Creative Teachers
Creative Teachers
Creative Teachers
Dennis Sale
Creative
Teachers
Self-directed Learners
Cognitive Science and Technology
Series Editor
David M. W. Powers, Adelaide, SA, Australia
This series aims to publish work at the intersection of Computational Intelligence
and Cognitive Science that is truly interdisciplinary and meets the standards and
conventions of each of the component disciplines, whilst having the flexibility to
explore new methodologies and paradigms. Artificial Intelligence was originally
founded by Computer Scientists and Psychologists, and tends to have stagnated
with a symbolic focus. Computational Intelligence broke away from AI to explore
controversial metaphors ranging from neural models and fuzzy models, to
evolutionary models and physical models, but tends to stay at the level of
metaphor. Cognitive Science formed as the ability to model theories with
Computers provided a unifying mechanism for the formalisation and testing
of theories from linguistics, psychology and philosophy, but the disciplinary
backgrounds of single discipline Cognitive Scientists tends to keep this mechanism
at the level of a loose metaphor. User Centric Systems and Human Factors similarly
should inform the development of physical or information systems, but too often
remain in the focal domains of sociology and psychology, with the engineers and
technologists lacking the human factors skills, and the social scientists lacking the
technological skills. The key feature is that volumes must conform to the standards
of both hard (Computing & Engineering) and social/health sciences (Linguistics,
Psychology, Neurology, Philosophy, etc.). All volumes will be reviewed by experts
with formal qualifications on both sides of this divide (and an understanding of and
history of collaboration across the interdisciplinary nexus).
Indexed by SCOPUS
Creative Teachers
Self-directed Learners
123
Dennis Sale
Singapore Polytechnic
Singapore, Singapore
This Springer imprint is published by the registered company Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.
The registered company address is: 152 Beach Road, #21-01/04 Gateway East, Singapore 189721,
Singapore
To my wife, Jane and daughters,
Adele & Lydia
Preface
vii
viii Preface
Chapter 1 frames the context for what follows in subsequent chapters, which is
the systematic analysis and evaluation of the pedagogic issues and necessary core
competencies for facilitating both teaching expertise and self-directed learning for
students.
Chapter 2 provides a comprehensive synthesis of the extensive research relating
to human learning, captured in terms of universal cognitive scientific principles,
which I frame as the Core Principles of Learning. These constitute an
evidence-based pedagogic framework—the essential knowledge bases underpin-
ning teaching expertise—what I refer to as Pedagogic Literacy.
Chapter 3 argues that Metacognitive Capability is the superordinate twenty-first-
century competence, essential for both creative teachers and self-directed learners.
I extensively analyse the components of metacognition—how they work as a
dynamic system in human psychological functioning—and, most importantly, how
this unique human capability can be used to maximize learning, self-regulation and
personal well-being.
Chapter 4 addresses the challenge of enhancing students’ intrinsic motivation in
school-based learning and, in essence, human motivation generically. Motivation
underpins learning, in that without motivation, people do not bother to learn—or
think—too much.
Chapter 5 tackles the psychological capability for creative thinking and how this
is contextualized to everyday practical teaching and learning contexts—what I refer
to as Creative Teaching Competence.
Chapter 6, using an evidence-based approach, demonstrates how the affordances
of EdTech can be used to positively impact specific aspects of the learning process,
hence providing better student attainment and engagement opportunities.
Chapter 7 critically analyses and evaluates what constitutes twenty-first-century
competencies, how these are best derived from an evidence-based approach and the
implications for framing educational aims and outcomes. I take as a core valuation
that while curriculum must support industry requirements and employability, there
is also a need to accommodate competencies for wider issues of well-being and
citizenship.
Chapter 8 focuses on assessing twenty-first-century competencies. Different
competencies, as well as different aspects of a competence, require different
assessment methods. Assessing complex competencies such as metacognition in
valid and reliable ways, in real application contexts, will be a big challenge for
curriculum planners and teachers.
Chapter 9 provides an evidence-based framework for implementing professional
development that is both practically viable in the real world of educational insti-
tutions and most likely to be perceived as such by teaching professionals them-
selves. While we increasingly know what to do and how to do it, high-quality
professional learning comes at a cost in terms of time and resources.
I have been privileged to have worked and lived in Singapore for the past 24 years.
Now acknowledged as the best educational system in the world, and much is to do
with the approaches taken, not least placing great value on teachers and their
professional development. I have been part of Singapore’s educational develop-
ment, both in terms of unlimited opportunities for personal learning and profes-
sional growth, as well as a contributor to many of its educational innovations—so
thank you Singapore.
Writing books on the realities of teaching is challenging, as it requires a wide
range of participating professionals to open up their classrooms for me to experience
their practice and engage in much mediation relating to its impact on learning.
Acknowledgement and thanks go to the thousands of teaching professionals, from
many educational/training sectors, countries and cultural contexts, who have shared
their experiences, thoughts and feelings with me. It has been enriching and enjoyable.
Special appreciation goes to my excellent Research Team (Ngoh Shwu Lan,
Cheah Sin Moh, Mark Wan, M. Fikret Ercan, M. Thiyagarajan, Roland Soh, Zhou
Shang Ping, Ng-Soo Geok Ling and Wong Yunyi) who allowed me full access to
observe their lessons, talk extensively to their students, as well as conducting their
own Evidence-Based Reflective Practice.
I would also like to specifically acknowledge the following individuals who have
made significant contributions to my thinking, research work and writing:
Geoff Petty, one of Britain’s leading experts on teaching methods and author of
Teaching Today and Evidence-Based Teaching: A Practical Approach. Apart from
being inspired by Geoff’s pioneering work in this area, I am especially grateful to
him for his feedback on my work.
Ochan Kusuma-Powell and Bill Powell, veteran global international educators,
and authors of numerous books on teaching and educational leadership. Their
feedback and friendship over the years were invaluable in framing the style and
direction of my writing.
ix
x Acknowledgements
xi
xii Contents
Dennis Sale has taught across all sectors of the British educational system, and for
the past 24 years trained and coached over 10,000 teaching/training professionals in
Singapore and most countries in the Asian region. He has invented curriculum and
pedagogic models in the areas of metacognitive capability, creative teaching, and
blended learning. His most recent books include Creative Teaching: An
Evidence-Based Approach (Springer, 2015) and The Challenge of Reframing
Engineering Education (Springer, 2013). While in Singapore, Dennis worked as
Senior Education Advisor at Singapore Polytechnic for over 15 years, conducted
research for the Ministry of Education, and provided consultancy and training for
both private and public institutions across Asia. His work focuses on how humans
learn best, translating evidence-based findings from cognitive science and related
fields into professional development programs that offer high impact learning
outcomes for both teachers and students. Dennis has set himself the personal goal of
enhancing teacher expertise and learner capability globally.
xv
Chapter 1
Making Sense of Teaching: From
Mystery to Heuristics
Abstract This introductory chapter frames the context for establishing an evidence-
based pedagogic framework for developing Creative Teachers and Self-Directed
Learners. Self-directed learning is now considered an educational priority for stu-
dents who will need new skills and attributes to successfully navigate the challenges
and uncertainties of today’s rapidly changing technological society. Equally, teach-
ers will need heightened expertise, most notably in pedagogy and technology related
skills, as many teaching roles will be challenged in an educational landscape where
expectations of value are rising, but financial resources are decreasing. The chapter
summarizes both the reasons why teaching has lacked the evidence-based approaches
to practice that are the norm in other professions such as medicine and engineering
and how it is now, from extensive research in the cognitive sciences and on what
teaching methods work best, in a learning revolution.
1.1 Introduction
Teaching is the only major occupation of man for which we have not yet developed tools
that make an average person capable of competence and performance. In teaching, we rely
on the ‘naturals’, the ones who somehow know how to teach.
(Peter Drucker)
The title of the book ‘Fifteen Thousand Hours’ by Rutter (1982) is based on the
approximate number of hours pupils spend in school. So, what did I learn in my
15,000 h? Well, I certainly acquired two useful skill-sets, football and boxing. What
makes football such a good learning experience and a useful skill set to learn? Travel
practically anywhere in the world and you will easily find soccer-players, who play
in teams either in organized leagues or social set-ups. They love the game and the
‘crack’ (I think this term has Irish origins, ‘craic’—loosely means fun; apologies
if I’m wrong). The essential point is that this offers the opportunity for immediate
membership and friendship in the local community.
Certainly, there were things I learned about teaching and teachers. Most significantly,
the teachers were not alike, far from it. The 1966 film “The Good, the Bad and the
Ugly”, which starred one of my favourite actors, Clint Eastwood, comes readily to
mind when thinking about my teachers. Let’s lump the “bad and the ugly” together
and this was my maths teacher for ‘O’ level. Unintelligible on all counts, the lesson
may just as well have been taught in a Malagasy dialect. I sat the ‘O’ level mathematics
exam in June 1968 and achieved the undistinguished grade of 9 (6 being the lowest
passing grade at that time). You may be wondering what a grade 9 means in the
context of mathematical competence? Well, if my Jack Russell would have sat the
same exam, he could not have fared worse. Jack (what else would you call a Jack
Russell) is a clever dog but still hasn’t worked out how to open the fridge door and
get to his chicken meal autonomously.
Fortunately, in the following academic year, I had a change in maths teacher, Mr.
Edrich, and he represented an example of ‘the good’. We could understand what
he was saying, and he recognized that we were far from confident or competent
in maths, which was not rocket science to ascertain. However, most importantly,
instead of communicating negative and pessimistic views, he communicated to us
that with effort and some hard work we could learn this stuff. In terms of his teaching,
as I remember it, he slowed down the pace and kept providing examples and non-
examples (I did not recognize this strategy then) and we gradually began to understand
and eventually do those basic factorization procedures. I was able to make some sense
of how simultaneous and quadratic equations worked, and I could increasingly solve
the questions set. However, it was not a deep understanding as I re-sat the maths ‘O’
level in the November series and still failed it—but only just, a grade 7. The happy
ending was that in the following June exam series in 1969, I passed with a grade
3, which was very credible in context. If I am honest, I don’t think I ever achieved
a deep understanding of maths at that time, but passing it was crucial as it was a
high stakes exam; without it, I could not have got a place at university on a B.Sc.
Programme. Mr. Edrich will never know his specific and positive impact on my life,
and this is the norm for many teachers. Unfortunately, I did not consider thanking
him at that time, as it’s only retrospectively, and much later, as I came to fully realize
how important such teachers are.
In my 40 plus years as a teaching professional of various genres, similar experi-
ences emerge and play out in terms of different perceptions of teacher’s abilities. For
example, as a classroom teacher, there were many instances of parents asking me
something akin to, “Is there any way I can avoid my daughter Linda being taught by
Mr. Lee next term? Everybody knows he’s dead boring and students can’t understand
what he’s on about.” Parents certainly know that teachers vary greatly in competency
(however defined) and that their children’s performance is not just a reflection of a
fixed innate capability in the subject, but varies considerably depending on who
teaches them. Similarly, as a parent, it was very apparent that both my daughters’
4 1 Making Sense of Teaching: From Mystery to Heuristics
enjoyment and grades reflected, in no small part, the experiences that certain teachers
created for them throughout the course programme. In one situation, for my youngest
daughter, I ended up paying for private tuition, on top of the already expensive expa-
triate private school fees. In the previous year, she was meeting the attainment targets
comfortably and enjoyed the subject. However, in this particular year, she found the
teacher less friendly, not easy to follow and the experience generally dull. Her inter-
est waned and her grades fell significantly. Whatever one’s views on educational
equity, and I favour providing as much equality of opportunity as possible for all, by
not paying for this extra tuition from a different (maybe better) teacher could have
resulted in an outcome similar to my first two attempts at the ‘O’ level maths exam,
back in the 1960s. I was lucky I had Mr. Edrich to teach me that darn math, and he
was not on extra salary.
It is not surprising, therefore, that an increasing body of research shows the massive
impact that teachers have on student attainment. Izumi and Evers (2002), from an
overview of research on the impact of teachers on student achievement, summarized:
…nothing is as important to learning as the quality of a student’s teacher. The difference
between a good teacher and a bad teacher is so great that fifth-grade students who have poor
teachers in grades three to five score roughly 50 percentile points below similar groups of
students who are fortunate enough to have effective teachers. (ix)
Hargreaves and Fullan (2012), which is even more damning, documented that:
… the Los Angeles Times reporters gained access to 7 years of value-added test performance
data for 6,000 third through fifth-grade teachers in English and Mathematics in the Los
Angeles Unified Public-school District – one of the poorest districts in the United States.
They passed the data to expert economists, who came up with an even more remarkable
finding. There were differences of up to 41% in value-added performance between teachers
of the same kind of children in the very same school. (p. 15)
Much of the confusion about what is or should be good teaching can be explained in
large part by Sallis and Hingley’s (1991) assertion that “…education is a creature of
fashion” (p. 9). I like this analogy as it is so grounded in my experience of fashion.
While I have relatively little interest in fashion now, my teenage years were spent in
the 1960s, the era of great musical bands, full employment, a real sense of optimism
about the future and, of course, the famous fashion icon, the mini-skirt. This was the
world as I knew it, and this was ladies fashion as I experienced it, and it seemed an
objective reality of what was natural. I was, of course, unaware of such notions as
‘socially constructed realities’ (Berger and Luckman 1967). The reality was exactly
as I perceived it, what else could it be? Equally, I never considered what it might
have been like for the ladies wearing such attire in the winter months. However, one
evening, my mother was showing me pictures of herself when she was young, and
the thing I noticed was the long skirts she wore. I vividly remember commenting
that this seemed strange and I was glad that evolution had moved on from then. It
never dawned on me that fashion was the product of a deliberate industry ploy that
systematically creates, manages and periodically changes images of desirable attire.
After all, it must do this once the marketed item is saturated—how many pairs of
flair bottom trousers can you fit in a typical male wardrobe? In defence of my lack
of understanding on such matters, I don’t think many 14-year-olds at that time were
well versed in such sociological imagination either. It’s no big deal that fashion in
clothes is manipulated to ensure new revenue is generated and novelty is added to an
aspect of human experience. However, I am far less comfortable when this is applied
to professions, especially where human well-being is concerned, and teaching meets
that criteria full on. It is inevitable that any profession, indeed any aspect of human
activity, can only be as good as the most current knowledge bases—conventional
wisdom—of the time. Let’s not blame Ptolemy for thinking that the Earth was the
centre of the universe, it would have made perfect sense at his time. Similarly, before
6 1 Making Sense of Teaching: From Mystery to Heuristics
we could have known otherwise, the simple notion of gravity, it makes perfect sense
to believe that the Earth is flat. In the case of teaching, I take the stand that much
professional activity has not sufficiently and consistently reflected knowledge bases
that could have improved practice at the level of student learning opportunities and
outcomes (e.g., attainment, engagement, well-being). For example, for those of us
who have been in, or around, the profession over the past 30–40 years, one could
not have failed to notice such major shifts in teaching focus from ‘traditional’ to
‘progressive’ education and then ‘back to basics’, as well as, more recently, the
teacher’s role allegedly changing from ‘sage on the stage’ to ‘guide on the side.’
A negative consequence of this contested nature and periodic radical reframing of
what constitutes good teaching is that it does little to convince anybody that teaching
is truly a profession with well-constituted bases of professional knowledge, as is
the case for medicine or engineering. This is not to say that the medical profession,
or other well-established professions, have not gone through similar epochs of fads
masquerading as practice, as Thomas’ (1979) depiction of the medical profession
before the drive towards evidence-based practice portrayed:
It is hard to conceive of a less scientific enterprise among human endeavours. Virtually
anything that could be thought up for treatment was tried out at one time or another, and,
once tried, lasted decades or even centuries before being given up. It was, in retrospect, the
most frivolous and irresponsible kind of experimentation, based on nothing but trial and
error, and usually resulting in precisely that sequence. (p. 159)
One would probably be both shocked and frightened if, on a visit to a modern
medical centre, the doctor produced a saw, some leeches and asked you to drink a
large dose of alcohol. We now see increasing sophistication of practice through a
whole range of complex technology infrastructure. This is not to argue that all is
well in the medical profession and there are probably still some ‘dodgy’ practices.
However, it feels like the profession, in most modern societies anyway, is now largely
driven by established and rigorous standards of research and validation, which seems
to be relatively lacking in the context of education. Indeed, one may argue that this is
visibly apparent as many classrooms look pretty similar to what they were decades or
even centuries past. However, the major reason for the slower acceleration towards
accepted high professionalism in teaching is that much of practice is still largely
driven by dominant paradigms or perspectives in psychology and pedagogy, rather
than a solid empirical base. Paradigms are ways of looking at things in the world
(e.g. the meaning of life, human conduct, educational aims and practice) and contain
certain premises and methodologies relating to those particular domains of reality.
These, in turn, shape how we perceive and orientate ourselves to such realities. Kuhn
(1996) famously noted that when socialized into a paradigm, it becomes a prerequisite
to perception itself:
What a man sees depends both upon what he looks at and also upon what his previous
visual-conceptual experience has taught him to see. (p. 113)
World religions are other notable examples of a paradigm in that they typically
contain explicit assumptions about the nature of reality (e.g., a belief in a metaphys-
ical being, absolute codes of conduct, building a relationship with that being) which
1.3 A Short Tour into Educational Jurassic Park 7
shape the thinking and behavioural aspects of adherents to specific faiths. In educa-
tion, much has been similar, though lacking adherence to a metaphysical being, only
the occasional psychological guru, which may have had similar impacts in terms of
practice. Prominent paradigms in education have included ‘behaviourism’, ‘cogni-
tivism’ and, probably the most dominant one in terms of ‘current vogue’, ‘construc-
tivism’. These paradigms do offer insights relating to aspects of the learning process
and provide some useful overall framing for approaching teaching. However, they
are far from constituting a comprehensive evidence-based framework that has strong
predictive value in terms of enhancing student attainment. The danger of limiting
practice to one paradigm is well captured by Pratt (2002):
Perspectives are neither good nor bad. They are simply philosophical orientations to knowl-
edge, learning and the role and responsibility of being a teacher. Therefore, it is important
to remember that each of these perspectives represents a legitimate view of teaching when
enacted appropriately. Conversely, each holds the potential for poor teaching. (p. 14)
Anderson et al. (1998) are more explicit in identifying the problem when they
argued that:
What is needed more than a philosophy of education is a science of education. Modern
attempts at educational improvement point back to theorists (Piaget, Vygotsky, and Dewey)
whose theories are vague by current psychological standards and lack the strong connection
to empirical evidence that has become standard in the field. (p. 237)
Mayer (2004) is even most blunt in advocating the necessity of making the kind
of changes in approach to practice that have occurred in other more established
professions. He argued that we must:
...move educational reform efforts from the fuzzy and unproductive world of ideology - which
sometimes hides under the various banners of constructivism – to the sharp and productive
world of theory-based research on how people learn. (p. 18)
Finally, the problem appears systemic, both in terms of policy and practices, and
shapes the socialization of recruits into the profession, which is the hallmark of a
paradigm. Stone’s (2000) criticism of some teacher education programmes further
illustrates the continuation of paradigms rather than evidence-based practice in the
training of teachers:
What teachers are told, however, is that student differences are important and if their teaching
is truly creative, energetic and engaging, they will succeed in individualising and bringing
forth the best from all students. In effect, teachers are being taught to make diagnoses that
heighten their awareness of differences without advancing their ability to teach. (p. 43)
In consequence, this has created much confusion for many teaching professionals
as to what is good pedagogy (indeed, what is pedagogy) and what are truly useful
knowledge bases from which we can design and facilitate instructional strategies with
high predictive value in terms of meeting desired learning outcomes. It is unlikely
that many in the teaching profession believe that this is the result of limited available
literature on teaching and how to teach , just as there is no shortage of writings on
8 1 Making Sense of Teaching: From Mystery to Heuristics
other topics of educational relevance, such as parenting. However, the confusion does
not seem to abate. Hattie (1999), for example, stated:
A glance at the journals on most shelves of most libraries, my colleagues’ shelves, and
on web pages would indicate that the state of knowledge in the discipline is healthy. The
worldwide picture is certainly one of plenty. (p. 1)
A particularly notable example, that fully illustrates the above analysis, is that
of Learning Styles which has shaped the thinking and practices of many teachers
worldwide. Over the years, I have had many heated debates on this topic and always
refused to conduct workshops or seminars on it, as I felt it was, at best, an ephemeral
entity in the learning and attainment stakes. From an evidence-based point of view,
it now seems little more than ‘folk psychology’, and I can take some solace in that.
As Hattie (2009) summarized:
One of the more fruitless pursuits is labelling students with ‘learning styles’. This modern fad
for learning styles, not to be confused with the more worthwhile notion of multiple learning
strategies, assumes that different students have differing preferences for particular ways of
learning.
Often, the claim is that when teaching is aligned with the preferred or dominant learning style
(for example, auditory, visual, tactile, or kinesthetic) then achievement is enhanced. While
there can be many advantages by teaching content using many different methods (visual,
spoken, movement), this must not be confused with thinking that students have differential
strengths in thinking in these styles. (p. 89)
1.4 Moving Out of Educational Jurassic Park 9
Much is changing as far as teaching is concerned and it may, as Petty (2009) argued,
be ready to:
…embark on a revolution, and like medicine, abandon both custom and practice and fashions
and fads, to become evidence-based (cover page).
Invariably, one cannot escape the essential subjectivity of experience, and sugges-
tions of a value-free science are untenable. However, I feel it is necessary to retain
at least a critical operational notion of objectivity as a ‘regulatory ideal’; otherwise,
there is little point in conducting an inquiry, whether it be about good teaching or
good soccer, or whatever. As Phillips (1990) argued:
If we abandon such notions, it is not sensible to make inquiries at all. For if a sloppy inquiry
is as acceptable as a careful one, and if an inquiry that is careless about evidence is as
acceptable as an inquiry that has taken pains to be precise and unbiased, then there is no
need to inquire… (p. 43)
In this context, to argue that there are no better nor worse ways in which to design
student learning experiences is both absurd and dangerous. As Ramsden (1992)
wrote:
It is a folly to suggest that there are no better or worse ways of teaching, no general attributes
that distinguish good teaching from the bad. (p. 87)
It is now firmly established that there is a strong evidence base relating to how best
to design and facilitate the various practices we call teaching that can significantly
enhance student learning opportunities, attainment levels and the experience of learn-
ing (e.g. intrinsic motivation). This change is an inevitable result of our increasing
knowledge relating to how humans learn, what teaching methods and practices work
best and why, and the unpacking of what the best teaching practitioners do and how.
Much of this significant research on learning has already been documented in the
literature (e.g., Bransford 1999; Marzano 2007; Mayer and Alexander 2010; Hat-
tie and Yates 2014). Collectively, the research evidence is now providing us with a
heightened pedagogic understanding of the various facets of highly effective teaching
and, when this is used creatively in context, it will optimize attainment and engage-
ment for a wider range of student groups. In most basic terms we can now engage
10 1 Making Sense of Teaching: From Mystery to Heuristics
There is no doubt that our understanding of how humans learn is rapidly increas-
ing, especially as the fields of cognitive and social neuroscience provide further
insights into brain functioning at the neurological level, and how this plays out in
terms of human cognition and behaviour relating to learning. Equally, and fully
consonant with this heightened understanding of human learning, is the accumula-
tion of extensive and rigorous research activity, which is uncovering from a strong
empirical base what teaching methods tend to work best and on what basis. Perhaps
most publicized in this area is the work of Hattie (e.g., 2009, 2012), though many
others have been providing significant contributions over recent years (e.g., Brans-
ford 1999; Marzano 2007; Mayer and Alexander 2010; Petty 2009). Mansell (2008)
referred to Hattie’s seminal work on the effectiveness of different teaching methods
and strategies as:
… perhaps education’s equivalent to the search for the Holy Grail - or the answer to life, the
universe and everything.
Table 1.1 Examples of effect sizes in learner attainment from Hattie’s Meta-analysis
Influence Mean effect size
Whole-class interactive teaching (direct instruction) 0.81
A specific approach to active learning in class, which is highly teacher led,
but very active for students. This involves summaries reviews and a range of
active learning methods, including questioning
Feedback 0.73
Students getting feedback on their work from the teacher or from
themselves (self-assessment or from peers or some other sources
Metacognitive strategies 0.69
Explicit teaching and use of metacognitive strategies (e.g., conscious
planning, monitoring, and evaluating of thinking and learning
Challenging goals for students 0.59
Goals that students can meet through effort on their part—they should be a
specific as possible, and meaningful to the students involved
Advance organizers 0.41
Giving students a summary in advance and a purposes for the learning
For readers not familiar with ‘Russian Dolls’, they are a set of different sized dolls,
usually around 5, and they fit one inside another from the smallest to the biggest.
Figure 1.1 provides a visual example of high effect method combination.
12 1 Making Sense of Teaching: From Mystery to Heuristics
The Russian Doll analogy provides an easy to remember generic advance orga-
nizer for planning lessons as it should easily evoke the key question of what strategy
or method combination is likely to be most effective for the particular student group.
However, as will be explored in subsequent chapters, some methods may have a bet-
ter overall impact on student attainment but in learning, as in all aspects of life, too
much of a good thing often leads to habituation and boredom and subsequently loses
its impact. Also, in designing the overall instructional strategy, we must take into
account the learner profile (especially prior competence and motivational status), the
learning outcomes to be attained, and the available resource facilities that can be
accessed.
The interested reader can refer to Hattie’s original works (e.g., Hattie 2009, 2012)
for the extensive detailed coverage of the research methodology employed and the
full range of effect sizes for different instructional methods and learning strategies.
Human Immunodeficiency Virus (HIV), the virus that causes AIDS, was discov-
ered. As is well documented, the origins of the virus, as well as the specific means
and rates of transmission, evoked much controversy and debate at that time. Also,
treatments, despite the initial optimistic hype of azidothymidine (AZT), were con-
troversial and driven more by desperation rather than hard science which, in context,
was understandable. Today, HIV is clearly in the realm of heuristics, in that we
have a comprehensive understanding of the behaviour of the virus and its impact on
the immune system, as well as how to mitigate its most deleterious effects—in an
increasing number of cases—to the progression into full-blown AIDS. At present,
our knowledge is not Algorithmic, as this level of knowledge would require both a
sterilizing cure (where the virus is completely eradicated from the body), as well as
an effective vaccine. However, as Avert (2019), which provides global information
on HIV and Aids, summarizes:
There is no cure for HIV, although antiretroviral treatment can control the virus, meaning that
people with HIV can live long and healthy lives. Most research is looking for a functional
cure where HIV is reduced to undetectable and harmless levels in the body permanently, but
some residual virus may remain.
world of mystery, while others master its heuristics. The beauty of heuristics is that they
guide us toward a solution by way of organized exploration of possibilities. (p. 12)
It is to be noted that heuristics in this context retains the more generic notion of
‘rules of thumb’ that enables people to solve problems and make judgments quickly
and efficiently, but extends the concept to include existing (but as-to-yet, incomplete)
knowledge about phenomena in the world. In this way, good heuristics will enable
teaching professionals to design and facilitate learning experiences effectively and
efficiently from a sound pedagogic base but, as the term denotes, not with the certainty
of outcomes in all situations. How this works in practice will be explained and
illustrated in the forthcoming chapters. It certainly constitutes a significant shift
towards a more substantive evidence-based profession, and reflects very strikingly the
description by Perkins (1992) of the ‘unequal distribution of knowledge’ concerning
what we know about learning and teaching and what happens in many classrooms:
...we do not have a knowledge gap – we have a monumental use-of-knowledge gap. (p. 2)
1.6 Summary
References
Anderson JR et al (1998) Radical constructivism and cognitive psychology. In: Ravitch D (ed)
Brookings papers on education policy: 1998. Brookings Institution, Washington, D.C., pp 227–
255
Avert (2019) Global information on HIV and Aids. https://www.avert.org/. Last accessed 30 Nov
2019
Berger PL, Luckmann T (1967) The social construction of reality: a treatise in the sociology of
knowledge. Anchor Books, New York
Bransford J et al (1999) Brain, mind, experience & school. National Academy Press, Washington,
D.C.
Darling-Hammond L, Bransford J (2005) Preparing teachers for a changing world: what teachers
should learn and be able to do. Jossey-Bass, San-Francisco
Eisner EW (1995) The art and craft of teaching. In: Ornstein AC, Behar LS (eds) Contemporary
issues in curriculum. Allyn & Bacon, Massachusetts
Garreau J (2005) Radical evolution: the promise and peril of enhancing our minds, our bodies—and
what is means to be human. Doubleday, New York
References 15
Hargreaves A, Fullan M (2012) Professional capital: transforming teaching in every school. Teachers
College Press, New York
Hattie J (1999) Influences on student learning. Inaugural lecture. University of Auckland
Hattie J (2009) Visible learning. Routledge, New York
Hattie J (2012) Visible learning for teachers: maximizing impact on learning. Routledge, London
Hattie J, Yates GCR (2014) Visible learning and the science of how we learn. Routledge, New York
Izumi TL, Evers WM (2002) Teacher quality. Hoover Institutional Press, San Francisco
Kuhn TS (1996) The structure of scientific revolutions. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago
Lincoln YS (1990) The making of a constructivist: a remembrance of transformations past. In: Guba
EG (ed) The paradigm dialogue. Sage, London
Mansell W (2008) Research reveals teaching’s Holy Grail. TES Newspaper on 21 November
Martin R (2009) The design of business. Harvard Business Press, Massachusetts
Marzano RJ (1992) A different kind of classroom. ASCD, Alexandria, VA
Marzano RJ (2007) The art and science of teaching: a comprehensive framework for effective
instruction. ASCD, Alexandria, VA
Mayer RE (2004) Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning?. Am Psychol
59(1):14–19
Mayer RE, Alexander PA (2010) Handbook of research on learning and instruction. Routledge,
London
Ornstein AC (1995) Teaching: theory into practice. Allyn & Bacon, Needham Heights, Mas-
sachusetts
Perkins DN (1992) Smart schools. The Free Press, London
Petty G (2009) Evidence-based teaching: a practical approach. Nelson Thornes, Cheltenham
Phillips DC (1990) Postpositivistic science: myths and realities. In: Guba EG (ed) The paradigm
dialogue. Sage, London
Pratt DD (2002) Good teaching: one size fits all? In: Ross-Gordon JM (ed) Contemporary viewpoints
on teaching adults effectively, no 93. Spring 2002. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, pp 5–15
Ramsden P (1992) Learning to teach in higher education. Routledge, London
Rowe KJ, Rowe KS (1993) Assessing student behavior: the utility and measurement properties of a
simple parent and teacher-administered behavioural rating instrument for use in educational and
epidemiological research. Paper presented at the annual conference of the Australian association
for research in education. Fremantle, WA
Rutter M (1982) Fifteen thousand hours: secondary schools and their effects on children. Harvard
University Press, Cambridge, MA
Sallis E, Hingley P (1991) College quality assurance systems in Mendip Paper D20. Coombe Lodge,
Bristol
Stone JE (2000) Teacher training and pedagogical methods. In: Teacher quality conference. Hoover
Institution/Pacific Research Institute, May 12
Thomas L (1979) The Medusa and the Snail: more notes of a biology watcher. Viking Press, New
York
Chapter 2
Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic
Literacy: The Core Principles
of Learning
2.1 Introduction
Chapter 1 took you on a short tour into Educational Jurassic Park, and how EBT
(Petty 2009) can close its gates forever, keeping teachers safely on the outside. I
used Martin’s depiction of the ‘knowledge funnel’ as a conceptual frame to position
effective teaching as moving away from being a Mystery to one of potentially useful
Heuristics. Underpinning such heuristics are increasing knowledge bases on how
humans learn and extensive research on what teaching methods work best and on
what basis—enabling teaching to adopt evidence-based approaches to practice as in
the case of other professions. The pioneering work of Hattie (2009) outlined in that
chapter opened up the possibility of a more comprehensive evidence-based approach
to all aspects of pedagogy.
Pedagogy is a much-used term by educationalists and other personnel in the
learning industry when talking about matters of curriculum, teaching and learning. In
many curriculum-related meetings, I am still amused by the plethora of terminology
that surface in this area (e.g., pedagogical approach, pedagogic practices, pedagogical
content knowledge and, more recently, signature pedagogies). However, what is
equally apparent is that for many there is still a high level of conceptual confusion,
as I still get asked questions such as, “Is there one pedagogy or many”? This can
More recent definitions (e.g., The Free Dictionary 2019) have dropped the ref-
erence to the child and applied it more generically to “the principles, practice, or
profession of teaching” or “the activities of educating or instructing”. Pedagogy has
also been contrasted with the term Andragogy (Knowles 1973), which focuses on the
teaching of adult learners. This has invariably led to further confusion, and questions
being asked as to whether or not adults learn differently from children and should
they be taught differently, and in what ways and how. The issue of differential learn-
ing between adults and children is addressed in some detail in Chap. 9, but key points
are summarized here for purposes of context in this chapter.
Certainly, there are significant differences in the level of prior experience of adults,
as compared to children. Adults also choose what they want to learn, and this is
typically consciously directed to meet work or personal learning goals. Kids at school
are largely told what to learn, at least in the earlier years. However, whilst there are
important motivational and life experience differences for adults, it is questionable
whether the underlying learning process is structurally different from that of children
who have attained the stage of formal operational thought (Piaget 2001), typically
around 12–15 years of age. At this stage of brain maturation, children can reason
logically and use a range of thinking skills (e.g., analyze, compare & contrast, make
inferences and interpretations, and evaluate). In some ways, this has similarities with
the notion of different learning styles, which was popular in the educational literature
for a couple of decades. As outlined prior, research has far from validated such
theories, especially their usefulness in terms of pedagogically beneficial applications.
I agree with Schank (1997) who argued that:
Contrary to common belief, people don’t have different learning styles. They do, however,
have different personalities. The distinction is important because we need to be clear that
everybody learns in the same way. (p. 48)
In summary, much has been learned about the effectiveness of different methods
and strategies of teaching and their impact on student attainment. The big questions
now centre on what makes such methods and strategies work better and how they
operate in terms of productively structuring the subjective experience of learners. To
put it in simpler terms, what specifically goes on inside students’ heads and how does
this enhance their learning processes, resulting in better attainment, engagement and
well-being? The more we frame better evidence-based answers to these questions,
the more we move towards a pedagogy that is practically useful in terms of how we
teach, and all that this entails.
In the following sections, through an extensive synthesis of a wide range of knowl-
edge bases relating to human learning, I outline and illustrate certain key heuristics,
what I have referred to as Core Principles of Learning (Sale 2015) that underpin effec-
tive teaching. Together they constitute a pedagogic framework from which teaching
professionals can thoughtfully plan and facilitate learning experiences from a more
evidence-based perspective. The framework does not claim to be exhaustive or sum-
mative, as new knowledge and insights will continually enhance our understanding
of human learning and the implications for how we teach. However, from much val-
idation in practice across a wide range of educational sectors and cultural contexts,
I see them as contributing to a much-needed Pedagogic Literacy.
Furthermore, while each Core Principle of Learning focuses attention on a key area
or process relating to how humans learn and the specific implications for planning
20 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
instruction, they are not discrete or separate in that they should be considered indepen-
dently of each other. They are mutually supporting, interdependent and potentially
highly synergistic. As Stigler and Hiebert (1999) highlighted:
Teaching is a system. It is not a loose mixture of individual features thrown together by the
teacher. It works more like a machine, with the parts operating together and reinforcing one
another, driving the vehicle forward. (p. 75)
These core principles of learning are essential parts of this system, the underpin-
ning knowledge of how people learn and how to use this in the planning and delivery
of what we call teaching. Willingham (2009) used the term Cognitive Scientific
Principles to describe such principles of learning and illustrates their implications
for practice through an analogy with engineering:
Principles of physics do not prescribe for a civil engineer exactly how to build a bridge,
but they do let him predict how it is likely to perform if he builds it. Similarly, cognitive
scientific principles do not prescribe how to teach, but they can help you predict how much
your students are likely to learn. If you follow these principles, you maximize the chances
that your students will flourish. (p. 165)
The purpose of the above emphasis on how the brain works at the level of mind
is to emphasize that as teachers we are not able to simply transmit information into
students brains, like we can download information on our computers. Instead, we
must navigate many aspects—foibles—of human psychological functioning, much
of which is systemically not functioning as we would like it in terms of facilitating
desired learning outcomes in educational contexts.
However, it is through understanding how the mind works (and often doesn’t
work well) that can best arm us with useful knowledge for developing skills and
tools to teach in ways that are both brain compatible, as well as mitigating many of
its less helpful features. This is the territory that we must navigate if we are to be
expert teachers and, in navigation, it helps if we know the territory well and have
a half-decent map for getting from one location to another. Yes, it’s Heuristics, but
this is much better than Mystery.
The key message of the chapter is quite clear and simply captured by Hart (1983):
…designing educational experiences without knowledge about how human brains learn
naturally and most efficiently can be compared to designing a glove without any knowledge
of the human hand. (p. 4)
I fail to recall much by the way of consciously ever considering any learning goals
over my 15,000 h at school, beyond getting a regular place in the school soccer
team. Even for this desirable goal, I had little idea of what I specifically needed to
do to achieve it—except to be good at soccer. The physical education teacher never
helped me to understand my limitations as a soccer player and what I might do to
enhance specific skill areas. Indeed, my school life lacked an explicit structure for
learning beyond the fact that I was supposed to be there. In terms of the subjects I
studied (the word does not fit well), I had little notion of what I should be learning
in terms of specific outcomes and to what level of proficiency. For the exams I took,
I tried to memorize what I had written down in class. I had no benchmarks for my
performance, so it was a surprise, and delight, when I passed those ‘O’ and ‘A’ levels.
On leaving school, I had little direction on what I wanted to do. After all, my
school-friends had long since left school and were going out to local pubs and clubs
with money in their pockets. And there was a real motivational base to this—girls. For
my first 6 months or so, I worked as a labourer on a building site for the scaffolding
crew. Scaffolders were a tough bunch of guys, and they had to be able to carry those
22-foot poles—which were cast iron in those days. Anyway, the money was good,
and my boxing background meant I could match the scaffolders in the practices of
pole carrying.
However, somewhere around this time, my father, obviously concerned about
where his only son (in fact only child) was going in life, called me in for a ‘father
22 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
and son conversation’. These were not frequent, so I still can recall the main content
of this conversation. Most significant was him pointing out that while scaffolding
paid well now, I would be earning similar amounts, in real terms, some 20 years in
the future, and will not find it such a physically relishing challenge as the years pass.
Also, he pointed out that with my ‘O’ and ‘A’ levels, I should have plenty of choices.
The problem was, I did not know what I wanted—well not in occupational terms
anyway.
In response, I went to the local career office, where I must say, personnel were
helpful. I had many interviews, including at accountancy and legal firms, so my
options were good. Not sure which way to go, I was eventually introduced to the
possibility of going to university, something I had no meaningful frame on whatever.
I had never met anyone who had gone to university and my only prior knowledge
in this area was a weekly TV quiz show ‘University Challenge’ in which different
universities competed for some prize or other. It soon became apparent, however,
that there were some attractive aspects in going to university, not least government
grants, long holiday periods and opportunities to develop my soccer playing skills.
The only missing piece of this jigsaw was that one had to study a subject. Nothing
came to mind for me. Motivated to some extent to pursue this option, I browsed
through the university prospectuses and—hey presto, ‘psychology’. In all honesty, I
did not know much about psychology, but I guessed it was a bit like sociology, which
was one of my ‘A’ level subjects. Sociology was also my favourite school subject,
again made interesting by the teacher.
In summary, serendipity rather than any thoughtful sense of direction shaped my
learning and career to this point. However, studying psychology was a life-changing
experience as I realized that one’s learning is within a persons’ locus of control.
Invariably, the constraints of finance, time and commitments may significantly impact
the timing of career choices, but successful learning is very much in one’s own hands.
However, successful learning involves in no small part knowing what it is that you
want to learn and for what life goals. It also requires a strategy, and not least a fair bit
of effort, which in turn is aided by a belief system that sees attainment as a product
of these processes, not a predestined neurological state. There is a saying in soccer
circles that, “You are only as good as your last game.” That makes perfect sense.
I have noticed, over many years of watching professional soccer, how fickle soccer
fans are. When a player has had a few poor games, there are often sounds of derision
when his name is read out on the team sheet. Three weeks prior, the same player was
greeted with great applause. A similar frame plays out in life. I was once a grade 9
‘O’ level student in maths. I could still have been that, but I am not, and I know what
changed that reality and how it works. Hopefully, that has made me a better teacher.
Poor thinking, limiting beliefs and lack of competence are not existentially fixed for
the mainstream population of learners, but if no change in perception occurs, they
can become stable realities—even identities—for the people concerned. Learning
is about change, and productive change can be greatly helped by others, but these
others need to be good models. For example, in the case of thinking, as Dilts et al.
(1980) illustrates:
2.2 Core Principle 1: Learning Goals, Objectives and Proficiency … 23
Effective thinking strategies can be modelled and utilized by any individual who wishes to
do so. (p. 193)
The key point to this heuristic for effective teaching and enhancing learner attain-
ment is that students require structure for their learning, and this starts with having a
meaningful goal. While students are ultimately responsible for their learning, helping
them to frame clear and meaningful goals, as well as what is involved in meeting them,
is fundamental to providing structure, direction and motivation to their learning. As
Ramsden (1992) pointed out:
It is indisputable that, from the students’ perspective, clear standards and goals are a
vitally important element of the effective educational experience. Lack of clarity on these
points is almost always associated with negative evaluations, learning difficulties and poor
performance. (p. 127)
There is often a need for creative teaching to facilitate such high-level student
buy-in across divergent student groups, as this involves a major perceptual shift for
many students in terms of motivation and learning approaches. However, if this can be
attained, the focus can then be largely on the how of learning effectively, rather than
frequently revisiting the why. What constitutes challenging is, of course, subjective
in part, but most importantly, we are seeking the best contextualization to the learner
profile. Providing goals that are easy to attain results in little value in the learning
stakes. The idea of giving students such goals to ensure they get plenty of positive
feedback regarding their successful attainment, to promote self-esteem, is naïve at
best. Students know that they are being ‘dumbed down’ and will not be duped by
such token positive self-regard. Similarly, if the goals are not realistically achievable
in terms of student’s prior knowledge (e.g., level of conceptual understanding; skill
sets), and in the time frames defined, this creates frustration and stress which is
detrimental to learning and attainment. While it is sometimes challenging for the
teacher to establish meaningful and challenging goals for students, it is time well
spent, as Hattie (2009) concluded:
24 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
Educating students to have high, challenging, appropriate expectations is among the most
powerful influence in enhancing student achievement. (p. 60)
Netscape. Yes, I started to feel a bit silly, but this was not a concern, as I knew exactly
where I was in the learning stakes—a complete novice. In this learning situation, I
was very aware of my limited prior knowledge of email and the internet. Furthermore,
as a novice, it’s natural to experience feelings of uncertainty, even dependency, and
performance will be erratic at best. That’s the profile of a novice in any unfamiliar
learning situation, irrespective of whether one has great expertise in other fields.
Aside, I am also very much a novice as a guitar player and on the one occasion I
did a public performance, fortunately in a minor venue, even my basic chord playing
went out of synchronization. I have never played publicly since.
Looking back on that one-day internet training programme highlights the diffi-
culties faced by any learner who is confronted with a learning situation in which
there is little prior knowledge to connect to, and where the instruction is far too
fast to build any useful understanding of what is being taught. I went back to my
office tired, confused, and with no useful understanding or competence to use the
internet. However, what I did know was that this was a typical and almost inevitable
result given the learning context and, most importantly, I knew how to deal with it
effectively.
Learners come to any new learning situation (whether it be the classroom or
elsewhere) with preconceptions about how the world works based on their life expe-
riences. Within this framing, they may have developed some generalized beliefs
about themselves as learners (which may or may not have been favourable) in terms
of their capability for learning. As described prior, post my grade 9 math ‘O’ level
result and the preceding learning experience, I did not feel competent or confident
in learning mathematics. The problem is that prior learning can create a whole host
of misconceptions and motivational dispositions that lead people to avoid any fur-
ther attempts at learning in a specific area. This takes on an added significance in
that all learning, whether accurate or otherwise, exists as a relatively permanent
structure in our neural architecture. I was fortunate in that my final ‘O’ level math
teacher, Mr. Edrich, was able to challenge and disrupt my existing knowledge and
beliefs relating to learning mathematics. For many, they become stuck in an abyss
of misconceptions and perceived limited capability. The important point is that new
learning cannot avoid being connected to prior learning. As Shulman (1991) pointed
out:
All new knowledge gains its form and meaning through its connection with pre-existing
knowledge and its influence on the organization and reorganization of prior knowledge.
(p. 10)
working—as I have no useful prior knowledge on what causes cars to break down,
and even less on how to fix them.
