to changes inside the brain. Already in the late 19th century
theorists like Santiago Ramón y Cajal argued that the most plausible explanation is that learning
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 20 of 37
and memory are expressed as changes in the synaptic connections between neurons.[100] Until
1970, however, experimental evidence to support the synaptic plasticity hypothesis was lacking.
In 1971 Tim Bliss and Terje Lømo published a paper on a phenomenon now called long-term
potentiation: the paper showed clear evidence of activity-induced synaptic changes that lasted
for at least several days.[101] Since then technical advances have made these sorts of
experiments much easier to carry out, and thousands of studies have been made that have
clarified the mechanism of synaptic change, and uncovered other types of activity-driven
synaptic change in a variety of brain areas, including the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, basal
ganglia, and cerebellum.[102] Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and physical activity
appear to play a beneficial role in the process.[103]
Neuroscientists currently distinguish several types of learning and memory that are
implemented by the brain in distinct ways:
◾ Episodic memory is the ability to remember the details of specific events. This sort of
memory can last for a lifetime. Much evidence implicates the hippocampus in playing a crucial
role: people with severe damage to the hippocampus sometimes show amnesia, that is, inability
to form new long-lasting episodic memories.[105]
◾ Semantic memory is the ability to learn facts and relationships. This sort of memory is
probably stored largely in the cerebral cortex, mediated by changes in connections between
cells that represent specific types of information.[106]
◾ Instrumental learning is the ability for rewards and punishments to modify behavior. It is
implemented by a network of brain areas centered on the basal ganglia.[107]
◾ Motor learning is the ability to refine patterns of body movement by practicing, or more
generally by repetition. A number of brain areas are involved, including the premotor cortex,
basal ganglia, and especially the cerebellum, which functions as a large memory bank for
microadjustments of the parameters of movement.[108]
Research
Main article: Neuroscience
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 21 of 37
diagnoses and treats diseases of the nervous system. The
brain is also the most important organ studied in psychiatry,
the branch of medicine that works to study, prevent, and treat
mental disorders.[109] Cognitive science seeks to unify
neuroscience and psychology with other fields that concern
themselves with the brain, such as computer science
(artificial intelligence and similar fields) and philosophy.[110]
The Human Brain Project is a large
The oldest method of studying the brain is anatomical, and scientific research project, starting in
2013, which aims to simulate the
until the middle of the 20th century, much of the progress in
complete human brain.
neuroscience came from the development of better cell stains
and better microscopes. Neuroanatomists study the large-scale structure of the brain as well as
the microscopic structure of neurons and their components, especially synapses. Among other
tools, they employ a plethora of stains that reveal neural structure, chemistry, and connectivity.
In recent years, the development of immunostaining techniques has allowed investigation of
neurons that express specific sets of genes. Also, functional neuroanatomy uses medical
imaging techniques to correlate variations in human brain structure with differences in cognition
or behavior.[111]
Neurophysiologists study the chemical, pharmacological, and electrical properties of the brain:
their primary tools are drugs and recording devices. Thousands of experimentally developed
drugs affect the nervous system, some in highly specific ways. Recordings of brain activity can
be made using electrodes, either glued to the scalp as in EEG studies, or implanted inside the
brains of animals for extracellular recordings, which can detect action potentials generated by
individual neurons.[112] Because the brain does not contain pain receptors, it is possible using
these techniques to record brain activity from animals that are awake and behaving without
causing distress. The same techniques have occasionally been used to study brain activity in
human patients suffering from intractable epilepsy, in cases where there was a medical
necessity to implant electrodes to localize the brain area responsible for epileptic seizures.[113]
Functional imaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging are also used to
study brain activity; these techniques have mainly been used with human subjects, because
they require a conscious subject to remain motionless for long periods of time, but they have the
great advantage of being noninvasive.[114]
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 22 of 37
Design of an experiment in which brain activity from types of damage. In humans, the effects of
a monkey was used to control a robotic arm[115]
strokes and other types of brain damage have
been a key source of information about brain function. Because there is no ability to
experimentally control the nature of the damage, however, this information is often difficult to
interpret. In animal studies, most commonly involving rats, it is possible to use electrodes or
locally injected chemicals to produce precise patterns of damage and then examine the
consequences for behavior.[116]
Computational neuroscience encompasses two approaches: first, the use of computers to study
the brain; second, the study of how brains perform computation. On one hand, it is possible to
write a computer program to simulate the operation of a group of neurons by making use of
systems of equations that describe their electrochemical activity; such simulations are known as
biologically realistic neural networks. On the other hand, it is possible to study algorithms for
neural computation by simulating, or mathematically analyzing, the operations of simplified
"units" that have some of the properties of neurons but abstract out much of their biological
complexity. The computational functions of the brain are studied both by computer scientists
and neuroscientists.[117]
Computational neurogenetic modeling is concerned with the study and development of dynamic
neuronal models for modeling brain functions with respect to genes and dynamic interactions
between genes.
Recent years have seen increasing applications of genetic and genomic techniques to the study
of the brain [118] and a focus on the roles of neurotrophic factors and physical activity in
neuroplasticity.[103] The most common subjects are mice, because of the availability of technical
tools. It is now possible with relative ease to "knock out" or mutate a wide variety of genes, and
then examine the effects on brain function. More sophisticated approaches are also being used:
for example, using Cre-Lox recombination it is possible to activate or deactivate genes in
specific parts of the brain, at specific times.[118]
History
See also: History of neuroscience
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 23 of 37
blood. Democritus, the inventor of the atomic theory of Illustration by René Descartes of how
the brain implements a reflex response
matter, argued for a three-part soul, with intellect in the head,
emotion in the heart, and lust near the liver.[120] Hippocrates, the "father of medicine", came
down unequivocally in favor of the brain. In his treatise on epilepsy he wrote:
“ Men ought to know that from nothing else but the brain come joys, delights,
laughter and sports, and sorrows, griefs, despondency, and lamentations. ...
And by the same organ we become mad and delirious, and fears and terrors
assail us, some by night, and some by day, and dreams and untimely
wanderings, and cares that are not suitable, and ignorance of present
circumstances, desuetude, and unskillfulness. All these things we endure
from the brain, when it is not healthy...
The first real progress toward a modern understanding of nervous function, though, came from
the investigations of Luigi Galvani, who discovered that a shock of static electricity applied to an
exposed nerve of a dead frog could cause its leg to contract. Since that time, each major
advance in understanding has followed more or less directly from the development of a new
technique of investigation. Until the early years of the 20th century, the most important
advances were derived from new methods for staining cells.[123] Particularly critical was the
invention of the Golgi stain, which (when correctly used) stains only a small fraction of neurons,
but stains them in their entirety, including cell body, dendrites, and axon. Without such a stain,
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 24 of 37
brain tissue under a microscope appears as an impenetrable tangle of protoplasmic fibers, in
which it is impossible to determine any structure. In the hands of Camillo Golgi, and especially
of the Spanish neuroanatomist Santiago Ramón y Cajal, the new stain revealed hundreds of
distinct types of neurons, each with its own unique dendritic structure and pattern of
connectivity.[124]
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 25 of 37
recorded from behaving animals has steadily moved theoretical concepts in the direction of
increasing realism.[84]
One of the most influential early contributions was a 1959 paper titled What the frog's eye tells
the frog's brain: the paper examined the visual responses of neurons in the retina and optic
tectum of frogs, and came to the conclusion that some neurons in the tectum of the frog are
wired to combine elementary responses in a way that makes them function as "bug perceivers".
[128]
A few years later David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel discovered cells in the primary visual
cortex of monkeys that become active when sharp edges move across specific points in the
field of view—a discovery for which they won a Nobel Prize.[129] Follow-up studies in higher-order
visual areas found cells that detect binocular disparity, color, movement, and aspects of shape,
with areas located at increasing distances from the primary visual cortex showing increasingly
complex responses.[130] Other investigations of brain areas unrelated to vision have revealed
cells with a wide variety of response correlates, some related to memory, some to abstract
types of cognition such as space.[131]
In the second half of the 20th century, developments in chemistry, electron microscopy,
genetics, computer science, functional brain imaging, and other fields progressively opened
new windows into brain structure and function. In the United States, the 1990s were officially
designated as the "Decade of the Brain" to commemorate advances made in brain research,
and to promote funding for such research.[135]
In the 21st century, these trends have continued, and several new approaches have come into
prominence, including multielectrode recording, which allows the activity of many brain cells to
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 26 of 37
be recorded all at the same time;[136] genetic engineering, which allows molecular components of
the brain to be altered experimentally;[118] genomics, which allows variations in brain structure to
be correlated with variations in DNA properties[137] and neuroimaging.