Prior knowledge then is the lens through which students will perceive and react
to new information provided in a learning event. If prior learning is inaccurate,
incongruent or limited, it is likely to interfere with the meaningful integration of
the new knowledge presented. This provides real challenges for teachers. Ausubel
(1978) went as far as arguing that:
If I had to reduce all of educational psychology to just one principle, I would say this: the most
important single factor influencing learning is what the learner already knows. Ascertain this
and teach him(sic) accordingly. (p. 163)
Making student’s prior knowledge explicit helps not only to deal with misconcep-
tions and facilitate better linking of new knowledge to existing knowledge structures
but also saves an enormous amount of time in terms of duplicated learning (e.g.,
Nuthall 2005), boredom for students, as well as frustration for teachers. Finding out
what students already know, understand and can do is fundamental to teaching in
any context. Hattie (2012) argued that:
…we must know what students already know, know how they think, and then aim to progress
all students towards the success criteria of the lesson. (p. 44)
There is then the challenge of designing ways to connect new knowledge to the level of
knowledge and understanding that learners already have. This requires both a good under-
standing of the subject matter content and the students being taught, as well as some creativity
in order to design the most appropriate instructional strategy to best facilitate such connec-
tivity. Wlodkowski (2008), using the language of cognitive neuroscience, suggests that this
involves the following:
…begin with what they already know and biologically assemble them with the new
knowledge or skill by connecting the established networks and the new networks. (p. 13)
This heuristic has an effect size of 0.41 (Hattie 2009) and in combination with
clear goals and advance organizers, provides a strong foundation for subsequent
learning, and can be seen as significant components of an impactful “Russian Doll”
instructional strategy—to reiterate this metaphor introduced in Chap. 1. Once stu-
dents have clarity of purpose in the learning goals, a sense of direction for meeting
them, appraised their existing knowledge and dealt with any restrictive misconcep-
tions, they are in a much better position to tackle new concepts effectively. Of course,
this is an ideal scenario and it is unlikely to happen so easily for all students in all
situations. However, it is a much better strategy than going straight into the new
content delivery, for all the reasons outlined above.
Activation of students’ prior knowledge can be done in many ways, but all involve
eliciting specific feedback concerning what they already know, understand and can
do (and to what level of proficiency) in relation to new learning goals and specific
outcomes. This can be done through written and oral pre-tests, and by way of open
discussion with students to explore more fully their mental models and ways they
are thinking about the topic area to be covered. It is important to recognize that
many students are unlikely to be clear on specific things they don’t know and may
not even be able to effectively make this explicit. For this reason, it is important
2.3 Core Principle 2: Learners Prior Knowledge Is Activated … 27
One area of general agreement among writers on the types of knowledge, which
provides a useful understanding of what knowledge entails, is the categorization of
knowledge into Declarative Knowledge and Procedural Knowledge, as summarized
below:
Declarative Knowledge: As the term implies, it refers to knowledge that can
be clearly stated as facts, concepts, generalizations or principles within a content
knowledge field. For example, once acquired, we might be able to clearly assess that
a learner knows or understands:
• the concept of democracy
• the defining attributes of a dog
• the conventions of punctuation
• Cristiano Ronaldo plays football for Juventus (at the time of writing).
Procedural Knowledge: This refers to knowing how to do something, typically
involving performing a process or demonstrating a skill. For example, once acquired,
we might be able to clearly assess that a learner is able to:
• Add and subtract
• Write a paragraph
• Juggle
• Set up an experiment
• Read music
• Search for a database.
In many practical tasks both types of knowledge are involved, as to do something
typically involves knowing something about it. For example, while the amount of
declarative knowledge involved in being able to play soccer is not extensive, no
amount of skill in procedural terms would be useful if one did not know what goal
to score into. Invariably, there is much variation in terms of both the number of
knowledge components and level of complexity involved in knowledge acquisition
and deployment when procedural. For example, to acquire a single piece of factual
knowledge such as England won the soccer world cup in 1966 is very straightfor-
ward. Around 5 repetitions should put it firmly into long-term memory. How memory
works and its crucial role in effective learning is outlined in detail in Core Principle
7: Learning design takes into account the working of memory systems. In excep-
tional circumstances, a little idiosyncratic knowledge may be amazingly useful to
an individual, as was powerfully illustrated in the 2008 film ‘Slumdog Millionaire’.
The film featured a young man (Jamal) from the slums of Mumbai who appears on
the Indian version of ‘Who wants to be a millionaire?’ and answers all the ques-
tions correctly, though arouses suspicions that he must have cheated. However, in
the film, Jamal recounts in a flashback how he knows the answer to each question,
each one linked to a key event in his life. His learning of these specific bits of fac-
tual knowledge happened idiosyncratically, but through great serendipity resulted
in the illusion of him being highly knowledgeable, which ran counter to his slum
living existence. In most real-world contexts, we are very unlikely to get such highly
favourable results from limited knowledge bases. The building of accurate organized
2.4 Core Principle 3: Content Is Organized Around Key Concepts … 29
Notions that today, all we need to do to get content is to search the internet and
find it, and ‘hey presto’ we have knowledge and understanding—even expertise—
is highly dubious, as Keen (2007) exposed in his book, ‘The cult of the Amateur:
How Today’s Internet is Killing our Culture’. In the days when we used physical
encyclopaedias and went to the library and read books, information was there, but it
did not mean that we could understand it. Good teachers get good results for a reason
and, amongst other important aspects, they can identify the key concepts essential to
understanding the structure of a topic area in the context of learners’ prior knowledge
and experience, and then work with this for extending their learning. Willingham
(2009) makes an interesting point in relation to the importance of key knowledge in
the learning process:
The very processes that teachers care about most – critical thinking processes such as reason-
ing and problem-solving – are intimately intertwined with factual knowledge that is stored
in long-term memory (not just found in the environment). (p. 22)
First, you should know that much of the time when we see someone apparently thinking,
he or she is actually engaged in memory retrieval…memory is the cognitive process of first
resort. (pp. 28–29)
In terms of our everyday learning, such cognitive science explains why we can
read, understand and easily apply new knowledge in areas that we have the expertise,
30 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
and do this quickly. In contrast, when our knowledge is limited and idiosyncratic,
we struggle to make sense of what we are reading and feel that we are not so bright
(in those situated moments). For example, I can breeze through research papers in
the field of psychology and education but always struggle with any instructional
booklet on how to assemble any do-it-yourself (DIY) equipment such as furniture
or a mechanical device—much to my wife’s annoyance. Quite simply, I have such
limited knowledge or interest in this area. The same applies to gardening, which I
once thought I should do, as we had a garden, and other folk often said how relaxing
and enjoyable such activity was. Well, I tried to grow lettuce, and guess what, the
slugs ate them; I tried to grow strawberries, which the birds ate, and my attempt at
growing runner beans, as I did not secure their structures against a strong wind, was
destroyed. Basically, I lacked too much basic necessary gardening knowledge. Was
it relaxing and enjoyable? Well, you can infer the answer—I had the whole garden
ripped up and concretized.
So, what are the pedagogic implications of this? In Willingham’s words:
Factual knowledge must precede skill. Critical thinking is not a set of procedures that can
be practised and perfected while divorced from background knowledge. Cognitive science
leads to the rather obvious conclusion that students must learn the concepts that come up
again and again – the unifying ideas of each discipline. (p. 33)
prior knowledge) all contribute in some significant way to facilitating the process of
building understanding. Through a careful analysis of the learning goals, the specific
outcomes and proficiency standards that we seek to achieve with our students, it
is possible to identify the key declarative and procedural knowledge (especially
core concepts and principles) that underpin an understanding of the key structure
of the topic areas we are teaching. Bruner (1966) identified what are essentially
key evidence-based principles underpinning the importance of good structure in
enhancing learning:
The first is that understanding fundamentals makes a subject more comprehensible. (p. 23)
The second point relates to human memory. Perhaps the most basic thing that can be said
about human memory, after a century of extensive research, is that unless detail is placed in
a structured pattern, it is rapidly forgotten. (p. 24)
Third, understanding of fundamental principles and ideas…appears to be the main road to
adequate “transfer of training.” To understand something like a specific instance of a more
general case – which is what understanding of a more fundamental principle of structure
means – is to have learned not only a specific thing but also a model for understanding other
things like it one may encounter. (p. 25)
For example, in this chapter, the Core Principles of Learning constitute key con-
cepts and principles fundamental to understanding the structure of what constitutes
Good Teaching (e.g., effective, efficient, creative teaching). Once these are under-
stood, the more specific factual content relating to how they enhance aspects of
the learning process will become increasingly easier to accommodate into a solid
and meaningful mental model in long-term memory. Over time, with a thoughtful
application, the knowledge base becomes more refined, elaborated and practically
useful. In the wider context of this book, as the key structure becomes increasingly
understandable in terms of how to enhance the practice of teaching from an evidence-
based approach, the more abstract notion of ‘Pedagogic Literacy’ starts to become a
meaningful and useful proposition (he says, hopefully). Just as clear and meaningful
learning goals and advance organizers provide structure to what is to be learned,
this heuristic focuses our attention to the most appropriate selection of knowledge
32 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
components and their best organizational structuring and sequencing for facilitating
the learning experience to maximize attainment opportunities for learners. While the
mind has a natural tendency to organize information into meaning wholes, as Gestalt
psychology established in the early twentieth century (e.g., Koffka 1915; Köhler
1929), this is greatly aided and enhanced when there is a clear and logical structure
in the presentation of knowledge in the first place. Hattie and Yates (2014) pointed
out:
The mind does not relate well to unstructured data. We find it extremely taxing to learn
random lists or when coping with unrelated materials. We need to learn the organization,
structure, and meaning in whatever we learn. Meaningfulness, or relatedness, stems directly
from prior knowledge. We benefit enormously from being shown how to group information,
how to locate patterns, how to use order, and how to schematise and summarise. (p. 115)
Furthermore, it has long been recognized that different subject areas, by their very
nature, lend themselves to different teaching and learning approaches in terms of
effective student learning. For example, Shulman (1991) argues that teachers require
‘pedagogic content knowledge’, which is the ability to fully understand how their
specific disciplines are most effectively taught. This involves not only the identifi-
cation of core concepts and principles essential for building understanding but also
key areas where misconceptions and areas of difficulty are likely to be encountered
by students. In this way, the instructional strategy can be systematically tailored to
incorporate effective methods that are specifically contextualized to the nature of
the discipline and how practitioners in the field conduct their practices in real-world
contexts. The importance of applying not just pedagogical knowledge to the ways we
teach but also supplementing this with pedagogical content knowledge is captured
by Shulman when he argued:
When was the last time you saw a problem set in the study of Hamlet? Or in Asian History?
Can you have guided practice in a poem? Or for evolutionary theory? I would argue that we
have reflected in the differences among the disciples, different ways of knowing that are tied
to different ways of teaching. (p. 5)
It is essential, therefore, that teachers know their subjects especially well in order
to be able to identify the most appropriate method combinations to effectively teach
the key concepts and principles that are fundamental to understanding in the specific
context of their subject topic areas. In a similar vein, McTighe and Wiggins (1998)
refer to the importance of focusing content on the ‘big ideas’ and the ‘essential-
questions’ in making sense of the content knowledge and its importance within the
wider subject context. The big ideas relate to the more fundamental and enduring
understandings relating to a topic area, as they:
• Provide a conceptual “lens” for any student
• Provide a breadth of meaning by connecting and organizing many facts, concepts
and skills; serving as a lynchpin for understanding
• Point to key knowledge at the heart of the expert understanding of the subject
• Require “uncoverage” because its meaning or value is rarely obvious to the learner,
is counterintuitive or prone to misunderstanding
2.4 Core Principle 3: Content Is Organized Around Key Concepts … 33
• Have great transfer value; applying to many other inquiries and issues over time—
“horizontally (across subjects) and “vertically” (through the years in later courses)
in the curriculum and out of school. (p. 69)
Big ideas provide an excellent vehicle for helping students to understand both
the key structure of a topic area as well as its relevance to real-life contexts. As the
authors argue, they provide:
…a conceptual tool for sharpening thinking, connecting discrepant pieces of knowledge,
and equipping learners for transferable applications. (p. 70)
Essential questions are core to the subject and will stimulate thought, provoke
enquiry, and spark more questions relating to the essential core structure of the topic
area, further enhancing understanding. As the authors summarized:
The best questions point to and highlight the big ideas. They serve as doorways through
which learners explore the key concepts, themes, theories, issues, and problems that reside
within the content, perhaps as yet unseen: it is through the process of actively “interrogating”
the content through provocative questions that students deepen their understanding. (p. 106)
nearly did this with math but was fortunate to have a good teacher ‘to pull me out of
the pit’—so to speak, which made the difference.
There are some old sayings in this context, which provide easy to remember
analogies. We need to be able to:
“See the wood from the trees” and “Separate the wheat from the chaff”.
In summary, being able to identify the key concepts and principles of a subject
from the mass of tertiary information flying around is surely a core principle of
learning and must be a key heuristic in planning and facilitating instruction. Equally,
students need to be well informed and taught how to do this effectively. Brown’s
et al. (2014) reflection is worth ‘reflecting upon’:
Each of us has a large basket of resources in the form of aptitudes, prior knowledge, intel-
ligence, interests, and sense of personal empowerment that shape how we learn and how
we overcome our shortcomings, some of these differences matter a lot – for example, our
ability to extract underlying principles from new experiences and to convert new knowledge
into mental structures. Other differences we may think count for a lot, for example, having
a verbal or visual learning style, actually don’t. (p. 141)
In Chap. 1, I mentioned that thinking was not something I learned from my 15,000 h
at school. Well, my teachers can be easily forgiven, if Wagner’s (2010) conclusion
is correct:
In schools, critical thinking has long been a buzz phrase. Educators pay lip service to its
importance, but few can tell me what they mean by the phrase or how they teach and test
it… (p. 16)
For the most part, teachers haven’t been trained to teach students how to think. (xxiv)
There is often an assumption that thinking is simply common sense. Well, even if
it is, and I don’t think it is, it’s not that common. In most basic terms thinking is goal-
directed cognitive activity, which seems to occur not just at a conscious level (e.g., “I
just think this through”), but also subconsciously and unconsciously. The outcome
of good thinking is typically a heightened, or at least improved, understanding of
something. Certainly, thinking is essential to building understanding as it involves
the making of connections in the brain, and this is learning at the neural level.
However, thinking does not occur in isolation, but rather through connecting
and making sense of information, which ultimately (if successful) is retained as
neurologically useful mental models in long-term memory. As Willingham (2009)
summarized:
Thinking occurs when you combine information (from the environment and long-term
memory) in new ways…That combining happens in working memory. (p. 11)
2.5 Core Principle 4: Good Thinking Promotes the Building … 35
Hence, …we must ensure that students acquire background knowledge parallel with
practising critical thinking skills. (p. 22)
There is, despite differences in perspective and terminology in the literature, strong
agreement that thinking is crucial to the quality of human learning. As Paul (1993)
summarized:
The thought is the key to knowledge. Knowledge is discovered by thinking, analyzed by
thinking, organized by thinking, transformed by thinking, assessed by thinking, and, most
importantly, acquired by thinking. (vii)
Petty (2009) puts this into a very practical context when he argued that:
It is no exaggeration to say that almost every aspect of private and public life is driven by our
ability (or inability) to use these thinking skills effectively, and to ‘think straight’. (p. 325)
However, while good thinking may be beneficial in the learning stakes, there are
those who do not see the human mind as particularly well developed for such activity,
as Willingham (2009) concluded:
Humans don’t think very often because our brains are designed not for thought but for the
avoidance of thought. (p. 4)
On the other hand, the human brain for a significant proportion of the population
does not seem to want to do this too willingly. Kahneman (2012) provides a powerful
insight here, which has extensive implications in educational contexts and how we
teach. He argues that thinking can be conceptualized in terms of two systems; System
1 and System 2. These are, of course, metaphors, but they convey something that
instantly has strong face validity:
System 1 is a fast reflexive system that identifies the familiar, especially threaten-
ing elements in a situation and quickly activates automatic response patterns. This
system is the most essential for survival and is the default system. It typically works
well in familiar everyday life where most situations and problems are familiar, and
we have long-established patterned responses to them. However, this system also
36 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
The development of good thinking, then, has much in terms of similarity with
other desirable outcomes sought by people. For example, few people enjoy going
on a diet or working long hours of overtime. However, there is a benefit to weight
loss when obese and extra money is useful and often essential for some. The same
can be said for developing good thinking, as far as effective learning is concerned.
We clearly recognize the longer-term benefits, but the shorter-term cognitive strain
is often likely to short cut our perseverance to do this well in many situations.
If good thinking is hampered by it being a tiring activity and some of us have
‘lazy’ brains, this is further compounded by the impact of beliefs and emotions on
our capability for rational cognitive activity. Marcus (2009), from a cognitive neu-
roscience perspective, highlights how our belief systems further provide challenges
to the brain functioning as a good ‘thinking machine’:
Our beliefs are contaminated by the tricks of memory, by emotion, and by the vagaries of a
perceptual system that really ought to be fully separate – not to mention a logic and inference
system that is as yet, in the early twenty-first century, far from fully hatched. (p. 67)
It is therefore not that surprising that good thinking is more than just common
sense, or we may need to accept that common sense is a much rarer capability than
is typically assumed. However, despite the many barriers to good thinking, it can
be effectively modelled, understood, and improved. As Perkins (1995) pointed out,
“People can learn to think and act intelligently” (p. 18). In consequence, there is
little point in asking students to engage in good thinking if they have no accurate
and useful prior knowledge of what this means. In the absence of useful knowledge
in this area, as for any area of new learning, a whole host of misconceptions are
likely to come into play, and we know what this eventually leads to—a confused and
frustrated learner.
There are many models of thinking in the literature (e.g., Marzano 1988; Swartz
and Parks 1994; Perkins 1985) and the keen reader can find much of interest here.
2.5 Core Principle 4: Good Thinking Promotes the Building … 37
However, it is also full of different terms (e.g., ‘critical thinking’, ‘creative think-
ing’, ‘lateral thinking’, ‘analytical thinking’, ‘dialogical thinking’, ‘parallel think-
ing’, ‘design thinking’—even ‘thinking out of the box’) relating to thinking, that often
confuses rather than aids the development of good thinking in curriculum design and
practical teaching. Having spent many years researching this elusive human quality,
I have evolved a model of thinking (Sale 2014) based on extensive modelling of
how professionals, across a wide range of fields, actually solve problems in their
work contexts. It must be recognized at the outset that accurate conceptualization
of internal cognitive processes is inherently problematic and invariably unreliable,
especially across subject domains. However, without some valid practical frame on
what these elusive but desirable skills are, and how they work in terms of the wider
context of internal mental activity, there is little chance of the effective teaching and
assessment of them.
What this means, for example, is that while psychologists may solve problems in
some qualitatively different ways from engineers, both at the individual and collective
level, there is similarity in the types of cognitive activity involved. For example, they
will need to analyse situations (e.g., cases), make comparison and contrast with
similar past cases, build up inferences and interpretations from ongoing perceptions
and data accumulation, generate possible solutions and decide action based on chosen
criteria. Around this swirl of cognitive activity, there will be overall monitoring
of what is going on, typically referred to as metacognition. The summary model
is depicted in Fig. 2.1, and the typical cognitive heuristics involved are outlined
in Table 2.1. Note that the cognitive heuristics for each type of thinking are the
essential framing questions that are to be negotiated in making sense of information
and building understanding.
In this model, analysis, compare & contrast, inference & interpretation and evalua-
tion are typically employed during critical thinking; whereas generating possibilities,
as the term implies, is predominantly employed in creative thinking. However, it is
metacognition that is the higher executive capability and, as Flavell (1979) and Mar-
tinez (2006) maintain, critical thinking is subsumed under metacognition. Similarly,
Halpern (1998) argued that when people are engaged in critical thinking, they need to
use metacognitive skills to monitor the thinking process (e.g., checking the progress
being made towards a personal goal, ensuring accuracy, and making decisions relat-
ing to time and mental effort). Brown et al. (2014) is even more direct in this analysis
and argues that without metacognition, critical thinking is impossible to achieve.
Also, creative thinking is not simply a question of clicking on a creativity switch
in one’s brain. Such notions are both naïve and dangerous. While creative thinking
works along with different heuristics, it is closely linked to both critical thinking
and metacognition’s executive control. Without significant content knowledge in a
field (and preferably more than one field) and if metacognition and critical thinking
2.5 Core Principle 4: Good Thinking Promotes the Building … 39
are poor, little by way of creativity is likely to manifest itself in terms of real-world
usefulness.
Furthermore, and in the context of the above framing of how the mind works
(or doesn’t work), some writers see ‘good thinking’ not just in terms of cognitive
processes and heuristics’ but also in terms of the development of intellectual traits and
standards. However, while definitions of critical thinking vary in the literature, most
share a consensus that it involves certain dispositional factors as well as the cognitive
heuristics outlined above. For example, Paul et al. (2006) identify the following traits
as central to acquiring a high level of expertise in critical thinking:
• Intellectual humility—sensitivity to one’s own biases and the limitations of
knowing
• Intellectual courage—prepared to question own beliefs and those of others, even
if unpopular with dominant perspectives and people
• Intellectual empathy—awareness of the need to actively entertain different views
from one’s own
• Intellectual integrity—holding oneself to the same intellectual standards of others
(no double standards)
• Intellectual perseverance—working through intellectual complexities despite
frustration
• Confidence in reason—recognizing that humankind’s interests are best served by
giving free play to reason
• Intellectual autonomy—thinking for oneself in relation to standards of rationality
and not uncritically accepting the judgements of others
• Fair-mindedness—conscious of the need to treat all viewpoints alike and not be
influenced by vested interests.
Such dispositions are certainly desirable, but the extent to which they are inte-
gral to deep-seated personality traits and trainable by pedagogic interventions is
questionable and will be explored further in the following chapters.
In summary, these types of thinking run as overlapping and intertwined neural
programmes, moving from foreground to background as the focus of framing a
problem changes and new questions emerge. Certainly, when creativity is sought,
generating possibilities is at the mind’s forefront, but other types of thinking will
weave in and out of consciousness and, typically run continuously in the subconscious
mind. However, the good thinker will periodically take a conscious metacognitive
view and attempt to make sense of (understand) what is actually going on in his/her
mind, check various aspects of cognitive and affective processes (e.g., the types of
thinking; impact of beliefs and emotions) and adjust when necessary. Good thinking
can, therefore, be framed as the ability to navigate this ‘perpetual cognitive and
affective swirl’, and to be able to employ the various heuristics of these types of
thinking in a fluid, effective, efficient and highly synergistic manner. This is perhaps
the reason that good thinking is quite rare in many situations, and why we really need
to teach it to our students.
40 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
When I first arrived in Singapore, I took an instant liking for a local delicacy, ‘chilli
crab’—you must try it if you come to Singapore. In fact, I had this, and other local
dishes, almost every night. Indeed, on one occasion, I remember an elderly Chinese
lady at the local hawker centre (that’s a Singaporean term for food court) saying to
me, “Why you always have chilli crab, lah, why not spring roll.”—or something like
that. Well, the answer at that point in time was easy, “I like chilli crab.” However, one
night, and it was inevitable in retrospect, the chilli crab was served up in its typical
form, but my response was not the usual positive one. Suddenly, its appeal seemed
to have vanished completely. The chilli crab was no different, but my perception had
somehow changed and with this, my whole orientation to it was different. Invariably,
based on my East London values, I ate it; after all, it’s not right to leave good food—
a punishable offence by parents in my younger years, if caught. Quite simply, in
psychological terms, I had become habituated to chilli crab and its appeal had greatly
diminished. In most basic terms, I had become bored with it. Sadly, as humans, we
have an inherent tendency for this to happen, even for things we really like.
When asked what is the best teaching method to use and why, I tend to recite
a variant of the chilli crab story as an advance organizer. Yes, some methods are
more effective than others, but the overuse of any one method will create habituation
and students will get bored. I can recall academic faculty at a previous educational
institution attending a workshop on Project-based Learning. Many came back excited
and wanting to use it in their teaching. Well, imagine the students on a Monday
morning, when for the first time they get to choose aspects of their learning and
be more actively involved in the learning process, it was a novelty. However, after
2–3 weeks of such activity, when they have amassed several projects, the enthusiasm
for such pedagogy had long receded. Too much of the same thing gets boring, and
as Willingham (2009) concluded, “Change grabs attention, as you no doubt know”
(p. 17). This is often why we go on holiday—even though it often ends up stressful,
especially when taking young children who keep saying, “I wish we could go home”.
If the Gods struggle in vain, what chance for us mere mortals with this existential
nemesis?
2.6 Core Principle 5: Instructional Methods and Presentation … 41
Optimizing your learning experiences for attention is the first step towards optimizing it for
long-term memory and higher thought processes. (p. 87)
Mental activity is stimulated through our five senses, with the visual sense prob-
ably the most dominant. The relative dominance of our vision system may well be
the result of our evolution, as Mlodinow (2012) captures so interestingly:
…an animal that sees better eats better and avoids danger better, and hence lives longer. As
a result, evolution has arranged it so that about a third of your brain is devoted to processing
vision. (p. 35)
In many situations the greater the combination of our senses that are appropriately
stimulated in a planned learning event, the more potentially effective the experience
is likely to be in terms of gaining better attention and facilitating the desired learn-
ing outcomes. For example, it is estimated that when we see and hear something,
this doubles the sensory impact as compared to just hearing it. Direct experience
will increase the impact further and, teaching it, will enhance it further still. This
should not be surprising as the act of teaching, if conducted properly, will involve
much by way of preparation. Most specifically, it will involve developing a strong
understanding of the key content areas, especially those concepts and principles that
are fundamental to understanding the key structure of the topic area. It will also
involve identifying areas of potential difficulty and where the main misconceptions
are likely to be experienced by learners. Finally, there will a systematic structuring
and sequencing of how best to present this content in the most effective and effi-
cient method combination. In my experience, by assessing how well someone has
learned takes this process even further. When assessing students, one must firstly be
able to validly ascertain what they have learned and to what extent the key learning
components (e.g., the desired learning outcomes) have been met in the performance
evidence to ensure accurate judgement of performance. In making assessment deci-
sions, especially of a summative nature (e.g., when one is making a final assessment
decision or ascribing a grade), the assessor is claiming to know learners in some fun-
damental way that often has a significant impact on their access to future educational
channels and employment opportunities. Secondly, as assessment (formative) is a
key aspect of the learning processes, this requires assessors to accurately diagnose
students specific areas of weakness and then provide tailored feedback to help them
strategize effective future learning strategies.
Pedagogically there is logic in providing or enabling students to engage in real-
world tasks that they find meaningful and interesting, that gets them highly engaged
(i.e., behaviourally, emotionally, cognitively and agentically), proactively thinking
and asking questions—and assessing their learning collaboratively. This is a good
instructional approach. However, its rarely the reality in many classrooms, and cer-
tainly not for all students. The foibles of the human condition make this so. Many
teachers often bemoan the lack of student engagement and interest and feel it’s often
their fault. In the case of some teachers, this may be true, to varying degrees, but
teaching is not easy, and expecting full attention and engagement is wishful thinking.
One can only do one’s professional best. Remember teaching is heuristic not algo-
rithmic. Water always boils at 100 °C—correct? Well, not exactly, your elevation
2.6 Core Principle 5: Instructional Methods and Presentation … 43
relative to sea level can affect the temperature at which water boils, due to differ-
ences in air pressure. Hence, even algorithms don’t always behave 100% and don’t
worry, we won’t go on to discuss string theory. What we can say is that on a bad
day, students may not give much attention, no matter how well you teach. So many
factors can affect student behaviour, such as personal circumstances, moods, time of
day, etc., and if these negatives conflate, the best-planned and delivered lesson may
go nowhere useful. Just as many patients do not take ‘good’ medical advice, so many
students may not see the point in learning what is being taught—despite your best
attention and good teaching.
In terms of ascertaining the various ways in which teaching and learning arrange-
ments impact the senses, Edgar Dales’ famous ‘Cone of Learning’ (Fig. 2.2) is often
shown to illustrate how different senses and activities affect the learning process.
The percentages have a limited empirical base and are quite arbitrary; however, it
provides a generalized illustration of how different combinations of sensory input
may affect the type and quality of learning.
The use of audio-visual aids is common practice in seeking to enhance student
learning through different sensory modalities, and it is certainly the case that the
human mind responds positively to multimedia (Hattie and Yates 2014). The cinema,
of course, exploits this to its fullest impact. Our brain is set up well to integrate
information from different source inputs, especially from different modalities. Strong
learning occurs when words and images are combined, and these effects become
especially strong when the words and images are made meaningful through accessing
prior knowledge. Good visual representations work because:
• Recall is almost always visually triggered; hence visual representation acts as a
cue triggering the full memory system
Table 2.2 Key principles of good instructional design for audio-visual presentations
Key principles of good instructional design for audio-visual presentations
Five principles for reducing extraneous process
• Coherence (reduce extraneous words and pictures)
• Signalling (highlight essential words and pictures)
• Redundancy (do not add onscreen text to narrated graphics)
• Spatial contiguity (present printed words near corresponding graphics)
• Temporal contiguity (present corresponding words and graphics simultaneously)
Three principles for managing essential processing
• Segmenting (break down instruction into learner-paced segments)
• Pre-training (Provide pre-training in names and characteristics of each main concept)
• Modality (use spoken words for visualization rather than text)
Two principles for fostering generative processing
• Personalization (put words in conversational style)
• Voice (use friendly human voice for speaking words)
• Only structured information can go into Long-Term Memory, so this helps the
transmission from Working Memory into Long Term Memory and subsequent
recall
• They facilitate the ability of learners to see the relationship of a whole to its various
parts, which fosters understanding.
However, it is important not to over-use audio-visual aids or to create too much
variation in modes and mediums of presentation. I have seen many teachers using
audio-visual aids and varied presentation format, all with good intentions to enhance
the learning experience, but only to create confusion for students. There is now
much evidence-based research on how best to present visual material to facilitate
effective learning. For example, Mayer and Alexander (2011) summarized essential
key principles that specifically impact the effectiveness of multimedia on learning
(see Table 2.2).
As Mayer makes clear:
These practical implications are examples of evidence-based practice – basing instructional
methods on research evidence rather than on conventional wisdom, opinion, speculation,
fads, or doctrine. (p. 441)
This heuristic is not difficult to understand in terms of how it can enhance student
attention and attainment as it has strong face validity. For example, we have all
both experienced boredom and how it affects our attention and disrupts learning,
as well as being stimulated by high impact multi-media movies. I remember being
amazed by the film ‘Avatar’ because of the multi-media effects, even though the
story had some ridiculous concepts such as helicopter gunships, resembling what
are used today, on a planet in another solar system many light years away—really?
However, today’s multi-media and internet-rich resource pool is a double-edged
sword. On the one hand, it offers the creative teacher much in the way of capability
for building networks of integrated resources, differentiating the learning experience
and creating instructional strategies that provide better attentional, engaging and
2.6 Core Principle 5: Instructional Methods and Presentation … 45
Human memory is a little bit like having a Maserati sports car, but only being allowed
to use the first gear, except on special occasions. A Maserati will hit a top speed of 185
miles per hour, but certainly not in first gear. Our memory has two main systems, long-
term memory (LTM) and working memory (WM). These are depicted in Fig. 2.3. Our
LTM seems to have unlimited storage capability. It’s not that our brain gets bigger
as we learn more; rather it becomes denser in terms of neural connectedness, though
we can never live long enough to test its full capability. However, before information
can be stored in LTM, it must first pass through WM, which has limited immediate
capability when processing new information. The ‘magic’ 7 (able to process around
7 plus or minus 2 bits of information at one go) was originally documented by
Miller (1956), for what was then referred to as short term memory. However, more
recent research (e.g., Van Merrienboer and Sweller 2005) suggests that in everyday
situational use, this tends to be only 2–4 elements at a time. WM also needs quick
rehearsal for information to be effectively captured and processed, otherwise it is
typically lost (forgotten) after only a few seconds. The limited capacity of working
memory poses problems for learning, as Clark and Lyons (2005) point out:
…it is in working memory that active mental work, including learning, takes place. Working
memory is the site of conscious thought and processing. (p. 48)
Similarly, Ormrod (2011) summarizes the importance of this key memory system:
Working memory is the component of our memory system in which we hold attended-to
information for a short time while we try to make sense of it. More generally, it’s where
our thinking occurs. For example, working memory is where we think about the content of
a lecture, try to decipher a confusing textbook passage, or solve a problem. Whatever our
consciousness is, this is probably where it is housed. (p. 55)
Willingham (2009) further emphasizes that what we do with the information when
in WM, has implications for what we end up with in LTM—for better or for worse:
For material to be learned (that is, to end up in long-term memory) it must reside for some
period in working memory – that is, a student must pay attention to it. Further, how the student
thinks of the experience completely determines what will end up in long-term memory. (p. 49)
His descriptions of the internal mental processing involved demonstrate the cru-
cial dynamic relationship of thinking and knowledge in building understanding, If
knowledge is limited and thinking is poor, what we end up with in LTM will be of lit-
tle or no value. Hence, to develop well-structured, comprehensive, and useful mental
models of what we are learning, neurologically well-wired and cemented in LTM,
there is a need for the right knowledge (e.g., key concepts and principles) and good
thinking to be occurring in WM, and ongoing checking of what’s ‘building up’ in
LTM. To reinforce this process, Brown’s et al. (2014) summary is spot-on:
To be useful, learning requires memory, so what we have learned is still there when we need
it. (p. 2)
Practice at retrieving new knowledge or skill from memory is a potent tool for learning and
durable retention. (p. 43)
You may also remember from Core Principle 4 that the human mind is, for many
of us, inherently lazy in that System 2 thinking typically is draining on our cognitive
resources and results in what is often referred to as ‘ego-depletion’ (Kahneman 2012).
Quite simply, excessive cognitive activity, like excessive physical activity, is not the
norm for most people—one must choose to develop these capabilities. It is also the
case that, at a conscious processing level, the brain is relatively slow as a processing
system, especially when compared to computer technology. If you have any doubt,
do this simple exercise:
2.7 Core Principle 6: Learning Design Takes into Account … 47
However, the picture is not as bleak as it seems, as there are ways in which we
can use our memory akin to driving the Maserati in 4th gear. This becomes possible,
even easy, once we have acquired vast knowledge, understanding and expertise in a
specific field. Such capability is fully encoded as highly integrated neural networks
(e.g., cognitive schemata) in LTM. WM has no limitations when dealing with such
information retrieved from LTM, as it dramatically alters the functionality of what is
taking place within the memory systems. The two systems effectively merge into one
fluent dynamic entity working towards meeting the conscious goal of desired infor-
mation retrieval and solving the problem in hand. Furthermore, over time, this process
becomes increasingly automated, and as Hattie and Yates (2014) summarized:
When your knowledge becomes so automatic that you can access it quickly, with virtually
no effort, then the WM system is said to be bypassed through the automaticity stage – a most
desirable place to be. (p. 147)
This enhanced memory capability explains why a person very fluent in a language
can always find the words they want to use and assemble them in complex sentences
instantaneously. Contrast this with the novice trying to learn the days of the week in
a new language. It took me more than an hour to learn (as in encoding sufficiently
in LTM for later effective retrieval) the days of the week in Mandarin, and that was
quite good.
It has been popular in educational circles to downplay the importance of rote
learning and memorization. After all, we want flexible adaptive and creative thinkers
today—right? Yes, but such high-level human capability is largely based on what
we have already acquired in our LTM system. Quite simply, if there is not much
information there, and it’s not particularly well organized and connected, there is little
chance of creative or even useful outcomes. This could not have been levelled at the
neural arrangements of Einstein or Da Vinci, and it may have been a definitive factor
in their genius capabilities. It is not surprising that Kircher et al (2006) concluded
that:
…long term memory is now viewed as the central dominant structure of human cognition.
Everything we see, hear and think about is critically dependent on and influenced by our
long-term memory. (pp. 3–4)
48 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
Research clearly shows that a major factor that differentiates experts from novices
is that expert problem-solvers can draw on the vast knowledge bases in their LTM
and quickly select the best approach and procedures for solving a given problem. As
Kircher et al. further point out:
We are skillful in an area because our long-term memory contains huge amounts of informa-
tion concerning that area. That information permits us to quickly recognize the characteristics
of a situation and indicates to us, often unconsciously, what to do and how to do it. (p. 4)
This essentially means that the more you have effectively learned and appropri-
ately organized in LTM makes subsequent learning in that area or field more effective.
As Willingham (2009) noted:
…having factual knowledge in long term memory makes it easier to acquire still more factual
knowledge. (p. 34)
One of the main factors that contribute to successful thought is the amount and quality of
the information in long term memory. (p. 17)
This goes very much against the prevalent view among many educationalists that
we should not be encouraging rote learning but instead focusing on building under-
standing through the development of thinking. As documented earlier, understanding
is important, and the development of good thinking is essential to achieving this.
However, this is a bit like having a Maserati, knowing how to drive it, but not having
any petrol to put in the tank. Csikszentmihalyi (1990) was correct in arguing that, “it
is a mistake to assume that creativity and rote learning are incompatible” (p. 123).
Memory and thinking are equally important in the development of understanding,
share interdependent functionality in the learning stakes and there may be little point
in viewing them as distinctly different processes. It is the construction of elaborate
mental schemas in LTM that provides the conscious mind, operating in working
memory, with room to think when solving problems. Repetition and review are vehi-
cles enabling knowledge to be stored in reliable retrievable units which, over time,
accelerate mental growth through conceptual mastery and deeper understanding. As
Willingham (2009) argued:
As far as anybody knows, the only way to develop mental facility is to repeat the target
process, again, again. (p. 87)
a couple of hours had the reactor working as it should. Later she billed the company
$20,000. The company, not challenging the cost, given the alternative scenario, did
ask the consultant engineer for a breakdown of the bill. The reply went something
like this, “$1000 for the call out, $19,000 for what’s in my head”.
This heuristic has many implications for how we teach. Perhaps most apparent
is the need to chunk up information into manageable learning structures to prevent
cognitive overload on WM. Also, to take account of students’ prior knowledge, as
its level of completeness, integration and ease of access for retrieval will impact our
pace and focus when teaching. For example, students with limited prior knowledge,
when presented with new information in that area, may be especially vulnerable to
cognitive overload. In this situation, they will struggle to process it meaningfully,
feel confused, and fail to assimilate it meaningfully in LTM. Cognitive load has been
distinguished in terms of two main interrelated components: intrinsic cognitive load
and extraneous cognitive load (e.g., Van Merrienboer and Sweller 2005). Essentially
intrinsic cognitive load is related to the task complexity itself and the ability of
WM to deal with it. For the novice, a complex learning task will create cognitive
overload, simply trying to make sense of it. Extraneous cognitive load refers to
introducing information into the learning situation that is not relevant to the learning
(e.g., unnecessary text, graphic or colour change) or being poorly organized. This
can be significantly reduced by good instruction design. As the authors emphasize,
“There is no substitute for evidence-based instructional design” (p. 173). In contrast
to the novice learner, when teaching students who have a high level of knowledge
and expertise in an area, we can present information much quicker and in more
elaborated forms, as they already have highly developed mental schemata in that
knowledge field. In terms of analogy, therefore, I can read (and usually make good
sense of) several psychology journals in a day but cannot retrieve the television
picture when my dog sits on the remote-control device and scrambles the channels.
Students need time to rehearse new information in their minds and consolidate
to existing mental schemata, which is facilitated through application activities that
generate appropriate types of thinking (e.g., analysis, comparison, inference & inter-
pretation, and evaluation), as this facilitates understanding. The wise teacher will
provide this structure for students and adjust the pace of instruction accordingly.
Consolidation of learned material in LTM is further reinforced through providing
systematic reviews stimulating the retrieval of key information from LTM and bring-
ing it to conscious attention in WM, as illustrated in Fig. 2.4. Students and the teacher
can then do a quality check on what has been learned, remediate lost elements, clar-
ify overall understanding, as well as reinforce desired learning. This very act of
conscious retrieval from LTM to WM fires related neural structures, which result in
the secretion of myelin, an enzyme-based substance that forms an insulating sheath
around the axon in a neuron. In basic terms, this further strengthens the learning
bond in LTM. Talking to oneself, when memorizing for an exam, if it is about the
‘right stuff’, is far from madness; it is a good learning strategy.
Another aspect of how memory systems work, which has important implica-
tions for the design of learning and teaching practices, concerns how information
is selected when presented to learners. It is well documented that apart from the
50 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
Fig. 2.5 Serial position curve, incorporating the Von Restorff effect
can trick the brain into paying much more attention than it would customarily give
over a given time duration. Finally, the creative application of a Von Restorff effect,
will put the ‘icing on the cake’, metaphorically speaking.
Many teachers have long recognized the importance of presenting information in
manageable chunks and then structuring activities that give students time to make
sense (digesting) of it through discussion and/or other forms of application. Over
time they do periodic recap and review to increase the chances of effective transfer
and retention in LTM, as well as remediate gaps in learning. We are developing a
more precise science that underpins how this works and can now confidently predict
that when utilized thoughtfully in practice, there is likely to be significant gains in
student attainment, as well as better engagement. We can all remember the teachers
who bored us. Several behaviours can contribute to boredom in the classroom; many
are violations of memory processing. Teachers who consistently use practices that
conflict with the workings of the human memory system will experience frustration
with the gaps in many students learning, lack of attention and engagement, and
possibly acts of indiscipline. The frustration and consequences will invariably be
greater for the students themselves.
Most Saturday mornings I usually go into the gym at my apartment block and do
around 30 min of weight training; apparently, this is a good exercise regime. Whilst
doing my exercise programme, I occasionally look out of the window at people
52 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
playing tennis. I have noted that several players never seem to get any better even
after a few years. They play the same novice game every week. They are unable to
serve with any consistency, do not adopt proper body positions when striking the
ball and don’t even seem to focus attention on the ball when they hit it, and I’m not
a professional tennis coach.
The notion that learning inevitably improves over time and that experience is
central to improvement is highly questionable. Yes, time on task is important and
so is experience. However, it is more about what is done when on a task that really
makes a big difference in the experience. For example, why is it that some people, who
have many years of experience, still display limited competence, whereas relative
newcomers achieve good competence in a comparatively short time? The conclusion
of Berliner (1987) offers insight into such questions:
…experience will probably only instruct those who have the motivation to excel in what they
do and the metacognitive skills to learn from their experience…we believe that individuals
with that kind of motivation to learn and in possession of a set of strategies for learning from
experience are literally transformed by their experience. (p. 61)
It is certainly the case that motivation is a key factor in effective learning. However,
it’s also about being clear about learning goals and having the strategies and resources
to achieve them. Furthermore, in skill development, one must put in a lot of actual
doing, and much of this is what is typically called the practice. It seems obvious to
assume that practice is important. I like the quote from the legendary golfer Gary
Player, who once said, “The more I practice, the luckier I get.” However, from
research, we are becoming aware that it is not just practice per se that facilities
improvement but, more importantly, how it is structured, and the way feedback is
utilized. Colvin (2008) noted that exceptional performers were not necessarily the
most talented in terms of their earlier biographies but had certain attributes and
practices that distinguished their expertise over time. Of most importance was what
is now referred to as Deliberate Practice. According to Colvin, Deliberate Practice
is characterized by many interrelated key elements, which include:
• The activity (practice) is carefully designed to improve specific aspects of the
performance, often with a teacher’s help
• It requires much repetition
• Feedback on results is continually available
• It is highly demanding mentally (whether a physical or mental task)
• It isn’t much fun (in the main; but maybe for some).
If we analyse these components of deliberate practice it becomes apparent what
makes Deliberate Practice so important in the development of competence and
eventually expertise.
Firstly, deliberate practice fully utilizes the learning affordances of Core Prin-
ciple 1: Learning goals, objectives and proficiency expectations are clearly visible
to learners. Hence, there needs to be a desired goal that is both challenging and
achievable, with clear expectations of what is involved. It must also be highly con-
textualized to the individual who is to invest the necessary effort required to meeting
2.8 Core Principle 7: The Development of Expertise … 53
the goal. If learners take on unrealistic goals, this invites unnecessary failure, frus-
tration, and this can undermine confidence and belief in one’s potential. Equally, if
it is too easy to achieve, then we are dumbing down a person’s potential and creating
false confidence, who when faced with challenging tasks, as will inevitably happen
at some future time, will not well prepared for this. Hence, this process takes both
openness and good two-way feedback to frame the right goals for the right person at
the right time.
Secondly, it requires repetition, and this is often where perhaps the difficult bit
part of practice comes into play. Repetition is so important for building both clarity
of understanding and skill acquisition at the neural level. To build a strong mental
model in the brain at the neural level requires much rehearsal and retrieval activity in
and out of LTM to WM. This is generally referred to as Retrieval Practice. In most
basic terms learning at the level of the brain, involves the development of integrated
neural structures. As Lang (2016) describes:
Neurons form new connections with one another with every new experience we have: new
sensations, new thoughts, new actions. As the neurons are connecting to one another in
novel ways, growing and strengthening new connections, they are forming networks. The
first-time neurons link up in a new way, that connection is a temporary or fleeting one; if
that connection is used again (because we repeat the thought, or recreate an experience), the
link strengthens. The more times the pathway is used, the stronger the connection. The more
times the pathway is used, the stronger the connection. Neurons that fire together, goes the
saying in this corner of the biological world, wire together.
…we learn when our brains form new neural networks or modify existing ones as a result
of our experiences; this means that quite literally, learning requires the continual formation
of new connections between our neurons. (p. 95)
Retrieval practice then facilities this process of building stronger and better neural
networks, which results in a deeper and deeper understanding of that area of learning.