See also
◾ Brain–computer interface
◾ Neurological disorder
◾ Neuroplasticity
◾ Outline of neuroscience
References
1. ^ Pelvig, DP; Pakkenberg, H; Stark, AK; Pakkenberg, B (2008). "Neocortical glial cell
numbers in human brains". Neurobiology of Aging. 29 (11): 1754–1762.
doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.04.013 . PMID 17544173 .
2. ^ Yuste, Rafael; Church, George M. (March 2014). "The new century of the brain" (PDF).
Scientific American. 310 (3): 38–45. Bibcode:2014SciAm.310c..38Y .
doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0314-38 . PMID 24660326 .
4. ^ Sporns, O (2010). Networks of the Brain . MIT Press. p. 143. ISBN 978-0-262-01469-4.
6. ^ Singh, I (2006). "A Brief Review of the Techniques Used in the Study of Neuroanatomy".
Textbook of Human Neuroanatomy . Jaypee Brothers. p. 24. ISBN 9788180618086.
8. ^ Douglas, RJ; Martin, KA (2004). "Neuronal circuits of the neocortex". Annual Review of
Neuroscience. 27: 419–451. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144152 .
PMID 15217339 .
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 27 of 37
9. ^ Barnett, MW; Larkman, PM (2007). "The action potential". Practical Neurology. 7 (3): 192
–197. PMID 17515599 .
11. ^ Williams, RW; Herrup, K (1988). "The control of neuron number". Annual Review of
Neuroscience. 11: 423–453. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.11.030188.002231 . PMID 3284447 .
12. ^ Heisenberg, M (2003). "Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models". Nature Reviews
Neuroscience. 4 (4): 266–275. doi:10.1038/nrn1074 . PMID 12671643 .
13. ^ a b Jacobs, DK; Nakanishi, N; Yuan, D; et al. (2007). "Evolution of sensory structures in
basal metazoa" . Integrative & Comparative Biology. 47 (5): 712–723.
doi:10.1093/icb/icm094 . PMID 21669752 .
14. ^ a b Balavoine, G (2003). "The segmented Urbilateria: A testable scenario" . Integrative &
Comparative Biology. 43 (1): 137–147. doi:10.1093/icb/43.1.137 .
15. ^ Schmidt-Rhaesa, A (2007). The Evolution of Organ Systems. Oxford University Press.
p. 110. ISBN 978-0-19-856669-4.
16. ^ Kristan Jr, WB; Calabrese, RL; Friesen, WO (2005). "Neuronal control of leech
behavior". Prog Neurobiology. 76 (5): 279–327. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.09.004 .
PMID 16260077 .
17. ^ Mwinyi, A; Bailly, X; Bourlat, SJ; Jondelius, U; Littlewood, DT; Podsiadlowski, L (2010).
"The phylogenetic position of Acoela as revealed by the complete mitochondrial genome of
Symsagittifera roscoffensis" . BMC Evolutionary Biology. 10: 309. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-10-
309 . PMC 2973942 . PMID 20942955 .
18. ^ Barnes, RD (1987). Invertebrate Zoology (5th ed.). Saunders College Pub. p. 1.
ISBN 978-0-03-008914-5.
19. ^ a b Butler, AB (2000). "Chordate Evolution and the Origin of Craniates: An Old Brain in a
New Head". Anatomical Record. 261 (3): 111–125. doi:10.1002/1097-0185(20000615)
261:3<111::AID-AR6>3.0.CO;2-F . PMID 10867629 .
20. ^ Bulloch, TH; Kutch, W (1995). "Are the main grades of brains different principally in
numbers of connections or also in quality?". In Breidbach O. The nervous systems of
invertebrates: an evolutionary and comparative approach . Birkhäuser. p. 439. ISBN 978-3-
7643-5076-5.
21. ^ "Flybrain: An online atlas and database of the drosophila nervous system" . Retrieved
2011-10-14.
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 28 of 37
22. ^ Konopka, RJ; Benzer, S (1971). "Clock Mutants of Drosophila melanogaster" . Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 68 (9): 2112–6. Bibcode:1971PNAS...68.2112K .
doi:10.1073/pnas.68.9.2112 . PMC 389363 . PMID 5002428 .
23. ^ Shin, Hee-Sup; et al. (1985). "An unusual coding sequence from a Drosophila clock
gene is conserved in vertebrates". Nature. 317 (6036): 445–8. Bibcode:1985Natur.317..445S .
doi:10.1038/317445a0 . PMID 2413365 .
26. ^ Hobert, O (2005). The C. elegans Research Community, ed. "Specification of the
nervous system" . WormBook: 1–19. doi:10.1895/wormbook.1.12.1 . PMID 18050401 .
27. ^ White, JG; Southgate, E; Thomson, JN; Brenner, S (1986). "The Structure of the
Nervous System of the Nematode Caenorhabditis elegans". Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society B. 314 (1165): 1–340. Bibcode:1986RSPTB.314....1W .
doi:10.1098/rstb.1986.0056 . PMID 22462104 .
28. ^ Jabr, Ferris (2012-10-02). "The Connectome Debate: Is Mapping the Mind of a Worm
Worth It?" . Scientific American. Retrieved 2014-01-18.
30. ^ Kandel, ER (2007). In Search of Memory: The Emergence of a New Science of Mind.
WW Norton. pp. 145–150. ISBN 978-0-393-32937-7.
31. ^ Shu, D.-G.; Conway Morris, S.; Han, J.; Zhang, Z.-F.; Yasui, K.; Janvier, P.; Chen, L.;
Zhang, X.-L.; Liu, J.-N.; et al. (2003). "Head and backbone of the Early Cambrian vertebrate
Haikouichthys". Nature. 421 (6922): 526–529. Bibcode:2003Natur.421..526S .
doi:10.1038/nature01264 . PMID 12556891 .
33. ^ Armstrong, E (1983). "Relative brain size and metabolism in mammals". Science. 220
(4603): 1302–1304. Bibcode:1983Sci...220.1302A . doi:10.1126/science.6407108 .
PMID 6407108 .
34. ^ Jerison, HJ (1973). Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence. Academic Press. pp. 55–74.
ISBN 978-0-12-385250-2.
35. ^ Parent, A; Carpenter, MB (1995). "Ch. 1". Carpenter's Human Neuroanatomy. Williams
& Wilkins. ISBN 978-0-683-06752-1.
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 29 of 37
36. ^ Pardridge, W (2005). "The Blood-Brain Barrier: Bottleneck in Brain Drug
Development" . NeuroRx. 2 (1): 3–14. doi:10.1602/neurorx.2.1.3 . PMC 539316 .
PMID 15717053 .
37. ^ Northcutt, RG (2008). "Forebrain evolution in bony fishes". Brain Research Bulletin. 75
(2–4): 191–205. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.10.058 . PMID 18331871 .
38. ^ Reiner, A; Yamamoto, K; Karten, HJ (2005). "Organization and evolution of the avian
forebrain". The Anatomical Record Part A: Discoveries in Molecular, Cellular, and Evolutionary
Biology. 287 (1): 1080–1102. doi:10.1002/ar.a.20253 . PMID 16206213 .
39. ^ Siegel, A; Sapru, HN (2010). Essential Neuroscience. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
pp. 184–189. ISBN 978-0-7817-8383-5.
40. ^ Swaab, DF; Boller, F; Aminoff, MJ (2003). The Human Hypothalamus . Elsevier.
ISBN 978-0-444-51357-1.
43. ^ Saitoh, K; Ménard, A; Grillner, S (2007). "Tectal control of locomotion, steering, and eye
movements in lamprey" . Journal of Neurophysiology. 97 (4): 3093–3108.
doi:10.1152/jn.00639.2006 . PMID 17303814 .
44. ^ Richard Swann Lull; Harry Burr Ferris; George Howard Parker; James Rowland Angell;
Albert Galloway Keller; Edwin Grant Conklin (1922). The evolution of man: a series of lectures
delivered before the Yale chapter of the Sigma xi during the academic year 1921–1922 . Yale
University Press. p. 50.
45. ^ Puelles, L (2001). "Thoughts on the development, structure and evolution of the
mammalian and avian telencephalic pallium" . Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society
B. 356 (1414): 1583–1598. doi:10.1098/rstb.2001.0973 . PMC 1088538 . PMID 11604125 .
46. ^ Salas, C; Broglio, C; Rodríguez, F (2003). "Evolution of forebrain and spatial cognition in
vertebrates: conservation across diversity". Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 62 (2): 72–82.
doi:10.1159/000072438 . PMID 12937346 .