Hence, if you are doing plenty of retrieval practice as you read through the various
sectors of this book, you will be linking together the content knowledge in the various
areas, as well as the key concepts and principles, (hopefully in multiple ways) and, as
a result, will be building understanding. This will be growing neurologically in your
brain, and as you continue the retrieval practice, maintain interest and thinking about
how this applies in different contexts and situations, the conceptual understanding
will grow, getting deeper, until you have that total ‘aha’ feeling—often referred to
as the Eureka Effect. Every time you do a retrieval practice activity from LTM into
WM, these neural pathways will fire and when they go back to LTM the connections
get stronger. There are biochemical processes involved in such activity, which the
interested reader can research. From a pedagogical perspective, we don’t need to
know the neuroscience—but we do need to understand the cognitive behaviour and
ensure that our teaching practice includes the necessary opportunities for retrieval
practice. Hence, Brown et al. (2014), in summarizing this process concludes:
…while the brain is not a muscle that gets stronger with exercise, the neural pathways that
make up a body of learning do get stronger when the memory is retrieved, and the learning
practised. Periodic practice arrests forgetting, strengthens retrieval routes, and is essential
for hanging on to the knowledge you want to gain. (pp. 3–4)
54 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
I explicitly teach students how this works and why they should do it. In fact, I teach
students all the core principles of learning, of course, customized to their context. If
we want students to learn well, we should teach them how learning occurs and how
they can make this part of their own self-regulation. If we are seeking to develop self-
directed learners and we want them to become increasing agentic in their learning
(e.g., be proactive, seek out resources and develop their own personalized learning
strategies) this is surely a no-brainer—no pun intended. In practical terms, the more
students do retrieval practice, the better they learn and retain that knowledge gained.
Furthermore, as Brown et al. (2014) points out:
Testing doesn’t need to be initiated by the instructor. Students can practice retrieval anywhere;
no quizzes in the classroom are necessary. (p. 44)
Another consideration in doing practice concerns how often you do it and for how
long. For example, we may do our practice every hour, every-day, every week—or
whatever; this is typically referred to as Spaced Practice. Also, there is the question
of how long should we do our practice sessions? For example, we may do it for a
designated amount of time or until we are exhausted or fed up—or both. Doing long
practice sessions is usually referred to as Massed Practice. We can, of course, do
both. In the movie Desperado, the main male actor, Antonio Banderas, who apart
from other things, was a guitarist who in one scene was asked by a young boy in the
street how to get good at playing this instrument. The actor replied with the statement
“practice all day, every day”. Well, that combines spaced practice and massed practice
into continuous practice—is this the best option? The answer is, of course, no, it’s
not possible to do that—there is a limit physiologically and psychologically to the
amount of massed practice one could put in, though this would vary depending on
the activity and person.
However, research shows that both types of practice, when done effectively in con-
text, can enhance learning, but Spaced Practice has been shown to be most beneficial
for learning. As Lang (2016) summarized:
Research has demonstrated the power of spaced learning. Carey wrote, “nothing in learn-
ing science comes close in terms of immediate, significant, and reliable improvements to
learning”. (p. 76)
In addition, when done effectively and is ongoing (as and when needed) is another
core principle of learning and will be examined in some detail in the next section. For
now, let’s see feedback as a two-way communication process between the learner
and others who can contribute to his/her learning in some useful way. This would
certainly include the teacher/coach, but also peers and any other persons who can
2.8 Core Principle 7: The Development of Expertise … 55
contribute input for an individual learner. In the feedback process, irrespective of who
the others are, the learner must go through the retrieval practice process, whether
it is pulling out knowledge from LTM or displaying skill and engaging in dialogue
as to its development in terms of the desired goal. Essentially, this is a process
of testing (not summative assessment), but formative assessment (assessment for
learning). The purpose of retrieving knowledge from memory (or displaying one’s
skill in a visible performance) is that the learning becomes visible to both learner and
others (especially for the expert teacher/coach). This has two important benefits for
future learning. Firstly, it provides direct performance evidence on what the learner
knows, doesn’t know, and any fuzzy areas or misconceptions. It is then possible,
through mediation between the teacher (or other), to identify where the learner is
now in terms of meeting designated goal(s), what is needed to move the learner
progressively forward, and how best to do this (e.g., enhance the learning strategy in
specific ways). Remember, every time students do these retrieval practice activities,
they are not only going through the testing processes with feedback but also further
cementing the learning neurologically in the brain, which makes it easier and easier
to recall in future testing activities.
The pedagogic implications of this are significant. For the purposes of teaching,
the use of performance tasks that directly test the key concepts and skills involved
will facilitate the learning process, especially with supportive feedback. Hence, in
terms of the core principles covered so far, clear goals, knowing what students already
know, focusing on key concepts, getting students to think, managing cognitive load,
and doing deliberate practice is offering a potentially high impact instructional design
framework. There’s more to come, so we’ll stay with the retrieval practice concept,
and re-fire those neurons in spaced practice—seems like a well-reasoned approach.
Research also strongly supports the use of what is referred to as Mastery Learn-
ing. For example, Bloom (1985) in evaluating optimal conditions for learning,
summarized its impact on learning:
One example of such conditions is mastery learning where the students are helped to master
each learning unit before proceeding to a more advanced learning task. In general, the
average student taught under mastery-learning procedures achieves at a level of above 85%
of students taught under conventional instructional conditions. (p. 4)
It is important that there are clear and realistic improvement targets for the partic-
ular learner. This involves stretching the individual beyond an existing performance
56 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
level to a recognizably improved level in some aspect but a level that is achiev-
able with effort and coaching from a teacher. As outlined in Core Principle 1, it is
important to have as much clarity—visibility—of the learning goal, objectives and
proficiency level as possible. In this way, motivation is maintained as the learner
will have a perceived experience of a higher mastery in at least some aspect of the
performance, which further reinforces the belief and sustains effort in continuing this
learning strategy. To reiterate the point, “nothing breeds success like success”. It is
often noted in professional sport that when a player finally wins that elusive major
tournament, more seem to quickly follow. Andy Murray winning the men’s tennis
tournament at the Olympics, the US Open and Wimbledon, is perhaps an illustrative
recent example. Prior to that, he had failed to win a major tournament, losing in 4
finals.
Of key importance is the role played by expert teachers in helping the learner
identify what specific aspects of the performance to improve, structure the practice
programme accordingly and provide ongoing quality feedback to maintain focus on
the skill development. Again, to use the Andy Murray example, the appointment
of Ivan Lendl in this role may have been more than coincidental in his attainment
of two major titles within one year. Lendl himself had gone through the experience
of losing his first 4 finals in major tournaments but eventually went on to win 8
singles titles in such events. Certainly, he had learned something important and this
may have helped in coaching Andy Murray. It seems that even the very best in the
world still desire and need an expert teacher. It is necessary to emphasize that while
the deliberate practice is fundamental to effective and efficient learning, it is not a
shortcut to expertise or even competence (however defined).
What is particularly interesting is that in the process of developing expertise, not
only is there an enhancement in understanding and skill, but significant changes in
neurology and sensory acuity relating to the field of expertise. Many years of inten-
sive deliberate practice changes the body and the brain, enabling great performers
to perceive more, to know more and to remember more than most people. Colvin
particularly noted the following key attributes of great performers:
They all possess large, highly developed, intricate mental models of their domains. (p. 122)
…observe themselves closely… monitor what is happening in their own minds and ask how
it’s going. Researchers call this metacognition …top performers do this more systematically
than others do; it’s an established part of their routine. (p. 118)
As you become skilled in a task, its demands for energy diminishes. Studies of the brain
have shown that the pattern of activity associated with action changes as skill increases, with
fewer brain regions involved. (p. 35)
Hattie (2012) from extensive research supports this heightened capability of expert
teachers as well as providing insight into how they are likely to be more creative:
Experts possess knowledge that is more integrated, in that they combine the introduction
of new subject knowledge with students’ prior knowledge; they can relate current lesson
content to other subjects in the curriculum; and make lessons uniquely their own by chang-
ing, combining, and adding to the lessons according to their students’ needs and their own
teaching goals. (p. 28)
This heuristic focuses attention on the important role of deliberate practice in skill
development and attainment. From an evidence-based perspective, we are now able to
be much more precise and specific in terms of what types of practice and how best to
structure and manage practice to enhance attainment. The saying that “practice makes
perfect” is not quite right, though well-intended. Simply getting students to practice
and spend more time on task may have limited value in optimizing competence and
expertise without the systematic structuring of the practice activity, calibrated to the
learner’s proficiency level, and with expert feedback. Practice on its own may simply
lead to consistent proficiency at not doing an activity well, as Berliner noted above,
and I observe from the gym window. It is through deliberate practice over time that
is most likely to lead to higher proficiency levels and eventually expertise. However,
deliberate practice is very much intertwined with the building of dense and well-
integrated mental schemata in LTM and the ability to use metacognitive capabilities
at heightened levels. As emphasized earlier, each Core Principle of Learning, while
focusing on a specific aspect of the learning process, is ultimately part of a dynamic
and synergistic system in which specific areas of learning capability become mutually
supporting in enhancing human learning (e.g., attainment, engagement and well-
being).
In applying this core principle of learning in practical teaching it is important to
ensure that the process of using deliberate practice is adhered to as best as is practi-
cally feasible. Invariably, in working with large classes it is harder to be as precise
in diagnosis, task structuring and providing the time for ongoing feedback, as in the
case of purely individualized coaching. However, by making the process of deliber-
ate practise visible and meaningful to students, it is possible with some thoughtful
58 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
At school, I don’t recall the word ‘assessment’ being used, and certainly not ‘learn-
ing outcomes’ or ‘feedback’. We had to sit end of year exams and we were given
homework each week, which was marked by teachers. On receiving homework back,
we got a graded mark often with a ‘+’ or ‘−’ sign next to it, and a short comment
such as, “fairly good”, “could do better”, etc. I also never recall giving this much
thought in terms of what I might have done well and what I had not done well, and
certainly not what I needed to do in order to improve and how. It was done and
out of the way and that was that. I attach no blame to the teachers as that was the
assessment practice in that context. The assessment was largely seen in terms of
summative grading and not as a key facilitating aspect of the learning process. The
question, in the present context, is what do we now know about assessment practices
that are evidence-based in terms of providing an important heuristic for significantly
improving student learning and attainment?
Firstly, it is now clearly recognized that assessment is not simply a means to
measure learning that has already occurred but a major facilitator in the learning
process itself. As Boud (1988) illustrated:
There have been a number of notable studies over the years which have demonstrated that
assessment methods and requirements probably have a greater influence on how and what
students learn than any other single factor. This influence may well be of greater significance
than the impact of teaching or learning materials. (p. 35)
self-directed lifelong learners will mean little when the marks on test papers suggest
otherwise.
Assessment serves many purposes for different stakeholders (e.g., selection, main-
taining standards, identifying and diagnosing learning difficulties, and enhancing
teaching). Most significant, in this context, is the important role that formative assess-
ment (e.g. where learning is focused on supporting the learning process) plays in
influencing student attainment and engagement, especially through the process of
ongoing two-way feedback. This contrasts with summative assessment (e.g., where
a terminal assessment decision is made and the learner either passes or fails or is
graded accordingly). The high impact of feedback on attainment (e.g., the average
effect size of 0.79, which is twice the average effect of all other schooling effects)
is well documented by Hattie (2009). However, it is only relatively recently that this
has been subjected to detailed scrutiny in terms of its impact and how it works on
specific aspects of the learning process.
There are many interrelated aspects that contribute to the high impact potential
of feedback on learning. Nicol and MacFarlane-Dick (2006), in synthesizing the
research literature suggest the following seven principles:
Good feedback practice:
1. helps clarify what good performance is (goals, criteria, expected standards);
2. facilitates the development of self-assessment (reflection) in learning;
3. delivers high-quality information to students about their learning;
4. encourages teacher and peer dialogue around learning;
5. encourages positive motivational beliefs and self-esteem;
6. provides opportunities to close the gap between current and desired performance;
7. provides information to teachers that can be used to shape teaching. (p. 203)
As prior learning (Core Principle 2: Learners’ prior knowledge is activated and
connected to new learning) is always the entry point for new learning (and feedback is
new learning) it must find some anchor point in prior learning to connect meaningfully
to it, in order to result in some enhancement of understanding in that given area of
knowledge/ skill-building. For example, if students are unclear about what they are
supposed to be learning (e.g., the goals, criteria, expectations) even good feedback
may not make much sense. Indeed, research (e.g., Hounsell 1997) has found that
teachers and students often have quite different conceptions about what is involved
in meeting the goals and what the criteria mean in specific terms. The significance
of this is, as Nicol and MacFarlane-Dick (2006) explain:
Weak and incorrect conception of goals not only influence what students do but also the value
of external feedback information. If students do not share (at least in part) their teacher’s
conceptions of assessment goals (and criteria and standards), then the feedback information
they receive is unlikely to ‘connect’. (p. 206)
Hence, good feedback is very much an ongoing dialogue between teacher and
learners (as well as between learners) to identify gaps in knowledge, understanding
and skills, as well as directing the necessary action to resolve these gaps. As Hattie
(2009) states, effective feedback must:
60 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
It’s not surprising that quality feedback has such high impact in terms of effect
size on student attainment, as it connects to so many aspects of the learning process.
However, to maximize the positive impact of feedback on attainment a few conditions
need to be effectively met. Sadler (1989) summarized these as follows:
• What good performance is (i.e. the learner must possess a concept for the goal or
standard being aimed for)
• How current performance relates to good performance (for this, students must be
able to compare current and good performance)
• How to act to close the gap between current and good performance.
A useful approach for helping students to identify what good performance is in
relation to a goal, what assessment criteria are and how they work, and to make com-
parisons between present competence and the competence needed (e.g., knowledge
bases, skills and levels of performance) is to provide various exemplars of perfor-
mance at these different levels. As we know, providing students with examples (and
non-examples) is an effective instructional method. With deliberate practice, includ-
ing plenty of testing, such capability with emerge—given student motivation and
volition. In the most basic terms, students must understand the nature and qualities
of good work if they are to create it themselves. Specifically, they need to know:
• The meaning of key tasks language (e.g., what does ‘describe’ and ‘evaluate’
mean)?
• The meaning of assessment criteria (e.g., “what does give evidence”, “show your
working” mean)?
• How actual tasks and criteria can be demonstrated in practice (e.g., what are
acceptable evidence formats)?
The manner and types of questions asked during feedback sessions are also impor-
tant. A friendly supportive mediating approach is essential to create a level of rapport
in which learners feel comfortable in providing feedback to the teacher. Once estab-
lished, teachers can then ask students focused questions in order to ascertain what
they know and understand, identify specific gaps in knowledge and understanding,
as well as misconceptions, thus enabling learning to become more visible to both.
Furthermore, effective teachers, just as they adjust their communication style to
different student personalities, also adjust their provision of feedback accordingly
based on students’ need in different contexts. For example, Hattie and Yates (2014)
suggest that novices require more specific task-related corrective feedback, to be
gradually replaced with more process feedback as they become increasingly profi-
cient and self-regulated in their learning. What this means is that initially, feedback
will focus on detecting errors in what students are doing on a task and help to reduce
and eventually eliminate these errors. Such feedback will include showing students
what went wrong, examples of correct performance and ways to improve on these
types of learning tasks. Process feedback is more focused on how the students are
2.9 Core Principle 8: Assessment Practices Are Integrated into … 61
tackling the tasks given, such as their thinking (e.g., analysing, comparing, making
inferences & interpretations, evaluating) and the learning strategies they are using.
In providing feedback it is often the case that both aspects are needed, and this is
where the teacher’s judgement and skilful action are most impactful. As students
become increasingly proficient, feedback is usually more focused on their abilities
to monitor and evaluate their own learning, both at cognitive and affective levels
(e.g., metacognition). Questions of how much feedback and the frequency of feed-
back, as with all aspects of differentiated instruction, will depend on the situation
and learners’ readiness. As Hattie (2012) summarized:
The key is the focus on decisions that teachers and students make during the lesson, so most
of all the aim is to inform the teacher of student judgements about the key decisions: ‘Should
I relearn…Practice again…To what?’ and so on. (p. 143)
The strategic use of ongoing formative assessment is an essential part of the overall
assessment strategy and, as Perkins (1992) suggests, once considered thoughtfully:
Teaching, learning, and assessment merge into one seamless enterprise. (p. 176)
Furthermore, feedback is not something that occurs only between the teacher and
individual students but can, and should be, an ongoing collaborative process with
students as active participants. Hence, any activity that tests what students know/dont
know, etc., is retrieval practice which, as detailed prior, is essential for memory
consolidation and building understanding—with good thinking of course. This can
be done through quizzes, conversations, or any performance task that authentically
enables students to elicit/display the knowledge and skills involved.
A method that I have found particularly useful is that of peer assessment. As Petty
(2009) summarizes the procedure:
1. Students come to understand the nature of good work more deeply, as they must
use this understanding to judge peers’ work. This helps them understand their
goals as learners, for example how marks are gained and lost. These goals are
learned from concrete to abstract; this is the most powerful way to learn.
2. They learn other ways of approaching a task than the approach they used.
3. They become more reflective about their own learning and gain understand-
ing by discussing disagreements. For example, if students realize they did one
calculation wrong because they confused a sine with a tangent that is very helpful.
4. Students can do more work than you can mark.
5. Students tend to take pride in work that will be peer-assessed: they are more
likely to complete it and to write more neatly than if you assess it.
6. Students accept criticism from each other that they would ignore if given by you!
For example, ‘Your writing is really hard to read.’
7. Students greatly enjoy this method, and both ‘helpers’ and ‘helped’ learn if
they support each other constructively. (The standard of discussion is commonly
higher than you expect!)
8. It helps to develop the skills required for self-assessment. (p. 263)
62 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
approach for addressing them. Good feedback, when used effectively is another of
those “Russian Dolls” (Hattie 2009) and it supports learning, both for students and
the teacher.
The importance of this heuristic is fundamental to the whole instructional process
as our goal is to help develop in our students the capability to more agentic in how
they learn so that they can increasingly (and this does take considerable time) to
become self-directed learners; that is to be able to plan, monitor and evaluate their
learning—and do this well. As Hattie (2012) concluded:
…all students should be educated in ways that develop their capability to assess their learning.
(p. 141)
In Visible Learning (Hattie 2009), the importance of the climate of the classroom
was noted as among the more critical factors in promoting learning, emphasizing that
teacher-student relationships were the major determiner of such climates—having
an effect size of 0.73.
We are all very clear on what constitutes a physical climate, and its various fea-
tures. It was a typical everyday conversation in England, especially when in a lift with
a stranger. How many times have I heard the comment, “Looks like rain shortly”?
One of my reasons for leaving the UK was quite simply the climate. I did not like
the long winter months, which seemed to last forever. I prefer the perpetual summer
weather in Singapore, and what an easy job weather forecasting is here: “26–33
centigrade with some chance of rain in the afternoon” is a 90% correct call on most
days. In the UK, as I remember it some 20 years back, weather forecasting was a
combination of thoughtful roulette and serendipity, at best.
Now defining a psychological climate is a bit like defining thinking, as we can’t
see, touch or smell it. However, when it is very good or very bad, we can certainly feel
it. People typically use terms like, “The atmosphere is terrible in there”, or “Every-
thing’s cool here”. Essentially, it’s about the nature and types of interactions that are
going on—or not going on—between people in each social and geographical context
(in educational contexts it’s typically a classroom) and their impact on perception,
feeling and subsequent behaviour. The ability to create and facilitate a positive psy-
chological climate in a range of informal interpersonal situations is a great skill set
to have. If you have such capability, it’s likely that you will always be high on the
invite list for socially orientated parties, as you have the skill of creating lively con-
versational content which helps folks to relax and feel comfortable. As classrooms
are not fundamentally different from other social interaction situations in that there
are human actors (teachers and students) involved in interpersonal communication
over time for a purpose (e.g., teaching and learning), a psychological climate will
inevitably result. Furthermore, there is no doubt that certain types of psychological
64 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
climates are much more conducive to attainment, engagement and well-being than
others, which may have adverse effects. Research suggests that several key factors are
very important for promoting a positive psychological climate. First and foremost,
as Hattie and Yates (2014) summarized, this entails the teacher exhibiting attributes
that:
…promote positive and open human communication. Students value being treated with
(a) fairness, (b) dignity, and (c) individual respect. These threefold aspects have emerged
strongly in all studies in which students are interviewed and surveyed as to what they expect
of their teachers. (p. 26)
Very much in the context of this Core Principle, Jensen (1996) found that:
Learners in positive, joyful environments are likely to experience better learning, memory
and feelings of self-esteem. (p. 98)
However the really important questions concern what are the specific things that
teachers can do, and how best to do this, in order to create and sustain a psychological
climate that results in the students perceiving and feeling that they are being treated
with ‘fairness’, ‘dignity’ ‘individual respect’, developing a ‘you can do it attitude’ and
experiencing some sense of joy in participating in the classroom learning activities?
It is easy, though somewhat limited, to address these questions in terms of intent
or generalizations. For example, we might say, “show respect”, “Be enthusiastic in
how you teach” or “Display passion about learning”. This is in many ways like going
on a first date and having little idea on what to say or do and being told by a friend
to “Be interesting”. Such statements are, in both the above contexts, valid and will
make sense to both the cognitive neuroscientist and the layperson alike—but there is
something significantly missing. It is interestingly and annoyingly (for me anyway)
captured in the saying “Everything is easy when you can do it.” Being interesting
certainly was not the case for me on my first ever proper date with a girl as a seventeen-
year-old. Getting ready to meet Geraldine (that was her real name—it will give her a
chuckle if she ever reads this book) at a local cinema on a Saturday night, I suddenly
posed myself the essential question, “What do I talk to her about?” Instantaneously,
I became anxious, which quickly escalated to panic (we have all been there, and we
know what this does for good thinking and confidence). In delving into my LTM
system it was not long before I realized that all I ever talked about was football,
boxing and fishing with my friends, who were all boys. I had no idea at all on what
to talk to a girl about, a real lack of prior knowledge containing, in retrospect, only
2.10 Core Principle 9: A Psychological Climate Is Created … 65
misconceptions. The inevitable happened and the date was a disaster. I had nothing
to say, was visibly uncomfortable all night, and this clearly contributed to her feeling
equally uncomfortable. At the end of the film, the encounter quickly ended with a
statement from me like, “How do you get home?” I had a reply something like, “I
get the bus from over there.” Geraldine never contacted me again, and that’s not
too difficult to explain. That was my first date and my last for a while; I was afraid
to go through that again. If there was an ‘O’ level in conversational literacy with
females, another grade 9 was an absolute certainty for yours truly, at that time. My
Jack Russell dog would have fared better, and you will know why shortly. Despite
my intention to be interesting and build rapport, I had no knowledge in LTM on how
to do this. As Molden (2001) makes clear:
It is our behaviour that directly connects to results, even though our thinking may be
responsible for generating behaviour. (p. 59)
skill sets. Good understanding plus deliberate practice over time will get one to this
desirable state. The converse is equally true. In most basic terms, to be effective at
something, having intent is only an initial motivator, you must know how to do it well,
and be able to do it at the behavioural level. Ultimately perception and judgements
about other people, accurate or otherwise, is the product of their behaviour, and of
course, our pre-existing beliefs.
In several teaching situations, I have seen novice teachers tremble at the front of
a classroom, even run out in fear and despair when faced with challenging students
or sometimes from forgetting the details of their teaching plan. Quite simply, they
don’t know what to do next and lack the strategies in their long-term memories
that might be effective in such situations. In contrast, highly competent and creative
professionals when confronted with a challenging group of students or even noticing
boredom developing on some of the students’ faces, while never complacent, can
typically and smoothly change the teaching strategy in situ (re-create the pedagogy
situationally). In most cases, such action results in regaining attention, settling the
group down and changing the psychological climate to one that is more positive,
and task-focused. To a novice teacher or outsider, this may seem almost like magic,
as creativity in any domain often feels a bit like that. However, as for most things
(including magic), once things are made explicit at the behavioural and cognitive
level, it all seems rather obvious and logical.
Of course, understanding is not competence, and deliberate practice is needed in
skill acquisition, but it certainly helps if one knows very clearly what is involved in the
learning process. What I have been describing may seem somewhat behaviouristic
and contrived, and that is partly true. However, customer service professionals don’t
learn how to speak, smile and use their voice in certain specific ways just to fill up
training hours on their staff development plans. As Mlodinow (2012) summarized:
The gestures we make, the position in which we hold our bodies, the expressions we wear
on our faces, and the nonverbal qualities of our speech, all contribute to how others see us.
(p. 110)
It’s therefore not surprising that politicians and other high-profile media peo-
ple employ communication specialists and psychologists to create certain positive
appearances to influence the public at large. They do this because it works in large
part with many people, and there is an underlying set of reasons why it works. For
many years, I mentored and coached ‘underperforming’ teachers. These were aca-
demic faculty who received below 3.25 on a rating scale (where a score of 5 was ‘very
good’ and a score of 1 was ‘very poor’) from student feedback for 2 semesters on the
formal end of semester online questionnaire. Over the years this highlighted how, in a
communication encounter, the relationship between a communicator’s intention and
the perception and meaning by others can be so incongruent. Many of these teachers
2.10 Core Principle 9: A Psychological Climate Is Created … 67
also had very negative qualitative comments relating to such things as “shows little
interest”, “no care and concern” etc. In conversation with them, some were very dis-
turbed by such student responses, and could not explain on what basis and how they
might have been perceived in such negative light. They seemed unaware that such
perceptions originate from specific behavioural aspects of personal presentation.
Essentially, the psychological climate is largely shaped based on how the teacher
behaves on an everyday basis with the student group. Hattie and Yates (2014) sum-
marized the specific behaviours that are positive in this respect; they are noteworthy,
but obvious when made explicit:
The key aspects, as described by a significant body of research involve the teacher’s positive
open gestures when dealing with the class, physically moving around the room, relaxed body
orientation, frequent use of smiles, direct eye contact, and using a variety of friendly and
encouraging vocal tones, especially when dealing with an individual student. (p. 28)
Mlodinow (2012, p. 118) quotes research by Ekman and Friesen (1971) who
showed people in an isolated Neolithic culture in New Guinea pictures of Ameri-
can faces displaying a range of typical emotions. These primitive people had never
been exposed to outside cultures, used no written language, were still using stone
implements, and very few had seen a photograph let alone television or films. How-
ever, when they were shown American faces of basic emotions, they were as able, as
people from the twenty-one literature countries who participated in the research, at
recognizing happiness, fear, anger, disgust, sadness and surprise in the faces of the
emoting Americans.
Certainly, from my experience of facilitating many workshops in a wide range of
cultural contexts, I would also make the case for there being much similarity in terms
of people’s perception and comprehension of what constitutes good human conduct,
a positive psychological climate, as well as the way they learn. Several years ago, I
was attending a conference in which one of the keynote speakers was emphasizing
how people from different cultures learned very differently, and that we should be
thinking of culturally relative pedagogies. In listening, I was reflecting on my own
experiences and feeling a bit confused and somewhat annoyed. Yes, of course, there
are cultural differences, and pedagogy must consider relevant culturally determined
situated factors for obvious reasons. However, in large part, the main specific cultural
factors relate more to specific social norms and custom, rather than pedagogic or
fundamental interpersonal communication practices. For example, I am mindful of
touch, even handshakes in certain cultural contexts, as well as the humour I use. I
also notice that in different cultural and ethnic contexts, one must modify the level
of informality accordingly. For example, I tend to be more informal quicker in the
Philippines than other countries, as participants seem to respond well to this. In
68 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
certain countries, I tend to retain formality longer as I feel that the early display of
humour may be detrimental to a perception of high professional credibility.
However, my experience is that irrespective of cultural context, learners will
become more informal and appreciate some humour, once they feel comfortable
and perceive high credibility in terms of what is on offer in the learning stakes. Cul-
ture is impactful, but it may be less so than personality configurations. In terms of
how people learn, I find little difference, and that’s because we share the same brain
structure and we learn structurally in the same way. In most basic terms, learners must
acquire knowledge through memory processing, make meaning of it (build under-
standing) through thinking, and acquiring skills by doing. In this context, there is
motivation and beliefs that will come into play, but the essential principles of human
conduct and learning seem largely universal, based on my experience.
I am convinced that highly competent and creative teachers will be positively
impactful anywhere, but not with everyone all the time—that’s impossible. Equally,
very poor teachers will be similarly experienced in negative ways, wherever, in most
cases. What is often of noticeable difference is how learners across cultures and
contexts respond to the variety of teachers they experience. The best are generally
always appreciated. However, how the worse teachers fare may vary significantly
depending on cultural contexts. In some cultures, it seems that few learners will
disrupt or react negatively even in the face of poor teaching, as there is a deep respect
for the profession of teaching. They probably remain just internally bored or upset,
depending on whether the teacher is just incompetent technically or socially, or both.
The latter is a sorry state to experience. In summary, Sale and Mukerji (2006) were
delighted to report:
…in our experiences of co-facilitation over several years, we were initially surprised but
ultimately delighted to find that there appears to be several generic principles and practices
that facilitate rapport and effective learning irrespective of cultural and ethnic contexts,
(abstract)
Humour makes us feel better, and this has a positive effect on our psychological
state. Of course, humour must be used thoughtfully and in context. However, far
from limiting the learning experience, humour is now seen to have a wide range of
positive impacts on aspects of the learning process, such as:
• Refreshing the brain
• Creating mental images that retain learning
• Reinforcing the desired behaviour and making classroom management easier
• Developing positive attitudes
• Promoting creativity
• Contributing to the enjoyment of teaching.
Furthermore, humour seems to have a role in learning more generally. Earleywine
(2011) summarized:
Funny instructors get higher ratings perhaps because humour affects immediacy – the sense
that an instructor is present and attentive with students…
…a full semester of instruction that includes relevant jokes that illustrate key concepts lead
to better scores in final exams. (p. 138)
When I first arrived in Singapore some 24 years prior, there was a Smile Cam-
paign, and since then there have been campaigns for Graciousness and, more recently,
Kindness. Cynics may say this is ‘social engineering’. Well, any act of socialization
is social engineering, and my response to such folk is that it’s much nicer to have
smiling, gracious and kind people, than the opposite. In terms of moral values, there
is much that can be learned from neuroscience as Harris (2010) Suggests. People
generally feel better in a psychological climate that encourages kindness and gra-
ciousness, and smiling is both uplifting and builds rapport. In the context of education,
70 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
the teacher’s interactions with students will largely shape the psychological climate
of the classroom, and as Rogers (1998) described:
…the facilitation of significant learning rests upon certain attitudinal qualities that exist in
the personal relationship between the facilitator and the learner. (p. 121)
Many of the important components that underpin the shaping of this relation-
ship have been outlined and illustrated in this chapter and some key areas will be
developed further in subsequent chapters. Most significantly as a teacher, shaping the
psychological climate is in large part your responsibility, though it can be challenging
in many situations. However, as Hattie and Yates (2014) argued:
As their teacher, you are an inevitable coach in interpersonal mannerisms. Hence a deep
understanding of how these social processes operate will prove of inherent value in your
professional work. (p. 269)
For the uninitiated, fly fishing involves a sophisticated fishing technique in which
an artificial fly is cast to catch trout. However, whether the fisherperson catches
trout, involves much more than this. Choosing the strategy, type of fly, identifying
the species of trout in the location, interpreting the impact of weather conditions
are some of the critical considerations in catching trout. The expert fisherperson
negotiates these almost intuitively and catches fish regularly. Suffice to say, as a
novice fly-fisherman, I did not catch many trout and never reached any great heights
of expertise.
Fly fishing is a useful analogy when applying the core principles of learning to the
context of teaching in that both involve a solid knowledge base relating to the design
and conduct of the respective activities. Similarly, they are also mediated by the situ-
ated context in which they are enacted in that both the fly fisherperson and the teacher
must deal with the here and now environmental situation. For the fly-fisherperson,
there is a need to carefully consider such factors as the nature of the water locality
(e.g., river, lake or sea), type of trout inhabitants in the locality, the season of the
year and prevailing weather conditions. For the teacher, key considerations include
the nature and composition of the student group (e.g., prior knowledge and compe-
tence levels, motivational status), classroom resources and time of the day. Based
on their knowledge and framing of the situated context, both fly fisher-persons and
teachers select methods and resources, and create strategies to try to produce good
results—whether defined in terms of ‘trout caught’ or ‘students taught’.
In teaching, while the core principles of learning are enduring heuristics in the
design of the learning experience and the conduct of teaching, their relative impor-
tance as focal points in the design and teaching process is typically mediated by such
situated factors. For example, if I am aware that a learning group has many students
2.11 Using the Core Principles Thoughtfully: The Fly-Fishing Analogy 71
who have a generally low intrinsic motivational level for the subject, I will give more
thought concerning how best to incorporate appropriate motivational strategies and
work on creating a positive psychological climate as the central consideration. In
this situation, I may ‘sacrifice’ cognitive considerations for better motivational or
affective outcomes, at least in the short term. However, I would maintain a strong
focus on avoiding cognitive overload and developing some mastery of key skills as
priority pedagogic features. In contrast, when teaching fee-paying students on higher
degree programmes motivating them may not be such a central concern, though they
typically appreciate it anyway. This thoughtful and situated application of the core
principles of learning has been well captured by Darling-Hammond and Bransford
(2005):
…teachers not only need to understand the basic principles of learning but must also know
how to use them judiciously to meet diverse learning goals in contexts where students differ
in their needs. (p. 78)
The cognitive scientific principles (Core Principles of Learning) presented and illus-
trated in this chapter are not meant to be exhaustive or summative—as noted in the
introduction. However, they do constitute powerful universal heuristics in the design
and facilitation of learning in all contexts (e.g., face-to-face teaching, blended learn-
ing, fully online). What this means is that teaching can be designed, conducted, and
evaluated from a sound pedagogic base. In other words, teaching can be subjected to
a systematic evidence-based pedagogic analysis which will increase both diagnoses
of learning events in terms of their effectiveness and efficiency, and well as designing
learning events with a high probability of successful outcomes. This will significantly
enhance all teaching and training professional’s ability to conduct a more rigorous
and useful process of reflective practice; whereby they can not only identify what
has worked well, or not so well, but also the underlying psychological principles that
have led to such outcomes. Evidence-Based Reflective Practice is fully explained
and illustrated in Chap. 9.
Whatever frameworks or models of teaching are used, teachers must inevitably
design learning events, and it makes better sense to be as evidence-based as possible
in relation to the situated context as documented above. Kilbane and Milman (2014)
argued that teachers should be educational designers, and define this in these terms:
An educational designer is a teacher who approaches instructional planning with purpose,
uses knowledge of specialized systematic processes to identify and frame instructional chal-
lenges related to learners and content, and competently addresses these challenges through
the skillful application of a broad repertoire of instructional models, strategies, and technolo-
gies. Educational designers approach the work of teaching with a new mindset, a broadened
skill set, and a high-quality tool set – all of which assist them in developing instruction that
72 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
responds to their learner’s needs. The new mindset enables teachers to approach their work
as empowered problem solvers who are aware of their ability to direct important dimen-
sions of practice. The expanded skill set allows them mastery over systematic approaches
to instructional planning and assessment processes. The high-quality toolset consists of a
collection of models, strategies, and technologies for teaching that can make learning more
efficient, effective, and engaging. (xxi)
2.13 Summary
This chapter has outlined and illustrated key heuristics—Core Principles of Learn-
ing—for planning and conducting the practices of teaching. They are underpinned
by current and established knowledge relating to human learning and research on
what methods are most effective. The extent to which cognitive scientific principles
(e.g., Core Principles of Learning) can be said to constitute an essential Pedagogy
Literacy for the planning and facilitation of learning may rest on how other literacies
are framed and on what basis. The term literacy has been typically used in the con-
text of language acquisition and use. For example, persons who cannot read, speak
2.13 Summary 73
References
Apter MJ (ed) (2001) Motivation styles in everyday life: a guide to reversal theory. American
Psychological Association. ISBN: 1-55793-739-4, Washington, D.C.
Ausubel DP et al (1978) Educational psychology: a cognitive view. Holt, Rhinehart and Winston,
New York
Bandler R, Grinder J (1990) Frogs into princes: the introduction to neuro-linguistic programming.
Eden Grove Editions, Middlesex
Berliner DC (1987) Ways of thinking about students and classrooms by more and less experienced
teachers. In: Calderhead J (eds) Exploring teachers’ thinking. Cassell, London
Bloom BS (ed) (1985) Developing talent in young people. Ballantine Books, New York
Boud D (ed) (1988) Developing student autonomy in learning. Kogan Page, London
Brown PC, Roediger III HL, McDaniel MA (2014) Make it stick: the science of successful learning.
Harvard University Press, Massachusetts
Bruner JS (1966) The process of education. Harvard University Press, Cambridge
Clark RC, Lyons C (2005) Graphics for learning: proven guidelines for planning, designing, and
evaluating visuals in training materials. Pfeiffer, San Francisco
Colvin G (2008) Talent is overrated: what really Separates world-class performers from everybody
else. Penguin, London
Csikszentmihalyi M (1990) Flow: the psychology of optimal experience. Harper Row, New York
Darling-Hammond L, Bransford J (2005) Preparing teachers for a changing world: what teachers
should learn and be able to do. Jossey-Bass, San-Francisco
De Bono E (2003) Serious creativity 2. Allscript Establishment, Singapore
74 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
Dilts R et al (1980) Neuro linguistic programming Vol. 1: the study of the structure of subjective
experience. Meta Publications, California
Dougherty MR, Robey A (2018) Neuroscience & education: a bridge astray. Association for
Psychological Science. Curr Dir Psychol Sci, 1–6. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/
326143095_Neuroscience_Education_A_bridge_astray
Dweck CS (2006) Mindset: the new psychology of success. Ballantine, New York
Earleywine M (2011) Humour 101. Springer, New York
Ekmann P, Friesen WV (1971) Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. J Pers Soc Psychol
17(2):124–129
Flavell JH (1979) Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area of cognitive-developmental
inquiry. Am Psychol 34(10)
Garner LA (2006) Humor in pedagogy: how haha can lead to aha! Coll Teach. Last accessed 7 Nov
2019
Gregory G, Kaufeldt M (2015) The motivated brain: improving student attention, engagement, and
perseverance. ASCD, Alexandria, USA
Goulston M (2009) Just listen: discover the secret to getting through to absolutely anyone.
AMACON, New York
Halpern DF (1998) Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains: dispositions, skills,
structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. Am Psychol 53(4):449–455
Hart LA (1983) Human brain and human learning. Longman, New York
Harris S (2010) The moral landscape: how science can determine human values. Free Press, New
York
Hattie J (2009) Visible learning. Routledge, New York
Hattie J (2012) Visible learning for teachers: maximizing impact on learning. Routledge, London
Hattie J, Yates GCR (2014) Visible learning and the science of how we learn. Routledge, New York
Hounsell D (1997) Understanding teaching and teaching for understanding. In: Marton F, Hounsell
D, Entwistle N (eds) The experience of learning: implications for teaching and studying in higher
education. Scottish Academic Press, Edinburgh
Jensen E (1996) Brain based learning. Turning Point Publishing, Del Mar, CA
Kahneman D (2012) Thinking fast and slow. Penguin Books, London
Keen A (2007) The cult of the amateur: how today’s internet is killing our culture and assaulting
our economy. Nicholas Brealey Publishing, London
Kilbane CR, Milman NB (2014) Teaching models: instruction for 21st century learners. Pearson,
London, England
Kircher PA et al (2006) Educ Psychol 41(2):75–86
Knowles MS (1973) The adult learner: a neglected species. Gulf Publishing Company, Houston
Koffka K (1915) Zur Grundlegung der Wahrnehmungspsychologie: Eine Auseinandersetzung mit
V. Benussi. Beiträge zur Psychologic der Gestalt, No. 3. Zeitschrift fur Psychologie 73:11–90
Kohler W (1929) Gestalt psychology. Liveright, London
Lang JM (2016) Everyday lessons from the science of learning. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA
Lei SA, Cohen JL, Russler KM (2010) Humor on learning in the college classroom: evaluating
benefits and drawbacks from instructor’s perspectives. J Instr Psychol
Marcus G (2009) Kluge: the haphazard evolution of the human mind. Mariner Books, New York
Martinez ME (2006) What is metacognition? Phi Delta Kappan, 696–699
Marzano RJ et al (1988) Dimensions of thinking: a framework for curriculum and instruction.
ASCD, Alexandria, VA
Marzano RJ et al (2007) Designing and teaching learning goals and objectives: classroom strategies
that work. Marzano Research Laboratory, Colorado
Mayer RE, Alexander PA (2011) Handbook of research on learning and instruction. Routledge,
London
McTighe J, Wiggins G (1998) Understanding by design. ASCD, Alexandria, VA
Miller GA (1956) The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for
processing information. Psychol Rev 63(2):81–97
References 75
Miller MD (2016) Minds online: teaching effectively with technology. Harvard University Press,
Massachusetts
Mlodinow L (2012) Subliminal: how your unconscious mind rules your behaviour. Vintage Books,
New York
Molden D (2001) NLP business masterclass. FT-Press, New Jersey
Mortimore P (ed) (1999) Understanding pedagogy & its impact on learning. Paul Chapman, London
Murdock BB (1962) The serial position effect of free recall. J Exp Psychol 64(5):482–488
Nicol DJ, MacFarlane-Dick D (2006) Formative assessment and self-regulated learning: a model
and seven principles of good feedback practice. Stud High Educ 31(2):199–218
Nuthall GA (2005) The cultural myths and realities of classroom teaching and learning: a personal
journey. Teachers Coll Rec 107(5):895–934
O’Doherty J et al (2004) Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental
conditioning. Science 304:452–454
Ormrod JE (2011) Our minds, our memories: enhancing thinking and learning at all ages. Pearson,
London
Ornstein AC, Behar LS (1995) Contemporary Issues in Curriculum. Allyn & Bacon, Massachusetts
Paul RW (1993) Critical thinking. Foundation for Critical Thinking, Santa Rosa, CA
Paul R, Niewoehner R, Elder L (2006) The thinker’s guide to engineering reasoning. Foundation
for Critical Thinking, Tomales, CA
Perkins DN (1985) ‘What creative thinking is’. Excerpt from Perkins DN. Creativity by design.
Educ Leadersh 42(1):18–24
Perkins DN (1992) Smart schools. The Free Press, London
Perkins DN et al (1995) Software goes to school: teaching for understanding with new technologies.
Oxford University Press, Oxford
Petty G (2009) Evidence-based teaching: a practical approach. Nelson Thornes, Cheltenham
Piaget J (2001) The psychology of intelligence. Routledge, New York
Pinker S (2003) The blank slate: the modern denial of human nature. Penguin, London
Pugh KJ, Bergin DA (2006) Motivational influences on transfer. Educ Psychol 41(3):147–160
Ramsden P (1992) Learning to teach in higher education. Routledge, London
Resnick LB (1989) Assessing the thinking curriculum: new tools for educational reform. In: Gifford
BR, O’Connor MC (eds) Future assessments: changing views of aptitude, achievement, and
instruction. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Boston
Rogers A (1998) Teaching adults. Open University Press, Buckingham
Sale D (2014) The challenge of reframing engineering education. Springer, New York
Sale D (2015) Creative teaching: an evidence-based approach. Springer, New York
Sale D, Mukerji S (2006) Establishing rapport in facilitating workshops in different cultural and
ethnic contexts: key principles of effective human conduct and engagement. In: Paper presented
at ERAS conference, Singapore
Sadler DR (1989) Formative assessment and the design of instructional systems. Instr Sci 18:119–
144
Schank R (1997) Virtual learning. McGraw-Hill, London
Schank R (2011) Teaching minds: how cognitive science can save our schools. Teacher College
Press, New York
Shulman L (1991) Pedagogic ways of knowing. Institute of Education, London
Stigler JW, Hiebert J (1999) The teaching gap. Free Press.
Sullo B (2007) Activating the DESIRE to learn. ASCD, Alexandria, VA
Swartz RJ, Parks S (1994) Infusing critical and creative thinking into content instruction. Critical
Thinking Press & Software, Pacific Grove, CA
Sylwester R (ed) (1998) Student brains, school issues. Skylight Training and Publishing Inc,
Arlington Heights, IL
The Free Dictionary (2019). www.thefreedictionary.com. Last accessed on 8 Nov 2019
Turner-Bisset R (2001) Expert teaching: knowledge and pedagogy to head the profession. David
Fulton, London
76 2 Towards an Evidence-Based Pedagogic Literacy …
Van Merrienboer JJG, Sweller J (2005) Cognitive load theory and complex learning: recent
development and future directions. Educ Psychol Rev 17(2)
Wagner T (2010) The global achievement gap. Basic Books, New York
Willingham DT (2009) Why don’t students Like school: a cognitive scientist answers questions
about How the Mind works and what it means for the classroom. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Wlodkowski RJ (2008) Enhancing adult motivation to learn: a comprehensive guide for teaching
all adults. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Chapter 3
Metacognitive Capability: The
Superordinate Competence
for the Twenty-First Century
3.1 Introduction
Akturk and Sahin (2011), in reviewing the literature on metacognition, argued that:
Metacognition is a structure that is referred to as fuzzy by many scholars and has very diverse
meanings. Much research has been conducted for more than 30 years to access the inner side
of this structure, which is hard to grasp. (p. 3731)
Having conducted numerous workshops with adults, I am still surprised that many
people have little or no knowledge relating to metacognition; some are even totally
unfamiliar with the term. Just using the word metacognition often results in glazed
eyes as the initial hearing of this unfamiliar term seems to evoke feelings akin to
‘not more psychobabble’. Brown’s (1987) framing of metacognition, while making
sense to me having surveyed the literature extensively, may also do little to enthuse
the lay reader to explore it further:
Metacognition is not only a monster of obscure parentage but a many-headed monster at
that. (p. 105)
However, at the same time, metacognition is being heralded as one of the main
twenty-first century competencies. For example, Lai (2012) noted that while many
frameworks mapping such skills have been developed, underpinning these frame-
works, and related to student attainment, positive learning and career development, 5
key research-based competencies have been identified: Critical Thinking; Creativity;
Collaboration; Motivation; Metacognition.