47. ^ a b Grillner, S; et al. (2005). "Mechanisms for selection of basic motor programs—roles for
the striatum and pallidum". Trends in Neurosciences. 28 (7): 364–370.
doi:10.1016/j.tins.2005.05.004 . PMID 15935487 .
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 30 of 37
48. ^ Northcutt, RG (1981). "Evolution of the telencephalon in nonmammals". Annual Review
of Neuroscience. 4: 301–350. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.04.030181.001505 . PMID 7013637 .
50. ^ a b Barton, RA; Harvey, PH (2000). "Mosaic evolution of brain structure in mammals".
Nature. 405 (6790): 1055–1058. doi:10.1038/35016580 . PMID 10890446 .
51. ^ Aboitiz, F; Morales, D; Montiel, J (2003). "The evolutionary origin of the mammalian
isocortex: Towards an integrated developmental and functional approach" . Behavioral and
Brain Sciences. 26 (5): 535–552. doi:10.1017/S0140525X03000128 . PMID 15179935 .
52. ^ Romer, AS; Parsons, TS (1977). The Vertebrate Body. Holt-Saunders International.
p. 531. ISBN 0-03-910284-X.
53. ^ a b Roth, G; Dicke, U (2005). "Evolution of the brain and Intelligence". Trends in Cognitive
Sciences. 9 (5): 250–257. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.005 . PMID 15866152 .
55. ^ Shoshani, J; Kupsky, WJ; Marchant, GH (2006). "Elephant brain Part I: Gross
morphology, functions, comparative anatomy, and evolution". Brain Research Bulletin. 70 (2):
124–157. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.03.016 . PMID 16782503 .
56. ^ Finlay, BL; Darlington, RB; Nicastro, N (2001). "Developmental structure in brain
evolution". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 24 (2): 263–308.
doi:10.1017/S0140525X01003958 . PMID 11530543 .
57. ^ Calvin, WH (1996). How Brains Think . Basic Books. ISBN 978-0-465-07278-1.
58. ^ Sereno, MI; Dale, AM; Reppas, AM; Kwong, KK; Belliveau, JW; Brady, TJ; Rosen, BR;
Tootell, RBH (1995). "Borders of multiple visual areas in human revealed by functional magnetic
resonance imaging" (PDF). Science. AAAS. 268 (5212): 889–893.
Bibcode:1995Sci...268..889S . doi:10.1126/science.7754376 . PMID 7754376 .
59. ^ Fuster, JM (2008). The Prefrontal Cortex. Elsevier. pp. 1–7. ISBN 978-0-12-373644-4.
61. ^ a b Wong, R (1999). "Retinal waves and visual system development". Annual Review of
Neuroscience. 22: 29–47. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.29 . PMID 10202531 .
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 31 of 37
63. ^ Ridley, M (2003). Nature via Nurture: Genes, Experience, and What Makes Us
Human . Forth Estate. pp. 1–6. ISBN 978-0-06-000678-5.
64. ^ Wiesel, T (1982). "Postnatal development of the visual cortex and the influence of
environment" (PDF). Nature. 299 (5884): 583–591. Bibcode:1982Natur.299..583W .
doi:10.1038/299583a0 . PMID 6811951 .
66. ^ Cooper, JR; Bloom, FE; Roth, RH (2003). The Biochemical Basis of
Neuropharmacology . Oxford University Press US. ISBN 978-0-19-514008-8.
67. ^ McGeer, PL; McGeer, EG (1989). "Chapter 15, Amino acid neurotransmitters". In G.
Siegel; et al. Basic Neurochemistry. Raven Press. pp. 311–332. ISBN 978-0-88167-343-2.
68. ^ Foster, AC; Kemp, JA (2006). "Glutamate- and GABA-based CNS therapeutics". Current
Opinion in Pharmacology. 6 (1): 7–17. doi:10.1016/j.coph.2005.11.005 . PMID 16377242 .
70. ^ Mehler, MF; Purpura, DP (2009). "Autism, fever, epigenetics and the locus coeruleus" .
Brain Research Reviews. 59 (2): 388–392. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.11.001 .
PMC 2668953 . PMID 19059284 .
71. ^ Rang, HP (2003). Pharmacology. Churchill Livingstone. pp. 476–483. ISBN 0-443-
07145-4.
72. ^ Speckmann, E-J; Elger, CE (2004). "Introduction to the neurophysiological basis of the
EEG and DC potentials". In Niedermeyer E, Lopes da Silva FH. Electroencephalography: Basic
Principles, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. pp. 17–31.
ISBN 0-7817-5126-8.
73. ^ a b Buzsáki, G (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-
530106-9. OCLC 63279497 .
74. ^ a b c Nieuwenhuys, R; Donkelaar, HJ; Nicholson, C (1998). The Central Nervous System
of Vertebrates, Volume 1. Springer. pp. 11–14. ISBN 978-3-540-56013-5.
75. ^ Safi, K; Seid, MA; Dechmann, DK (2005). "Bigger is not always better: when brains get
smaller" . Biology Letters. 1 (3): 283–286. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2005.0333 . PMC 1617168 .
PMID 17148188 .
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 32 of 37
76. ^ Mink, JW; Blumenschine, RJ; Adams, DB (1981). "Ratio of central nervous system to
body metabolism in vertebrates: its constancy and functional basis". American Journal of
Physiology. 241 (3): R203–212. PMID 7282965 .
77. ^ Raichle, M; Gusnard, DA (2002). "Appraising the brain's energy budget" . Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99 (16): 10237–10239. Bibcode:2002PNAS...9910237R .
doi:10.1073/pnas.172399499 . PMC 124895 . PMID 12149485 .
78. ^ Mehagnoul-Schipper, DJ; Van Der Kallen, BF; Colier, WNJM; Van Der Sluijs, MC; Van
Erning, LJ; Thijssen, HO; Oeseburg, B; Hoefnagels, WH; Jansen, RW (2002). "Simultaneous
measurements of cerebral oxygenation changes during brain activation by near-infrared
spectroscopy and functional magnetic resonance imaging in healthy young and elderly
subjects". Hum Brain Mapp. 16 (1): 14–23. doi:10.1002/hbm.10026 . PMID 11870923 .
79. ^ Ebert, D.; Haller, RG.; Walton, ME. (Jul 2003). "Energy contribution of octanoate to intact
rat brain metabolism measured by 13C nuclear magnetic resonance spectroscopy" . J
Neurosci. 23 (13): 5928–35. PMID 12843297 .
80. ^ Marin-Valencia, I.; Good, LB.; Ma, Q.; Malloy, CR.; Pascual, JM. (Feb 2013).
"Heptanoate as a neural fuel: energetic and neurotransmitter precursors in normal and glucose
transporter I-deficient (G1D) brain" . J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 33 (2): 175–82.
doi:10.1038/jcbfm.2012.151 . PMC 3564188 . PMID 23072752 .
81. ^ Boumezbeur, F.; Petersen, KF.; Cline, GW.; Mason, GF.; Behar, KL.; Shulman, GI.;
Rothman, DL. (Oct 2010). "The contribution of blood lactate to brain energy metabolism in
humans measured by dynamic 13C nuclear magnetic resonance spectroscopy" . J Neurosci.
30 (42): 13983–91. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2040-10.2010 . PMC 2996729 .
PMID 20962220 .
82. ^ Deelchand, DK.; Shestov, AA.; Koski, DM.; Uğurbil, K.; Henry, PG. (May 2009). "Acetate
transport and utilization in the rat brain" . J Neurochem. 109 Suppl 1 (Suppl 1): 46–54.
doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2009.05895.x . PMC 2722917 . PMID 19393008 .
83. ^ Soengas, JL; Aldegunde, M (2002). "Energy metabolism of fish brain". Comparative
Biochemistry and Physiology B. 131 (3): 271–296. doi:10.1016/S1096-4959(02)00022-2 .
PMID 11959012 .
85. ^ a b Carew, TJ (2000). "Ch. 1". Behavioral Neurobiology: the Cellular Organization of
Natural Behavior . Sinauer Associates. ISBN 978-0-87893-092-0.
86. ^ Dafny, N. "Anatomy of the spinal cord" . Neuroscience Online. Retrieved 2011-10-10.
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 33 of 37
88. ^ Gurney, K; Prescott, TJ; Wickens, JR; Redgrave, P (2004). "Computational models of
the basal ganglia: from robots to membranes". Trends in Neurosciences. 27 (8): 453–459.
doi:10.1016/j.tins.2004.06.003 . PMID 15271492 .
89. ^ Shima, K; Tanji, J (1998). "Both supplementary and presupplementary motor areas are
crucial for the temporal organization of multiple movements" . Journal of Neurophysiology. 80
(6): 3247–3260. PMID 9862919 .