Certainly, voices advocating the importance of metacognitive activity within edu-
cational contexts have resulted in placing metacognition high on educational research
agendas. For example, Martinez (2006) argues:
Metacognitive ability is central to conceptions of what it means to be educated. The world
is becoming more complex, more information-rich, more-full of options, and more demand-
ing of fresh thinking. With these changes, the importance of metacognitive ability as an
educational outcome can only grow. (p. 699)
In this chapter, I will get to grips with this ‘many-headed monster of obscure
parentage’, as it is an issue of pedagogic necessity. While accepting that there is much
by way of conceptual confusion in the literature—yep, it’s fuzzy—metacognition is
not just a twenty-first century competence, it is the superordinate competence of the
twenty-first century. The ability to be highly effective in this competence, I frame as
Metacognitive Capability (MC). While this focus on MC is in the field of education
and training, it is of relevance to all professional and human activity, as it is central
to how humans can learn better, be more self-directed in their learning and careers
as well as experience higher levels of personal well-being.
Learning to be competent in any area of human performance always requires a
combination of knowledge, skills, and attitudes (however defined). Also, there are
universal principles of learning that underpin such processes, and MC is no exception.
To develop competence and expertise in MC, it is necessary to fully understand what it
is, the benefits for learning, well-being and, over time, self-directed lifelong learning.
Invariably, one must then invest the motivational and volitional strategies, as well as
the spaced and deliberate practice, to achieve such competence.
Let’s start with direct experience as an anchor point. Whether we like it or not, we
inevitably reflect on our experiences and actions in living in the world. We also
typically do this in relation to the actions of others, making judgements of worth,
often of right or wrong, based on our beliefs and perception. There is variation in
3.2 Making Sense of Metacognition: Unpacking… 79
terms of the extent, form and nature of how humans do this, and it is influenced
both by hereditary and experiential factors. As you are reading this text you may
choose or simply drift into reflecting on what you already know, or don’t know about
metacognition. Hence, you are thinking about it and you’ll probably be doing some
analysis, comparing and contrasting, making inferences and interpretations, even
some evaluation on what you know and what you are reading. These are the key
cognitive heuristics of critical thinking which help to build understanding—when
done well with the right content knowledge, as we saw in Chap. 2.
This capability to be able to consciously think about our actions, be aware of
the potential consequences of action, and the likelihood of experiencing a range
of feelings from heightened pleasure to extreme dread, seems to define mankind
as distinct from other animal species. Over the years, my family has acquired a
few domestic cats as pets, which they see as lovely friendly creatures who are so
affectionate—after all they are pussycats, right? For me, I am ok with cats, but the
occasional gecko or cockroach, not to mention mouse, that happened to venture into
our apartment would not share such positive experience of the pussycats. Of note,
the cats never show any visible signs of remorse for torturing these small creatures.
Fromm (1987) referred to man “as a freak of nature, being within nature and yet
transcending it” which makes humans beset with existential paradoxes. Humans, in
contrast to animals, must consciously live with the consequences of their actions, as
well as deal with knowing that suffering and finality to life (in the existential sense)
is probably inevitable. As Ursula, the sea witch, said to Ariel, the little mermaid,
in the Disney film The Little Mermaid (1989), when she bargained with Ariel an
exchange of her beautiful voice for a pair of legs to be fully human, “…life is full of
tough choices innit?”.
It is this distinctive human capability to be able to self-reflect, think, plan, monitor
and evaluate our thinking and actions that underpin metacognition. Central to self-
directed learning is the capability to use metacognition to self-regulate one’s thinking,
affect and behaviour in meeting learning goals. There is no significant evidence that
other creatures possess such a capability, though of course they experience pleasure
and pain and other emotions that seem to have similar correlates to those in humans.
Flavell originally coined the term metacognition in the late 1970s to mean “cogni-
tion in relationship to cognitive phenomena,” or more simply “thinking about think-
ing” (Flavell 1979, p. 906). He went on to suggest that while metacognition mainly
focuses on knowledge and cognition about cognition, the concept could be broadened
to anything psychological, rather than just anything cognitive. It could be related to
include executive processes, formal operations, consciousness, social cognition, self-
efficacy, self-regulation, reflective self-awareness, and the concept of psychological
self or psychological subject (Flavell 1987).
Flavell’s suggestion that metacognition could be more than just thinking about
thinking, but “broadened to anything psychological” opened-up the ‘many headed
monster’ analogy. Recent research tends to confirm both Flavell and Brown’s anal-
ysis. The most current framing of metacognition, especially from neuropsychology,
goes beyond monitoring and evaluating cognition, but also regulating affective and
motivational aspects of being—the whole person—such as beliefs and emotions (e.g.,
80 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
Dweck and Legget 1989). There is now an integration of metacognitive and motiva-
tional approaches to explaining the development of student’s success (or otherwise)
in school learning. Of note, Martinez (2006) argues that metacognition entails the
management of affective states, and that metacognitive strategies can improve per-
sistence in the face of challenging tasks. Paris and Winograd (1990) support this
view as they see affect as an inevitable element of metacognition because as students
monitor and appraise their cognition, they will become more aware of strengths and
weaknesses and can take the necessary action to enhance learning capability and
well-being.
This makes perfect sense as we know cognition (thinking) does not exist as an
independent entity in human decision—making and behaviour. Apart from hereditary
factors and personality configurations, beliefs, emotions, and motivation all play
out as a dynamic system in determining perception and behaviour. Also, there is
increasing evidence that much of this activity (which may give us the illusion of
conscious self-control) is operating sub/unconsciously (e.g., Mlodinow 2012).
Education and many branches of psychology abound with perspectives, theories
and models, and this poses problems for even the theoreticians and researchers,
let alone practitioners in the field of teaching and training, as well as the wider lay
public. It is, therefore, necessary to firstly clarify key conceptual issues in the field
of research on metacognition. For example, the term metacognition is often used
simultaneously or alternately with other frameworks relating to mental processing
and self-regulation. The major frameworks include:
• Self-Directed Learning
• Self-Regulated Learning
• Meta-Learning.
Self-Directed Learning (SDL) is becoming somewhat of a buzzword in the edu-
cational landscape. Certainly, in the rapidly changing and volatile world of today,
the aim of developing persons, in-school environments or elsewhere, who are self-
directed lifelong learners would seem a pertinent educational aim. There is nothing
new about the need for self-directed and lifelong learning. It’s just that it is more
essential in the modern context than yesteryear. A useful reference point in framing
SDL is the enduring definition of Knowles (1975):
Self-Directed Learning describes a process in which individuals take the initiative, with
or without the help of others, in diagnosing their learning needs, formulating learning
goals, identifying human and material resources for learning, choosing and implementing
appropriate learning strategies, and evaluating learning outcomes. (p. 18)
A highly self-directed learner…is one who exhibits initiative, independence, and persistence
in learning; one who accepts responsibility for his or her own learning and views problems
as challenges, not obstacles; one who is capable of self-discipline and has a high degree of
curiosity; one who has a strong desire to learn or change and is self-confident; one who is
able to use basic study skills, organize his or her time, set an appropriate pace for learning,
and develop a plan for completing work; one who enjoys learning and has a tendency to be
goal-oriented. (Guglielmino 1978, p. 73)
In practice, SDL and SRL have often been used interchangeably in the litera-
ture. However, Saks and Leijen (2013), in comparing and contrasting SDL and SRL,
suggest that while both incorporate task definition, goal setting, planning and enact-
ing strategies, monitoring and evaluation—as well as metacognition and Intrinsic
motivation, they differ in certain important ways:
1. SDL originates from Adult Education; whereas SRL originates from Cognitive
Psychology
2. SDL is practiced mainly outside school environments; whereas SRL is practiced
more in school environments
3. SDL involves the learner more in the design of the learning environment and its
trajectory; whereas in SRL, outcomes and tasks are usually set by the teacher
4. SDL is a broader macro-level construct than SRL.
Another term, that of Meta-Learning, originally framed by Maudsley (1979),
has now been re-surfaced by the Center for Curriculum Redesign, (e.g., Fadel and
Trilling 2015) who define it in terms of two main components:
• Metacognition—the process of thinking about thinking.
• Growth Mindset—the inner belief that abilities can be developed through hard
work.
Meta-Learning has a ‘catchy’ tone to it, but these components—metacognition and
growth mindset—are already embedded in more recent framings of metacognition.
From the above comparisons, it is not surprising that there has been confusion
relating to what is metacognition and how it relates to other frameworks such as SDL
and SRL. Indeed, the confusion is enhanced further as a result of some researchers
considering self-regulation to be a subordinate component of metacognition (e.g.,
Kluwe 1987), whereas others regard self-regulation as a concept superordinate to
metacognition (e.g., Winne 1995).
Metacognition may indeed be that many-headed-monster of obscure parentage.
The latter aspect is more the domain of evolutionary theory and may be of academic
interest only; the many heads are of major pedagogical, social and global interest.
82 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
Research suggests that human brains were pretty much the same (morphologi-
cally) some 50,000 years ago—indeed recent data shows that even 300,000 years
ago, the brain size in H. sapiens already fell within the range of present-day humans
(Neubauer et al. 2018). It is arguable, therefore, that such folk (let’s be gentle on
the language occasionally) had similar cognitive abilities and motivational disposi-
tions—just played out in different contexts, with different resources, and different
contingencies. I note that the Paleo diet is now becoming highly popular with certain
sectors of the healthy-eating community, so there is probably still much in common
between Homo sapiens today and those from the aeons of yesteryear.
One thing that certainly differentiates us from our ancestors is the rate of knowl-
edge production. It’s a cliché to refer to the exponential knowledge explosion of
modern times, but the problem of constant cognitive overload is a problem for many
of us. Before information can be stored in our long-term memory, it must first pass
3.2 Making Sense of Metacognition: Unpacking… 83
through our working memory, which has very limited immediate capability when
processing new information, as we explored in some detail in Chap. 2, Core Prin-
ciple 6: Learning design takes into account the working of memory systems. This
limitation is further hampered in terms of learning effectiveness and efficiency by
our brain’s inherent systemic biases and tendency to be lazy (e.g., Kahneman 2012),
and it not being particularly well designed for thinking (e.g., Willingham 2009),
and our limited amounts of willpower (e.g., Baumeister and Tierney 2012). Dealing
with them, at least in terms of mitigating their negative impacts on learning and
well-being, will be imperative in future curriculum planning and instruction.
In terms of the twenty-first century underpinning research-based competencies
identified by Lai (2012), I suggest that they are interrelated in complex ways. For
example, metacognition is inevitably related to both critical and creative thinking
and is the executive function monitoring and evaluating them—all being the main
cognitive components of ‘Good Thinking’ as framed in Chap. 2. Also, motivation
and metacognition are reciprocally related and synergistic and, finally, providing
students with opportunities to work together may stimulate students’ motivation and
thinking.
MC in this context can be validly and usefully framed as the superordinate com-
petence that encompasses a range of sub-competencies and skill sets (including those
of self-regulation) that facilitates the development of SDL. MC fuels the process of
becoming a self-directed learner in that it can develop and facilitate many functions
that are highly beneficial for effective learning and well-being—helping learners to:
• Set key goals for learning (e.g., short term, long-term, and appropriate challenge)
and deciding what needs to be learned for what purpose
• Know how to learn and plan a successful learning strategy (e.g., what, how, when
and where)
• Use specific metacognitive, cognitive, and motivational strategies to achieve the
learning goals
• Maintain positive beliefs and managing emotions to remain calm under pressure
• Persist, exercise volition to stay on track in the face of challenges and/or setbacks
• Monitor and review one’s progress and modify/change aspects of strategy based
on feedback (if necessary).
Research has shown that the effective use of metacognitive strategies has a high
impact on student attainment (e.g., Effect Size of 0.69, Hattie 2009). Similarly, Dig-
nath et al. (2008), quoted in Lai (2012), meta-analysed 48 studies investigating the
effect of training in self-regulation on learning and the use of strategies among
students from grades 1–6, as shown in Table 3.1.
The most effective metacognitive strategies included the combination of planning
and monitoring (mean effect size = 1.50) and the combination of planning and eval-
uation (mean effect-size = 1.46), both of which were more successful than teaching
any of the skills in isolation or teaching a combination of all three metacognitive
skills (planning, monitoring, and evaluation). Training approaches that combined
metacognitive components with other aspects of self-regulation, such as cognitive
84 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
or motivational strategies were also successful, with effect sizes of 0.81 and 0.97,
respectively (pp. 22–23).
Also, metacognition impacts other aspects of human psychological functioning,
and their behavioural consequences, simply as a result of its executive function. For
example, Critical Thinking—also one of the so-called twenty-first century competen-
cies, is very much under the ‘supervision’ (for better or for worse) of metacognition.
As Mango (2010) summarized:
Higher-order thinking (like critical thinking) requires executive control and executive pro-
cesses (that comes in the form of metacognition). Note, specifically, metacognition helps
in developing critical thinking, because it is likely that critical thinking requires a form of
meta-level operation. (p. 149)
Similarly, Pintrich and De Groot (1990) have shown that the use of cognitive
and self-regulatory strategies is an important component of student learning. For
example, students who use cognitive strategies like elaboration (e.g., summarizing,
paraphrasing) and organization (e.g., mind maps, concept maps) engage the content
at a deeper level of processing and are more likely to be able to recall the information,
understand it, and use it purposefully at a later time.
3.2 Making Sense of Metacognition: Unpacking… 85
The view that metacognition can be developed through carefully planned instruc-
tion is supported in the literature. For example, drawing from the research of Ericsson
et al. (1993), Schraw (1998) argues that:
Well organized instruction or the use of effective learning strategies may in large part compen-
sate for differences in IQ. In many cases, sustained practice and teacher modelling lead to the
acquisition of relevant task-specific knowledge as well as general metacognitive knowledge
that is either independent or moderately correlated with traditional IQ scores. (p. 117)
While the case is made for MC being the superordinate twenty-first century compe-
tence, based on its significance for effective learning and well-being, it is also widely
accepted that metacognitive knowledge is insufficient to promote student achieve-
ment. Students must also be motivated to use their metacognitive knowledge and
skills. In the context of metacognition, motivation is typically framed in terms of the
“beliefs and attitudes that affect the use and development of cognitive and metacog-
nitive skills” (Schraw et al. 2006, p. 112). According to them, motivation has two
primary components: (1) self-efficacy, which is confidence in one’s own ability to
perform a specific task and, (2) epistemological beliefs, which are beliefs about the
origin and nature of knowledge.
86 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
These two components of motivation are essentially concerned with beliefs, which
affect perception, feelings and behaviour, though have their origins in prior percep-
tions and experiences. They work as an internal processing system for decision-
making when people are confronted with new tasks. For example, Bandura (1997)
referred to self-efficacy as:
…beliefs in one’s capabilities to organize and execute the courses of action required to
produce given attainments. (p. 3)
and if they are poor pictures, the consequences may turn out just that way also.
Fortunately, they are changeable based on new experiences which is hardly surprising,
if we think back to what we believed to be true as children. Do you still believe in the
‘tooth fairy’, Santa Claus, or the bogeyman under the bed? As Adler (1996) cleverly
noted:
We forget that beliefs are no more than perceptions, usually with a limited sell-by date, yet
we act as though they were concrete realities. (p. 145)
It is not difficult to understand how beliefs profoundly affect the way people
approach their learning and the subsequent impact on attainment and engagement
levels. Beliefs act as major neurological filters that determine how we perceive exter-
nal reality (Fig. 3.2). In this way, they provide the inner maps we use to make sense
of the world around us. When we have a belief about something in our world, we
act as though it is true. It is what is in our Inner Personal Map of Reality that deter-
mines our perception, emotional responses and orientation to people and things in
the External World. While the External World is only knowable through our senses
and therefore can never be fully ascertained in purely objective terms (whatever this
is), our challenge as evidence-based teaching practitioners is to build increasingly
more useful Internal Maps of how best to facilitate learning and attainment for our
students (part of our External World) and improve the quality of their Inner Personal
Maps of Reality through the ways we teach and interact with them.
Motivation is fundamental in activating metacognition and both are essential for
learning. Most importantly, their impact is likely to be synergistic. For example,
once motivation is enacted, learners are more likely to want to be successful in their
learning tasks, and this is where metacognitive strategies are particularly useful. As
Martinez (2006) points out, metacognitive strategies can improve persistence and
motivation in the face of challenging tasks, which means the learner can maintain a
strong growth mindset, achieve successful task completion that, in turn, reinforces
self-efficacy. As an old saying goes, “nothing breeds success like success”.
Marzano’s (2007) research is of real pedagogic interest in terms of explaining how
different aspects of human psychological functioning interact in terms of influencing
an individual’s motivation to learn. His new taxonomy focuses on three internal
systems, all of which are important for learning. These are summarized below:
• The Self-system—This relates to the set of beliefs (and related feelings) the student
holds about his or her capabilities, the meaning attributed to the task in hand, along
with the perceived likelihood of success
• The Meta-cognitive system—This relates to the higher-level self-regulation of
the student in terms being able to monitor and evaluate his or her own thinking
process (e.g., setting goals, monitoring progress towards these goals and adapting
to difficulties)
• The Cognitive system—This is the system that reasons, and thinks in specific ways
(e.g., analyses, compares and contrasts, makes inferences and interpretations, and
evaluates) with the information at its disposal, to achieve the desired goals.
When faced with the option of participating in a new learning project or activity, it
is the Self-system which initially decides (whether consciously or subconsciously) to
give attention and then activates the Meta-cognitive and Cognitive systems to provide
structure and direction for the appropriate learning strategies and skills to acquire the
necessary knowledge, build understanding and skills to move progressively to goal
attainment. He found that teaching strategies that activated the Self-system had the
greatest effect on student learning, the Metacognitive system the next most effect,
and the Cognitive system least, though it is still substantial. What this means is
that it is the Self-system that activates the Meta-cognitive system, which actives the
Cognitive system, which creates learning. In the ideal situation for effective learning,
we would like to get all systems fully ‘up and running’ towards meeting the demands
of the desired learning goal. What we now can be reasonably sure of is that without
a desire to meet a task’s outcomes, belief in one’s capabilities to attain the necessary
knowledge and skill components and perception of likely success, there is probably
little effort invested to commit to task requirements. Quite simply, unless the Self-
system is firmly activated, the other important systems are not likely to be working
at anywhere near optimal levels.
Schunk and Zimmerman (2012), in summarizing the research findings conclude
that:
3.3 Metacognition and Motivation: Two Bedfellows … 89
Fig. 3.3 Relationship of motivation, metacognition and learning outcomes. Adapted from the work
Lens and Vansteekiste (2012)
Clearly, motivational processes play a vital role in initiating, guiding and sustaining student
efforts to self - regulate their learning. (p. 3)
The relationship between motivation and metacognition and its impact on learning
outcomes is summarized in Fig. 3.3.
In the practical pedagogic context, when selecting and using cognitive strategies,
the following heuristic is helpful:
• What is my learning purpose (e.g., extracting information from a text; organiz-
ing information from a variety of learning resources; building understanding;
cementing mental models in long term memory)
• What are useful strategies for this area of learning
• How to use the strategies effectively and efficiently
• When to use them for best results.
For example, I find mind-mapping very useful for writing research papers, as I
can identify the key structure and content areas on one sheet of paper and do ongoing
good thinking—critical, creative, metacognitive—to develop the paper content areas
from this central advance organizer. However, there is a skill in doing good mind-
mapping and one does need to know the subject domain quite well; otherwise, the
mind maps can become no more than mazes, confusing rather than aiding thinking
and learning.
Another strategy that I introduce to students early in a course of instruction is
Retrieval Practice (see Table 3.3). This strategy helps to build understanding and
cement knowledge in LTM. It involves students using their WM in response to a
question posed internally or externally (e.g., what do I know about agentic engage-
ment?) and searching LTM for what’s there (or not there). This is further enhanced
3.4 What Is the Difference Between Cognition and Metacognition? 91
when students engage in self-talk (talking aloud) and verbalizing with others as they
can assess what they know already that’s useful, identify gaps and/or misconcep-
tions, and take appropriate learning action (e.g., employ specific cognitive strategies
if needed). The process of extracting what’s in LTM and subjecting it to scrutiny in
WM, and then putting it back (often in an improved form—richer mental schemata)
is a well-validated strategy for effective learning from a cognitive neuroscience per-
spective. Repeated retrieval practice, especially when spaced out over time, builds
the learning as a solid neural network and mental model for understanding. Hence
future recall, when needed, becomes increasing quicker, as well as more effective
for connecting to new learning and enhancing understanding in that topic area.
Metacognition, in contrast, can be viewed as how we mentally act in relation to our
cognition (e.g., monitoring and evaluating the quality of the self-regulatory process,
including the choice and use of the cognitive learning strategies being employed).
Martinez (2006) put it very succinctly:
Metacognition can be seen as evaluation turned inward, especially turned toward our ideas.
(p. 698)
It typically comes into play when cognition becomes problematic (e.g., when tasks
are more challenging) or when a new learning challenge has been identified. Many
researchers in the field acknowledge that they are probably mutually dependent on
each other—hence cannot be entirely separated (e.g., Flavell 1979; Veenman et al.
2006). Figure 3.4 summarizes the key concepts and their likely relationship.
An important summary point is that students need to understand the distinc-
tion between cognition and metacognition to become self-regulated (e.g., Schraw
1998, p. 118). Schraw argues that metacognition is a multidimensional phenomenon,
domain-general in nature, and that metacognitive knowledge and regulation can be
improved through instructional strategies. His view is that cognitive skills tend to
be encapsulated within domains or subject areas, whereas metacognitive skills span
multiple domains, even when those domains have little in common (p. 116). With-
out a clear understanding of these distinct mental processes, what each can do for
effective learning in different learning contexts, and how to work them in unison, is
like driving a manual gear car without knowledge and skill in clutch control.
92 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
Fig. 3.4 Relationships between metacognition, cognition and the Real World
Hence, individuals who have developed high MC are likely to be more focused
and successful in their approach to learning—possibly in life generally—than those
lacking in this capability. They can run ‘quality assurance’ checks on what they know,
don’t know, need to find out, as well as ensure that they are utilizing, monitoring and
evaluating the necessary cognitive, motivational and affective strategies needed for
goal attainment. Therefore, while metacognition and cognitive (thinking) skills have
a high impact on student attainment, the situation is still largely as Ambrose (2010)
notes:
Unfortunately, these metacognitive skills tend to fall outside the content area of most courses,
and consequently, they are often neglected in instruction. (p. 191)
In the context of teaching and learning, a major pedagogic goal is to teach students
how to develop and use their MC to effectively select, use and evaluate appropriate
cognitive and motivational strategies for meeting their learning goals in different
situations, enhancing their well-being and developing the ability to be self-directed
lifelong learners. This constitutes perhaps the gold standard in terms of educational
aims. In practice, educational systems (and institutions) vary greatly in terms of
policy, practices, and resources. Hence, Stenhouse’s (1989) observation is as relevant
today as it was at the time of his writing:
…the central problem of curriculum study is the gap between our ideals and our attempt to
operationalize them. (p. 3)
Despite many approaches and models in the literature, there is general agreement
that SDL (or SRL) involves the following iterative stages, irrespective of the specific
terminology employed:
1. Planning Learning
2. Managing Learning Performance and Process
3. Reviewing and Evaluating Learning.
1. Planning Learning
As an old saying goes, ‘fail to plan, plan to fail’. This applies equally to learn-
ing almost anything. Essentially, when we seek to learn something new or extend
our existing learning in some way, we are going about the business of enhancing
aspects of our long-term memory system (e.g., in the language of neuroscience,
building on existing neural networks in terms of the richness of pathway connec-
tions). Once established, this enhances our understanding of what we are intending
to learn and, hopefully, our competence in applying this new learning skilfully in
real-world contexts.
Hence, in this planning stage, the learner (you, me, whoever) must do the neces-
sary cognitive and metacognitive work, and it’s necessary to have key underpinning
knowledge on how we learn and how we think—as the latter involves the cognitive
and metacognitive stuff.
Here’s a summary of the key areas/questions to address in the planning stage:
• Assess the task at hand and set key realistic challenging goals for learning
• Identify interest/value (and personal strengths and weaknesses) for the learning
involved
• Evaluate existing knowledge and skills to identify gaps, and how to address these
• Design a successful learning strategy (e.g., specific strategies to be used, when and
how).
The importance of goals was identified and outlined in Chap. 2: Core Principle
1: Learning goals, objectives and proficiency expectations are clearly visible to
learners.
At the initial stage, before setting clear goals, it is useful to assess the task(s) at
hand and have some idea of what may be involved in terms of time and resources
needed (e.g., knowledge, skills to be acquired). Goals need to be challenging but
must equally be a ‘viable proposition’ in terms of one’s life situation. If they are too
challenging, one may be left with feelings of frustration and this may not be good in
terms of self-efficacy in that area. This situation is well captured in an old English
saying (it may be elsewhere also), “you have bitten off more than you can chew”.
Equally, if goals are too easy to attain, they won’t stretch competence, build volition
or grit, and may feed mediocrity and complacency.
3.6 The Self-regulation Cycle 95
Research shows that academic performance gains range from 16 to 41% in class-
rooms where students are explicitly taught how and why it is important to set learning
goals (e.g. Marzano 2007). Setting specific and challenging goals furthers strength-
ens learning by providing students with benchmarks to measure their progress and
to motivate themselves to exert the effort to accomplish their aims. As Hattie (2009)
points out:
A major reason difficult goals are more effective is that they lead to a clearer notion of
success and direct the student’s attention to relevant behaviours of outcomes. (p. 164)
Furthermore, when teachers help students connect to classroom goals in ways that
have personal meaning for them, there is a much greater chance they will be motivated
to engage in the necessary hard work often required in achieving challenging learning
goals. Where possible, allowing students to set some goals for themselves adds a
further intrinsic motivational element to the learning process, as student derived
goals can:
• enhance one’s sense of autonomy (Ryan and Deci 2017) through choice and more
focused attention toward goal-relevant tasks and the most effective strategies to
attain them
• increase one’s effort and volition to attain them through motivation and desire for
mastery
• increase one’s affective reactions to targeted outcomes (i.e. the investment of
emotions and desire to maintain self-efficacy).
Goals then provide the important role of framing cognitive representations of a
future event and, as such, influence motivation through certain key processes. More
specifically, goals, as Alderman (2008) identifies:
• Direct instruction and action toward an intended target. This helps individuals
focus on the task at hand and organize their knowledge and strategies toward the
accomplishment of the goal;
• Mobilize effort in proportion to the difficulty of the task to be accomplished;
• Promote persistence and effort over time for complex tasks. This provides a reason
to continue to work hard even if the task is not going well;
• Promote the development of creative plans and strategies to reach them;
• Provide a reference point that provides information about one’s performance
(p. 107).
Identify Interest/Value and Personal Strengths and Weaknesses for the Learning
Involved
by Eccles and Wigfield. While many writers focus on the importance of intrinsic
motivation (e.g., Ryan and Deci 2017; Reeve and Tseng 2001), and this should be
a main pedagogic focus, we must recognize that other extrinsic factors are equally
important. Measuring precisely how much a persons’ motivation is determined by
intrinsic or extrinsic factors may be of academic interest though less impactful in
terms of pedagogic practice. In many cases for people, their motivation for different
activities will have extrinsic and intrinsic aspects, whose levels and focus vary over
time, even over situations. In writing this book, is my motivation intrinsic or extrinsic?
Both, but I would not be able to put a percentage on this. I like it when all the value
components are strong, and the costs are low. I’ll not argue the compositions too
critically.
Cost-Benefit Analysis is a core concept in decision making, which appears to
apply in some form in most contexts and cultures I have experienced. Of course, how
rational is this process enacted by people is a completely different issue. Often the
costs and benefits cannot be framed (or as clear as we may like) before the decision
is taken, as certain information and outcomes are not available and/or cannot be
predicted with high accuracy. Also, this process requires Good Thinking, and as we
have explored in some detail earlier, which is far from a given pre-requisite in human
psychological functioning.
Identifying what you know and don’t know is fundamental for effective learning.
Specifically, this applies to evaluate one’s strengths and weaknesses, both in terms
of knowledge and skill levels for a specific learning goal, as well as appraising one’s
dispositions for learning (e.g., belief systems; ability to maintain volition in the face
of challenges). A major area of motivational theory has focused on how attributions
(what we believe to be true) impact motivation and learning. For example, research
(e.g., Weiner 1992) shows that students who attribute their successes and failures to
internal and controllable sources (such as one’s effort) are more likely to persist on
difficult tasks and experience success than those who attribute success and failure to
external or uncontrollable sources (such as innate ability, luck, task difficulty).
Perhaps the most notable research in the literature in recent years is that of Dweck
(2006) on the impact of a Growth Mind-set (as compared to a fixed Mind-set) on
98 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
student learning and attainment. This was outlined and explained earlier. Equally
important are what Bandura (2004) refers to as Self-Efficacy Beliefs, which “are
rooted in the core belief that one has the power to effect change by one’s actions”
(p. 622). As Pajares (2012) summarizes:
People with a strong sense of self-efficacy approach difficult tasks as challenges to be mas-
tered rather than as threats to be avoided. They have a greater intrinsic interest and deep
engrossment in activities, and they set themselves challenging goals and maintain a strong
commitment to them. (p. 113)
The ability to use metacognition, selecting, using and evaluating cognitive strate-
gies is of key importance in terms of goal attainment and becoming a self-directed
learner. Therefore, students must develop a deep understanding of how to use
metacognitive and cognitive strategies for learning and well-being. (e.g., what these
are, how they benefit specific aspects of the learning process and how to use them
effectively). In generic terms, if we don’t know how to do something, we are unlikely
to attain competence even with much practice. Students need to learn how metacog-
nition works in the context of good thinking and how this is part of the process of
effective learning.
In terms of metacognition, most theorists would agree in distinguishing two main
aspects: Metacognitive Knowledge, and Metacognitive Regulation:
• Metacognitive Knowledge refers to the information that individuals hold about
their cognition and about strategies which impact on it. It also includes one’s
self-efficacy beliefs, motivation or interest in relation to learning demands and
3.6 The Self-regulation Cycle 99
goals. This knowledge provides a plan or guides for processing, the rules of which
may be more (explicit) or less (implicit) in terms of being amenable to conscious
awareness and verbal expression.
• Metacognitive Regulation involves the execution of control strategies to regulate
our cognitive activities to ensure successful planning, monitoring and evaluation
of our actions towards desired goals. As we engage in metacognitive activities,
based on our metacognitive knowledge, this creates ‘metacognitive experiences’
(e.g., Flavell 1981; Schwarz 2010). This is the conscious learning we derive from
applying our metacognitive knowledge in new metacognitive regulation tasks. It
is essentially a process of assimilation and accommodation of new metacogni-
tive knowledge with prior metacognitive knowledge. For example, in writing this
chapter I have probably been metacognitive throughout (not always consciously)
and not just cognitively but also in terms of affect. Some days I feel more pro-
ductive, even excited; others, less so. I have certainly learned that metacognition
is, in fact, ‘a many-headed monster of obscure parentage’. Has it enhanced my
MC?—well, I finished this book. Just writing that statement enhanced my volition
a bit.
Through both retrieval practice and deliberate practice in self-regulation, students
will acquire both the understanding and competence relating to metacognitive knowl-
edge and regulation, to the extent that it becomes a habit of mind (e.g., Costa) and
an adaptive competence across a range of learning contexts.
Similarly, in using cognitive strategies, students will need to have gone through
the same learning process, so that they have acquired:
• Strategic knowledge on how cognition works in facilitating the learning pro-
cess (e.g., information processing through the working of memory systems). This
enables knowing what a cognitive strategy can do in terms of assisting learning in
tackling a specific learning task. For example, in using a mind map one needs to
understand what the tool can do, how to use it effectively, and how this helps the
learning purpose (e.g., summarize information, show relationships/connections,
aid memory processing)
• Knowledge of task, which enables the ability to understand, analyse and make
inferences and interpretations of what a lesson objective, activity or procedure
(as explained by a teacher) specifically involves in terms of what learning (and
performance) is needed
• Knowledge of self, which is essential for identifying and evaluating personal (exist-
ing) strengths and weaknesses as a learner and can choose appropriate learning
strategies (knowledge of strategy) that are aligned with the task(s) at hand (knowl-
edge of task). This will involve both an understanding of systemic barriers to
cognition and learning, as well as openness of personal recognition of specific
traits that may mitigate effective learning (e.g., impulsivity, procrastination).
Cognitive strategies, by definition, will focus on specific aspects of the learning
process and types of learning. These will essentially fall into one or more of the
following activity categorizations:
100 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
• Identifying, clarifying and verifying what is already known about a topic or activity
(e.g., activating prior knowledge)
• Framing clear challenging desired goals
• Planning ways to obtain relevant knowledge, resources and develop the neces-
sary understandings and skills to achieve desired goals—create/produce a learning
plan/strategy
• Finding and accessing relevant information sources and resources
• Extracting, organizing and summarizing information
• Processing information between WM and LTM (connecting new information to
prior knowledge)—thinking—to build understanding in LTM. The use of retrieval
practice (pulling information back and forth from LTM to WM to check under-
standing and knowledge completion and cement the learning in LTM at the neural
level
• Using new knowledge and skills in different situations to elaborate understanding
and skill transfer
• Managing one’s motivation and volition in the often frustrating and boring process
of learning, especially the thinking part which is often tiring.
Strategy Evaluation Matrixes (e.g., Schraw 2006) are useful, especially for identi-
fying, analysing and evaluating cognitive, metacognitive and motivational strategies
(see Table 3.5).
As students develop both metacognitive and cognitive skills, both their mastery
levels and self-efficacy in using such skills will likely increase. This, in turn, helps
to maintain/enhance motivation (whether intrinsic or extrinsic) for learning.
2. Managing Learning Performance and Process
Good planning is crucial, and the key components have been outlined and illustrated
above. However, the best plan in the world does not mean success without the appro-
priate action, as we all know. A great lesson plan is not a great lesson; enacted by a
poor teacher it is likely to have little impact in terms of desired learning.
In this stage of the self-regulatory process, metacognition focuses on the use
of cognitive and motivational strategies to ensure the successful maintenance of the
overall learning strategy towards goal attainment. Hence, it is important to be mindful
of:
• Ensuring the necessary skilful use and effort is put into the implementation of the
cognitive learning strategies employed. Unless they are implemented effectively
and conscientiously, there may be little benefit in terms of the desired learning
outcomes.
• Maintaining a Growth Mindset. We are fragile creatures and can easily slip into
self-doubt and loss of confidence. This happens to even the top sportspeople, so
the rest of us had better be mindful.
Students need to be explicitly taught to expect challenges and setbacks that may
seem to push them backwards in what they are trying to learn. This is where self-
control and willpower are important to avoid or at least mitigate procrastination and
maintain persistence. As Borkowski et al. (2000) made clear:
3.6 The Self-regulation Cycle 101
Students need to learn that classroom activities require them to work hard to achieve
understanding. (p. 33)
Many terms have been used to capture the underlying traits and characteristics
that facilitate self-control and willpower, including perseverance, conscientiousness,
resilience, to name but a few. The new vogue seems to be ‘Grit’. I remember it well
in my boxing classes at school, as the instructor would say words akin to “show
some grit, Dennis”. I was not good enough to box professionally, but the training did
develop some grit. Going to university was a relative breeze after years of getting up
at 5 a.m. on cold English winter days and doing a 10 km run, followed by several
hundred push-ups and sits ups. There was never any sympathy from the boxing
102 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
instructor—he was a second world war veteran, and had little time for excuses not to
push oneself to the limit (as he saw it). From an educational perspective, Duckworth
et al. (2007) defined grit as “perseverance and passion for long-term goals” (p. 1087).
Tough (2013) postulates that in the real world, learning to react to failure is as critical
to success as academic achievement. Noncognitive character traits such as resilience,
drive and delayed gratification are as important as cognitive skills (Farrington et al.
2012).
Of wider concern, Baumeister and Tierney (2012) conclude that:
…most major problems, personal and social, centre on the failure of self-control: compulsive
spending and borrowing, impulsive violence, underachievement in school, procrastination
at work, alcohol and drug abuse, unhealthy diet, lack of exercise, chronic anxiety, explosive
anger. (p. 2)
However, research suggests (e.g., Baumeister and Tierney 2012) that we have
limited amounts of willpower and distractions, especially in this digital age, are all
around us. After studying thousands of people inside and outside the lab, researchers
found that experiments consistently demonstrated two lessons:
1. You have a finite amount of willpower that becomes depleted as you use it.
2. You use the same stock of willpower for all manner of tasks. (p. 35)
As they note:
Emotional control is uniquely difficult because you generally can’t change your mood by
an act of will. You can change what you think about or how you behave, but you can’t force
yourself to be happy. (p. 37)
When researchers compared students’ grades with nearly three dozen personality traits, self-
control turned out to be the only trait that predicted a college student’s grade-point average
better than chance. Self-control also proved to be a better predictor of college grades than the
student’s IQ or SAT score. Although raw intelligence was an advantage, the study showed
that self-control was more important because it helped the students show up more reliably for
classes, start their homework earlier, and spend more time working than watching television.
(pp. 11–12)
The results couldn’t be clearer: self-control is a vital strength and key to success in life.
(p. 13)
the journey to expertise, as we are not born with a ‘Blake Slate’ as Pinker (2003)
so comprehensively documented. In summary, on the question of how much time
is necessary to develop expertise, there are certainly no ‘quick fixes’ and it will
involve much time and commitment, but 10,000 h may be more of a metaphor than
an evidence-based heuristic.
To develop an effective approach to developing SDL, all components of this
framework need to be addressed thoughtfully in the context of student profiles. As
an analogy, think of the mind like a car, which is a system, and, if we don’t know the
parts of the system, what they do and how they work, and their relationship to each
other—we are like the poor chap in this cartoon:
On the motivational stakes, we can only provide the rationale, learning strategies
and the opportunities to develop MC. It’s ultimately their call.
3.7 A Curriculum Model for Developing Metacognitive … 105
It has been a tough call getting to grips with the many heads of the metacognition
monster. Yes, it certainly is a monster, but as Arnold Schwarzenegger said in Predator,
when he noticed the yellow ‘blood’ of the alien on nearby vegetation, “If it bleeds,
we can kill it”. Predator was a film in which a group of expert combat veterans were
recruited to check out some mysterious killings in a jungle and discovered a strange
presence that turned out to be an alien creature who seemed to enjoy hunting in the
intergalactic context (the latter is my interpretation). Hence, once we have a clearer
evidence-based frame on the nature and components of these heads, and how they
work (and often don’t work), we are in a better position to develop and facilitate
strategies to help students manage their own ‘Many-Headed Monster’. Given the
above analogy, we can subject these ‘many heads’ to rigorous pedagogic analysis,
finding ways of managing them better, and develop MC.
Veenman et al. (2006), in reviewing the literature, identified three fundamental
principles that are necessary for successful metacognitive instruction:
(a) embedding metacognitive instruction in the content matter to ensure connectiv-
ity,
(b) informing learners about the usefulness of metacognitive activities to make them
exert the initial extra effort, and
(c) prolonged training to guarantee the smooth and maintained application of
metacognitive activity.
He suggests following the WWW & H rule (What, When, Why, and How to do)
in implementing these principles (p. 9). This rule is essentially a cognitive strategy
that creates a structured thinking heuristic in the planning and development process.
To be successful, one would need to know what constitutes the What (always a big
question), and Why, possess the necessary knowledge (i.e., conceptual understand-
ing) of knowing When and Where to apply it, and the other big question—How to
do this effectively and efficiently.
I have created a curriculum design model, incorporating the heuristics of Good
Thinking outlined earlier—(what I euphemistically called the ‘Thinking Curricu-
lum’), (Sale 2013) that guides the whole curriculum development cycle and related
processes. Figure 3.6 summarizes the key process.
In basic terms, this means that the types of thinking incorporated in the Learning
Outcomes must be effectively taught through the Instructional Methods/Strategies
used and accurately measured in the Assessment System.
In developing a curriculum that incorporates MC, it is first necessary to estab-
lish a sound theoretical framework that establishes what Good Thinking entails—as
detailed in Chap. 2: Core Principle 4: Good thinking promotes the building of under-
standing. Without a sufficiently valid and practical model of good thinking, notions
of effectively teaching it, let alone assessing it, are tenuous at best. Not an easy task,
as we have seen, and it’s not surprising that many teaching professionals are confused
106 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
in this area. The next big question concerns the extent to which thinking is amenable
to development through pedagogic means, and how best we can do this from an EBT
approach.
Thinking is, essentially, a human performance activity. How we have learned to
think will determine in large part how we think, much the same as for any kind of
learned activity.
Furthermore, as Perkins (1995) points out, “People can learn to think and act
intelligently.” (p. 18). Similarly, Coles and Robinson (1989) argue that:
The problem is not whether we can teach thinking. The evidence suggests we can. The
problem continues to be whether we are willing to make the pedagogic changes necessary
to do so and if we are, which changes might be the most effective. (p. 20)
Paul (1993) provides an interesting analogy between the development of mind and
physical fitness. He points out that the mind, like the body, “has its form of fitness or
excellence” which is “caused by and reflected in activities done in accordance with
standards (critically).” (p. 103). He goes on to argue that:
A fit mind can successfully engage in the designing, fashioning, formulating, originating,
or producing of intellectual products worthy of its challenging ends…Minds indifferent to
standards and disciplined judgment tend to judge inexactly, inaccurately, inappropriately,
prejudicially. (pp. 103–44)
Swartz and Perkins (1990) identify six areas of ‘improvement’ that become
apparent when students’ thinking gets better:
1. Awareness of one’s thinking
2. Investment of effort in one’s thinking
3. Attitude towards thinking processes
4. Organization of thinking processes
5. Development of subskills
6. Smoothness in the thinking process. (p. 24)
The learning outcomes can be specially written to cue specific type(s) of thinking
or in terms of direct performance, as illustrated below:
Type of Thinking
• Analyse the impact of pollution on water quality
• Compare and contrast a range of retaining structures
• Generate new design options for marketing a health food product
• Predict the outcomes of specified legal scenarios
• Evaluate a policy for animal protection.
Real Work Performance
• Conduct product packaging tests for a specified product
• Prepare a voyage passage plan
108 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
Fig. 3.7 Summary heuristics for identifying and structuring metacognitive and cognitive strategies
into a curriculum plan
3.7 A Curriculum Model for Developing Metacognitive … 109
to the world of work, both now and in the conceivable future. The essential thing
about performance-based learning outcomes is that the subject content can be orga-
nized in ways that are most likely to be experienced as meaningful for learners. In
this context, the types of thinking, as well as other process skills (e.g., communica-
tion and teamwork), can be naturally embedded in the learning tasks that students
are required to master. For example, The National Vocational Qualifications (NVQ)
framework, which a few decades ago revolutionized vocational education in the
UK, are essentially competency-based standards derived from functional analysis of
industrial roles. Their attainment is not governed by curriculum duration or modes
of delivery, but solely on the learners’ ability to meet the full range of competencies
in a vocational area.
However, while competency models are more tailored to the present need for
effective and efficient learning, there has been a criticism of these approaches. A
major concern has been the extent to which learners possess sufficient depth of both
content and process knowledge (i.e., accurate neural structures/mental models) of
what is necessary for conceptual understanding and adaptive learning—transfer of the
competences in a range of real work situations, even when achieving competence in
the assessment framework. Much of the problem lies in the reduction of occupational
roles into units and elements of competence, based on a methodology of functional
analysis. This typically provides an atomized view of real work activity and falls into
the trap of the decomposability assumption, which as Resnick and Resnick (1989)
note:
…has been seriously challenged by recent cognitive research, which recognizes that com-
plicated skills and competencies owe their complexity not just to the number and compo-
nents they engage but also to interactions among the components and heuristics for calling
upon them. Complex competencies, therefore, cannot be defined just by listing all their
components. (p. 5)
The learning outcomes for a thinking curriculum, then, must avoid the decompos-
ability trap by ensuring that performances represent holistic and complete activities,
not just bits of facts and skills. The essence of this important point is captured by
Fennimore and Tinzmann (1990) when they argue that a thinking curriculum must:
…always treat tasks as indivisible wholes; variations that acknowledge the novice status of
the learner are changes the teacher can make in the environment. (p. 4)
that require metacognitive and cognitive activity (e.g., critical and creative thinking
skills), and when and where they are to be taught in the curriculum. Use the frame-
work and method outlined here as an evaluative guide—and some Good Thinking!
You may need to rewrite certain learning outcomes to make specific types of think-
ing explicit where relevant in the curriculum structuring. Using the building analogy
prior, this sets a solid foundation for the rest of the curriculum design and development
process.
The term SignaturePedagogies (Schulman 2005) has become popular recently, which
emphasizes that different professional fields have their distinct pedagogy in terms
of the appropriateness of methods used for enhancing learning. In most basic terms,
there is a need to contextualize pedagogy to the subject content—what Shulman refers
to as Pedagogic Content Knowledge. This emphasizes on domain contextualized
pedagogy rather than generic pedagogy.
However, while it is necessary to select methods and facilitate learning in the con-
text of subject domains, I see this as no more than a feature of EBT (e.g., combining
high effective methods to the subject content and student profile). For example,
in teaching literature there may not be many learning areas where problem-based
learning would be an effective or efficient method. In contrast, in engineering, which
is very much concerned with solving real-world problems, it could be seen as a
‘signature method’ in curriculum planning. However, having worked extensively in
engineering education, I could equally make the case for case-based learning (no
pun intended) or simulated practice—as well as many other method combinations.
Hattie’s (2009) extensive meta-analysis found little support for the use of teaching
specifically focusing on subject content knowledge, as the overall effect size was
relatively small (0.11). Hence, while the term signature pedagogies is useful in terms
of identifying core methods for a field of study, and planning instruction accordingly,
it is better seen in a wider EBT context re method blending.
There cannot be a single ‘silver-bullet’ (simple direct solution to a problem;
derived from the folklore of killing werewolves with a silver bullet) teaching method
that will work with all students in all contexts. Apart from effectiveness, students
(and the rest of us) will get bored with over repetition of one method. I remember
3 decades ago, when teachers were being introduced to Project-Based Learning in
a college in the UK, there great enthusiasm for its use. And, it did seem to work
well initially, as students showed greater engagement and made positive responses
to having more choice and variety in their learning; rather than the traditional lecture
format every lesson. However, after 2–3 weeks and several projects on the go, student
interest waned. I remember hearing a student saying something akin to, ‘can we just
have some teaching rather than another project’. The point is that any method, no
3.8 Developing Metacognitive Capability: Evidence-Based Strategies 111
matter its logical effectiveness, will lose impact when overused. Remember my Chilli
Crab story from Chap. 2: Core Principle 5: Instructional methods and presentation
mediums engage the range of human of senses.
However, students through appropriate instruction can learn to:
1. Understand essential knowledge on how the mind works in terms of human
learning, our unique species-specific capability for being metacognitive and
its potential for learning, well-being and life enhancement through better
self-regulation
2. Thoughtfully use metacognitive, cognitive and motivational learning strategies
and skills
3. Maintain volition and willpower to achieve personal goals.
While there is still a current vogue for constructivism underpinning much of class-
room practice, research shows that the most significant gains in student achievement
result when students are taught the use of metacognitive strategies in explicit ways
(e.g., Haidar and Al Naqabi 2008; Kistner et al. 2010). Kistner et al. noted that
while explicit strategy instruction of metacognitive skills is positively correlated with
achievement gains, as compared to implicit instruction, teachers use implicit meth-
ods more often than explicit ones. In an analysis of 60 lessons, involving 20 teachers,
they found that on average, teachers taught strategies through implicit instruction in
comparison to explicit instruction at a ratio of 5 to 1. Implicit instruction is where an
explanation is made of how the strategy is effective and students need to work out
how to do it; in contrast, explicit instruction involves the simultaneous verbalizing
of the teachers’ thought processes and/or asking specific questions to the students
during the demonstration and modelling of the strategy.
From my experience the characteristics of explicit strategy instruction can be
framed to include the following integrated instructional activities:
• Direct instruction on the strategies—what they are, when and how to use them
effectively. This requires a clear explanation and demonstration on how metacog-
nition and cognition work, and the benefits of using the strategies for learning.
This can be illustrated with a range of examples and stories (both yours, others
and the students)
• Modelling the strategies—Making ‘Thinking Visible’ (e.g., eliciting the students’
thinking, modelling expert thinking, thinking aloud and verbalizing, and mind-
fulness). From this Good Thinking can be made explicit, modelled, practised and
learned
• Retrieval, Deliberate and Spaced Practice opportunities for using the strategies in
real-life/work contexts (e.g., using questions and activities to cue the different types
of thinking, develop and extend their application and cement deep understanding
112 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
in long term memory; real life performance tasks that incorporate the range of MC
strategies in the context of the subject content).
Direct Instruction
Direct instruction (DI) often conjures up images of teachers talking and students lis-
tening, and one in which there is little opportunity for student engagement, questions,
and an inevitable loss of attention after some 10–15 min. In this situation, it may
seem that students are not active and therefore are unlikely to be doing the necessary
cognitive work—thinking—to make connections with prior knowledge and develop
a deep understanding of the concepts to be learned. The notion that students need to
be actively involved in the learning process to be more engaged and to learn better is
well documented in the literature. Active learning has been identified as one of the
seven principles of good practice in undergraduate education (e.g., Chickering and
Gamson 1987). For learning to be active, students must do more than listen, they
must read, write, discuss, or be engaged in solving problems. Most importantly, to
be actively involved, students must engage in such higher-order thinking tasks as
analysis, synthesis, and evaluation. Students must be doing things, and then think-
ing about why they are doing them. These kinds of activities can include case study,
cooperative learning, debates, drama, role-playing and simulation, and peer teaching.
However, getting students to do activities while engaging—at least at the
behavioural level—does not mean that they are effectively learning what is intended.
Active learning methods must be aligned to desired learning outcomes and get stu-
dents thinking about the key concepts to be learned. It’s the thinking that does the
work in building understanding—forming neurologically-based mental models in
long-term memory. Equally, it cannot be assumed that students listening to a well-
constituted lecture, with clear concise explanation of key concepts, illustrated with
interesting real-life examples (and non-examples), striking stories and good visuals,
does not get students engaged at both cognitive and emotional levels—which are
more important for learning than simply behavioural engagement.
Indeed, while a long, disorganized lecture with a monotonous voice tone can be
labelled a sin of teaching, so in the same vein of thinking, this can be applied to
activity for activities sake.
Notions of the teacher no longer being the ‘sage on the stage’ but the ‘guide on the
side’ so aptly reflects this dichotomy. Similarly, Brown et al. (2014) refer to ‘student-
directed learning’ (a theory now current among some parents and educators), which
holds that students know best what they need to study to master a subject, and what
pace and methods work best for them, as “laudatory” (p. 123). It’s nonsense at best,
dangerous nonsense at worse. Just more excursions back into ‘Educational Jurassic
Park’. Expert teaching is neither of these—it is much, much more. Learning to be
self-directed requires student engagement and effort, but the evidence suggests—and
the very premises of teaching are based on—that they need much by way of expert
help in getting there.
DI as a method, assumes that all students can learn new material when (a) they
have mastered prerequisite knowledge and skills and (b) the instruction is unambigu-
ous (e.g., Stockard et al. 2018, p. 480). The importance of student’s possessing the
3.9 The Need for Explicit Teaching of Metacognition 113
necessary prior knowledge is a core principle of learning, essential for connecting and
assimilating new knowledge to prior knowledge in long-term memory. Clear (unam-
biguous) explanation supports the thinking process to build further understanding
and, ‘mastery learning’ ensures the necessary skill acquisitions. In this scenario,
they will have developed the necessary neural networks and clear mental schemata
(model) of what is to be learned. Furthermore, DI emphasizes the importance of test-
ing, which uses retrieval practice, to ensure that the learning is well established in
long-term memory. The same conditions apply to skill development. Recent research
by Stockard et al. (2018), supported earlier reviews of the DI effectiveness literature.
The estimated effects were consistently positive. The authors note that:
…despite the very large body of research supporting its effectiveness, DI has not been
widely embraced or implemented. In part, this avoidance of DI may be fuelled by the current
popularity of constructivism. (p. 502)
Certainly, our nation’s children deserve both effective and efficient instruction. As one of
the anonymous reviewers of our article put it, “Researchers and practitioners cannot afford
to ignore the effectiveness research on DI”. (p. 503)
In this process, I can check their understanding, deal with knowledge gaps and
misconceptions, and provide the retrieval practice for them to cement this learning in
long-term memory. The important point here is that the explicit teaching of MC is not
a passive lecturing scenario, but a highly effective and active teacher-led instructional
approach.
Modelling MC Strategies
Schraw (1998) specifically emphasizes:
Modelling of regulatory skills such as planning, monitoring, and self-evaluating, is espe-
cially important. Every Teacher should make a concerted effort to model explicitly these
behaviours. (p. 122)
Modelling MC strategies extend the knowledge from the explicit instruction to the
how of using the strategies, showing what is to be done at the specific strategy level.
For example, if mind mapping as a cognitive strategy is explained and illustrated,
modelling goes through the essential stages, procedures of how to do it, from the
central framing of the learning area to the selection of the superordinate branches,
and then to extensions. Other features that enhance the mind map, such as colour
and images are them modelled and demonstrated.
Using a Whole-Part-Whole Strategy (see Fig. 3.9) is useful here, as it provides a
key structure to the process, utilizes retrieval practice and reduces cognitive load.
The WPW strategy is analogous to completing a jig-saw puzzle, in that we can
continually refer back to the whole picture while grouping various components based
on similarity (e.g., in a jig-saw puzzle, there are always specific features, such as sky,
objects, people, animals). As we use the strategy, we are doing ongoing retrieval
practice, and as more of the segments are put correctly into place, the task becomes
easier and the cognitive load less. One could say that life is one big jigsaw puzzle,
albeit a more difficult one than a country scene, and there always seem to be pieces
missing but we don’t know what they are.
As students work through this learning strategy, metacognitive, cognitive and
motivational strategies can be systematically infused and facilitated as students tackle
3.9 The Need for Explicit Teaching of Metacognition 115
different learning challenges. Educational research (e.g., Schraw 1998) and practice
support the generic teaching of metacognitive and cognitive skills, though customiz-
ing cognitive skills to the subject domain context. This makes perfect sense as we
want our students to be mindful not just in the biology lesson, but across all subjects;
in-fact much more than that, in their lives holistically. There are serious limitations
in displaying good thinking in one life situation, but not in others.
Making Thinking Visible
In the process of helping students to build an understanding of what constitutes good
thinking and how to develop MC, making thinking visible (both student thinking and
teacher thinking) is essential. Questioning is a key strategy in developing all types
of thinking. As the famous success coach Anthony Robbins (2001) wrote:
Questions are the primary way we learn virtually everything. Thinking itself is nothing but
the process of asking and answering questions. (pp. 179–180)
The use of questions creates a stimulus to get students searching (both consciously
and sub/unconsciously) through their LTM system to find appropriate responses to
the stimulus question. The process of questioning, when done skillfully, makes the
invisible internal neural representations occurring when thinking visible to some
extent. As Ritchhart et al. (2011) point out:
We need to make thinking visible because it provides us with the information we as teachers
need to plan opportunities that will take students’ learning to the next level and enable
continued engagement with the ideas being explored. (p. 27)
In this process, it’s necessary to ensure that good thinking is clearly and explic-
itly explained, modelled and then practised through application in a range of rel-
evant activities that incorporate key subject content learning outcomes. Students
must develop their accurate representation of the heuristics of good thinking, which
116 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
involves the forming of rich integrated neural networks in the brains to have use-
ful models for doing good thinking in real-life contexts. As Sheppard et al. (2009)
recognized in advocating that:
…teachers have to make their intellectual processes (their performances) visible. This means
that the teacher-expert has to make visible to learners the otherwise invisible processes of
thinking that underlie complex cognitive operations …
Teachers have to articulate and demonstrate rather than assume the thought processes they
want students to learn. (p. 188)
Many studies (e.g., Pintrich and De Groot 1990; Schraw and Dennison 2004) have
found that having metacognitive knowledge, such as knowing what factors affect
one’s thinking (person variables), how to make sense of a problem or how different
3.9 The Need for Explicit Teaching of Metacognition 117
problems demand different cognitions (task variables) and knowing when and why to
use a skill (strategy variables) facilitate metacognitive regulation. However, having
only an awareness for the need to apply metacognitive strategies is not enough for
good performance; one must also know when, how and which strategy to use in
different learning contexts. The same reasoning applies across most learning fields
and domains.
To facilitate this effectively, the key terminology relating to the various heuristics
of the types of thinking needs to become part of the language of learning (to con-
solidate a language of thinking). For example, I often hear teachers, who I assume
are seeking to encourage student thinking, use terms like “What are your comments
on this” or “Let’s discuss this”—even “I want good thinking on this”. This latter
example assumes that students have a mental model (schemata) of what constitutes
thinking. However, in practice, this may vary widely (e.g., Fig. 3.10). If they have
no prior useful mental model, then they are either blank or in the process of just
commenting, which typically results in statements like, “It’s ok”, or “I don’t like it
much” that has little underpinning thoughtful analytical or evaluative base to it.
In contrast, when students understand the different types of thinking and the
cognitive heuristics involved, they can respond thoughtfully (no pun intended) to
their teacher’s systematic use of language to specifically cue these types of thinking.
This provides essential modelling and practice to develop competence over time.
This is illustrated in Table 3.6.
The use of appropriate curing questions, in which the types of thinking are nat-
urally infused into the content of the topic, helps students to become familiar with
the ‘language of thinking’. For example, when asked to evaluate options, whatever
the subject context, they will have already internalized that this requires the deriving
of relevant criteria to be used in evaluation, the likely prioritizing of these criteria in
terms of relative importance in making the decision, and finally to apply the criteria,
based on the available information, to the option or range of options.
In teaching this strategy, I am often posed the scenario that this takes time and
eats into the teaching of content. However, a medical friend of mine has frequently
said to folk something akin to, ‘eating better and taking exercise is, in the longer
term, less work and cheaper, than treating type 2 diabetes or a stroke’. Poignant, I
know, but the analogy is there. Teaching the heuristics of good thinking, does not,
in practice take that long, and it can be done incrementally, using spaced, retrieval
and deliberate practice. Once students are familiar with the terms, it’s then just a
bit of good EBT pedagogic design to infuse this effectively into a lesson structure.
For example, for a couple of years, I was given (though I did not ask for it) the role
of AIDS educator/counsellor, back in the days when the condition, its causes and
treatment were ambiguous. In this role, I had to do the necessary research to be as
current as possible. In my teaching role in this area, I would typically infuse some
specific thinking questions, tailored to the learning outcomes, as illustrated below:
• What are the similarities and differences between Hepatitis A and HIV?
• In what ways are these differences significant?
• What is the relationship between HIV infection and poverty?
• What inferences and interpretations can be drawn from the data on HIV infection
in Asia?
• How might we evaluate the effectiveness of the present HIV prevention pro-
gramme?
• What other ways might we make people more aware of HIV infection?
I recall revising for my final exams at university, having been told by one of the
psychology professors that talking to yourself while going through notes helps the
learning process. Initially, I thought this was somewhat strange. However, having
used self-talk and verbalization for a few decades, as well as teaching my students
to use it, I am assured to know that if there is a valid cognitive-neuroscience base to
what I was doing. For example, Treadwell (2017) points out:
Throughout our lives, we are conscious, non-consciously and subconsciously creating, refin-
ing and modifying our identity through our self-talk. The influence of self-talk is foundational
in establishing who we are, how we learn, and what we expect of our-self. (p. 40)
3.9 The Need for Explicit Teaching of Metacognition 119
It certainly made the revision process less tedious, the actual learning of content
seemed quite easy, and we all did well in the exams. Of interest, many components of
this broad process have been refined and formalized in ‘Flipped Classroom Learning’
and the now-famous method of ‘Peer Instruction’ developed by Mazur (1996). The
typical format is as follows:
1. Students are posed a challenging (key concept question) based on prior learning
2. Ask students to provide their answers (e.g., capture through an e-tool, response
cards, even show of hands ) and display the results:
120 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
It is now recognized that Retrieval Practice, which involves the retrieval of infor-
mation from Long-Term Memory to Working-Memory and then back to Long-Term
Memory, is a key cognitive and metacognitive strategy (e.g., Lang 2016). The pro-
cess of reciprocal retrieval, apart from gap analysis, and clearing up misconcep-
tions, strengthens the connections between neurons; hence building stronger neural
networks for this area of learning. In doing so this creates increasingly accurate
and useful mental models for the application of learning. As Brown et al. (2014)
summarizing from the research concluded:
In virtually all areas of learning, you build better mastery when you use testing as a tool to
identify and bring up your areas of weakness. (p. 5)
Retrieval practice can be done anytime, in most places, and as one becomes more
familiar and fluent with content (e.g., understanding is becoming richer) the process
can be super-fast. As Willingham (2009) demonstrated, the more factual knowledge
you have in long-term memory, the easier it is to acquire more factual knowledge.
To quote:
It means that the amount of information you retain depends on what you already have. So,
if you have more than I do, you regain more than I do, which means you gain more than me.
(p. 34)
The importance and workings of Deliberate and Spaced practice were outlined
and illustrated in Chap. 2: Core Principle 7: The development of expertise requires
deliberate practice.
Also, according to Lang (2016), interleaving helps to learn in the overall practice
process. Interleaving refers to the practice of spending some time learning one thing
and then pausing to concentrate on learning a second thing before having quite
mastered that first thing, and then returning to the first thing, and then moving on to
a third thing, and then returning to the second thing, and so forth. This utilizes the
brains sub/unconscious processing in terms of the dynamic of memory systems. It
also reduces ‘habituation’—attention disengagement/loss of interest in an activity—
factors that typically creeps in when staying with a single task for too long. Moving
to a new task introduces some novelty and may energise conscious thinking. At the
same time our brains are still indwelling (e.g., Moustakas 1990) sub/unconsciously
3.9 The Need for Explicit Teaching of Metacognition 121
on the previous task, but with a different neural focus in a more relaxed manner, as
documented by Claxton (1998).
In writing, I use interleaving quite extensively. I rarely finish a chapter before
moving on to the next one. In space/time between the first and making some inroad
into the second, I typically come up with new ideas and areas of content for enhancing
sections of the previous chapter—and so on.
Practice doesn’t always make perfect. However, the thoughtful use of the types
of practice outlined above certainly enhances learning capability and makes future
learning easier with a ‘bit of practice’—again, no pun intended. They can be infused
into any instructional strategy, as well as used situationally when needed. These
practice methods are certainly a well-constituted Russian Doll in themselves—in
terms of Hattie’s analogy.
Real World Performance Tasks
Performance-based learning tasks are perhaps the most authentic means for devel-
oping MC as key selected skills (e.g., metacognitive, cognitive, volitional) can be
embedded in the task to be accompanied. These skills would have already been
decided and infused into the content curriculum and should be familiar to both fac-
ulty and students. In this way what is to be learned, as well as what will be assessed
is made explicit and transparent. As Tombari and Borich (1999) explain:
A performance assessment is a test that tries to determine if a learner “really knows” about
something, or has a deep understanding. It does this by challenging learners with tasks
that ask them not simply to recall knowledge but to construct or organize it, not just to
solve problems but to demonstrate a disciplined approach requiring strategic thinking and
metacognition. (p. 148)
The goal is to develop schooling that will have a major impact on student moti-
vation and learning, and will better prepare students for the complex world they
are entering (p. 9). The extent that performance tasks reflect real work activity, the
greater the opportunities to develop the selected twenty-first century competencies
and to what level in what vocational context. It is for this reason that more and
more vocational and higher education institutions are using internships as an essen-
tial part of the learning experience for their programmes. Internships can take many
forms, in terms of format and duration, but the goal is to provide learners with direct
authentic work experiences in a specific vocational sector where they can acquire
workplace experience, key functional competencies and greater knowledge of that
industry. Invariably, this relies heavily on the arrangements between the educational
institution and the employing industry firm. It is to be noted that an internship is not a
novel learning model. The notion of students having work experience has long been
part of the curriculum landscape. However, this can be largely a ‘hit-or-miss’ expe-
rience, for both students and employers. Often students don’t feel they learn much
122 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
of value, apart from doing photocopying, making tea and generally running errands.
Equally, some employers feel that these novices offer little value to the company
effort, especially when they don’t have a ‘good attitude’, and are more trouble than
what they are worth. In contrast, of course, many students have felt that this was
the most valuable and enjoyable part of the curriculum programme. Similarly, many
employers have been delighted with some interns and have offered them employ-
ment post-graduation. Having been involved in this work for many years, this is my
experience and, from numerous conversations with teaching faculty and students,
it’s pretty much the norm.
However, increasingly, the internship is being used in more pedagogically-
structured ways. This involves certain important planning and conduct features:
• Close liaison between the educational institution and employing industrial per-
sonnel. It is important to identify, clarify and agree on what types of learning
experiences the company can provide, and the relationship between these work
functions and curriculum learning outcomes
• Close liaison between the educational institution supervisor and the interns. Interns
must be aware of their role and responsibilities in this learning event. It is useful
to clarify with interns what they are likely to have to do in the company and work
role, and how this relates to other aspects of their course, as well as the specific
learning outcomes that can be met during the internship period
• Ongoing communication and collaboration between all stakeholders to monitor
and evaluate the learning experience and ensure the smooth running of the intern-
ship. This involves many aspects, including how interns are progressing in terms
of key learning outcomes, work performance and attitude, and agreed assessment
arrangements. It is also important to keep regular contact with workplace super-
visors to ensure that interns are meeting expectations and taking any necessary
corrective action if needed.
Apart from internships, a wide range of methods can be used to provide students
with meaningful real-work learning activities, these include:
• Projects
• Problem-Based Learning
• Case Studies
• Simulated Practice
• Role-Play
• Presentations and Critique
• Experiments.
Essentially, any activity that reflects real work/life skills can be used. What is
essential is that it relates to curriculum outcomes and employs some skill(s) and/or
attribute(s) of desired twenty first-century competencies. For example, in the infu-
sion model introduced earlier, metacognitive strategies should run throughout the
activities.
3.9 The Need for Explicit Teaching of Metacognition 123
An example of framing the main knowledge and skill components for an electrical
and electronic engineering project is presented in Table 3.7.
learning (and assessment tasks) are perceived as meaningful and useful from the
students’ perspective, greater is the potential for intrinsic motivation to be evoked. In
this situation, coupled with good teaching, there is the potential for creating synergy
in the utilization of both metacognition and motivation. As an Advance Organizer
for the next chapter, which addresses the meaty challenge of making learning more
motivating for students, especially intrinsic motivation, here’s a positive frame from
Wlodkowski (1999) who suggests:
…if something can be learned, it can be learned in a motivating manner…every instructional
plan also needs to be a motivational plan. (p. 24)
3.10 Summary
In this chapter, I have attempted to get to grips with Metacognition; to repeat, with
some linguistic rejigging, Brown’s (1987) reference to it as a ‘many-headed monster
of obscure parentage’. One purpose has been to provide the reader, without the pain
of going through the extensive and mazy literature on this ‘creature’, and with as
much clarity as possible on its nature, evolution and role in the world of education
and learning. The major purpose though is to propose Metacognition, framed here
as Metacognitive Capability as the most important twenty-first century competence.
Increasingly included in definitions of so-called twenty-first century competencies,
for the reasons explained and illustrated extensively in the chapter, it is still relatively
under-developed in schools. There is an urgent need to address such a significant
curriculum area. Hopefully, this chapter helps to better frame how to go about doing
this from an evidence-based perspective.
MC is both an essential teaching competence as well as a generic competence for
enhancing learning capability, well-being and better citizenship (however defined).
The potential results of this are, as Treadwell (2017) summarizes:
Once we learn how to learn, and we achieve that efficiently, we can learn independently and
anything becomes possible. (p. 49)
126 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
References
Farrington CA et al (2012) Teaching adolescents to become learners: the role of noncognitive factors
in shaping school performance: a critical literature review. The University of Chicago Consortium
on Chicago School Research (CCSR), Chicago
Fazey MA, Fazey JA (2001) The potential for autonomy in learning: perceptions of competence,
motivation and locus of control in first-year undergraduate students. Taylor & Francis, London,
UK
Fennimore TF, Tinzmann MB (1990) What is a thinking curriculum? Web Page: https://www.ncrel.
org/sdrs/areas/rpl_esys/thinking. Last accessed 22nd Nov 2019
Flavell JH (1976) Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In: Resnick LB (ed) The nature of
intelligence. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ
Flavell JH (1979) Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new era of cognitive developmental
inquiry. Am Psychol 34(10):906–911
Flavell JH (1981) Cognitive monitoring. In: Dickson WP (ed) Children’s oral communication.
Academic Press, New York, pp 35–60
Flavell JH (1987) Speculation about the nature and development of metacognition. In: Wernert FE,
Kluwe RH (eds) Metacognition, motivation and understanding. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale,
NJ
Fogarty R (2009) How to integrate the curricula. Sage, London
Fromm E (1987) The anatomy of human destructiveness [1973]. Penguin Books, Harmondsworth,
UK
Gladwell M (2008) Outliers: the story of success. Little Brown and Company, New York
Goleman D (2013) Focus: the hidden driver of excellence. HarperCollins, New York
Guglielmino LM (1978) Development of the self-directed learning readiness scale. Doctoral
dissertation, University of Georgia, 1977. Dissertation, Abstracts International, 38, 6467A
Haidar AH, Al Naqabi AK (2008) Emiratii high school students’ understanding of stoichiometry
and the influence of metacognition on their understanding. Res Sci Technol Educ 26(2):215–237
Hattie J (2009) Visible learning. Routledge, New York
Hattie J, Yates GCR (2014) Visible learning and the science of how we learn. Routledge, New York
Hidi S (2006) Interest: a unique motivational variable. Educ Res Rev 1(2)
Hidi S, Renninger KA (2006) The four-phase model of interest development. Educ Psychol 41:111–
127. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4102_4
Jones MG, Farq JD, Surry D (1995) Using metacognitive theories to design user interfaces for
computer-based learning. Educ Technol 35:12–22
Kahneman D (2012) Thinking fast and slow. Penguin Books, London
Kistner S et al (2010) Promotion of self-regulated learning in classrooms: investigating frequency,
quality, and consequences for student performance. Metacogn Learn 5(2):157–171
Kluwe RH (1987) Executive decisions and regulation of problem solving behavior. In: Weinert FE,
Kluwe RH (eds) Metacognition, motivation, and understanding. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
Inc., New Jersey
Knowles MS (1975) Self-directed learning: a guide to learners and teachers. Association Press,
New York
Ku KYL, Ho IT (2010) Metacognitive strategies that enhance critical thinking. Metacogn Learn
2010(5):251–267
Lai ER (2012) Metacognition: a literature review. https://images.pearsonassessments.com/images/
tmrs/Metacognition_Literature_Review_Final.pdf. Last accessed, 15 Nov 2019
Lang JM (2016) Everyday lessons from the science of learning. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA
Mango C (2010) The role of metacognitive skills in developing critical thinking. Metacogn Learn
2010(5):137–156. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-010-9054-4
Marcus G (2008) Kluge: the haphazard evolution of the human mind. Mariner Books, New York
Martinez ME (2006) What is metacognition? Phi Delta Kappan 87(9): 696–699. https://doi.org/10.
1177/003172170608700916
Marzano RJ et al (1988) Dimensions of thinking: a framework for curriculum and instruction.
ASCD, Alexandria, VA
128 3 Metacognitive Capability: The Superordinate Competence …
Marzano RJ et al (2007) Designing and teaching learning goals and objectives: classroom strategies
that work. Marzano Research Laboratory, Colorado
Maudsley DB (1979) A theory of meta-learning and principles of facilitation: an organismic
perspective. Thesis (E.D.), University of Toronto
Mazur E (1996) Peer instruction: a user’s manual. Prentice Hall, NJ, USA
Mlodinow L (2012) Subliminal: how your unconscious mind rules your behaviour. Vintage Books,
New York
Moustakas C (1990) Heuristic research. Sage, London
Neubauer S et al (2018) The evolution of modern human brain shape. Sci Adv 4(1)
Noushad PP (2008) Cognitions about cognitions: the theory of metacognition
Ormrod JE (2011) Our minds, our memories: enhancing thinking and learning at all ages. Pearson,
London
Pajares F (2012) Motivational role of self-efficacy beliefs in self-regulated learning. In: Schunk DH,
Zimmerman BJ (eds) Motivation and self-regulated learning: theories, research, and application.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, NJ
Panksepp J (2004) Affective neuroscience: the foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford
University Press, Oxford
Paris SG, Winograd P (1990) Promoting metacognition and motivation of exceptional children.
Remedial Spec Educ 11(6):7–15
Paul RW (1993) Critical thinking: foundation for critical thinking. Santa Rosa, CA
Perkins DN (1995) Outsmarting IQ: the emerging science of learnable intelligence. The Free Press,
London, UK
Pinker S (2003) The blank slate: the modern denial of human nature. Penguin, London
Pintrich PR, De Groot EV (1990) Motivational and self-regulated components of classroom
academic performance. J Educ Psychol 82:33–40
Reeve J, Tseng C (2001) Agency as a fourth aspect of students’ engagement during learning
activities. Contemp Educ Psychol 36(2011):257–267
Resnick LB, Resnick DP (1989) Assessing the thinking curriculum: new tools for educational
reform. In: Gifford BR, O’Connor MC (eds) Future assessments: changing views of aptitude,
achievement, and instruction. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Boston
Ritchhart R, Church M, Morrison K (2011) Making thinking visible. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Robbins A (2001) Unlimited power. Pocket Books, London
Rohn J (2019) Quote available at: https://www.quoteswise.com/jim-rohn-quotes-5.html. Last
accessed on 3rd Dec 2019
Ryan RM, Deci EL (2017) Self determination theory: basic needs in motivation, development, and
wellness. The Guilford Press, New York
Saks K, Leijen A (2013) Distinguishing self-directed and self-regulated learning and measur-
ing them in the e-learning context. In: International conference on education & educational
psychology
Sale D (2013) The challenge of reframing engineering education. Springer, New York
Schank R (2011) Teaching minds: how cognitive science can save our schools. Teacher College
Press, New York
Schraw G (1998) Promoting general metacognitive awareness. Instr Sci 26:113–125 (Kluwer
Academic Publishers, The Netherlands)
Schraw G, Dennison RP (2004) Assessing metacognitive awareness. Contemp Educ Psychol
19:460–475
Schraw G et al (2006) Promoting self-regulation in science education: metacognition as part of a
broader perspective on learning. Res Sci Educ 36:111–139
Schulman LS (2005) Signature pedagogies in the professions. Daedalus 134(3):52–59
Schunk D, Zimmerman BJ (2012) Motivation and self-regulated learning: theory, research, and
applications. Routledge, New York
Schwarz N (2010) Meaning in context: metacognitive experiences. In: Mesquita B, Barrett LF,
Smith ER (eds) The mind in context. Guilford, New York, pp 105–125
References 129
Sheppard SD et al (2009) Educating engineers: designing for the future of the field. Jossey-Bass,
San Francisco
Stenhouse L (1989) An introduction to curriculum research and development. Heinemann
Educational Books, Oxford
Stockard J et al (2018) The effectiveness of direct instruction curricula: a meta-analysis of a half
century of research. Rev Educ Res 88(4):479–507
Swartz RJ (1987) Teaching for thinking: a developmental model for the infusion of thinking skills
into mainstream instruction. In: Baron JB, Sternberg RJ (eds) Teaching thinking skills: theory
and practice. Freeman, New York
Swartz RJ, Perkins DN (1990) Teaching thinking: issues approaches. Critical Thinking Press &
Software, CA
Tombari M, Borich G (1999) Authentic assessment in the classroom. Prentice-Hall, New Jersey
Tomlinson CA (2005) The differentiated classroom: responding to the needs of all learners, 2nd
edn. ASCD, Alexandria, Virginia
Tough P (2013) How children succeed: grit, curiosity, and the hidden power of character. Random
House Books, London, UK
Treadwell M (2017) The future of learning. The Global Curriculum Project, Mount Maunganui,
NZ
Veenman VJ et al (2006) Metacognition and learning: conceptual and methodological considera-
tions. Metacogn Learn 2006(1):3–14
Weiner B (1992) Human motivation: metaphors, theories and research. Sage, Newbury Park, CA
Whitehead AN (1967) The aims of education. Free Press, New York
Willingham DT (2009) Why don’t students like school: a cognitive scientist answers questions
about how the mind works and what it means for the classroom. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Winne PH (1995) Inherent details in self-regulated learning. Educ Psychol 30(4):173–187
Wlodkowski RJ (1999) Enhancing adult motivation to learn: a comprehensive guide for teaching
all adults. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Zimmerman BJ, Kitsantas A (1999) Acquiring writing revision skill: shifting from process to
outcome self-regulatory goals. J Educ Psychol 91(2):241–250. psycnet.apa.org
Zimmerman BJ, Martinez-Pons M (2012) Student differences in self-regulated learning: relating
grade, sex, and giftedness to self-efficacy and strategy use. J Educ Psychol 82(1):51–59
Zimmerman BJ et al (1992) Self-motivation for academic attainment: the role of self-efficacy beliefs
and personal goal setting. Am Educ Res J 29(3):663–676
Chapter 4
Motivation and Well-Being:
An Evidence-Based Frame
Abstract This chapter addresses the challenge of enhancing student’s intrinsic moti-
vation in school-based learning environments and, in essence, motivation generically.
Motivation underpins learning in that without motivation, people don’t bother to learn
(or think) too much. It connects extensively to the pedagogic framework developed in
Chap. 2, as well as the superordinate twenty-first century competence of Metacogni-
tive Capability explored and illustrated in Chap. 3. Metacognition and Motivation are
extensively and dynamically interlinked. Without motivation people are unlikely to
do much by way of metacognition. However, without metacognition and the related
thinking and self-regulatory skills, people may not fare well in the learning stakes as
tasks become more complicated—and that’s an existential problem we face today.
Intrinsic motivation is explored in detail, especially the Self-Determination Theory
(SDT) of Ryan and Deci, as it offers much potential for enhancing learning and
well-being in educational contexts.
4.1 Introduction
Often, as teachers, what we get is varying amounts (and levels) of attention from stu-
dents. Unfortunately, without sustained focused attention on a topic being explained,
students may remember very little. Attention is, to all intent and purpose, both a quan-
titative and qualitative human capability and from a pedagogic point of view—a big
challenge. As Miller (2016) States:
Optimizing your learning experiences for attention is the first step towards optimizing it for
long-term memory and higher thought processes. (p. 8)
Motivation and attention are very much connected in the world of the classroom, as
in all areas of human activity. When students are motivated, they are much more likely
to give a higher level of attention and engagement than in situations where motivation
is poor. They are also more likely to put effort into the learning process, especially
when difficulties are encountered. In such scenarios, especially if supported by a good
teacher, students will typically experience better learning outcomes which, in turn,
provides the basis for further motivation, as well as enhancing self-efficacy. Over
time increased mastery is likely to be achieved, which further reinforces positive
beliefs, confidence and motivation in such activities or area of learning.
However, while motivation is recognized as fundamental to learning, there is
much debate about how it works and, more significantly, how we as teachers can
harness such human energy in the pursuit of educationally desired learning goals.
The literature is rich in terms of theories and models of human motivation (e.g.,
Maslow 1962; Herzberg 1966; Ryan and Deci 2017; Dweck 2006) but, as Kim
(2013) argues:
Existing theories on motivation bear three limitations. First is the vagueness of the con-
cept of motivation. It is practically impossible to draw a clear line between motivation and
other concepts such as drive, need, intention, desire, goal, value, and volition. Due to this
conceptual vagueness, it is difficult to come to a consensus on whether motivation refers
to a psychological state or process, let alone the definition. Various constructs in different
theories of motivation are overlapping and often create confusion. For instance, the vague
conceptual distinctions between intrinsic motivation and interest, self-efficacy and perceived
competence, value and reward, self-regulation and volition hinder effective communication
and constructive arguments on the identical phenomenon of motivation.
I would concur on the above and have some empathy with the frame of the
management guru Peter Drucker, who made the challenging assertion that:
We know nothing about motivation. All we can do is write books about it.
Indeed, this may seem to have a fair measure of face-validity at least in terms of
widespread practice in educational institutions, as Levin (2008) concluded:
…boredom and lack of engagement remain endemic in schools around the world, and
seemingly unmotivated students are a main complaint of teachers. (p. 99)
In countless focus groups, I’ve conducted with high school students, “boring classes”- which
include so-called advanced classes – are among the main complaints about the school. (p. 114)
On many occasions, I have heard teachers being told by various sources that they
should ‘ignite the passion for learning in every child’—or something akin to this. The
assumption is that by activating passion (e.g., a strong emotional desire for some-
thing—in this case learning) it will provide the sustained attention and motivation to
do the necessary hard work that learning often entails. A nice ideal and it should be
a goal we seek to attain. However, it’s a bit like saying doctors should be able to cure
all diseases and sickness, and this may be a goal that many seek. I would particularly
like that, especially if they can reverse the ageing process also. However, it’s not the
world as I know it. The evidence would also support this, as people are still getting
sick and dying, and I am not getting any younger. Referring to motivating students—
is it possible to ignite a passion for learning in all students? Well, I’m going to play
my ‘get out of jail’ card (this is a card used in the game of Monopoly to enable your
moving counter icon to immediately get out of jail when it, unfortunately, through
the throw of the die, lands on a space that denotes ‘Go to jail’). I don’t know the
answer to the question, and I’m not sure there is one. It is like asking the question of
whether people are born good or neutral in terms of dispositions, or are some simply
badly wired to be difficult or even dangerous?
However, while we might like algorithmic answers to our big questions, whether
it’s how best to motivate our students or other areas of life that are meaningful to
us, in reality we may have to settle for a well-framed evidence-based set of useful
heuristics; otherwise we may simply go with personal preference or fad, albeit more
philosophical than empirical. In most basic terms, from my experience, I would not
dispute the English philosopher Jeremy Bentham’s framing of human motivation in
terms of:
Nature has placed mankind under the governance of two sovereign masters, pain and pleasure.
Invariably, what is a pleasure to one person may be a pain for another, but little in
my life has seriously questioned these underlying premises. Indeed, such a perspec-
tive, with the additional component of ‘novelty’ has been supported by the field of
cognitive neuroscience (e.g., Cloninger 1997). The avoidance of pain is inherent in
the notion of punishment, whether for the infraction of laws, regulations or mores.
Punishment is based on the fact that folk won’t do certain acts of deviance for fear of
4.2 The Components of Motivation 135
the painful consequences of such acts. For example, long prison sentences are framed
both in terms of being retributive and justice providing, as well as a deterrent to others
who might otherwise transgress. When I was at school, the cane was used for specific
misdemeanours such as fighting, bullying, being disrespectful—whatever the school
establishment framed as meriting such treatment. There was also some notion of
severity; the worse the infraction, the more strokes of the cane administered. Did it
work?—probably for most of us. However, I remember one of my classmates who
seemed intent on being caned by all teachers in the school, probably as a mark of
masculinity. The point is that most students, whether they would admit it or not, don’t
enjoy failing in their learning. Those who want to learn the schoolwork are likely to
experience pain in failure, and this can come in many forms (e.g., loss of self-esteem
and self-efficacy, punishment by parents).
Let’s move on to the pleasure aspect of motivation. People like doing things
that lead to pleasurable feelings—albeit variation in the things deemed pleasurable.
We can, therefore, assume that people are more likely to be interested in things
that provide pleasurable feelings for them; hence interest drives, in no small part,
motivation. We actively seek to do stuff we like. It’s as simple as that. As explored in
the previous chapter interest is fundamental to motivation and has biological roots.
Futhermore, interest is very much bound up with the experience of a reward.
For example, I have a passion for soccer—which means I am interested in playing
it, watching it—even talking about it. I also often experience a range of emotions
when playing and watching soccer, which can change from negative to positive emo-
tions—especially the latter when the game goes well (e.g., the team wins, especially
when the quality of the soccer is good); it’s psychologically rewarding. Rewards are,
according to Kim (2013), one of the most powerful variables influencing motiva-
tion, irrespective of reward type (physical or social reward; extrinsic or intrinsic).
The main function of a reward is to induce positive emotions, which then releases
the neurotransmitter dopamine in the brain—this creates a great emotional feeling
of pleasure. Emotions, whether positive or negative, are highly impactful on the
learning process, as Wlodkowski and Ginsberg (2017) summarized:
Research in the neurosciences and the field of intrinsic motivation indicates that emotions
are critical to learning (Reeve and Lee 2012). Not only do emotions largely determine what
we pay attention to and help us to be aware of our mind-body states also affect what we
remember. We are much more likely to remember things that engage us emotionally. (p. 18)
Value and goal are concerned with the importance of the activity. For example, if
a highly valued goal is being sought, there is more likelihood of sustained motivation
being activated and maintained. Invariably, of course, other factors come into play,
but that’s what makes predicting human behaviour heuristic at best; never fully
algorithmic.
136 4 Motivation and Well-Being: An Evidence-Based Frame
teaching. The evidence is clear that what teachers do and how they do it significantly
impacts student’s attainment. Does the same logic and heuristics apply to enhancing
students’ intrinsic motivation? According to Ryan and Deci (2017):
School curricula or materials are often not packaged to be intrinsically motivating, nor
in any way made to be particularly meaningful or relevant to the students’ daily lives or
purposes. In addition, under various top-down policy pressures, many modern schools have
become extremely focused on a very narrow set of cognitive goals, often to the neglect of the
varied interests, talents and more holistic psychological and intellectual needs of students.
(pp. 352–353)
In educational contexts, few would disagree that students who perceive classroom
learning as painful and boring are unlikely to contribute much, except to absenteeism
rates and disruptive behaviour. The converse is also true. When students experience
the learning as personally interesting, or place value on the outcomes to be obtained
from successful completion of a programme (e.g., qualification/certification), they
are more likely to participate meaningfully in the learning activities. For many adult
learners, there are clear goals associated with their learning. These may have both
extrinsic and intrinsic motivational components. Extrinsic motivation typically refers
to the motivation coming from external factors to the activity (e.g., money, status or
power, rather than the specific work activity). In contrast, intrinsic motivation is
where motivation is derived from doing the task itself (e.g., passion for teaching).
For example, having conducted more than 100 teacher education programmes, it
was apparent that many participants had joined the programmes largely for purposes
of accreditation (e.g., no certificate, no job). However, even for such extrinsically
motivated persons many did, over the duration of the programme, find intrinsic
interest which resulted in added value to their overall learning experience.
Where there are strong extrinsic motivators, it is always likely that adult learners,
even with little intrinsic interest will try to maintain a level of attention and the neces-
sary volition to achieve success on the programme (typically certification). Even for
non-adult learners, grades and passing the examinations are strong extrinsic moti-
vating anchors. However, for many school students, there may be limited extrinsic
motivators (e.g., passing the exams does not get them a desired job) as well as little
or no intrinsic interest in the content of the school subjects. This makes teaching such
students highly challenging and potentially frustrating. I need say no more on this.
The seeking of pleasure, the avoidance of pain, and a desire or need for variation
and novelty are existential aspects of motivation, in that they drive people to do
things—often referred to as needs. Indeed, we need to eat, and when feeling hungry,
we have the drive to find food. It may also be pleasurable and novel—even painful.
I have many favourite foods, but will usually try something new when in different
138 4 Motivation and Well-Being: An Evidence-Based Frame
countries—I won’t bore you with examples—but I have found some pleasurable,
others not pleasurable (painful would be a bit of an exaggeration) but certainly novel.
I retain the motivation to explore eating new food items in different countries and
contexts.
As noted, prior, there are many models and theories of motivation. One that has
been probably the most prevalent for the past three decades has been that of Abraham
Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs, summarized in Fig. 4.1.
However, despite its appeal in terms of providing a clear and appealing structure
for framing human motivation in terms of progressive need enhancement towards
self-actualization, it has been criticized on a number of counts; notably its absence to
include social cohesion and collaboration, which are fundamental to human survival,
as well as the notion of hierarchy (e.g., Rutledge 2012). Most significantly, as Ryan
and Deci (2017) summarize:
…despite the appearance of Maslow’s (1943) pyramid-shaped hierarchy in almost every
introductory psychology textbook and its intuitive appeal, empirical evidence for his
hierarchy of psychological needs is quite thin. (p. 93)
The theory posits only three basic and universal psychological needs: competence,
relatedness and autonomy. As Ryan and Deci (2017) explain:
…a basic need is essential for growth, wellness and integrity. Accordingly, optimal develop-
ment, supported by basic need satisfaction, will be manifested in the motivational process of
(1) intrinsic motivation, a fundamental psychological growth process; (2) the internalization
and integration of behavioral regulations and social prescriptions and values, which results in
psychological coherence and integrity and (3) an experience of vitality and wellness. (p. 98)
As a psychological need, competence is not only functionally important but is also experi-
entially significant to the self. Phenomenally, feelings of effectance nourish people’s selves,
whereas feelings of effectance threaten their feelings of agency and undermine their ability
to mobilize and organize action. (p. 95)
Relatedness refers to the feeling of belonging and being significant in the eyes of
others. Ryan and Deci (2017) define it in terms of a need to:
…feel responded to, respected, and important to others, and, conversely, to avoid rejection,
insignificance and disconnectedness. (p. 96)
While what constitutes well-being is open to contestation, the framing of the likely
preferences of people by Pinker (2003) makes perfect sense to me:
Most people agree that life is better than death. Health is better than sickness. Sustenance
is better than hunger. Abundance is better than poverty. Peace is better than war. Safety
is better than danger. Freedom is better than tyranny. Equal rights are better than bigotry
and discrimination. Literacy is better than illiteracy. Knowledge is better than ignorance.