90. ^ Miller, EK; Cohen, JD (2001). "An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function". Annual
Review of Neuroscience. 24 (1): 167–202. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167 .
PMID 11283309 .
91. ^ Antle, MC; Silver, R (2005). "Orchestrating time: arrangements of the brain circadian
clock" (PDF). Trends in Neurosciences. 28 (3): 145–151. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2005.01.003 .
PMID 15749168 .
92. ^ Kleitman, N (1938). Sleep and Wakefulness. revised 1963, reprinted 1987. The
University of Chicago Press, Midway Reprints series. ISBN 0-226-44073-7.
94. ^ Gross, CG (1998). "Claude Bernard and the constancy of the internal
environment" (PDF). The Neuroscientist. 4 (5): 380–385. doi:10.1177/107385849800400520 .
96. ^ Chiel, HJ; Beer, RD (1997). "The brain has a body: adaptive behavior emerges from
interactions of nervous system, body, and environment". Trends in Neurosciences. 20 (12): 553
–557. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01149-1 . PMID 9416664 .
98. ^ Ardiel, EL; Rankin, CH (2010). "An elegant mind: learning and memory in
Caenorhabditis elegans" . Learning and Memory. 17 (4): 191–201. doi:10.1101/lm.960510 .
PMID 20335372 .
99. ^ Hyman, SE; Malenka, RC (2001). "Addiction and the brain: the neurobiology of
compulsion and its persistence". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2 (10): 695–703.
doi:10.1038/35094560 . PMID 11584307 .
100. ^ Ramón y Cajal, S (1894). "The Croonian Lecture: La Fine Structure des Centres
Nerveux". Proceedings of the Royal Society. 55 (331–335): 444–468.
doi:10.1098/rspl.1894.0063 .
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 34 of 37
101. ^ Lømo, T (2003). "The discovery of long-term potentiation" . Philosophical Transactions
of the Royal Society B. 358 (1432): 617–620. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1226 . PMC 1693150 .
PMID 12740104 .
102. ^ Malenka, R; Bear, M (2004). "LTP and LTD: an embarrassment of riches". Neuron. 44
(1): 5–21. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.012 . PMID 15450156 .
103. ^ a b Bos, I; De Boever, P; Int Panis, L; Meeusen, R (August 2014). "Physical Activity, Air
Pollution and the Brain" . Sports Medicine. 44: 1505–1518. doi:10.1007/s40279-014-0222-6 .
PMID 25119155 .
104. ^ Curtis, CE; D'Esposito, M (2003). "Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during
working memory". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7 (9): 415–423. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)
00197-9 . PMID 12963473 .
105. ^ Tulving, E; Markowitsch, HJ (1998). "Episodic and declarative memory: role of the
hippocampus". Hippocampus. 8 (3): 198–204. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)
8:3<198::AID-HIPO2>3.0.CO;2-G . PMID 9662134 .
106. ^ Martin, A; Chao, LL (2001). "Semantic memory and the brain: structures and
processes". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 11 (2): 194–201. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(00)
00196-3 . PMID 11301239 .
107. ^ Balleine, BW; Liljeholm, Mimi; Ostlund, SB (2009). "The integrative function of the basal
ganglia in instrumental learning". Behavioral Brain Research. 199 (1): 43–52.
doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.034 . PMID 19027797 .
108. ^ Doya, K (2000). "Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and
motor control". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 10 (6): 732–739. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(00)
00153-7 . PMID 11240282 .
109. ^ Storrow, Hugh A. (1969). Outline of clinical psychiatry. New York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts, Educational Division. ISBN 978-0-390-85075-1. OCLC 47198 .
110. ^ Thagard, P (2008). Zalta, EN, ed. "Cognitive Science" . The Stanford Encyclopedia of
Philosophy. Retrieved 2011-10-14.
111. ^ Bear, MF; Connors, BW; Paradiso, MA (2007). "Ch. 2". Neuroscience: Exploring the
Brain. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ISBN 978-0-7817-6003-4.
112. ^ Dowling, JE (2001). Neurons and Networks. Harvard University Press. pp. 15–24.
ISBN 978-0-674-00462-7.
113. ^ Wyllie, E; Gupta, A; Lachhwani, DK (2005). "Ch. 77". The Treatment of Epilepsy:
Principles and Practice. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ISBN 978-0-7817-4995-4.
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 35 of 37
114. ^ Laureys, S; Boly, M; Tononi, G (2009). "Functional neuroimaging". In Laureys S,
Tononi G. The Neurology of Consciousness: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropathology.