Intelligence is better than dull wittedness. Happiness is better than misery. Opportunities to
enjoy family and friends are better than drudgery and monotony. (p. 51)
Such a viewpoint can be supported in many similar veins and contexts. For exam-
ple, Harari (2016) argues that “the most real thing in the world is suffering” (p. 307).
This may sound somewhat extreme—even shocking—but I don’t think many of the
inmates of Auschwitz would take issue with such a claim. And, we don’t need to look
for archetypal examples, suffering is embedded in most peoples’ lives at some time, in
various ways. The point is not to be morbid here, but if suffering has a realism beyond
the arguments of social relativists, then there is value in considering Well-Being as a
necessary consideration in framing twenty-first century competencies.
140 4 Motivation and Well-Being: An Evidence-Based Frame
In the moral domain, there is much that reason and science can contribute to
our understanding of well-being. Pinker (2019) argues, with a few unexceptional
convictions, that:
…all of us value our welfare, and that we are social beings who impinge on each other and
can negotiate codes of conduct – the scientific facts militate toward a defensible morality,
namely principles that maximize the flourishing of humans and other sentient beings. (p. 395)
Similarly, Harris (2010) argues the case for looking at science for ways to better
understand human values and what aspects of the moral domain may enhance human
action, relationships and well-being. He takes as a premise that:
Human well-being entirely depends on events in the world and states of the human brain.
Consequently, there must be scientific truths to be known about it. A more detailed under-
standing of these truths will force us to draw clear distinctions between different ways of
living in a society with one another, judging some to be better or worse, more or less true
to the facts, and more or less ethical. Such insights could help us to improve the quality of
human life – and this is where the academic debate ends and choices affecting millions of
lives begin. (p. 3)
Conceptions of well-being can vary, and there is subjectivity here, but extensive
research on the impact of a whole host of physical, social and emotional experiences
have massive implications for brain development, physical and mental well-being.
For example, Swaab (2015) summarizing the evidence, highlights:
Children who are seriously neglected during their early development …have smaller brains;
their intelligence and linguistic and fine motor control are permanently impaired, and they
are impulsive and hyperactive. (p. 27)
Certainly, we would not argue the case that such experiences and outcomes
contribute to the well-being of these children. As Harris (2010) points out:
(1) Some people have better lives than others, and (2) these differences relate, in some lawful
and not arbitrary way to states of the human brain and states of the world. (p. 15)
findings in all articles underscore the fact that environments that provide autonomy sup-
port lead to qualitatively superior forms of motivation characterized by high levels of self-
determination (i.e., intrinsic motivation and identified regulation) that, in turn, are conducive
to more adaptive cognitive, affective, and behavioural outcomes. (p. 257)
Most significantly, and specifically, as Schunk and Zimmerman (2012) point out:
…unless people believe that their actions can produce the outcomes they desire, they have
little incentive to act or to persevere in the face of difficulties. (p. 113)
It is not difficult to understand how beliefs profoundly affect the way people
approach their learning and the subsequent impact on motivational (and eventually
attainment) levels. Beliefs act as major neurological filters that determine how we
perceive, and respond to, external reality (the world around us and the people in it),
and as Henry Ford famously wrote:
If you think you can or think you can’t, you’re right.
4.4 Perception, Thinking, and Meaning Making 143
Notions of purpose and meaning are of course central to learning and existential
to well-being. For example, Pink (2009), while using Autonomy and Mastery as key
psychological needs, also includes Purpose in his motivational framework. One could
argue, that to some extent at least, purpose gives meaning to one’s activities—even
life itself. Frankl’s (2014) account of how he managed to survive the experience of
Auschwitz through his belief that one day he would share his psychological theories
(e.g., logotherapy) to the world and his ability to maintain a sufficiently enduring
positive psychological state (in the face of the terrible external realities around him),
defines “Man’s Search for Meaning” (the title of his book).
However, purpose and meaning, in the everyday context, can be interpreted in
different ways with different levels of importance, which may also be highly situa-
tional. Students are likely to experience and frame them in terms of what they feel are
important at that time in their life-worlds. For example, being in the school soccer
team was highly meaningful to me at around 14 years of age, and a major purpose at
that time was to have my hair straightened—how I hated that wave in my hair. Was I
indeed a younger version of ‘Shallow Hal’, a 2001 American romantic comedy film
starring Gwyneth Paltrow and Jack Black about a shallow man who, after hypnosis,
begins to see people’s inner beauty reflected in their outward appearance? Certainly,
helping students to find purpose and meaning in their schoolwork and facilitating
their well-being at this important developmental period are important aspects of a
good holistic education (e.g. supporting their purpose and meaning in school life
and future planning). Good career mediation is facilitative in this context. Indeed,
purpose and meaning are very much bound up with need satisfaction as students
who, in their everyday lives, are experiencing mastery, relatedness and autonomy are
likely to have a positive sense of well-being. This may give them sufficient purpose
and meaning to be both settled and proactive in their learning and life generally. They
may even be happy. Of course, these are generalizations, and personality and other
factors inevitably come into play in such existential matters. Ryan and Deci (2017)
illustrate the likely linkages in terms of these human states:
Empirical studies also show a linkage, with basic need satisfaction reliably predicting mean-
ing. Recent research suggests that one’s sense of meaning in life is largely accounted for
by SDT’s basic needs, along with feelings of benevolence, which itself is need-satisfying.
(p. 253)
4.4 Perception, Thinking, and Meaning Making 145
This is where students take a consciously active role in shaping the learning
context and instructional activities through collaborative learning relationships with
teachers. Agentic Learners are confident to:
• Offer input into the lesson content (e.g., based on a diagnosis of what they are
finding difficult and what might help their understanding for a topic area)
• Express preferences in terms of how they learn best (e.g., what methods/learning
resources are most useful)
• Communicate their thinking or learning need (e.g., ask specific questions about
what to learn and how)
• Communicate their level of interest (e.g., provide negative effect constructively
when bored or frustrated)
• Solicit resources to help their learning (e.g., feel comfortable in asking for help).
Agentic Learners can be framed as students who have learned how to learn and
have developed appropriate habits of mind (e.g., Growth mindset) that support active
engagement and autonomy in their learning relations and strategies. Framing the
characteristics of agentic students is not a difficult task, and these fully calibrate with
the components and features of MC. For example, we would expect agentic learners
to have the following skills and attributes:
• Aware of being metacognitive and what this entails in terms of personal self-
regulation and the ability to be self-directed
• Understand the key aspects of how people learn (e.g., cognitive scientific principles
from a user’s perspective)
• Competence in using a range of cognitive strategies; knows when to use them and
for what specific learning purpose and goals
• Frame clear, challenging realistic goals
• Plan, implement, monitor and evaluate a learning plan/strategy for meeting
learning goals
• Aware of the range of existential and personal barriers to maintaining motivation,
effort, and belief in meeting challenging goals
• Strategize and use techniques and tools for maintaining self-regulation in-the-face
of inevitable challenges
• Understand that failure is part of the learning process, and sees this as important
feedback for future learning
• Active seeking of feedback from a range of sources (teachers, peers, anyone who
might know), and ability to ask thoughtful focused questions
• Aware that showing empathy, respect and interest in the goals, thoughts and
feelings of others builds rapport, and that this is a key life skill
• Can use motivational strategies to keep a balanced perspective on managing self
in situations of poor mood, boredom, or challenges to a growth mindset.
148 4 Motivation and Well-Being: An Evidence-Based Frame
Agentic engagement adds much of value to the learning process, as agentic stu-
dents elicit (demand) greater mediation between the teacher and themselves, facilitat-
ing collaborative two-way feedback on what instructional strategies are best support-
ing their learning. Treadwell (2017) argues that empowering learners with the com-
petency to take agency over their learning is something every school and educational
system needs to address (p. 9). He goes on to argue:
If we give people agency over their world they generally rise to the occasion and increasingly
manage their world more successfully, but only if we provide them with the underlying
competencies to achieve this. (p. 9)
One of the key aspects of increasing agency in school is having the learners take responsibility
for their learning, where they can be aware of, and learn about, their mind-set and capacity
to show ‘grit’ (determination). Learners in schools get the opportunity to ‘play’ with taking
agency over their world and this is one of the most powerful learning lessons we can gift
them. (p. 146)
Finally, as Ryan and Deci (2008) point out, AS is not only beneficial to students
learning and engagement but has more generic benefits:
In short, both giving and receiving autonomy support in a close relationship are related to
relationship quality and well-being. (p. 671)
Throughout this book, there has been, and this will continue in subsequent chap-
ters, an extensive focus on the nature, underpinning components, features—even
nuances—of the learning process and that’s because this is the core business of
education and training.
In becoming competent and confident self-directed learners, students need to have
key understandings of how humans learn and are competent in using metacognition,
cognitive and motivational strategies. In this way, both the teacher’s and the students
thinking becomes visible and they have a shared language of learning, which makes
the mediation of learning between stakeholders (e.g., teacher, student, peers) much
more effective.
Teaching students the principles of learning (Saphier 2017; Sale 2015) is the
essential primary strategy for helping students to understand how their minds work
4.4 Perception, Thinking, and Meaning Making 149
(in the context of school learning and well-being), enhancing their learning capability
and, over time, empowering them to become self-directed lifelong learners. Saphier
(2017) recommends that educators:
…Share with them teaching and learning strategies we use ourselves. By including them in
the secret knowledge of teaching and learning strategies we give students choices, power,
and license to control their learning.
Principles of learning should be explicitly taught to students as they can use them to be more
powerful learners. (pp. 114–15)
Such knowledge and its application to everyday learning and life goals can be
effectively taught. This is ‘The Future of Learning’, as Treadwell (2017) described
it in a book of that title.
Communicate Positive Growth Mindset Messages in everyday interactions with
students
To develop good thinking, I emphasized the importance of making the cognitive
heuristics of the specific types of thinking explicit for students—visible—and through
retrieval practice in a range of application contexts, this becomes part of the language
of learning. The same pedagogic process is necessary to make growth mind-set
concepts visible and part of everyday classroom interactions. As Saphier (2017)
points out:
It is through daily interactions in everyday classroom life that we can:
impressions of us as teachers (as people), and of course how they think we perceive
them. Tomlinson (2005) captures this poignantly:
My students hear every message I send – whether overt or implied – about their capability
to learn and succeed. (p. 76)
Teachers are often pressurized into completing the curriculum in specified time
frames. However, students who lack prior knowledge and/or skills often fall behind,
have difficulties with subsequent topics, and risk becoming labelled (and may self-
identify as) ‘weak learner’s’. Mastery Learning focuses on ensuring that all students
master a topic area at a specified level of conceptual understanding or skill, before
moving on to a more advanced one (allowing students to move at their own pace in
this process). Hence, when students master the content, they experience a sense of
being competent, which is fundamental to supporting a growth mind-set as it pro-
vides a direct experience that perseverance and effort lead to success. Simultaneously
mastery promotes and reinforces self-efficacy in learning the topics, and the more
this is experienced across topics, enhances the possibilities of transfer and intrinsic
motivation.
Facilitating mastery learning inevitably involves some differentiation in the
instructional strategy but, with good pedagogic design and the thoughtful appli-
cation of technology, it is an achievable and desirable educational goal in the present
educational context.
A teachable moment is the time at which learning a specific topic or idea becomes
possible or easier. Great teachable moments for supporting a growth mind-set are
when students directly experience a learning breakthrough (e.g., achieve a learning
goal—a new understanding or skill level) and have that ‘aha feeling’—which essen-
tially means that they have ‘got it’—and will have developed a useful addition to
their neural networks in long-term memory. This is especially the case when this new
learning can be directly linked to their positive learning actions, such as extra effort,
persistence, using a learning strategy well. Nothing convinces like direct experience.
Once students understand how learning works, and how their actions impact the
results, they are more likely to adopt the beliefs and behave in ways consistent with
a growth mindset. The cartoon below is just for amusement.
4.4 Perception, Thinking, and Meaning Making 151
Hence, from an EBT approach, first and foremost, the design and facilitation of
learning experiences must involve, as an essential design heuristic, ways to gener-
ate and sustain learner motivation. The Core Principles of Learning, outlined and
illustrated in Chap. 2, are a significant part of the instructional design heuristics for
enhancing motivation to learn as they constitute the key underpinning knowledge—
Pedagogic Literacy—for making teaching more effective, efficient and engaging.
However, it is how the core principles are employed in the design and facilitation of
learning—the situated context of teaching—that is most important for maximizing
student motivation. This entails the creative blending of strategies and methods, uti-
lizing technologies, and teachers being active agents in the motivational process. As
Brophy (1987) makes explicit:
…teachers are not merely reactors to whatever motivation patterns their students had devel-
oped before entering their classrooms but rather are active socialization agents capable of
stimulating the general development of student motivation to learn and its activation in
particular situations. (p. 40)
This may just be the gold standard (e.g., Adaptive Expertise, Hatano and Inagaki
1986) of expert teaching, or what I refer to as Creative Teaching Competence.
For the purposes of this chapter, we can conclude that while motivation is a
complex and dynamic entity, influenced by a range of external environmental factors
and internal features of the human brain and mind (and not necessarily working in
unison) as well as much of its processing operating unconsciously (the extent of
which is contested)—we can derive useful heuristics for enhancing motivation and
well-being As Miller (2016) summarizes:
…the combination of an atmosphere of self-determination and a connection to student’s
personal goals and values is a potent formula for motivating college students. (p. 73)
152 4 Motivation and Well-Being: An Evidence-Based Frame
4.5 Summary
Finally, in this framing of student motivation, don’t forget yourself in this endeav-
our. As professional educators, while we are paid to do this challenging work, there
is no harm, in fact, massive benefit, in enjoying the experience. There is little plea-
sure or novelty, and certainly considerable pain in teaching groups of unmotivated
learners. However, when we have learners who show interest in what we are teach-
ing (not necessary all the time), positively interact with us as human beings, and are
successful in the attainment stakes, it is a highly rewarding experience, and it’s why
many of us do this job. As Levin (2008) summarized:
Greater engagement is a vehicle that improves students’ work and makes teachers’ lives
easier as well.
…increased student motivation is very positive for teachers’ experience of their work. (p. 99)
References
Alderman MK (2008) Motivation for achievement: possibilities for teaching and learning. Taylor
& Francis Inc., Milton Park, Oxfordshire, UK
Bentham J (1789) Quote. Available at www.goodreads.com/quotes/1013033-nature-has-placed-
mankind-under. Last accessed, 3rd Nov 2019.
Brophy J (1987) Synthesis of research on strategies for motivating students to learn. Association
for Supervision and Curriculum Development, Alexandria, Virginia
Cloninger CR (1997) A psychobiological model of personality and psychopathology. Jpn Psycho-
som Med 37:91–102
De Bono E (2019) Quote available at https://www.brainyquote.com/quotes/edward_de_bono_
124615. Last accessed on 2nd Dec 2019
Deci EL et al (1991) Motivation and education: the self-determination perspective. Educ Psychol
26(3 & 4):325–346
References 153
Dweck CS (2006) Mindset: the new psychology of success. Ballantine, New York
Frankl VE (2014) Man’s search for meaning. Beacon Press, Boston, US
Gallwey TW (1987) The inner game of tennis. Jonathan Cape, London
Gregory G, Kaufeldt M (2015) The motivated brain: improving student attention, engagement, and
perseverance. ASCD, Alexandria, Virginia
Harari YN (2016) Homo Deus: a brief history of tomorrow. Vintage, Penguin, London
Harris S (2010) The moral landscape: how science can determine human values. Free Press, New
York
Hatano G, Inagaki K (1986) Two courses of expertise. Child development and education in Japan,
pp 262–272
Herzberg F (1966) Work and the nature of man. World Pub. Co., Cleveland
Kim SI (2013) Neuroscientific model of motivational process. Front Psychol 4:98. https://doi.org/
10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00098
Ladd G, Dinella LM (2009) Continuity and change in early school engagement: predictive of
children’s achievement trajectories from first to eighth grade? J Educ Psychol 101(1):190–206
Langer HJ (2016) The power of mindful learning. Da Capo Lifelong Books, Boston, US
Levin B (2008) How to change 5000 schools. Harvard Education Press, Cambridge
Maslow A H (1943) A theory of human motivation. Psychol Rev 50(4):370–396
Maslow A (1962) Toward a psychology of being. Van Nostrand, New York
Miller MD (2016) Minds online: teaching effectively with technology. Harvard University Press,
Cambridge, MA
Pink DH (2009) Drive: the surprising truth about what motivates us. Riverhead Books, New York
Pinker S (2003) The blank slate: the modern denial of human nature. Penguin, London
Pinker S (2019) Enlightenment now. Penguin, UK
Reeve J (2015) Giving and summoning autonomy support in hierarchical relationships. Soc Pers
Psychol Compass 9(8):406–418
Reeve J et al (2004) Enhancing students’ engagement by increasing teachers’ autonomy support.
Motivation Emotion 28(2)
Reeve J, Lee W (2012) Neuroscience and human motivation. In: Ryan RM (ed) Oxford library of
psychology. The oxford handbook of human motivation (pp 365–380). Oxford University Press
Reeve J, Tseng C-M (2011) Agency as a fourth aspect of students’ engagement during learn-
ing activities. Contemp Educ Psychol 36(4):257–267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2011.
05.002
Robbins A (2001) Unlimited power. Pocket Books, London
Rutledge P (2012) Social networks: what Maslow Misses. https://www.psychologytoday.com/gb/
blog/positively-media/201111/social-networks-what-maslow-misses-0
Ryan RM, Deci EL (2008) Self-determination theory and the role of basic psychological needs
in personality and the organization of behaviour. In: The handbook of personality: theory and
research. Guilford Press, New York
Ryan RM, Deci EL (2017) Self determination theory: basic needs in motivation, development, and
wellness. The Guilford Press, New York
Sale D (2015) Creative teaching: an evidence-based approach. Springer, New York
Saphier J (2017) High expectations teaching. Sage, New York
Schunk DH, Usher EL (2012) Social cognitive theory and motivation. In: Ryan RM (ed) The Oxford
handbook of human motivation. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK
Schunk DH, Zimmerman BJ (2012) Motivation and self-regulated learning: theory, research, and
applications. Routledge, New York
Swaab D (2015) We are our brains: from the womb to Alzheimer’s. Penguin, London
Tomlinson CA (2005) The differentiated classroom: responding to the needs of all learners, 2nd
edn. ASCD, Alexandria, Virginia
Treadwell M (2017) The future of learning. The Global Curriculum Project, Mount Maunganui,
NZ
154 4 Motivation and Well-Being: An Evidence-Based Frame
Vallerand RJ, Pelletier LG, Koestner R (2008) Reflections on self-determination theory. Can
Psychol/Psychol Canadienne 49(3):257–262
Wagner T (2010) The global achievement gap. Basic Books, New York
Wlodkowski RJ, Ginsberg MB (2017) Enhancing adult motivation to learn: a comprehensive guide
for teaching all adults. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Zepke N, Leach L (2010) Improving Student Engagement: ten proposals for action. Act Learn High
Educ 11:167
Zig Zigler https://www.brainyquote.com/quotes/zig_ziglar_387369. Last Accessed 30th Nov 2019
Chapter 5
Creative Teaching Competence: The
SHAPE of Creative Teachers
Abstract This chapter analyses the psychological capability for creative thinking
and how this is most usefully contextualized to everyday teaching and learning con-
texts—what I refer to as Creative Teaching Competence. As creativity or creative
thinking appears in most frameworks of twenty-first century competencies, it is nec-
essary to reduce misconceptions and frame this area in more evidence-based specific
practical terms. While creative teaching can take many forms, and be both a planned
experience and/or a situated invention in the teaching context, there is an underpin-
ning syntax to how it works and what are typical outcomes of such behaviour in terms
of specific practices. In summary, creative teaching can be demystified, explained
and illustrated. Hence, it is a learnable competence/expertise for motivated teaching
professionals.
5.1 Introduction
Chapter 1 argued for teaching to become evidence-based, as is the case for other
professions such as engineering and medicine—you may remember the Jurassic Park
analogy! Chapter 2 established cognitive scientific principles—the core principles of
learning—as a foundational base for an emerging and increasing Pedagogic Literacy.
Chapter 3 argued that Metacognitive Capability is the superordinate twenty-first
century competence, and what makes it an essential component of student learning
and expert teaching. Chapter 4 unpacked motivation, especially intrinsic motivation,
to better understand how it works at the level of subjective experience and human
psychological needs.
However, while the core principles of learning provide evidence-based heuristics
for the design and facilitation of learning experiences, they do not dictate the form or
structure of any specific learning event. This is always mediated by a whole host of
situated factors, as portrayed in the ‘fly fishing analogy’ (e.g. learning outcomes, sub-
ject content, student competence, resource access). Hence, teaching is not a highly
prescriptive activity, governed by a finite range of algorithmic teacher behaviours;
rather it is an act of situated design—but not determined by whim or fashion. Design
in teaching is essentially similar to design in other fields in that it must involve
Art and creativity have much in common and are sometimes seen as synonymous.
Indeed, art by its very nature is always seeking new forms and genres, and the most
valued in financial terms are often those exhibits that are deemed ‘creative’. However,
in the context of teaching, while recognition has long been there, creativity has proved
elusive in terms of clear framing and how its works pedagogically. As Schon (1987)
notes:
…outstanding practitioners are not said to have more professional knowledge than others
but more “wisdom”, “talent”, “intuition”, or “artistry”. (p. 13)
5.1 Introduction 157
In the following sections, I will explore the notion that the art of teaching can also
be better understood from an EBT approach in that aspects of human psychological
functioning, which appears elusive—almost ephemeral—but impactful in terms of
learning and well-being, can be effectively modelled, learned and applied like other
more overt aspects of behaviour. It just may be the case that this applies to all
forms of art, and works as a result of its subtle impact on human sensation and
perception, creating positive aesthetic and affective responses (both conscious and
sub/unconscious). The dichotomy of science and art of teaching may cease to be
a useful one; we may simply talk about Creative Teaching or Creative Teaching
Competence.
In the context of teaching, key heuristics such as the core principles of learning
provide the systematic approach with rules, based on evidence. When planning the
design of any learning experience (e.g., module, workshop) these heuristics should
be at the forefront of planning decisions. For example, in planning a professional
development workshop, I will typically generate and address the following kinds of
questions:
• What are the learning goals and key outcomes for this learning group?
• What prior knowledge do these learners already have, and what activities might
best capture their present understanding of these areas of learning?
• What are the key concepts that need to be negotiated to facilitate understanding,
and how is this best organized and managed with this group of learners?
• What essential questions connected with this topic might get them thinking in
critical or creative ways about areas of interest or problem-solving?
• What presentation mediums and resources do I have that will generate and maintain
interest?
• How do I organize the learning sequences to avoid cognitive overload, facilitate
the ‘digesting’ and understanding of the content to enable effective transfer into
long-term memory?
• How best can I make the learning experience active and experiential for supporting
application-based learning outcomes?
• What specific skills need to be developed and how might effective spaced and
deliberate practice be organized?
• Are there areas where feedback is likely to be most critical for effective learning;
and what strategies will I employ to maximize efficiency?
• How best do I present myself to this group of learners to encourage the building
of rapport, positive interpersonal interactions and a ‘can do’ feeling for the tasks
in hand?
158 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
• What activities, stories, examples, artefacts do I have that will generate interest
and engagement around the key areas of learning?
Ok, I’ll confess, the process may not always be as explicit, meticulous or pristine as
the above description depicts. Furthermore, as with all learning over time, especially
with expertise, it becomes a largely unconscious competence and can be done very
quickly. The key point is that there is a systematic process in design; it is far from
ad hoc.
In teaching, the term practices can relate to many activities, resources and arrange-
ments designed to enhance desired learning outcomes. At a technical level, and
typically for competency-based teacher education programs, practices are often seen
in terms of specific functional competencies such as:
• Write clear and appropriate learning outcomes
• Prepare a teaching/training plan
• Produce learning resources
• Conduct teaching/training
• Design differentiated learning activities
• Produce a scheme of assessment
• Conduct assessment
• Produce an online module
• Facilitate an online module.
Such functional competencies are foundational to teaching/training as they com-
prise—metaphorically speaking—the ‘tool-box’ and underpinning knowledge relat-
ing to key technical practices. However, I have conducted numerous teaching obser-
vations in which the designated competencies were technically met, but I did not
perceive the actual learning experience as being effective in terms of student learn-
ing—especially concerning cognitive and emotional engagement. I might sum up
the experience as ‘just ok’. The reason is that practices cannot just be seen solely
in technical terms, but also in the way teachers conduct themselves, interact with
learners and mediate the situated learning experience to engage, motivate and, on the
best occasions, inspire them. As Andrews et al (1996) pointed out:
…the hallmark of excellent teaching is more than adequate content expertise and effective
technical performance. (p. 82)
Andrews et al. (1996) develop this further in their description of excellent teachers:
…excellent teachers seem to want to facilitate a meaning approach (deep) to learning rather
than a reproducing (surface) approach. Moreover, they tend to engage in instructional pro-
cesses that are congruent with their preferred approach and have values and beliefs, and
characteristics (for example, honesty, integrity, genuineness and respect for self, students,
materials and the process of teaching) that are considered foundational to a meaning approach
to teaching. (p. 101)
Equally the most powerful methods, in terms of their potential for enhancing stu-
dent attainment, may be ineffective when employed by less competent practitioners,
just as the best tools are often wasted in the hands of DIY (do-it-yourself) novices—as
I have learned from personal experience.
Similarly, while universal human conduct principles such as equity, fairness,
respect, concern for the person are fundamental to the practices of teaching, the
160 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
actual style and the content of human interaction often requires much contextualiza-
tion and adaptation. Even simple greetings, which are pretty much universal, need
careful contextualization across cultural and ethnic groups. A kiss on the cheek when
being introduced to a lady may be expected in France, but it would be highly risky in
many other countries. Fortunately, we are not confronted with such decision-making
in class on an everyday basis.
When people are skillful in an area, the inference is that they can perform a range
of specific activities in a highly effective and efficient manner. For example, to say
that the soccer player, Cristiano Ronaldo, is very skillful means that he is considered
highly proficient or expert in employing such skills as controlling, passing, heading,
shooting and dribbling with a soccer ball when playing soccer. However, while such
skills are essential for a high level of performance in an activity, they are not the only
components in determining an individual’s actual performance in real-life situations.
Other attributes, such as aptitude, personality traits, and attitudinal components also
play an important part in determining performance. To capture the range of attributes
involved in a complex performance the term competency is often used (Fig. 5.1), as
it attempts to capture the wider configuration of attributes that determines actual
performance in real work and life contexts.
Aptitude
This term is generally used to refer to a person’s innate tendency for competence
in an area. For example, a female basketball coach once asked me, perhaps with
some humour in mind, if I knew any 6 6 women who have an athletic physique.
Interesting question. My answer was “none”, but I did ask her, “why did you ask?”
The reply was something like ‘if I had one in my team, she would probably become a
millionaire’. Interesting, I thought, so I asked, “How does that work?” The rationale
provided was simple; very few women reach that height, so given some motivation,
good training, and playing time for a year or two, she is likely to be recruited by a top
team, and top players earn large salaries. How many 5 2 professionals basketball
players have you seen?
A similar framing can be applied in terms of psychological/neurological aspects
related to learning. For example, in the contested field of defining human intelligence,
Spearman (1950) proposed a two-factor theory of intelligence that posited that all
cognitive performance can be explained by two variables: one general ability (g)
and the many specific abilities (s). The General ability or G factor intelligence refers
to inborn aptitude, which varies from individual to individual and facilitates mental
operations across all situations. People vary in terms of this G factor intelligence,
and it underpins the basis of much of present-day intelligence testing. However, indi-
viduals typically possess specific abilities (S factors), also innate, and these facilitate
their learning in these areas (e.g., singing, painting, drawing). People can also have
more than one special ability and in varying degrees of innate potentiality. How-
ever, of interest, one form of specific ability may not help in the development of
others. For example, If you are very good at painting and you can also sing well,
this does not mean that your ability in painting helps you sing—or vice-versa. The
extent to which this intelligence can be impacted by instructional interventions has
been argued extensively over the years—often depicted in terms of the nature-nurture
debate. The stance taken here is not to engage in discussions of extent, as clearly both
are important determiners of behaviour, and we can only impact what is impactable—
so to speak. Most significantly, in this context, is the notion that intelligence can be
developed through a positive mindset (e.g., Growth Mind-set, Dweck 2006), student
effort and perseverance and, of course, good instruction. There is, little doubt that
an innate disposition for a specific S factor, other things being equal, has a natural
advantage over someone not possessing that S factor. For example, in my case, I
have tried to learn to sing, and while I have improved, it’s been minimal at best. I
would not claim even the most basic of competence (however defined). In contrast, I
have worked extensively in the Philippines and am astonished at how well so many
of the locals can sing. How much is innate, and how much is cultural, I don’t know.
I quake in fear (as novices do) when they ask me to sing a song after a workshop
session in a social event. In summary, from Spearman’s perspective, most tasks you
perform are likely to be influenced by G factor, and all individuals have some spe-
cific abilities, which may help in task success, though varied in nature and intensity.
Most importantly, however, both G and S intelligence are highly subject to positive
environmental influence. As Bloom (1985) concluded:
After forty years of intensive research on school learning in the United States as well as
abroad, my major conclusion is: What any person in the world can learn, almost all persons
can learn if provided with appropriate prior and current conditions of learning. This general-
ization does not appear to apply to the 2% or 3% of individuals who have severe emotional
and physical difficulties that impair their learning. At the other extreme, there are about 1%
or 2% of individuals who appear to learn in such unusually capable ways that they may be
exceptions to the theory.
162 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
He goes on to illustrate:
This middle 95% of school students become very similar in terms of their measured achieve-
ment, learning ability, rate of learning, and motivation for further learning when provided
with favourable learning conditions.
One example of such favourable learning conditions is mastery learning where the students
are helped to master each learning unit before proceeding to a more advanced learning task.
In general, the average student taught under mastery-learning procedures achieves at a level
above 85% of students taught under conventional instructional conditions. (p. 4)
Traits/Dispositions
The area of traits and dispositions often creates some conceptual confusion, not
dissimilar to that of comparing & contrasting metacognition, self-regulated learning,
self-directed learning and meta-learning. The term trait is used to describe a typical
characteristic that a person has or is perceived to possess. For example, we may refer
to a person in such terms of ‘friendly’, ‘selfish’, ‘unreliable’, etc. Sometimes the term
personality is attached to the description, e.g., she’s an extrovert personality or he is
introverted. Essentially, personality is generally framed, though with variation and
focus, as the more generalized set of key traits that a person displays. In mainstream
psychology, these are often referred to as the Big 5:
5.4 Teaching as Skillful Creative Activity 163
• introverted or extroverted
• neurotic or stable
• incurious or open to experience
• agreeable or antagonistic
• conscientious or undirected.
Personality traits are hereditary and are relatively permanent and stable over
time. The innate basis of personality traits has been extensively validated, especially
through the study of monozygotic (identical) twins separated at birth (e.g., Mit-
tler 1971; Bouchard 1990). Such twins show remarkable similarities, even though
reared in very different environments. Hence, people often postulate on how much
of personality, our trait configurations, are hereditary and how much is influenced
by environmental experience. This nature-nurture debate has raged (note: I do not
use this term loosely) over the decades, swinging back and forth depending on the
dominant ideology of the time. Over the past two decades the dominant ‘social sci-
ence model’ (i.e., focus on culture and socialization as the main determinants of
behaviour, and perhaps most prevalently voiced in terms of gender and race issues)
is now being challenged by a wide range of research findings (e.g., Pinker 2003;
Swaab 2015). The important issue from a pedagogic point of view is that while we
have finally ‘put the learning styles issue to bed’, we cannot avoid the reality that our
learners have different personalities that can vary greatly—and some configurations
may be easier (much easier) to teach than others. O’Connor and Paunonen (2007)
have noted that:
…there are behavioural tendencies in personality traits that can affect certain habits that influ-
ence academic achievement (e.g., perseverance, conscientiousness, talkativeness)…While
cognitive ability reflects what an individual can do, personality traits reflect will do. (p. 40)
For example, we may notice that some students tend to be impulsive when given
a challenging task—which can be framed as a maturational issue or a disposition.
It’s probably some of each, and there is little point in trying to ascertain how much
to allocate to either aspect. What is important is that dispositions can easily lead to
the forming of habits, which are particular-behavioural sets, often involuntary and
automatic. We tend to think of habits in negative terms, such as ‘that person has some
shocking-habits’. In practice habits can be productive for learning, as in the case of
students who have the habit of doing key reading and critical thinking when doing
their assignments. MC can help individuals to analyse their dispositions, identify
what one’s constitutes strengths and weaknesses for learning and well-being, and
then take the necessary self-regulatory action. Hence, there is little need to change
positive dispositions. These include what Costa (1991) referred to as Habits of Mind
or Intelligent Behaviours, which include such dispositions as ‘persisting’, ‘manag-
ing impulsivity’, ‘listening with understanding and empathy’, ‘creating’, ‘innovat-
ing’, ‘thinking flexibly, ‘thinking about thinking’, ‘taking responsible risks’, ‘striving
for accuracy’, ‘finding humor’, ‘questioning and posing problems’, ‘applying past
knowledge to new situations’, and ‘remaining open to continuous learning’.
Whatever the terminology employed, Positive Dispositions, Habits of Mind
or Intelligent Behaviours, they are essentially referring to the same attributes
(traits/dispositions) that facilitate better learning and well-being. However, equally,
many negative dispositions can mitigate one’s learning capability and in doing so,
impact self-efficacy and well-being. These can be seen in terms of the absence of the
above positive dispositions/ habits of mind. For example, impulsivity, poor thinking,
sloppiness in doing work, etc. are not good on the learning front, and are likely to
have negative impacts across all of life’s domains. However, many poor disposi-
tions can be addressed through specific cognitive strategies and tools, supported by
metacognitive strategies. Over time, better habits will form, and these will develop
neurological correlates that will further support the behaviour—even reducing the
innate valency of the disposition over time.
Skills
Skill in the most basic sense is the ability to do something well. For example, one
could say that a person has a good skill in heading a soccer ball, presenting a speech,
creative thinking, cake-baking, etc. Skill is often confused with competence, as we
could equally apply very similar terminology to the above examples (i.e. competent
at heading a football, etc.). However, as we know, a person may be very good at doing
something (i.e., skilful), but not choose to (or be able to) display his/her skill in real
work or life situations, for whatever reasons (e.g., impulsivity, introverted). Hence,
the term competence is generally used to describe the work-related performance or
complex performances in any field that involve more than skilful activity, but also
other important attributes for successful performance—as identified and explored
above.
In the case of teaching, and this would apply to any complex activity, there
are many skills involved as part of the overall performance (i.e., in the observable
behaviour, from which we can infer and interpret a person’s competence), and these
5.4 Teaching as Skillful Creative Activity 165
can vary significantly in terms of complexity. For example, if we talk about presen-
tation skills, which are important in teaching, one aspect of the skill is voice tone.
Mlodinow (2012), for example, quoting research by Apple et al. (1979), highlighted
the power of voice on person perception. This involved an experiment in which vol-
unteers were asked to judge the attributes of speakers’ voices (without seeing them),
who were using the same content, but with a specific variation in the vocal qualities.
In this way, the listeners’ assessments would be based on the influence of those vocal
qualities and not the content of the speech. Mlodinow’s summary captures the key
results:
…speakers with high-pitched voices were judged to be less truthful, less emphatic, less
potent, and more nervous than speakers with lower-pitched voices. Also, slower-talking
speakers were judged to be less truthful, less persuasive, and more passive than people who
spoke more quickly. …And if two speakers utter the same words but one speaks a little faster
and louder and with fewer pauses and greater variation in volume, that speaker will be judged
to be more energetic, knowledgeable, and intelligent. Expressive speech, with modulation in
pitch and volume and with a minimum of noticeable pauses, boosts credibility and enhances
the impression of intelligence. (p. 113)
However, it is not just the use of a voice that is impactful in terms of influenc-
ing how people experience a speaker, but also a whole host of non-verbal aspects
of interpersonal communication, many operating unconsciously, that significantly
affect perception and attention. For example, a key behavioural aspect of human
interaction which I have long believed to be fundamental in creating a good psy-
chological climate, whether in the classroom or the local coffee shop, is Smiling.
Not surprisingly, cognitive neuroscience is providing a strong evidence base for its
impact, which has been summarized by Hattie and Yates (2014):
…the smile is one of the most powerful tools to use in interpersonal teaching situations.
(p. 259)
The above analysis has important implications for identifying, defining, and sub-
sequent professional development in building teacher expertise. Firstly, it is valid
and useful to conclude that voice tone and smiling can be framed as skills (e.g.,
she is skillful in voice modulation; he has a skill in the way he smiles). While not
typically found in competency-based teacher education frameworks, I see them as
key human communication (even human conduct) skills, and given that teaching is
essentially an act of communication, they are definitely in my toolbox of skills for
teacher expertise.
166 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
They are told it will deliver quality. They believe it will deliver managerial control over their
performance, leave less room for professional judgement and reduce their status to that of a
technical operative. (pp. 118–119)
I have empathy with such critiques of CBTE, as having used this for over a decade
in the UK context, I often experience cognitive dissonance in making judgements
of teacher competence based on the criteria provided, which often seemed to focus
on easy to assess aspects like the font size in PPT slides, positioning in the class-
room, rigid sequencing of content, etc. Now, these things impact learning and are
important. However, I found from much experience, that while I was accrediting
competence based on the performance criteria provided on the assessment proforma,
my personal feelings were that this did not constitute good teaching in many cases,
especially in terms of emotional, cognitive and agentic engagement. Yes, they met
the stated criteria in minimalistic terms, and they were not incompetent, but I would
not have employed them. CTC must extend beyond functional competence, as there
are essential qualitative aspects to expert teaching, though more difficult to frame
and, therefore high inference in terms of assessment.
The Reflective Practitioner Model
Carr (1989) called for a fundamentally different approach to addressing quality in
teaching from that of a competency-based model, focusing on teachers’ reflecting
on their practice. He argues that:
When teaching is interpreted in this way, ‘quality’ has little to do with the measuring up to
a list of performance criteria but instead is something that can only be judged by reference
to those ethical criteria which teachers tacitly invoke to explain the educational purpose of
their teaching. This means that teaching quality cannot be improved other then by improving
teachers’ capacity to realize educational values through their practice. It also means that this
process of improvement can be nothing other than a research process in which teachers
168 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
reflect on their practice and use the products of their reflections to reconstruct their practice
as an educational practice in a systematic and rational way. (p. 11)
Also, and most importantly, it does not recognize what Schon (1987) refers to as
artistry:
…the kinds of competence practitioners sometimes display in unique, uncertain and
conflicted situations of practice. (p. 22)
Teaching from this point of view cannot be seen solely in terms of the systematic
application of universal or licensed competencies; rather it involves a process of social
construction whereby acquired professional knowledge is dynamically negotiated
concerning the unique contexts of practical situations. Furthermore, implicit in such
a model is the notion of professional practice driven by professional judgement and
ethics. Brophy and Evertson (1976) richly captured the wide range of attributes
involved, and it’s our challenge to unpack this and provide teacher education that
facilitates the learning of such capability:
Effective teaching requires the ability to implement a very large number of diagnostic,
instructional, managerial, and therapeutic skills, tailoring behaviour in specific contexts and
situations to the specific needs of the moment. Effective teachers not only must be able to
do a large number of things: they must also be able to recognize which of the many they
know how to do applies at a given moment and be able to follow through by performing the
behaviour effectively. (p. 139)
Having worked with both frameworks, there are merits and limitations in each.
The competency-based approach provides a systematic structure for teachers to learn
pedagogic competencies such as lesson planning & preparation, the delivery of learn-
ing, design and using teaching/resources, classroom management, assessing learn-
ing, etc. The terminology may change but it’s essentially planning, teaching and
assessment, and there’s nothing wrong with that in broad terms. However, given the
focus on clear measurable aspects of these activities, it often does not capture the
more qualitative aspects of human interaction that influence student’s engagement,
beliefs and feelings, and ultimately the quality of their learning. However, I am not
arguing that a competency-based approach cannot do this, but in practice, it often
results is a more reductionist focus on the easier to define—more quantitative tech-
nical aspects of classroom management and methods—missing the more difficult to
5.5 Competency-Based Teacher Education 169
define qualitative interactional aspects that shape perception and behaviour. Simi-
larly, working with reflective, rather than highly prescriptive criteria, has the benefit
of a more holistic and interpretive framing of what is occurring in the classroom,
focusing more on how students are engaged emotionally and cognitively, rather than
just behaviourally (though not minimizing this aspect of engagement). The major
limitation of this approach is that without a strong underpinning pedagogic literacy,
the reflection may simply result in what Hattie (2009) described as ‘post hoc justifi-
cation’ (e.g., superficial coverage and generalities, lacking sound pedagogic analysis
and evaluation against evidence-based criteria—my interpretation).
The work of Hatano and Inagaki (1986) has generated interest in terms of differ-
entiating performance at the highest level of competence, which is expertise. They
distinguish between two broad categories of expertise, “routine expertise” and “adap-
tive expertise”. Routine expertise is characterized by a high level of technical profi-
ciency across the typical range of real-world problem-solving contexts. However, as
problems become less familiar or novel, the performance of routine experts can dip
significantly. In contrast, adaptive experts can reframe problems in different ways,
modify or invent strategies and combine skills to deal much more effectively in solv-
ing such problems. They suggest a range of factors that encourage adaptive expertise
in the context of education and may support creative teaching competence. These
include:
• the extent to which the situation has flexibility of options, rather than rigid
procedures, to enable exploration of new approaches
• the degree to which people can tackle problems with a degree of playfulness and
acceptance of some risk in terms of it not working out in practice
• an organizational culture that encourages better practice from professionals as a
key goal. For example, Hatano and Inagaki note:
…they are invited to try new versions of the procedural skill, even at the cost of efficiency.
(p. 270)
Creativity is one of today’s global buzzwords and figures high on lists of so-called
twenty-first-century competencies and skills. Furthermore, if it is such a necessary
attribute, so essential in the worlds of engineering, business and medicine, it should
also be similarly valued in education—and teaching is the core activity of education.
It is important, therefore, to make the best sense we can on what creativity entails in
the context of teaching, and the ways in which it can be utilized at the level of practice.
As a basic assumption, it would seem logical to approach creativity in teaching as
analogous to creativity in any domain, as it involves combining existing knowledge
in some new form to get a useful result. As Amabile (1996) suggested:
170 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
A product or response will be judged creative to the extent that (a) it is both a novel and
appropriate, useful, correct or valuable response to the task at hand, and (b) the task is
heuristic rather than algorithmic. (p. 35)
In any attempt to define creativity there are inevitable questions about what con-
stitutes the novel, in whose eyes, by what criteria, and to what extent? Furthermore,
the notion of useful, correct or valuable also involves subjectivity. Rap music may
meet certain criteria of novel, but in no way does it feel useful or valuable to me.
However, because something is difficult to define in precise and uncontested terms
should not detract one from working towards useful heuristics and their practical
application, as outlined earlier. Most things at some point in time were a mystery but
eventually move down the knowledge funnel (Martin 2009) as a result of systematic
inquiry and evidence-based practice—as illustrated in the case of HIV in Chap. 1.
If novelty, in some form and at some level, is foundational to creativity, then effec-
tive teaching may not entail creativity. For example, a teaching professional applying
the core principles of learning, selecting high effect size methods and using them
thoughtfully to the situated context may be teaching very effectively, even perhaps a
“routine expert”—though not an “adaptive expert” in terms of the differentiation of
experts by Hatano and Inagaki (1986). To frame creativity in the context of teaching,
it is essential to identify, in realistic and specific terms, what this might entail in both
the design and facilitation of learning experiences. For example:
• What specifically can be considered novel and useful in the context and practices
of teaching?
• What are the processes and activities that can generate novel resources for
incorporation into the design and facilitation of learning experiences?
Firstly, creativity, like wealth and beauty, are value-laden and relative. When I
travel to some countries, in certain locations I get a sense that some people think I
am very wealthy in financial terms. However, in certain social circles in Singapore
(and this is not specific to Singapore) I could feel relatively impoverished, as I don’t
own the condominium I live in and only have one modest car. In making sense of
creativity the same framing applies, in that novelty and usefulness is relative and one
of extent. Fasco’s (2006) creativity continuum, (identified prior, and refreshed here
for context), in which creativity can extend between two poles: Big C for ‘extreme
forms of originality’ (e.g., Nobel-prize winners in science) and Little c for ‘everyday
creativity’ (e.g., adding butter to coffee to make it tastier), has usefulness in framing
creativity for practical purposes. For example, if creativity is framed primarily in Big
C terms, then notions of developing a better creative competence for any professional
group (teachers included) becomes a very tall order indeed. In contrast, if we see
creativity in terms of such a continuum, then we enter a completely different arena
for conceptualizing creative teaching; one that is both challenging and realistically
achievable for any motivated teaching professional.
Secondly, it is important to understand how the creative process works, especially
the underpinning thinking processes, as these are fundamental to producing creative
outcomes. There is certainly an extensive research literature base on all aspects of
5.6 Expertise and Creativity in Teaching 171
creativity and many factors have been identified as contributing to such outcomes,
including biology, biography and the systematic use of creative techniques and tools.
Changing biology is difficult, and past biography is exactly that, hence a focus on
the creative thinking process and how the mind works may be the best avenue for
enhancing creative capability in practical ways.
I like travelling to different countries and have been fortunate to work and partici-
pate in a wide range of cultural contexts. I have also seen most of the acclaimed tourist
sites, especially in Asia. However, what I find most interesting is talking to people in
local eating places, sharing stories and finding mutually meaningful humour. For me,
stories provide the key narrative to understanding the human condition and building
rapport with people, irrespective of culture and location. How the creative process
works can also be framed in terms of stories, as each creative act has a story to tell.
One story that comes readily to mind and it fully fits a Big C categorization is that
of Percy Shaw and his invention in 1933 of the cat’s eye, a road stud for lighting
the way along roads in the dark. While there are a number of stories on what led to
him inventing the cat’s eye, a popular version (one that I like anyway) is that on a
foggy night in 1933, when he was driving back to his home in the Boothtown area
of Halifax from nearby Bradford, he hit a perilous stretch of road with a sheer drop
down a hillside to the right of the road. It was very dark and Percy could not see where
the road ended and the hillside began, until suddenly he spotted, in the darkness, the
reflections of his car headlamps in the eyes of a cat sitting by the road. It is then that
he is said to have hit upon the idea of replicating the reflection of a cat’s eyes to guide
drivers along dark and dangerous roads.
The main purpose of this story is to illustrate some important aspects of creativity.
Firstly, once invented, a creative act often seems so simple and logical. How many
people before Percy Shaw had, on a dark foggy night, noticed the reflective power
of cat’s eyes to light, but failed to make the internal neural connections to create a
new perception which may have generated the idea of a reflective road-stud? As De
Bono (2003) emphasized: “…every valuable creative idea must always be logical in
hindsight.” (p. 24).
Thirdly, creativity is not a process of ‘thinking out of the box’, which is impossible,
I think. Rather it is more useful to think of it as a process that changes internal neural
connections and creating new representations ‘in the box’, which is, of course, the
human brain. The process moves on further when these ‘new representations’ become
conscious and surface as a new perception of phenomena in the external world—as
happened to Percy Shaw above. Typically, without disruption, the brain will process
information through established learned neural networks, and that makes good sense
as life would be chaotic without a high degree of perceptual consistency. As De Bono
(1992) summarized:
What it all amounts to is a system in which incoming information sets up a sequence of
activity. In time this sequence of activity becomes a sort of preferred path or pattern. (p. 17)
So whenever we look at the world we are only too ready to see the world in terms of our
existing patterns… (p. 18)
172 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
For many people, everyday life is a fairly ordered series of activities in which
existing neural networks fire in relation to well-known and predictable stimulus
events, which further reinforce those connections. There is little need for creativity,
or the likelihood of it occurring. An interesting question is whether or not continually
enhancing one’s knowledge will eventually result in creativity. Such activity will
certainly increase neural density and elaboration in long term memory and the notion
would seem to have face validity in that many big C people fit this description.
Leonardo Da Vinci, for example, was no sloth on the knowledge stakes, frequently
referred to as a polymath (i.e., a person whose expertise runs across several subject
domains and professional fields).
However, creativity involves more than having rich knowledge bases and exper-
tise. Many experts are not noted in the creativity ratings. Hence, while expert knowl-
edge bases may enhance the likelihood of a new perception that results in a creative
outcome, it is far from guaranteed. Other factors are invariably important, such as
personality, other neurological features, belief systems, effort, and typically, some
luck. For example, as with successful learning generally, what may be of particular
significance is the desire and belief in one’s creative capability, and the persistence
to keep going with a problem scenario until a creative perception occurs. As Albert
Einstein is famously quoted:
It’s not that I’m so smart, it’s just that I stay with problems longer.
New ideas may be produced by an unusual coming together of events. New ideas may be
produced by a chance provocation provided by nature… (p. 67)
In terms of explaining Percy Shaw’s creative act of generating the idea of the ‘cat’s
eye’, the notion of ‘an unusual coming together’ and ‘chance provocation provided
by nature’ seems to fit nicely. Who knows, if the cat had not been there, on that night,
would the new perception have emerged? How methods of provocation can relate
to producing creative instructional strategies for teaching (i.e. developing creative
teaching competence) will be explored later. The important point to emphasize here
is that novel perceptions must inevitably have, as their basis, the elaboration and
restructuring of neural configurations. Creativity is essentially just another aspect of
the generic process of learning, but with a different cognitive spin (so to speak); the
building of more differentiated frames on reality. As de Bono makes fully explicit:
In my view learning, creative thinking is no different from learning mathematics and any
sport. (p. 57)
A similar analogy can be applied to the naturally funny people in life, whether
professional comedians or otherwise. Do such people have funnier experiences than
those lacking humour, or do they actively look for the funny side of the experience,
provoking new ways of experiencing everyday reality, hence creating the conditions
in which funny outcomes are more likely? For example, in East London humour
(well when I lived there some 30 years ago), there was a type of humour which was
referred to as ‘selling a dummy’. This involved making a silly statement in jest and
waiting to see the response of others. For example, if someone talks perhaps too
positively about someone else (e.g., they are very talented, kind, generous, etc.), a
listener may respond by saying, “Well, she must have some good points as well”.
If this results in the speaker, taking this seriously and responding with some mild
annoyance, he/she has been “sold a dummy”. Now, what’s clever, if the person who
was being sold the dummy initially does not falls for it, and sells a dummy back, it’s
called a ‘double dummy’ and that’s a very witty thing to be able to do. Can you do
this?
As a Cockney from East London, I think I know the answer to the question posed in
the last paragraph. If you look at things in the same way and do the same things in the
same situations, you will typically (unless there is a chance provocation) get the same
results. To get different results, it is necessary to do something differently. Hence,
it is not surprising that people who desire and persevere in deliberately connecting
things that may not initially seem to be naturally connected and look for new ways of
perceiving aspects of reality are likely to produce more creative outcomes, whether
in engineering design, teaching, or in making people laugh.
Finally, in terms of fostering creative outcomes, there appear to be recognizable
phases, mindsets and activities that can be systematically employed to stimulate and
enhance creativity. For example, Petty (1997) described the creative process as con-
sisting of six interrelated phases: inspiration, clarification, distillation, perspiration,
evaluation and incubation (p. 15). He also highlighted:
174 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
One of the main difficulties for creative people is that the different phases require radically
different, even opposite ‘mind-sets’, each of which is difficult to sustain without deliberate
effort. (p. 19)
Most significant in the context of this chapter is moving the focus of one’s mind
through the different stages, from generating new possibilities and applying more
critical thinking frames (e.g., analysis, comparison & contrast, inference and inter-
pretation, and evaluation) until the idea reaches fruition and practical application.
This is in many ways the result of good thinking, which not only involves manag-
ing the thinking process (cognition) but the whole swirl of beliefs, emotions and
other vagaries of the human mind. Perspiration, which is massive effort over time by
another name, is an expected necessity in most cases, especially for coming up with
something exceptionally novel and useful (e.g., a big C creativity outcome) as this
is far from easy as we all know. Thomas Edison made the point most bluntly:
Genius is one percent inspiration and ninety-nine percent perspiration.
Claxton’s (1998) analysis of the interplay between our fast conscious mind (which
has some similarities to Kahneman’s, 2011, description of “System 1 Thinking”,
outlined in Chap. 2) and a slower more fluid ‘undermind’ of “unconscious awareness”
(p. 10), which acts as an “intelligent unconscious” (p. 133) is particularly interesting
in this context. It sees creative ideas as being slowly and unconsciously brewed in
the neural re-configurations of long-term memory and when sufficiently structured,
flashing from the unconscious mind into conscious thought. In other words, while
we are not consciously seeking a creative solution our mind slows down, becomes
more relaxed and uninhibited, enabling it to do such creative work in its way, and
eventually switching on that elusive new perception on reality. Claxton (1998) may
have captured this internal process accurately:
Interesting intuitions occur as a result of thinking that is a low focus, capable of making
associations between ideas that may be structurally remote from each other in the brainscape.
(p. 148)
The above analysis on how creativity works in terms of psychological and neu-
rological functioning, and factors that may promote its development and capability,
helps to similarly frame creative teaching from a more evidence-based perspective.
Firstly, using Amabile’s (1996) definition earlier, I previously offered (Sale 2015)
the following operational definition of creative teaching:
Creative teaching occurs when a teacher combines existing knowledge in some novel form
to get useful or valuable results in terms of facilitating student learning and attainment. This
may be either planned before the act of teaching or invented as a response to the demands
of the here and now learning situation. (p. 100)
differently tends to work better. Over time, using an EBT approach with lateral think-
ing (e.g., deliberate practice using lateral thinking), most teachers, those motivated
to do so, will get better and quicker at coming up with more interesting and effective
components to infuse into their lessons. They will be developing creative teaching
competence.
For over two decades, I had responsibilities for the mentoring and coaching of
under-performing teachers, and many of them were ‘conscripts’ (e.g., sent to me
for improvement). Over a decade ago, having worked with hundreds of such folk,
in many different contexts, I noticed patterns of behaviours and thinking that made
understanding what ineffective teachers do quite easy to work out. It has variation in
nature and form, but there is an underlying syntax of ineffective teaching behaviours,
irrespective of whether they are intended or not, and in most cases, it is not intended.
Essentially, in the context of EBT, they violated many of the core principles of
learning very consistently and to some measure of negative effect. Eventually, hav-
ing cracked the ‘code’ of poor teaching—so to speak—it occurred to me, that it is
equally tenable to be able to unpack what effective creative teachers do and how
they do it (e.g., the work of Bain 2004, was pioneering in this context). Hence, for
more than a decade, I have continued in this vein as a researcher in Singapore. I must
confess, that developing the creative teaching framework documented in Creative
Teaching: An Evidence-Based Approach (Sale 2015) was not a question of switch-
ing on a ‘creative thinking switch’, but the very opposite; many thousands of hours
of hard work, probably building up—incubating—over many years (much occurring
unconsciously)—as documented earlier in the chapter. It was like that. As Winget
(2007), in his book titled “It’s Called Work for a Reason”—fits my experience well.
5.7 The Syntax of Creative Teachers 177
In observing teachers who consistently get high attainment results and positive
feedback (both quantitative and qualitative) I typically notice high levels of engage-
ment and, in particular, good rapport with students. Most significant, was that while
they might have different teaching styles, there is an underpinning syntax in what
they do in terms of certain behavioural elements that positively shape student expe-
riences, which lead to such results. While people make sense of the world based on
prior experience and selective perception, our common human apparatus and need
orientation typically results in shared ways of experiencing the world. Indeed, with-
out this commonality, the inter-subjectivity of everyday life would be even more
problematic than it is already. As Marton (1981) argued:
…we have repeatedly found that phenomena, aspects of reality, are experienced (or
conceptualized) in a relatively limited number of qualitatively different ways. (p. 181)
You may recall from Chap. 1 that even the defining of effective teaching has
proved contentious and problematic over the decades, so what chance is there of
achieving an adequate definition of creative teaching? I can only offer a frame on
this and let’s initially recognize fully that we have little choice but to live with a
fair measure (whatever that is) of subjectivity in making sense of the world. For
example, we have beauty contests and there are judges, contestants, decisions made,
and winners identified. Well, how does that work when supposedly, “Beauty is in
the eye of the beholder?” Well, it is and it isn’t—right? Yes, beauty is subjective, but
there’s a lot of common agreement, explicit or otherwise, about what its key features
are, at least at a heuristic level; and that’s why I have yet to win one.
Now, what’s important is that in the mediation between teacher and students,
and between students, some things facilitate learning and well-being much better
than others—as we have documented, and this is the basis of EBT. However, much
is subtle and working unconsciously, and appears almost ephemeral—but that is
because the mind ‘is as the mind is’—essentially chaotic and open to subtle influences
in terms of impacting mood or psychological state. However, combinations of certain
behaviours, often linked to specific activities, create positive perceptions and feelings
about what is occurring, and we know what positive feelings do—create a nice little
shot of dopamine.
The following sections outline and illustrate some specific notable features and
aspects of CTC, which I have captured in the acronym SHAPE (Stories, Humour,
Activities, Presentation Style and Examples). This acronym provides a useful and
easy to remember metaphor as it is so much a term in our everyday vocabulary for
qualitative descriptions of things. For example, when a person has attained a high
level of physical fitness, which is visible in terms of muscle tone, etc., we might say
to the person that he or she is in “good shape”. The converse is also true at the level of
perception, though we are highly unlikely to say this to the person. In the context of
creative teaching, SHAPE was just something that came into my conscious mind as
a result of indwelling about what creative teachers do (based on students’ qualitative
feedback on these teachers) at the behavioural level in their classrooms. There were
so many references to ‘stories’, ‘humour’, ‘interesting activities’, ‘personality of the
teacher’ and ‘good examples’ in the students’ qualitative responses about teachers
178 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
that were perceived as creative or interesting. These same teachers also got responses
very much in tune with Willingham’s (2009) description of the teacher as ‘being a
nice person’. It is interesting that Willingham, in reviewing researchers’ analysis of
feedback questionnaires to figure out which professors get good ratings and why,
noted that one of the interesting findings is that most of the question items are
redundant. He suggests that:
A two-item survey would be almost as useful as a thirty-item survey because all the questions
really boil down to two: Does the professor seem like a nice person, and is the class well
organized…Although they don’t realize they are doing so, students treat each of the thirty
items as variants of these two questions. (p. 50)
While academics may break up the components of highly effective teaching into
a wide range of sub-components or constructs, this is not how students perceive and
apprehend the experience of their teachers; rather their perception is based on more
holistic generic constructs such as personality and organization. As Willingham
summarized:
When we think of a good teacher, we tend to focus on personality and on the way the teacher
presents himself or herself. But that’s only half of good teaching. The jokes, the stories, and
the warm manner all generate goodwill and get students to pay attention. But then how do
we make sure they think about meaning? That is where the second property of being a good
teacher comes in - organizing the ideas in the lesson plan in a coherent way so that students
will understand and remember. (p. 51)
5.7.1 Stories
When there are no more stories to tell, we may be in that perfect world where
thinking is redundant and there are no problems to solve. At the personal level, we
communicate our experience through the stories we tell: they reflect who we are,
the sense we have made of our experiences and they become a stimulus for other
people’s perception and the quality of attention they are likely to give us.
Watch the very best speakers in any field and there is typically a story in their
presentation, invariably a very poignant one to their main purpose and audience. From
the perspective of enhancing a learning experience, stories connect powerfully with
others as they immediately associate with their own experiences, especially at the
emotional level. From an evidence-based perspective, as the cognitive neuroscientist
Willingham (2009) suggested:
The human mind seems exquisitely tuned to understand and remember stories – so much so
that psychologists sometimes refer to stories as “psychologically privileged,” meaning that
they are treated differently in memory than other types of material. (p. 51)
Learners may forget the factual content of our lessons but stories that are embed-
ded with meaning, especially when it connects to their own experiences, needs and
interests, are committed easily to memory and provide a powerful anchor for recall.
Stories can also be transformative in that they connect with people emotionally and
are a key means of enhancing positive beliefs. As noted previously, beliefs are no
more than perceptions that have been around a long time, but they are real to the
believer. There is often little point in telling students that they are smart when they
believe they are not. Changing limiting beliefs most readily occurs when people
are confronted with evidence, over time, that consistently contradicts the belief, and
which is perceived as real and meaningful. This is where stories can provide an
effective means for initiating alternative ways of looking at the world (i.e., refram-
ing). Reframing refers to looking at things in different contexts and, in doing so,
give them different meanings. For example, in certain contexts, if I feel it may have
relevance and meaningful learning impact for students, I have deliberately used per-
sonal stories relating to my own experiences at school (you will have noted this from
Chap. 1). Has this led to some students actually significantly reframing aspects of
their belief systems and then going on to make the necessary behavioural changes
(e.g., put in personal effort, acquire key knowledge and skills, develop metacognitive
capabilities, persist when things get tough and seek good feedback) and, as a result,
becoming more successful (however defined)?
180 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
5.7.2 Humour
The importance of humour and its uses to enhance learning were identified in Core
Principle 9: A psychological climate is created which is both success-orientated and
fun. The ability to create and use humour productively for the benefit of others is a
rare and highly sought-after skill, which may explain why professional comedians
earn significantly more than teachers. Useful specific definitions of humour have
proved problematic, as it takes many forms and is always situated to persons and
context. What some people may find extremely funny, others may simply find deeply
offensive. Earleywine (2011), in this context, frames humour generically to focus on
its interpersonal and outcome features:
Humour is an intricate interaction between the perceiver and the perceived…
…humour is anything that somebody deems funny. (p. 21)
Similarly, Tamblyn (2003) sees humor as “a state or quality”, which has a number
of aspects:
…humor is openness, optimism – a sort of yes-saying to life. Humor is creativity. Humor is,
above all, play. Humor, creativity, and play are the same thing because they all involve the
same act: Finding new connections between things. (pp. 9–10)
182 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
While there are many genres or types of humour (e.g., jokes, anecdotes, wise-
cracks, witticisms, banter, wordplay), they all typically play out in an interpersonal
context. If the humour is perceived by some as funny, it will result in smiles or laugh-
ter in some form. You may also have noticed that when some people laugh, it often
has a contagion effect causing others to join in, often unknowing of the exact source
of the humour. It is as though laughter of others, just as listening to sad songs or
watching emotional scenes in a film, evokes our mirror neurons (Rizzolatti and Sini-
gaglia 2008). These are neurons that fire automatically when observing someone else
having an experience, creating an inner feeling of having the experience oneself and
we respond with sad emotion, often with tears, almost instinctively. Mirror neurons
may also be an important consideration for how we interact with students in other
ways. For example, showing enthusiasm and displaying good equity in dealing with
students may have similar productive unconscious influences at the neural level.
In a similar vein, Martin (2007), drawing from brain imaging research, reports
that exposure to humorous cartoons activates the well/known reward network in the
limbic system of the brain (e.g., Mobbs et al. 2003). The funnier the particular cartoon
is rated by a participant, the more strongly these parts of the brain are activated. As
he summarizes:
This explains why humour is so enjoyable and why people go to such lengths to experience
it as often as they can; whenever we laugh at something funny, we are experiencing an
emotional high that is rooted in the biochemistry of our brains. (p. 7)
Perhaps the most critical aspect of humour for learning is its impact on attention
and perception in the learning situation. Firstly, humour, by definition is typically
unpredictable and often is a surprise element in human experience. Hence, it has
both a Von Restorff effect as well as creating a sense of immediacy (e.g., Anderson
1979). Martin (2007) summarizing the research on the impact of immediacy, notes:
Past research has indicated that greater levels of immediacy are associated with more positive
student attitudes towards the class and instructor, greater enjoyment and motivation, and
greater perceived learning. (p. 353)
5.7 The Syntax of Creative Teachers 183
However, a new joke that we find funny, is funny because of its novelty. If we hear
someone tell it a second time, it may be boring. My wife, for example, will berate
me for repeating a joke or funny story with “We’ve heard that one before.” Now, of
course, when told to a new audience, it will be novel and if their mindset is ‘in sync’
with that type of humour it will get the usual positive response. If something is new
to a particular person, it is creative in their eyes. The essential point is that humour
typically catches the attention of the brain as it creates a strong Von Restorff effect,
as we explored in Chap. 1 and as Morrison (2008) explained:
The surprising aspect of humour affects the attentional centre of the brain and increases the
likelihood of memory storage and long-term retrieval. Humour has the potential to hook
easily bored and inattentive students. As brain food, humour can’t be beaten. (pp. 2–3).
Note, in teaching, while a good piece of humour will work well in getting attention
and helping to build a positive perception of you in the students’ minds, but it has no
or limited positive impact once repeated. However, the good news is that once one
can use humour, there is an almost unlimited supply of resources. It’s not the genre
that becomes habituated to, it’s only the specific example, whether joke, cartoon or
story.
Humour is not just an attention grabber; it is also an experience shaper. I have
a good friend who has a far greater creative capability for humour generation than
anyone I know. He can tell the funniest of stories, display spontaneous wit, and
typically gets folk laughing almost at will. When he is not present at an event, people
notice immediately and ask, “Where is Tom?” If he is not coming, the groans of
disappointment can be audibly heard. Tom’s presence creates pleasure and novelty
and people feel comfortable in talking (and laughing) openly with him. He creates
that type of rapport.
In the context of teaching, the same patterns of human attention and perception
play out, again much of this is unconscious. Being liked is a big factor in positive
student evaluation of their teachers and humour shapes this perception helping to
foster a positive psychological climate and facilitates the building of rapport. There
are several subtle interacting components at play in this experience formation. Firstly,
as Morrison highlighted:
Humour thrives in an environment of trust and is a major factor that contributes to building
trust. (p. 6)
is also true, enthusiastic, humorous and fair-minded teachers are more likely to be
perceived as ‘nice people’. Morisson (2008) goes as far as arguing that:
Humour is a key element in building positive relationships with students that will make
classroom management an invisible element. (p. 59)
in terms of student learning. This teacher did win an excellence in teaching award,
and it was not difficult to see why. Apart from the ability to use humour, there were
several other aspects of his practice that, as a total experience, made perfect sense
from an evidence-based perspective. He also had the creative component—using his
honeymoon experience (a Von Restorff effect) as the foundation for teaching optional
pricing. That’s everyday Little c creativity, and it’s not that difficult to do when one
understands the underpinning syntax and heuristics.
Humour then, like other aspects of human capability can be understood, and,
while difficult to define, can be described in very specific terms and therefore, as
Morisson (2008) argued:
Humor is a procedural skill that can be learned. (p. 58)
For example, when telling a joke, as when telling a story, there are key aspects
of an effective presentation. These include keeping it moving fairly quickly, using
movement and expressive voice tone when modelling a conversation between people,
and a quick pause before the punchline. This is not difficult to model, understand
and, with some deliberate practice, show a reasonable proficiency in delivery. When
people tell me they can’t do this, I can usually change their minds in around 1–2 h,
through modelling of the story/joke and getting them to do the necessary practise
with appropriate feedback. They are not quite ready for a professional career as a
comedian but are good to go in terms of adding this humour component to their
teaching skill repertoire of resources. Even easier is the use of materials’, such as
audio or video clips. The important point is that you don’t have to be funny, just the
communication of this intent is a rapport builder and communicates your humanness.
I recall one teacher who, every week, shared either an amusing story about his dog or
a cartoon with his students. It was hardly highly sophisticated humour, but it always
lifted the mood in the classroom and communicated to the students that he was
making an extra effort to make learning more interesting (or at least, less painful).
Quick wit is a really powerful humour resource but requires more skill in terms
of recognizing when and how to use it. I have seen this used by skilful teachers to
manage a wide range of potentially disruptive behaviours. Quick wit can break up
situations of potential conflict, as it’s hard to build up an aggressive psychological
state once interrupted by something funny. This often works simply by slowing down
the negative response long enough for the evoked emotions to settle at the neurolog-
ical level and a more rational state of mind to take prominence in consciousness. I
often use wit to ‘call the room’, an old comedian’s term for being straight and honest
with the audience. For example, in a long session on a dry subject, I might say to
the students something like, “I know you would rather be on an exotic Island with
‘Person x’ (I identify a local celebrity of noted glamour as exemplar) than here with
me doing this on a Tuesday afternoon, but what can we do about it?” This needs to
be done with a bit of ‘playfulness’ in terms of voice-tone and gesture but typically
works as intended because it communicates your empathy and that you share some
of their learning ‘pain’. Quick wit is probably the most difficult of all humour to do
consistently well, and the ‘quick’ component can lead to saying something that may
not be intended and which may be perceived as offensive by some. However, I use
186 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
this type of humour extensively and have yet to be slapped or reprimanded in any
way, so it’s not that risky—if one is thoughtful of context.
Even for the teacher who is genuinely self-conscious and chooses not to experi-
ment with any form of humour, there is a fairly easy solution, and this is to allow the
students to generate some humour and simply participate with a genuine smile. The
teacher does not need to be, nor should be, the seat of all humour, it’s much better
when it’s collaborative. Table 5.2 provides a guide for using humour as part of one’s
creative teaching repertoire of resources.
Finally, it is important to recognize that infusing some humour into everyday
classroom interactions is not taking valuable time away from learning the subject.
Quite the contrary, as Dewey argued:
To be playful and serious at the same time is possible, and it defines the ideal mental condition.
5.7.3 Activities
In designing an instructional strategy, one of the most impactful aspects of the learn-
ing experience is the activities that students are engaged in to facilitate key learning
outcomes. Activities are usually used in unison with methods, and can sometimes
refer to the same thing. For example, case studies are considered a method of instruc-
tion, but the actual case is an activity in itself. In most basic term’s activities provide
specific structures for students to engage their thinking skills with selected content
knowledge and work towards understanding and subsequent application. Activities
can take numerous forms, varying from a single question posed to a large project or
dissertation, but they all share a common purpose which is to enhance the learning
process towards designated learning outcomes.
In practice, all instructional strategies are a sequence of planned activities. Some
are predominantly teacher-centred, such as explanation (typically framed as lecturing
or previously ‘chalk and talk’); others involve greater participation and student auton-
omy in terms of choice and management. There is much talk about the need to make
learning more student-centred, rather than teacher-centred and, as noted in Chap. 1,
‘The role of the teacher being changed from the “sage on the stage” to “the guide
on the side”. Frankly, I find this quite disturbing as all instructional strategies should
be student-centred, whether they are more teacher-directed or student-managed. For
example, lecturing, while typically involving more teacher talk than that of students,
does not inevitably mean a lack of student engagement and thinking. Invariably,
long periods of teacher talk, especially if it lacks organization and presentation style
is likely to be both boring and ineffective. However, where lectures are delivered
in ways consistent with an evidence-based approach (e.g., appropriate chunking, a
variation of presentation medium, focus on key concepts, questions to encourage
thinking) and good presentation skills, they can be highly effective and creative in
terms of enhancing student learning and attainment. It is a serious misconception to
perceive lectures as a passive experience for learners. As Hattie and Yates (2014)
make clear:
Within the world of psychology, there is no such thing as passive learning, unless this term
implies learning to do nothing, in a manner akin to learned helplessness. When we are
learning from listening or watching, our minds are highly active…People will often learn
more effectively from watching a model perform than from doing and performing that same
action in the flesh. Although we note that learners need to be active, this does not mean being
active in the physical sense of having to respond overtly. (p. 47)
In contrast, activity for activity’s sake is both un-motivating in many cases, and not
useful for enhancing learning and attainment. The important point is that an effective
instructional strategy is typically an organized series of well-chosen methods and
activities designed to meet the learning goal/outcomes for a given student profile;
the main decisions should relate to the design and facilitation of the overall strategy,
rather than a generic framing of it being teacher-centred-versus student-centred. The
aim, over time, is to enable students to develop the necessary learning-to-learn skills
and sufficient content knowledge to increasingly initiate, direct and manage their
188 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
The most basic form of activity is the question, which powerfully impacts all
aspects of the learning process. Robbins (2001) went as far as arguing that:
Thinking itself is nothing but the process of asking and answering questions. (pp. 179–8)
The very nature of posing a question suggests some gap in knowledge in long term
memory, hence the question. Once the answer is not found in long term memory,
other possible sources of information are then identified and sought to provide the
necessary information. If we ask the right questions about what we need to learn,
find appropriate resources and persist in building the necessary understanding and
competence, we should be well equipped to meet necessary attainment targets.
Questions can take many forms and serve different purposes. For example, they
can be closed, and focused on the memory of key factual knowledge, such as “What
is the currency used in Brazil?” This will provide immediate specific feedback on
whether or not students know this particular fact. However, helping students to build a
solid understanding of the subject content—that is memorizing relevant information
and making the necessary connections through good thinking—requires the use of
open-ended questions that specifically cue the relevant types of thinking documented
in Chaps. 2 and 3. Questions that can effectively promote these types of thinking are
typically what and how questions. The following are some examples relating to
aspects of this book:
• What is the relationship between deliberate practice and expertise?
• What are the similarities and differences between whole-class interactive teaching
and problem-based learning, and how does this affect the role of the teacher?
• What inferences and interpretations can be drawn from Hattie’s meta-analysis of
the research on the impact of learning styles on student attainment?
• How might we evaluate the effectiveness of our evidence-based teaching in terms
of enhancing student attainment?
• What other ways might we encourage our colleagues to take on the challenge of
being more creative in their teaching?
You will have noticed that the first four questions focus on critical thinking skills
(e.g., analysis, compare and contrast, inference and interpretation, evaluation) and the
fifth on creative thinking (generating possibilities). Of particular importance, students
need to clearly understand what good thinking entails (e.g., the cognitive heuristics
identified and outlined in Chap. 2), have opportunities for deliberate practice to apply
5.7 The Syntax of Creative Teachers 189
these in authentic real-world contexts, as well as receive clear and useful feedback
from expert professionals.
The creative challenge with activities is to produce authentic learning tasks that are
sufficiently challenging as well as to systematically infuse key concept knowledge
and appropriate thinking skills, tailored to desired learning outcomes. While thinking
may not be a desirable activity in all situations, for reasons outlined earlier, the brain
responds well to interesting mental challenges, and in some way novel. Hence, we
may be naturally curious and take pleasure in solving problems. For whatever other
reason would people do crosswords and other puzzles on underground railway system
and buses, apart from relieving boredom? However, apart from personal dispositions,
other factors influence our responses to activities, whether school-based or otherwise.
Firstly, what is sufficiently challenging will vary depending on the student group and
even for individuals within it. Secondly, as Willingham (2009) pointed out:
…curiosity prompts people to explore new ideas and problems, but when we do, we quickly
evaluate how much mental work it will take to solve the problem. If it is too much or too
little, we stop working on the problem if we can. (p. 10)
Activities that facilitate a range of differentiation (e.g., where all students can
be successful, but the activity enables more competent or motivated students to go
further in terms of depth or breadth of the knowledge and skill areas involved),
is particularly challenging and requires creativity on the part of the teacher. Such
activities can take various formats (e.g., cases, projects, problem-based learning,
simulations and experiments) and are based on real work applications of the subject
content How to design of real world tasks was explained and illustrated in Chap. 3.
Table 5.3 provides a guide for enhancing student learning through the creative
use of activities.
It must be emphasized that activities need to be appropriately integrated within the
overall instructional strategy which may also include other methods (e.g., cooperative
learning, reciprocal teaching, peer assessment). Also, just as a funny story can always
be ruined by poor presentation skills, a potentially high impact activity can fall
relatively flat with poor facilitation skills.
The importance of good presentation skills is obvious in the case of teaching. Unclear
voice, disorganized sentences, monotonous tone, dull body language, irritating man-
nerisms (however defined) etc., all contribute to a quick loss of attention, boredom,
disengagement and, for those wanting to learn, frustration. Increasing research evi-
dence is highlighting very specifically how aspects of our presentation style impact
on others’ perception of who we are and what we are like. The powerful impact
of voice tone and smile were identified prior, and it is certainly my experience that
smiling, often supported with appropriate humour, provides important feedback to
students about your mood and approachability, both at conscious and subconscious
190 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
levels, and has contagion effects. For example, if you smile at a student, he or she is
likely to smile back at you and this can quickly spread to his or her classmates. This
has such face validity—excuse the pun—but it is not so easy for many people to do
this and, even more importantly, to do it well. In my first year in Singapore, there
was a National Smile Campaign, as it was felt that local people did not smile much,
and this would be a good thing to encourage in the community. The intention was
well-founded, and it provided me, a former teacher of social psychology, to conduct
one of those ‘strange experiments’ that social psychologists often partake in. The
basic experimental design was simple, I smile at everybody I walk past in the local
‘hawker centre’ (this is a Singaporean term for an area comprising many vendors
providing food outlets, usually but not always outdoors). Social psychologists have
thick skins and can deal with embarrassment. What other occupational group goes
around with bold faces asking people to give up their seats in crowded underground
stations, to investigate the impact of social norms on people’s response behaviour?
(Milgram 1977). Many did get up, without asking for reasons. It might on the surface
seem unlikely that few people would ask why, but would you like a response such
5.7 The Syntax of Creative Teachers 191
as, “My colostomy bag has just broken and….”? Anyway, I conducted the smiling
experiment and held firm despite the great majority of local people, for the first day or
two, looking at me as though I was from another world. Persistence is another trait of
social psychologists, so I continued the daily smiling routine, and guess what? Some
started smiling back to me and, for one week (my designated time for the coverage
of the experiment), I had several brief conversations with local Singaporeans. A few
even congratulated me on my bravery. Of interest, no one during this week initiated
smiling at me. That experience may support the view that smiling is not particularly
easy for many people.
Furthermore, there is a skill in smiling, as in most aspects of interpersonal com-
munication. In the context of classroom teaching, it should be brief, natural and
unforced, involves scanning the whole group with quick friendly eye contact. Yes,
it’s a skill requiring deliberate practice, but one well worth developing both for effec-
tive teaching and social interaction generally. Of course, smiling is not the content
of one’s interaction with others but facilitates setting the climate or context of the
interactions, especially towards building rapport. And, as the famous success coach,
Anthony Robbins (2001) once wrote:
Rapport is the ultimate tool for getting results with other people. (p. 231)
Rapport is very much bound up with positive feelings towards someone you like
and results from your perceptions of what they do and how. There is a saying that
goes something like this, “People like people like themselves.” It also includes liking
people, who you might like to be like. This is often an unconscious bonding process
and reflects the human tendency for identification with desired social models. I like
Molden’s (2001) frame:
Rapport is long-lasting, elegant, respectful, and acknowledging in nature. Rapport con-
nects emotional centres and creates enjoyable bonds between people. Rapport is the intelli-
gent approach to influencing, regardless of positional power, whereas power and authority
are defaults for people in positions of power who have poor interpersonal skills and little
flexibility. (p. 72)
Presentation style is not just the ability to use one’s voice and specific aspects of
body language to maintain positive attention and build rapport, but also to quickly rec-
ognize the reactions of other people, and modify one’s communication style accord-
ingly to encourage desired changes in their behaviour. This ability to quickly notice,
monitor, and to make sense of the external cues from other people, through careful
observation and empathic listening, is often referred to as sensory acuity. These skills,
while typically associated with professionals working in various fields of applied psy-
chology and detective or military work are now increasingly becoming part of the
training of salespeople and customer service personnel. The field of neuro-linguistic
programming (NLP) which focuses on influencing other people’s behaviour through
the use of language, voice tone and a range of non-verbal communication strategies
has much to offer teaching professionals in terms of enhancing presentation style.
It involves skill in recognising patterns in body language and voice characteristics
to understand the states of mind of other people and to be able to make accurate
192 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
inferences about their perception and feelings. In this way, it is then possible to
communicate more effectively and get better results in terms of building mutual
understanding and rapport Ultimately, perception and judgements about other peo-
ple, accurate or otherwise, are the product of their behaviour, and of course, our
pre-existing beliefs. The use of NLP is explored further in Chap. 9, in the context of
developing teacher expertise.
In teaching, as in other human interaction situations, the processes of perception,
apprehension and response play out and lead participants to construct their realities,
and these can be favourable or not for the teachers concerned. For example, Wadd
(1973) warned:
In establishing the order he has decided upon, the teacher must be fully aware that what
happens in the first few encounters with the pupils is likely to establish the relationship
which he will have to live with for the rest of his contact with that particular class. (p. 87)
Like on a first date, for a teacher encountering a new class for the first time, it can
be the first step toward a long-lasting positive relationship, or it can be a disaster. In
the language of psychology, as introduced prior, this is a Primacy effect, which has
its roots in our evolution as a survival mechanism. For example, it probably wasn’t
helpful to take too long in ascertaining that the large sabre tooth tiger at the entrance to
our cave may not be there offering us a meaty barbeque item. Similarly, the Recency
effect also discussed earlier, is impactful in these ways. What you say or do at the
end of a teaching session, or on your first date, may have a similarly strong impact
on the other’s perception, feelings and subsequent response to you. Hence, good
Primacy and Recency effects can put one in a potentially strong position in terms of
the ‘person perception stakes’. To make this even more impactful, introduce a novel
and interesting aspect—a Von Restorff effect—to the communication encounter, and
you may well be on your way to achieving positive framing by others, in many (not
all) situations.
For example, enthusiasm is typically considered a positive feature of a teacher’s
presentation style. However, the inference and interpretation of a person’s enthusi-
asm can only be made by others based on their observation of behaviour, not the
person’s intent. Hence, if you want to be perceived as an enthusiastic teacher you
need to behave like one (e.g., displaying the range of behaviours that people, within
the cultural and normative context, typically frame as enthusiasm). We know that
variations in movement and proximity, voice and tone, certain types of posture and
gestures and eye contact, all contribute to positive perceptions of enthusiasm. We
also know that this needs to be done skillfully for it to work in practice. In other
words, the best-contrived performance works best, but it must not be perceived as
contrived—sounds kind of strange, but true. That’s why great actors earn such big
bucks.
What this means is that the way we structure and conduct our communication
behaviours to other people is crucial for influencing their perception of us and the
kind and amount of attention, if any, they are likely to give. The impact of good
presentation style is particularly significant in influencing students’ perception of
you as a teacher, especially in the area of being seen as a ‘nice person’.
5.7 The Syntax of Creative Teachers 193
Many factors and contingencies will affect how others perceive our behaviour
(e.g., personality configuration, cultural values and norms, prior experience, mood
and situation) but it is far from a random process. People with high proficiency
in emotional and social intelligence typically have a deep understanding of how
interpersonal communication works, and, perhaps more importantly, why it often
does not. As a result, they can, through good thinking and careful structuring of their
communication strategies; mitigate the likelihood of ambiguity and misperception.
Note, I say mitigate not eliminate. I remember a quote, but cannot trace the reference
(apologies), but it captures the context so nicely:
Life is a matter of perception, though more often than not a misperception.
5.7.5 Examples
In teaching students over three decades and across many educational and cultural
contexts, a generic response that sticks in my mind is students asking for examples.
It’s as though an example will provide that special key to open the door to understand-
ing the concept or principle being taught. Examples are a representation of a class
or a group of things and, as understanding requires classifying things to generalize
and differentiate, they play a key role in this organizing process. More complex con-
ceptual understanding is analogous (this is also a kind of an example) in many ways
to completing a large jigsaw puzzle. I can remember back to my childhood years
when opening the box of a large jigsaw puzzle containing many hundreds of pieces,
that feeling of both challenge and anxiety at the thought of the task ahead. However,
by using certain strategies (e.g., finding and assembling all the straight edge pieces
to form the border; putting together pieces of specific items in the picture) the task
becomes more manageable and eventually, towards completion, easy, as the last sev-
eral pieces can be quickly put in. Building complex understanding is a bit like this. In
terms of the jig-saw analogy, examples are key ‘instructional pieces’ to facilitate this
process, enabling the mind to organize information and build a clear and accurate
194 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
picture of reality (mental schemata) for the topic being learned. The use of a range
of worked examples (Effect Size of 0.57, Hattie 2009), in which students can clearly
see the full process or procedure for completing an activity, enables them to fully
connect their prior knowledge with new knowledge, build understanding and be able
to transfer learning to other relevant contexts in which it has useful applications.
According to Hattie and Yates (2014):
The worked example effect now stands as one of the most robust findings from applied
psychology research. Worked examples provide a form of modelling through demonstrations
of successful procedures or products. (p. 151)
Examples also include such things as analogy and metaphor. These liken one thing
with another for simplification and making things mean in terms of the particular
prior experience of the student involved. For example, I often use the analogy of
creative teaching as akin to good cake-making. A good cake requires a solid base
and an attractive topping. Creative teaching requires an evidence-based instructional
strategy, which has great SHAPE. It’s a very simple analogy, but easy to remember.
The reason I have selected examples as a key area for creative teaching is that there is
almost unlimited potential for finding or creating powerful examples, analogies and
metaphors to support the learning process. Like stories, examples cover all aspects of
5.7 The Syntax of Creative Teachers 195
human experience, and we can use them to creatively communicate difficult concepts
in simplified but authentic ways. Good analogies and metaphors are also very likely to
result in a strong Von Restorff effect, stimulating attention and interest. Like stories,
we can never find or create all the possible relevant examples for the topics we teach,
and this always provides us with a challenging and creative avenue for practice.
However, over time, with diligence and some creativity, it is not too difficult to
build an extensive and varied portfolio of these valuable pedagogic tools. Table 5.5
provides an advance organizer for this.
SHAPE is a metaphor for a range of activities and resources that can be utilized in the
creative design and facilitation of instructional strategies. The selection, blending and
enactment of such activities and resources determines, in large part, the experiences
of the learning group. Better the blend and facilitation, and its alignment to the
learning outcomes, student profile and situated context, the more likelihood of better
quality in terms of the learning experience and its outcomes (e.g., effectiveness,
efficiency and engagement). To use an analogy, it is like the very best chefs, who
can use a range and blend of ingredients to getting that special taste that customers
respond to, that can differentiate them from other expert chefs. Creative teachers use
their ‘ingredients’ (personal and professional knowledge and skills) to design and
facilitate lessons to better connect with student needs and interests than those less
creatively competent.
196 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
Creativity in lesson planning, in most basic terms, is the ability to combine meth-
ods, activities and resources in novel and useful ways that can significantly heighten
the learning process for students in terms of attainment and engagement. Often some
of the methods, activities and resources may seem to have little connectivity in them-
selves, but when creatively combined and contextualized to the subject content, they
make the learning of key concepts almost easy and fun. Again, another analogy,
this time from the field of environmental engineering: one may ask, “What is a
mirror got to do with solving a problem of tenants complaining about long elevator
waiting-times?” There seems no immediate connection. However, there is a well-told
story involving a multi-storey office building in New York, where many occupants
complained about the slowness of elevators at peak hours. Several of the tenants
threatened to break their leases and move out of the building because of this. In
response, the management authorized a study to determine what would be the best
solution. The study revealed that because of the age of the building no engineering
solution could be justified economically. The engineers said that management would
just have to live with the problem permanently. However, a young psychologist who
took on the challenge of solving this problem reframed it differently and concluded
that the complaints were as much a consequence of boredom as slowness. Therefore,
he took the problem to be one of giving those waiting for something to occupy their
time pleasantly. He suggested installing mirrors in the elevator boarding areas so that
those waiting could look at each other or themselves without appearing to do so. The
management took up his suggestion. The installation of mirrors was made quickly
and at a relatively low cost. The complaints about waiting stopped. Today, mirrors
in elevator lobbies and even on elevators in tall buildings are commonplace.
In the context of teaching, here’s an example of making creative connections
between what would appear to be unconnected aspects of reality to produce an
effective learning experience for a group of students. Many years ago, mentoring and
coaching a teacher who had received consistently low feedback scores, I remember
him lamenting on how students found his teaching of Newton’s Second Law of
Motion particularly boring and difficult to grasp. He agreed to me observing his
lesson on this topic, which began with a typical technical verbatim definition of the
law, which went something like this:
Newton’s second law of motion can be formally stated as follows: The acceleration of an
object as produced by a net force is directly proportional to the magnitude of the net force,
in the same direction as the net force, and inversely proportional to the mass of the object.
The definition was then followed by around 40 min of exposition and the writing
of formulae on the whiteboard. I was confused and bored, but no more than the stu-
dents, based on my observations. In our post-lesson discussion, he acknowledged the
students lack student engagement and interest, but could not see how it was possible
to make this topic area interesting or meaningful for students to learn effectively.
Having explored with him exactly what Newton’s Second Law of Motion entailed,
we designed the following strategy as a way of attracting increased attention and
putting the law into a more practical perspective for the students. In summary, this
involved showing a picture of the famous soccer player, David Beckham, then a
5.8 Creative Activity and Resource Blending: The Art of Teaching 197
Manchester United player, taking his trademark free-kick. As soccer is very popular
in Singapore and David Beckham is considered to be particularly good looking, this
seemed both a good Primacy and Von Restorff effect combined to get good initial
attention from both the male and female students, albeit for different reasons. The
students were then asked to consider the following two scenarios and the impact they
might have in terms of the acceleration of the soccer ball once struck by David:
Scenario 1 One of the opposition players changes the soccer ball before the free
kick has been taken with a ball that is 20% heavier than the original
ball.
Scenario 2 The ball remains the same, but David has been doing extra fitness
training and can now strike the ball with around 10% more power.
I’m sure you have worked this out, so I won’t need to bore you with my limited
display of physics jargon. While not a perfect analogy, it was sufficient to get the
students attention, create some interest and make the psychological climate a bit
more fun than usual. The strategy also included a lively and humorous presentation
style in which the scenarios were simulated by the teacher (e.g., putting a real soccer
ball down and asking the students if he looked like David Beckham). As the teacher
concerned lacked certain skills in terms of voice and gesture it was necessary to
provide some measure of coaching here before the teaching session. In summary,
the strategy worked in that student attendance was high and they quickly saw the
relationship between mass, force and acceleration. Also, it lifted the mood of the class
in an afternoon session noted for low student attention. From this basis, the teacher
then made connections between the free-kick analogy and other real engineering
contexts, inviting and answering questions, before proceeding with the mathematical
formula and how it worked. He also chunked up the session and conducted short
quizzes and activities to check to understand and provide feedback. This was creative
teaching (remember Little c) as it was novel and produced useful results in terms of
student learning.