Academic Press. pp. 31–42. ISBN 978-0-12-374168-4.
115. ^ Carmena, JM; et al. (2003). "Learning to Control a Brain–Machine Interface for
Reaching and Grasping by Primates" . PLoS Biology. 1 (2): 193–208.
doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0000042 . PMC 261882 . PMID 14624244 .
117. ^ Abbott, LF; Dayan, P (2001). "Preface". Theoretical Neuroscience: Computational and
Mathematical Modeling of Neural Systems. MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-54185-5.
119. ^ Bower, Bruce (12 January 2009). "Armenian cave yields ancient human brain" .
ScienceNews.
120. ^ a b Finger, S (2001). Origins of Neuroscience. Oxford University Press. pp. 14–15.
ISBN 978-0-19-514694-3.
121. ^ *Hippocrates (2006) [400 BCE], On the Sacred Disease , Translated by Francis
Adams, Internet Classics Archive: The University of Adelaide Library, archived from the
original on September 26, 2007
122. ^ Finger, S (2001). Origins of Neuroscience. Oxford University Press. pp. 193–195.
ISBN 978-0-19-514694-3.
123. ^ Bloom, FE (1975). Schmidt FO, Worden FG, Swazey JP, Adelman G, eds. The
Neurosciences, Paths of Discovery. MIT Press. p. 211. ISBN 978-0-262-23072-8.
124. ^ Shepherd, GM (1991). "Ch.1 : Introduction and Overview". Foundations of the Neuron
Doctrine. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-506491-9.
125. ^ Piccolino, M (2002). "Fifty years of the Hodgkin-Huxley era". Trends in Neurosciences.
25 (11): 552–553. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(02)02276-2 . PMID 12392928 .
126. ^ Sherrington, CS (1942). Man on his nature. Cambridge University Press. p. 178.
ISBN 978-0-8385-7701-1.
127. ^ von Neumann, J; Churchland, PM; Churchland, PS (2000). The Computer and the
Brain. Yale University Press. pp. xi–xxii. ISBN 978-0-300-08473-3.
128. ^ Lettvin, JY; Maturana, HR; McCulloch, WS; Pitts, WH (1959). "What the frog's eye tells
the frog's brain" (pdf). Proceedings of the Institute of Radio Engineering. 47: 1940–1951.
doi:10.1109/jrproc.1959.287207 .
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 36 of 37
129. ^ Hubel, DH; Wiesel, TN (2005). Brain and visual perception: the story of a 25-year
collaboration. Oxford University Press US. pp. 657–704. ISBN 978-0-19-517618-6.
130. ^ Farah, MJ (2000). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Vision. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 1–29.
ISBN 978-0-631-21403-8.
131. ^ Engel, AK; Singer, W (2001). "Temporal binding and the neural correlates of sensory
awareness". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 5 (1): 16–25. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)
01568-0 . PMID 11164732 .
132. ^ Dayan, P; Abbott, LF (2005). "Ch.7: Network models". Theoretical Neuroscience. MIT
Press. ISBN 978-0-262-54185-5.
133. ^ Averbeck, BB; Lee, D (2004). "Coding and transmission of information by neural
ensembles". Trends in Neurosciences. 27 (4): 225–230. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2004.02.006 .
PMID 15046882 .
134. ^ Forrest, MD (2014). "Intracellular Calcium Dynamics Permit a Purkinje Neuron Model to
Perform Toggle and Gain Computations Upon its Inputs." . Frontiers in Computational
Neuroscience. 8: 86. doi:10.3389/fncom.2014.00086 .
135. ^ Jones, EG; Mendell, LM (1999). "Assessing the Decade of the Brain" . Science. 284
(5415): 739. Bibcode:1999Sci...284..739J . doi:10.1126/science.284.5415.739 .
PMID 10336393 .
137. ^ Geschwind, DH; Konopka, G (2009). "Neuroscience in the era of functional genomics
and systems biology" . Nature. 461 (7266): 908–915. Bibcode:2009Natur.461..908G .
doi:10.1038/nature08537 . PMC 3645852 . PMID 19829370 .
Further reading
External links
Wikisource has the text of the 1911 Encyclopædia Britannica article Brain.
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26
Brain - Wikipedia Page 37 of 37
◾ BrainInfo , neuroanatomy database
https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Brain 2016/11/26