How many teachers have previously introduced Newton’s Second Law of Motion
through a simulation of David Beckham’s free-kick? Some may have, but that would
have been creative teaching also. The connections are not readily apparent until
you see them, as is the case with visual illusions, such as the famous ‘old’ and
‘young’ woman visual illusion. This is an example of lateral thinking in the context
of teaching, as one is unlikely to automatically connect a David Beckham free-kick
with Isaac Newton’s Second Law of Motion. As outlined previously, creativity, as for
good thinking generally, it is not necessarily something the human brain likes doing
naturally. For this reason, we need to provoke it into action and make a conscious
effort to think laterally.
The lesson incorporated several core principles of learning (e.g., focus on key con-
cepts, good thinking, psychological climate) and it also had a creative spin, which
made the experience more attention-grabbing and impactful (e.g., David Beckham
was a fairly powerful Von Restorff effect in this situated context). Also, when attempt-
ing creative activities in class with students, it communicates an important latent
198 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
message that you are genuinely interested in their learning and this is fundamental
in determining their perceptions of you as a person.
The teacher in question went on to receive significant improvements in terms of
feedback over the following two semesters. Invariably, this was not based on one
session but his realization that good teaching is much more than positive intentions
in a teacher’s mind, but the actual behavioural performances in class over time.
Most importantly, he was keen to improve his teaching and obtain better results,
both in terms of student learning and, of course, his feedback scores. Of particular
significance was his reframing of himself as a teacher, which was the result of the
direct experience of feedback from students. As he taught better and responded to
them more positively, they were more responsive and positive towards him, and good
rapport was developed over time.
Creative teaching can also occur in the here and now teaching situation in response
to the teacher’s perception and subsequent reframing of the learning situation. No
matter how well we try to plan an effective (evidence-based) instructional strategy,
there are occasions when the methods or activities do not work out as expected.
We may have made some incorrect inferences and interpretations about the prior
knowledge of the student group, not delivered the lesson activities as well as we can,
or it may simply be that some of the students are not in a good mood on that occasion,
for whatever reasons.
Even the weather can influence people’s behaviour. Mlodinow (2012) quoted
research by Cunningham (1979) in which waitresses in a shopping centre in Chicago
kept track of their tips and the weather over thirteen randomly chosen spring days.
Customers were probably unaware that the weather influenced them, but when it was
sunny outside, they were significantly more generous. It is therefore not surprising
that student attention and behaviour can vary so much, even with the same teachers.
Hence, don’t take it too personally, if things don’t always work out well; sometimes
they just don’t.
In situations where the planned strategy is not working the teacher, faced with
little by way of positive response, maybe at his/her most creative. As the saying goes,
“Necessity is the mother of invention”. For example, several years ago I was teaching
an elective module on learning strategies and skills at 2 p.m. on a Wednesday. These
electives were compulsory though students could choose which ones they took.
However, they were noted to be challenging in terms of getting student attention
and participation as students did not receive much by way of academic credit and
many thought this constituted unnecessary work. In my first lesson, before I had even
spoken a word, I quickly noticed the look of disinterest on the faces of many students.
It was apparent that if I just went ahead with the planned lesson, there might be little
value to their learning and tedious experience for all, including myself. I was acutely
aware of the need to change the students’ perception of the situation and was seeking
a strategy. Here’s the summary story in context. Firstly, of note, the students were
Singaporeans. In Singapore, education is highly valued and very well-funded, which
means that all students have good access to learning opportunities. A few years prior,
I was involved in a consultancy project in Kolkata, India. What’s the connection you
might ask? While working there I usually went for a walk after breakfast, just for some
5.8 Creative Activity and Resource Blending: The Art of Teaching 199
short exercise and mentally revising what I needed to do that day. On one occasion
I was approached by a teenage boy of around 13 years of age who asked me if I was
a businessman. Intrigued, I asked him why he was interested in that. In summary, he
pointed out that he was living on the streets, wanted to avoid getting into trouble and
was looking for an opportunity to get a job and learn some useful skills. He thought
I might be able to find him employment. Somewhat sadly, I explained that I was
not a businessman and could not provide him with any employment opportunities
(though I wish I could have done this). He left and that was that. My only significant
reflection at that time was that he did not ask me for any money.
Going back to the classroom situation in Singapore, a strategy flitted into my
mind. I walked around and looked at the students, one and all, and told them that
they were so lucky. One immediately perked up and said, “Why are we lucky?” I
replied, “Well, you are young, healthy and Singaporean”. They, of course, could not
dispute the first two assertions, but there was a quick response to the third, “What’s
so lucky about being Singaporean?” I told them the Kolkata story and while walking
around the classroom with a fairly serious expression on my face, making quick eye
contact with the students. On completion of the story, I asked them to discuss in
pairs what made them different from the boy in Kolkata. I used a verbal emphasis
on the word different by slightly raising the tone and slowing pace. It was not long
before they identified their situation of excellent learning opportunities and good job
prospects, which were lacking for the boy in Kolkata. I then said something like,
“Ok, well let’s not waste our time being negative” and started the lesson. To my
surprise, they settled down and the lesson seemed to progress quite well, especially
in the context of the earlier scenario.
I subsequently gave this little thought but was quite astonished by the response of
the students at the beginning of the next session. On my arrival, I was greeted by the
students with words akin to, “It’s ok, Cher (Singaporean slang for Teacher) we get
the message, no need to tell us the Kolkata story again”. They were quite good fun
to teach for the next 14 weeks and many gave feedback that they had learned some
useful stuff out of the elective. Was the story that impactful, or was I just lucky? Sure,
I exploited what is often referred to as a ‘teachable moment’; a situated unplanned
activity that I grasped as an opportunity to create an impactful learning experience
for the group at that specific time. In this case, the learning purpose was to change the
present negative attitude into one more conducive to learning and to do this I needed
to get some reframing of their present situation, by changing their perception of it.
In terms of lucky, who knows? On another day, I may not have thought about this
Kolkata story and I would have had to deal with the situation in a different way, which
may or may not have been as successful. However, without the story encoded in my
long-term memory, it could never have been part of my instructional strategy, albeit
constructed in situ. Hence, creativity requires both resources in long-term memory
as well as the creative competency to be able to see new combinations of methods,
activities and resources to structure a novel and effective instructional strategy for a
particular group of learners.
200 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
Similarly, while the core principles of learning have universality in terms of how
humans learn, learners come to the learning event with different biographies, per-
sonality configurations and prior knowledge which will influence their perception
and initial motivational status. Therefore, in planning the learning experiences, it is
really useful to ascertain as much as possible about the learners, both collectively and
individually. Invariably it is not possible to do this as thoroughly as one might like,
as it can be time-consuming. Equally, one must be cautious in making inferences and
interpretations from prior information about learners from secondary sources (e.g.,
attainment reports, other teachers’ framing) as objective or fixed. Often, I have found
that prior descriptions have been quite different and even at variance to what I experi-
enced. Teachers construct their realities, through their teaching practices and human
interactions with groups of students, as do the students themselves. I once inherited
a class of students where their prior teacher referred to them as, “That bunch of ani-
mals”. For the first two weeks, I could understand the basis of that teacher’s framing.
The students showed no interest in anything I tried to do, with many using a range
of negative responses to try to ‘wind me up’. It was obvious that many of them did
not like school or teachers. In this situation, there’s little point in trying to persuade
them verbally to see meaning in any aspect of the formal curriculum when they do
not. Furthermore, in my experience, there’s no point in doing anything that might
be perceived as confrontational as this will go nowhere useful for all concerned.
In such situations, my response, based on a strategy that has worked previously on
most occasions, starts with not showing fear or stress (of course, more easily said
than done) and maintaining a positive stance towards them. What this means at the
behavioural level is maintaining a friendly voice tone, smile and calibrated body
language. This will typically, over time, result in even the more vociferous of the
students losing interest in the activity of ‘winding up teacher’. At the psychological
level, a type of cognitive dissonance (Festinger 1957) will come into play. Cogni-
tive dissonance theory suggests that we have an inner drive to seek consistency in
our beliefs, perceptions and attitudes, and will experience inner conflict in situations
where two cognitions are inconsistent. For example, if students believe that teachers
are not particularly interested in them but are then consistently presented with one
who seems to be showing genuine interest, such dissonance may occur. In this sit-
uation, the person may either retain the existing belief (e.g., rationalization; denial)
or change the belief in some way. However, it is often not so clear-cut in terms of
perceptual change, and I would not expect students to suddenly completely reframe
and start liking teachers. I am only realistically looking for a slight shift in percep-
tion towards ‘Dennis is ok, for a teacher’. Once this has been attained, I am usually
able to engage in some informal non-confrontational chat with them and gradually
build a workable rapport. This is how most relationships develop over time, and
it is as much an unconscious as a conscious process. The key outcome is that the
reality of this situated context (e.g., negative confrontational student responses) will
change for the better. I remember hearing stories about how some people survived
the horrors of concentration camps by ‘being nice’ (at the behavioural level) to the
guards. It seems that it may be harder to kill someone who gives you a friendly word
and a well-calibrated smile. Whatever one’s views on this as a survival strategy, if it
5.8 Creative Activity and Resource Blending: The Art of Teaching 201
sometimes works in such situations, what can the genuine behaviour achieve in most
classrooms?
Having achieved a level of rapport, defined in terms of friendly banter with at
least a few individual students (this usually has a contagion effect over time), I am
then in a position to explore areas of possible interest and collaboratively identify
school-based activities that have at least a minimal buy-in from their perspective.
This is what happened with this particular class. By the end of the year, they were
quite responsive to learning and fun to teach. At the beginning of each session, I had
to run the gauntlet of jokes for several minutes, but they would always settle down
enough to do some ‘useful schoolwork’. The main significance of this story is that
at the beginning of the following semester something really interesting happened. I
was not timetabled for this group of students as another teaching faculty had been
was. I approached this colleague and asked if I could take them on, and he could
choose any one of my classes in exchange. He was somewhat surprised but readily
agreed. On arrival in class on their first session, the students were surprised to see
me, though visibly pleased. When I explained I had exchanged another class to teach
them, one stated that this had never happened before in their school life. Over the next
two years, they choose to do a City & Guilds qualification in Communication Skills
(with all passing and many getting distinctions) and the ‘O’ level English Language
(in which around 50% passed). Even the principal of the institution was shocked by
such results and congratulated me. This was not what seemed the likely outcomes
after the first few lessons, which were largely encounters of sarcasm and nihilism.
Had I tamed ‘that bunch of animals’, or were they not that bad in the first place?
Life is a matter of perception, and at any point time, it’s the reality. All I can say is
that I preferred the latter reality, and I think the students did. And I also think they
benefited in terms of learning and well-being—I certainly did.
When I have been asked, “What was your most significant achievement as a
teacher”, I often tell this story. After establishing a positive learning relationship
with these students, and seeing them develop a real sense of personal belief as able
learners, this fully reinforced my perception of the potential value of teaching and
the impact it can have on student attainment and well-being. It also taught me that it
was damn hard work.
A few years past my wife persuaded me to accompany her to see the magician David
Blaine perform a live show in Singapore. I rarely go to such events and have never
before seen a world-renowned magician perform live. I don’t believe in magic in the
metaphysical sense but can appreciate the illusion of the experience. David Blaine
did not disappoint on this count. However, I was a bit disappointed not to have been
selected to participate in one of his magic segments, not for reasons of ‘being on
stage’ but to get that close up view of how he does things. As a psychologist, who
should possess a good level of sensory acuity in terms of observation skills (he says
202 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
hopefully), I thought I might be able to work out how he performed the particular
piece of ‘magic’, at least in theory. Even though I did not get this opportunity of
a close-up view, I was impressed with David’s expertise—it was surely magic for
us mere novices. I can, in contrast, remember my father doing card tricks and other
bits of magic such as separating his thumb in two when I was a kid. It seemed quite
awesome when I was five years old, but by eight years old I had worked it all out.
The card tricks had a planned sequence (arranged beforehand) and the separating
thumb was the thumb of the other hand, disguised by two fingers.
How might we explain the apparent magic of such expertise? The answer lies
in the earlier discussion of Core Principle 7: The development of expertise requires
deliberate practice in Chap. 2. David has developed a level of expertise in his magic
acts that it has become part of his physiology (cognitively, effectively, and in terms
of neural wiring) that makes him different from the rest of us. It’s amazing, but it’s
not magic. For example, in his final act in Singapore, David immersed himself in a
tank of water for over 10 min. I would have died within a minute, but in 2012 Stig
Severinsen was awarded the record of “Longest time breath held voluntarily (male)”
by Guinness World Records for holding his breath for 22 min. This makes David’s
performance almost routine for such experts. Stig has a doctorate in medicine and
started experimenting with holding his breath as a child at the bottom of his parents’
pool. Hence, while his performance is exceptional and world-class, it is explainable.
Experts can do things far better than the general population because they are different
in significant ways and, therefore, it feels like magic to the rest of us.
There is a saying, “One swallow does not a summer make”. This essentially means
that seeing one swallow (swallows are birds that typically migrate toward warmer
weather) is not sufficient evidence that the summer is, in fact, on its way. I have no
idea how many swallows one must see to feel confident about the impending arrival
of summer in a given environment. The point in this context is that one creative act
of teaching does not make an effective learning experience for students, nor define a
creative teacher. There needs to be consistency in the overall evidence-based design
of instructional strategies over time. This is the case with all areas of expertise in
any field. However, while we would expect expert teachers to be able to teach to
consistently high standards in terms of their ability to maximize student learning
opportunity and achievement scores, it is unlikely that even the most creative can
come up with creative aspects (e.g., original components of SHAPE) every lesson.
Furthermore, creativity can arise in so many ways in teaching, as SHAPE illustrates.
For example, some teachers may be creative in terms of the activities and examples
they create for students, others in their presentation style and humour. Very few are
likely to be able to weave highly creative SHAPE, incorporating all components, in
most lessons they teach. That would constitute creative teaching competence at the
highest level of proficiency.
However, creative teaching competence, like other forms of competence, is based
on the same core principles of learning and involves the teacher developing from
novice to varying levels of proficiency towards expertise, and ultimately to adaptive
expertise. This may throw better light on differing conceptions of teaching as ‘art’,
‘craft’ or ‘science’ that have appeared in the research literature (e.g., Eisner 1995).
5.9 The Magic of Expertise: Getting into Great Shape 203
Creative teaching is science, art and craft combined. We now understand, in large
part, how this works in terms of the underlying syntax and heuristics involved. There
is no real dichotomy between the science and art of teaching, as both are underpinned
by strong evidence-bases from diverse fields in the human sciences. The art is the
capability for creative weaving of methods, activities and resources into high impact
instructional strategies. They can be seen in terms of great SHAPE, and just as David
Blaine creates the aura of magic in his performance, the most creative teachers create
similar experiences in their classrooms. It has the illusion of magic, but it is expertise
and can be learned by motivated teaching professionals over time. Once attained it
is exactly as Intrator (2003) depicts in his description of excellent teachers:
A potent teacher will skillfully and gracefully create conditions and stage activities that
inspire students to have a sustained and meaningful encounter with a subject – because they
can. (p. 7)
The notion of reflective practice has long been a buzzword in teacher education in
terms of how teachers can go about improving aspects of their practice. However,
reflection like thinking is a very general term and asking somebody to do good
thinking (or reflection) is making some very big assumptions about prior learning.
If teachers are as confused on what constitutes critical thinking, as Wagner (2010)
suggested in Chap. 2, we may similarly question the extent and quality of their critical
thinking when reflecting on aspects of professional practice.
However, let’s not ascribe blame to teachers for gaps in knowledge relating to
current research on human learning or even a lack of application of evidence-based
practices. Our earlier tour into Educational Jurassic Park in Chap. 1 provides ample
explanation for teachers’ reticence to buy into new initiatives. Furthermore, given
their busy schedules and the increasing plethora of demands placed on them, it’s a
wonder that many function as effectively as they do.
Scholarship, which involves research and sustained interaction with ongoing
developments and new knowledge relating to a field, is foundational to improve-
ment in any professional arena. Reflection, when underpinned by good thinking and
scholarship go ‘hand and glove’ in enhancing understanding and improving aspects of
practice. How teachers can thoughtfully use an evidence-based approach to improve
teaching, both at individual and collective levels will be further illustrated in Chap. 9.
204 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
5.11 Summary
This chapter, using the process and features of good design and building on the
pedagogic framework outlined in previous chapters have sought to unpack what
constitutes creativity in the context of teaching (e.g., the key underlying processes
and how they work in terms of producing creative outcomes). We no longer need to
view creativity as some mystical or ephemeral activity, limited to a few exceptionally
talented people. The creative process can be understood in terms of the underlying
cognitive processes involved, then modelled and applied to the design and facilitation
of learning. Therefore, Creative Teaching Competence can be learned by any teach-
ing/training professional through the acquisition of EBT knowledge and practices,
creative thinking as outlined here, and a strong volition to achieve such capability.
References
Amabile TAM (1996) The meaning and measurement of creativity, chap 2. In: Creativity in context.
Westview Press, Colorado, pp 19–40
Anderson JF (1979) Teacher immediacy as a predictor of teacher effectiveness. In: Nimmo D (ed)
Communication yearbook, vol 3. Transaction Books, New Brunswick, NJ, pp 543–559
Andrews J et al (1996) The teaching and learning transaction in higher education: a study of excellent
professors and their students. Teach Higher Educ 1(1)
Apple W et al (1979) Effects of speech rate on Personal attributions. J Pers Soc Psychol 37(5):715–
727
Bain K (2004) What the best college teachers do. Harvard University Press, Cambridge
Blank WE (1982) Handbook for developing competency-based training programs. Prentice-Hall,
Englewood Cliffs, NJ
Beetham H, Sharpe R (2007) Rethinking pedagogy for a digital age: designing and delivering
e-Learning. Routledge, London
Bloom BS (ed) (1985) Developing talent in young people. Ballantine Books, New York
Bouchard T et al (1990) Sources of human psychological differences: the Minnesota study of twins
reared apart. Science 250(4978):223–228. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2218526
Bransford J et al (1999) Brain, mind, experience & school. National Academy Press, Washington,
D.C
Brophy JE, Evertson CM (1976) Learning from teaching: a developmental perspective. Allyn and
Bacon, Boston
Carr W (1989) Quality in teaching: arguments for a reflective profession. The Falmer Press, London
Chickering AW, Gamson ZF (1987) Seven principles for good practice in undergraduate education.
Am Assoc High Educ Bull, 3–7
Claxton G (1998) Hare brain tortoise mind: why Intelligence increases when you think less. Fourth
Estate Limited, London
Costa A (1991) The search for intelligent life. In: Costa A (ed) Developing minds: a resource book
for teaching thinking. Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development, Alexandria,
VA
Cunningham MR (1979) Weather, mood and helping behaviour: quasi experiments with sunshine
Samaritan. J Pers Soc Psychol 37(11):1947–1956
De Bono E (1992) Serious creativity: using the power of lateral thinking to create new ideas. Ebury
Publishing, London, U.K.
References 205
De Bono E (2003) Serious creativity 2. Allscript Establishment and Harper Business, Singapore
and New York
Dweck CS (2006) Mindset: the new psychology of success. Ballantine, New York
Earleywine M (2011) Humour 101. Springer, New York
Edison T, Quote. Available at: https://www.brainyquote.com. Last accessed 6 Nov 2019
Einstein A, Quote. Available at: https://brainyquote.com. Last accessed 6 Nov 2019
Eisner EW (1995) The art and craft of teaching. In Ornstein AC, Behar LS (eds) Contemporary
issues in curriculum. Allyn & Bacon, Massachusetts
Elliot J (1991) Action research for educational change. Open University, Milton Keynes
Fasco D Jr (2006) Creative thinking and reasoning. In Kaufman JC, Baer J (eds) Creativity and
reason in cognitive development. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 159–176
Gardner H (1983) Frames of mind: the theory of multiple intelligences. Ingram, New York
Festinger L (1957) A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford University Press, Stanford, CA
Harris S (2010) The moral landscape: how science can determine human values. Free Press, New
York
Hatano G, Inagaki K (1986) Two courses of expertise. Child Dev Edu Japan, 262–272
Hattie J (2009) Visible learning. Routledge, New York
Hattie J, Yates GCR (2014) Visible learning and the science of how we learn. Routledge, New York
Intrator SM (2003) Tuned in and fired up: how teaching Can inspire real learning in the classroom.
Yale University Press, London
Katz LG (1993) Dispositions: definitions and implications for early childhood practices. Perspec-
tives from ERIC/EECE: A monograph series, No. 4
Liston DP, Zeichner KM (1990) Reflective teaching and action research in preservice teacher
education. J Educ Teach 16(3):235–254
Martin RA (2007) The psychology of humor: an integrative approach. Elsevier Academic Press.
ISBN 978-0-12-372564-6
Martin R (2009) The design of business. Harvard Business Press, Massachusetts
Marton F (1981) Phenomenography—describing conceptions of the world around us. Instr Sci
10:177–200
Mauzy J, Harriman R (2003) Creativity Inc: building an innovative organization. Harvard Business
School Press, Boston, Massachusetts
Milgram S (1977) The Individual in a social world: essays and experiments. Addison-Wesley,
Reading, MA
Mittler P (1971) The study of twins. Penguin Books, London
Mobbs D et al (2003) Humor modulates the mesolimbic reward centers. Neuron 40:1041–1048
Mlodinow L (2012) Subliminal: how your unconscious mind rules your behaviour. Vintage Books,
New York
Molden D (2001) NLP business masterclass. FT-Press, New Jersey
Morrison MK (2008) Using humour to maximize learning. Rowman and Littlefield, Lanham,
Maryland, USA
Nucci IP (2001) Education in the moral domain. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
O’Connor MC, Paunonen SV (2007) Big Five personality predictors of post-secondary academic
performance. Pers Individ Differ 43(5):971–990. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.03.017
Petty G (1997) How to be better at…creativity. Kogan Page, London
Petty G (2009) Evidence-based teaching: a practical approach. Nelson Thornes, Cheltenham
Pinker S (2003) The blank slate: the modern denial of human nature. Penguin, London
Rizzolatti G, Sinigaglia C (2008) Mirrors in the mind: how we share our actions and emotions.
Oxford University Press, New York
Robbins A (2001) Unlimited power. Pocket books, London
Sale D (2015) Creative teaching: an evidence-based Approach. Springer, New York
Schank R (2011) Teaching minds: how cognitive science can save our schools. Teacher College
Press, New York
Schon DA (1987) Educating the reflective practitioner. Jossey Bass, San Francisco
206 5 Creative Teaching Competence: The SHAPE of Creative Teachers
I must confess to previously being very sceptical concerning the early euphoria
relating to the supposed benefits of information-communication technologies (now
summarized as EdTech) in enhancing learning effectiveness, at least in the short term.
Like many others, I regularly experienced frustration when using technology-based
databases and software, often questioning, “Why is it that such a simple process
seems like the Mars mission?” I particularly remember attending an education con-
ference, waiting to listen to a keynote talk on the benefits of using technology in
teaching, only to see the speaker struggle with the applications and not able to even
get his PowerPoint slides up on the screen. After some 15 or so minutes he aborted
(or postponed) the presentation. I did not even bother to check. Anyway, such expe-
riences did little to inspire us non-techie folk to embrace technology for learning in
any sustained manner. Also, it does not seem many years past that e-learning was
being touted as, to use an old English metaphor, “The best thing since sliced bread.”
However, such early overhyping soon waned and it was not that long before a signif-
icant evaluation of the use of e-learning in education was referred to as a “Thwarted
Innovation” (Zemsky and Massy 2004). Similarly, Oliver (2007), commenting on the
However, much is now changing for the better concerning the use of EdTech
in teaching and learning. Several factors are contributing to this, and overall, their
impact is to merge pedagogy and technology into one seamless enterprise to offer
the increasing capability for highly effective, efficient and differentiated learning
experiences. This will become the arena in which professionals can fully display
their pedagogic knowledge (i.e., Pedagogic Literacy) and CTC. I would also like to
think that it may move teaching further towards the profession it has only fleetingly
threatened to become: one noted for its wide range of knowledge bases relating to
human learning, high skills in learning design and creative competence in practices
that significantly enhance learner attainment, engagement and well-being.
Firstly, and most significant, there is the recognition that technology tools alone
do not constitute anything near a learning revolution. No matter what we can create
6.1 My Early Scepticism Was Not Unfounded 209
Hence, no matter how much information we have in terms of gigabytes and ter-
abytes, it is not going to get quickly assimilated and nicely integrated into long-
term memory. The same Core Principles of Learning apply irrespective of mode or
medium. In most basic terms, looking into a computer screen does not change how
our memory systems work.
Secondly, there has been a significant reframing of the use of EdTech towards
pedagogic considerations and how they might enhance specific aspects of the learning
process, rather than the technologies per se. For many years I sat, and frowned, in
meetings on the use of technology tools. I listened to enthusiasts who showed that
with several clicks on fuzzily conceived icons one could read other people’s opinions
as well as offer one’s own opinion on an online discussion board. It seemed to them
that this technology affordance would exceed the frustration and inconvenience of its
complex technical use. It did not for most of us. The pain exceeded the relatively small
pleasure, and it was far from novel. From a pedagogic focus, and using an evidence-
based approach, we can now analyse and evaluate the use of various EdTech tools in
terms of how they can enhance aspects of the learning process. In this way, we can
select and creatively combine those e-tools which are most effective and efficient in
promoting learning, and not use technology simply because we have it.
Finally, the technologies are becoming more stable, much faster and, most impor-
tantly, user-friendly. For many years, apart from a lack of good pedagogic design,
there has been much criticism of online learning in terms of its ease of usability.
Shea-Schultz and Fogarty (2002) observed that very basic design failure is common
in e-learning environments:
When most learners complain about e-learning, it’s often not the training they object to but
the confusing menus, unclear buttons, or illogical links. (p. 117)
Taken together, while the initial hype of EdTech was premature and exaggerated,
there is now a strong evidence base for their potential to significantly enhance learn-
ing and attainment opportunities for a wider range of learners. EdTech tools are not
going to change brain capability and functioning in highly significant ways but driven
by a strong pedagogic literacy, they now provide an exceptional resource capability
for teachers to design learning experiences and teach in ways that are more effec-
tive, efficient and engaging. Miller (2016), from a cognitive psychology perspective,
summarizes the present scenario:
210 6 Creative Teaching Competence and EdTech: Total Pedagogy
I believe that technology gives us many advantages over and above traditional face-to-face
classroom techniques, but clarification is in order. I don’t believe that instructional technology
promotes learning by its mere presence. Nor does it let us evade some of the immutable truths
about how we learn – especially the fact that learning requires focused attention, effortful
practice, and motivation. Rather, what technology allows us to do is amplify and expand the
repertoire of techniques that effective teachers use to elicit attention, effort, and engagement
that are the basis for learning.
In a short space of time, technology in higher education has gone from a smattering of fully
online distance-only programmes and self-created web resources of a few individuals to
near-ubiquitous. (p. 1)
We can see, therefore, that many factors have led to more optimistic and viable use
of technology in education—but another significant factor is the increasing demand
for high-quality flexible access learning opportunities with reduced costs. As Miller
(2016) summarizes:
…one reason that interest in technology is exploding is that it is easier and cheaper than
before. (p. 12)
Even though this is a rapidly developing and changing arena, with new applications
and e-tools emerging almost daily, there is an underlying set of generic learning affor-
dances that remain relatively stable. All are related to enhancing some aspect(s) of the
learning process, whether the focus is more on providing subject content knowledge,
facilitating the building of understanding or skill acquisition. Furthermore, while
there may be many applications and specific e-tools available, they will inevitably
fall into a limited number of key genres or categories, relating to these broad learning
areas. For example, while tablets come in many formats and have different features,
they are essentially similar in terms of being compact mobile personal computers.
The same is true of the wide range of smartphones, and social media platforms such
as Facebook and Twitter.
In terms of EdTech genres, several broad categorizations have been suggested.
For example, Pacansky-Brock (2013) offered the following four main categories:
• Cloud-based applications—accessible from anywhere
• Web 2.0 tools, that make the creation and sharing of multimedia content simple
• Social media, technologies that enable communication and sharing
• Mobile apps.
This captures the essential range of EdTech options, which collectively provide a
rich resource base for teaching/training professionals to exploit in designing learning
experiences and facilitating their practices in effective, efficient, and creative ways.
I will be primarily focusing on e-tools that are user-friendly and can effectively
facilitate:
6.2 Framing EdTech Genres 211
As with most things relating to teaching and learning, there are different perceptions
of blended learning and any one particular definition will lead to some contestation.
Indeed, it could be argued that most learning designs are blended in the sense that
different methods and resources are typically combined in the creation of teaching
and learning strategies. As Littlejohn and Pegler (2007) pointed out:
Blending is an art that has been practised by inspirational teachers for centuries. It centres
on the integration of different types of resources and activities within a range of learning
environments where learners can interact and build ideas. (p. 1)
Effective teachers typically blend (or weave) methods, activities, and other
resources into pedagogically sound instructional strategies to meet desired learn-
ing outcomes for the students they teach. Furthermore, as we saw in the previous
chapter, the generation and blending of appropriate methods, activities and resources
are at the root of creativity in teaching. Here, the focus is specifically on the ped-
agogically driven blending of EdTech into learning design and teaching practices.
Over time, we will most likely talk less about the use of technology in teaching and
learning, as it will just be part of Creative Teaching Competence.
212 6 Creative Teaching Competence and EdTech: Total Pedagogy
Even in most lectures today, it’s rare not to see at least some use of EdTech, such
as PowerPoint slides or a video, blended into this traditional teacher centred format.
At the other end of the spectrum, we are seeing the growth of fully online courses,
in which there is no traditional face-to-face contact time. Invariably, discussions on
blended learning raise questions as to what content areas are best delivered online,
and on what basis, as compared to the face-to-face mode; as well as what percentage
of a programme should be delivered in these different modes. Such questions will be
addressed later in the chapter. In terms of operationally useful definitions of blended
learning, I find the following conceptions capture the essential framing:
Blended learning is the combination of different training “media” (technologies, activities,
and types of events) to create an optimum training for a specific audience. (Bersin 2004, xv)
In the best-blended learning design, the selection and organization of learning activities
and assessments support desired learning outcomes while maximizing the strengths and
minimizing the weaknesses - of both online and onsite environments. (Stein and Graham
2014, p. 28)
The concept of blended learning is attractive for several reasons. Firstly, from a
pragmatic point of view, there are affordances in terms of cost, time and convenience.
As the demand for higher education increases, in the face of public funding and
personal finances decreasing, high-cost long-duration face-to-face instruction may
become an option only for the wealthier minority. It’s not a high-level prediction to
forecast an explosion of low cost, even free, online or blended learning, as MOOC
(Massive Open Online Courses) have demonstrated. How this will eventually position
itself in the market context is open to anyone’s guess at present, but there’s little doubt
that blended learning, in whatever format (e.g., Flipped Classroom Learning) will be
a major curriculum option. The challenge will be to make it as effective, efficient, and
engaging as possible for increasingly diverse groups of learners (e.g., differentiation).
This is where creative teachers will be most impactful and needed.
There is evidence that blended learning can enhance learner attainment. For
example, The U.S. Department of Education, in a meta-analysis of online research,
reported that students in online courses performed modestly better, on average than
those in face-to-face courses, with blended students performing the best (e.g., Means
et al. 2010). Furthermore, according to the Educause Horizon Report (2019) Higher
Education Edition:
Students report a preference for blended learning, citing flexibility, ease of access and the
integration of sophisticated multimedia. (p. 12)
6.4 Designing and Facilitating Blended Learning 213
The EdTech design frame presented here can be applied both at the macro-curriculum
level (e.g., a module or unit of study), as well as for individual sessions or lessons.
Invariably, as in the face-to-face situation, even the best pedagogic design and prac-
tices will not engage all learners, and certainly not all of the time. As we know, when
dealing with humans, you will not, as the saying goes, “Please all the people all of the
time”. There are just better heuristics, but it’s an important ‘just’. From an evidence-
based approach, I use the following broad heuristics in the creative pedagogic design
and facilitation of blended learning:
1. Good learning design is based on Evidence-Based Practice (e.g., methods that
work best and consistent with how humans learn, i.e., embodied in the Core
Principles of Learning)
2. EdTech (e.g., internet, e-tools, multimedia) are used strategically and creatively
to enhance specific aspects of the learning process across the design, planning,
facilitation and evaluation of learning events
3. The completed blended learning design maximizes the affordances of a range
of learning modes, mediums and methods to enhance learning effectiveness and
efficiency (e.g., attainment levels, interest/engagement, differentiation, access
and flexibility).
For example, the selection and use of technology platforms and tools become one
of identifying the affordances of different categories of e-tools (e.g., content creation
& delivery tools; communication & collaboration tools; assessment & feedback tools)
and how they can be used to positively impact aspects of the learning process. The
following EBT guiding heuristics apply:
• In principle, if an EdTech facility (e.g., e-tool or an e-tool combination) enhances
any aspect(s) of the learning process (e.g., taking in, processing and applying
information in practice) for a group of learners, then there is potential use in terms
of infusion into the instructional strategy
• EdTech combinations that enhance a number of the core principles of learning
simultaneously are more likely to have a greater synergistic impact in terms of
enhancing student attainment opportunities and the experience of learning
• The creative blending of high effect size methods and cognitive scientific principles
(e.g., the core principles of learning) with appropriate e-tools is where teachers can
be most impactful, both on the motivational and attainment stakes. Such expertise,
both in planning and when the situation demands (e.g., when the planned strategy
is not working with a group of learners), can re-invent a new more effective strategy
in situ—is Creative Teaching Competence. In terms of Hattie’s (2009) ‘Russian
Doll’ analogy, the dolls are getting an added EdTech ‘makeover’, so to speak. We
would then be using the best method combination in terms of pedagogic design
and e-tools integration to support such methods.
214 6 Creative Teaching Competence and EdTech: Total Pedagogy
This guide can be systematically worked through in the learning design process.
Key focal areas for creative thinking and application will be in the strategic enhance-
ment of the learning process and maximization of the blend. You will often find
that while working through this design process in practice, new ideas or potential
resource blends will come into mind, making this as much an iterative process as
a linear one, and that’s where much of the creative connections will incubate and
hopefully flit into conscious thought—as we explored previously/That’s the way the
brain typically works in terms of creativity in any field.
The ‘brain is the brain’, whether it’s in a face-to-face situation or processing stuff
online. Do we need to adapt the teaching strategy, customizing what we know about
human learning and teaching methods to the online environment? Of course, we do,
just as there is always customization of instructional strategies in different face-to-
face contexts. However, we now have to effectively negotiate an added customization
and adapt it to the particular nature, affordances and limitations of this different
instructional mode. Clarke and Lyons’ (2005) analysis, in the context of human
learning, remains relevant for the foreseeable future:
The most robust instructional principles are those based on a model of human psychological
learning processes …Any given instructional method will be effective or ineffective depend-
ing on the extent to which it supports or disrupts basic-learning psychological processes
regardless of the delivery media. (p. 594)
Hence, no matter how much information we have in terms of gigabytes and ter-
abytes, it is not going to get quickly assimilated and nicely integrated into long-term
memory. As Moroder (2013) discovered from her experience:
Technology does not make learning more engaging or meaningful. A great lesson does
this…technology can make it more effective and efficient.
However, as summarized earlier, the situation is now changing for the better
concerning the use of technology in teaching and learning. Firstly, the technologies
are becoming more stable, much faster and, most importantly, user-friendly (and
many are free). Secondly, and most significantly, there is a reframing of the use of
technology towards pedagogic design considerations and how they might enhance
specific aspects of the learning process, rather than the technologies per se. Hence,
Horton (2006) makes the summative point in this context:
At its best, e-learning is as good as the best classroom learning. At its worst, it is as bad as
the worst classroom learning. The difference is design. (p. 3)
6.4 Designing and Facilitating Blended Learning 215
Quite simply, disorganized and over complex content in the online environment is
no less disruptive than in the face-to-face context—perhaps even more so. Similarly,
dull is dull, wherever, whenever; and we know how this works in terms of brain
processes. The framework developed in the previous chapters equally applies here.
EdTech simply provides a resource capability which, if thoughtfully used, has the
almost unlimited creative capability for enhancing learning opportunities for a wider
differentiated range of learners. Treadwell (2017) summarizes the present scenario
accurately:
The focus of technology is not to make the learner’s work look pretty or create far more
‘stuff’, but to give greater agency to the learner and drive their learning capacity deeper
by focusing on the new end-point of building conceptual frameworks of understanding that
learners can apply creatively to be innovative…
The key here is to leverage technology to make that happen, but the technology must be
simple to use, requiring no significant training. If ongoing training courses of any length are
required, then it is unlikely that in the long term, the technology will be used by educators
as it was intended. (p. 156)
EdTech does not change the fundamental ways in which the brain works and
therefore our focus must be on what the different e-tools can specifically do to
enhance aspects of the learning process. For example, at the most generic level,
we know that EdTech provides anytime and anyplace access to online resources.
Also, computers do not suffer from mental fatigue and we can, therefore, expect
consistency in performance, if the technical architecture is good. Hence, this is a
potentially good affordance for those who cannot attend class at designated times.
In understanding more fully the specific range of learning affordances that EdTech
offers, it is useful to consider what technology and human brains are disposed to do
well with information processing. This provides evidence-based guidance as to the
learning contexts in which technologies may be most effective. For example, we
know EdTech tools are much better than the human brain at:
216 6 Creative Teaching Competence and EdTech: Total Pedagogy
Based on the brief comparison and contrast above, certain inferences and inter-
pretations of what types of e-tools offer significant learning affordances for different
aspects of the learning process are readily apparent. For example, cloud-based appli-
cations such as Google Drive and Dropbox enable the storing, organizing, sharing
and collaboration of a wide range of content and applications. This enables teachers
to present extensive content resources in various formats and mediums, catering to a
wide range of learners’ needs and competency levels. The capacity to decentralise the
structure of knowledge bases and reconstruct them in dynamic customised digestible
bits (knowledge warehousing) makes knowledge even more directly accessible and
manageable. Similarly, and perhaps the most significant single learning affordance
of the online learning environment is the hyperlink which, at the click of the mouse,
can bring together a wide range of text-based, multimedia and personnel resources
way beyond what is possible in the traditional classroom. Hamilton and Zimmerman
(2002) illustrate this vividly when they wrote:
…the hyperlink, which is practicable without counterpart in the physical world of traditional
academics. Within an internet document, hyperlinks are used to bring multi-sourced infor-
mation into the primary text or to give the reader a path to alternative media. In essence,
this eliminates the physical separation of material messages that are logically connected. In
addition to text, hyperlinked messages may be pictures, sound files, animations, or video
clips. External links can refer students to other information-rich Internet sites, including
personal Web pages, specialized bibliographies, and professional specialists. (p. 270)
This provides the capability of creating networked resources that enable both
faculty and students to create, share and continually develop an extensive and varied
range of resources that can support the desired learning outcomes. These enable the
capability to:
• centralize key resources relating to a module syllabus (e.g., learning guides, module
maps, advanced organizers, annotated bibliographies of key resources, guidance
on how to negotiate potentially difficult topic areas)
6.4 Designing and Facilitating Blended Learning 217
• select prepared resources to support learning (e.g., notes, cases, videos, animations,
activities)
• select web links to provide a networked architecture of extended and dynamic
resources
• access, where appropriate, to other digital learning exchange portals (e.g., libraries,
specific learning communities).
This concerns determining what curriculum components and specific learning out-
comes can be effectively and efficiently met in the online environment, and what can
be better facilitated in a face-to-face context, the ‘balance of the blend’ so to speak.
From an evidence-based approach, the answer is primarily pedagogic and situated
rather than numeric. It is not a question of how much online learning versus how
much face-to-face learning; rather about how the face-to-face learning context can be
enhanced through ICTs and vice-versa. The real indicator of effective blended learn-
ing is not the amount of face-to-face or online learning but their effective integration
within a programme (Garrison and Kanuka 2004).
Therefore, if the previous two stages of the design process have been appropri-
ately negotiated, this final stage is essentially one of practicality and creativity. For
example, while we may have an ‘ideal’ blend in our mind, in practice the ‘right blend’
may depend on some other factors, which typically include the following:
• Programme type and focus (e.g., cost reduction, high impact on attainment)
• Learning group (e.g., prior competence, motivational level, cultural factors)
• Resources (e.g., budget and technology infrastructure)
• Content stability (e.g., enduring, relevance to key outcomes).
As the creative combination of methods, activities and resources underpin creative
teaching in the face-to-face context, the same design principles equally apply in the
online environment. We now have an increasing range and variety of e-tools that
provide affordances for different aspects of the learning process, hence the increasing
potential for more and more creative combinations. Furthermore, as we create and
develop effective and efficient method and e-tool combinations (blends), the creative
process will, over time, lead to highly synergistic embedded learning experiences that
will move us towards the ideal of maximising learning opportunities and attainment
for all students. This is creative teaching competence at the level of adaptive expertise.
The following section outlines the Flipped Classroom Learning approach (a vari-
ant of blended learning) and illustrates the EBT Blended Learning Design Model
with examples from a 3-year research project conducted at Singapore Polytechnic
218 6 Creative Teaching Competence and EdTech: Total Pedagogy
The following extracts are from two of the research-based cases on implementing
Creative Flipped Classroom Learning: An Evidence-Based Approach (Sale et al.
2017).
Supported Experiment 1: Digital Electronics
The Digital Electronics module in the School of Electrical and Electronic Engineer-
ing in SP has been flipped for 3 years. Apart from its impact generically, we have
become interested in how it specifically impacts weaker students. The learning design
process incorporated appropriate high ‘Effect Size’ teaching methods (Hattie 2009)
and cognitive scientific principles (e.g., Core Principles of Learning, Sale 2015).
Furthermore, the selection of EdTech tools was based on their predictive capability
to enhance specific aspects of the learning process. For example, the EdTech tool
Kahoot was chosen for its capability, when used effectively (especially creatively)
to get good student attention and engagement with the content learning, activate
prior knowledge and check conceptual understanding. In the language of cognitive
science, this had an excellent Von Restorff effect and hit the ‘sweet spot’ in terms of
an appropriate motivation strategy for many students.
Data from ‘Student Co-participants’ (e.g., Lincoln 1990) was collected to ascer-
tain how the students experienced the different components of the flipped class-
room innovation These were student volunteers who actively participated in the
research, providing regular feedback to the research team of their experience in
learning throughout the modules. The focus group of student co-participants com-
prised 11 of the class members. Based on agreed areas by the majority of students,
the following inferences and interpretations were recorded:
• The anytime, anyplace and opportunities for repeated exposure received a strong
majority affordance (which was to be expected).
• The use of the messenger app for smartphones like WhatsApp and EdTech tools
such as Socrative Exit Poll and Kahoot was widely noted as supporting learning.
Students found the provision of the short videos both supporting content under-
standing and interesting. They also identified and confirmed the learning benefits of
some of the key explicit EBT strategies employed (e.g. activation of prior knowl-
edge, checking understanding, timely and quality feedback, and the creation of
humour and fun as part of the learning experience).
• Of note, while the design of the instructional strategy is important, much of the
feedback seemed to be contextualized to how individual teachers facilitated the
learning process and interacted with students. In most basic terms the teachers’
style, personality and competence are key components determining the success or
otherwise of a flipped-classroom approach.
Student Performance
Apart from what the students told us about their learning experience, we also wanted
to find out the actual attainment levels in terms of the meeting stated outcomes. We
used both formative and summative assessment strategies to facilitate our learning
goals and objectives throughout the semester.
220 6 Creative Teaching Competence and EdTech: Total Pedagogy
The comparison was made between the flipped classes and the entire cohort of
students taking the same module, with overall results computed using marks from
general participation, continual assessment, laboratory tests, mid-semester test and
final exam. Figure 6.1 shows the comparison of student performance for over 3 years.
The results show better student attainment for the flipped classes as compared to
school overall. Furthermore, there is a consistent improvement in student attainment
for consecutive 3 years.
Supported Experiment 2: Plant Safety and Loss Prevention
Flipped Classroom Learning has been implemented for the Year 3 core module Plant
Safety & Loss Prevention in the Diploma in Chemical Engineering since Academic
Year (AY) 2015/2016.
The motivation for adopting flipped classroom stemmed from the observation
(and frustration) of the lecturer that students taking this module are often unable to
integrate knowledge and skills gained from earlier studies and developed a sufficient
understanding of the subject content. Students also lacked real-world experience in
the chemical process industries to fully appreciate the application of loss prevention
principles in the chemical plant. This, plus the ‘fact’ that safety itself is a relatively
dry topic due to its ‘common-sense-on-hindsight’ nature, makes teaching the module
especially challenging.
Like the Digital Electronics module, the learning design process utilized Hat-
tie’s high effect size strategies (2009) and Sale’s (2015) Core Principles of Learn-
ing. We specifically focused on the effectiveness of the following high effect size
interventions and using the ‘Russian Doll strategy’ (Hattie 2009) outlined earlier:
• Learning Outcomes: Advance Organizer (start-of-class) and Checklist (mid-point
and end-of-class) (effect size 0.40)
• Whole-class Interactive Teaching: Challenging Goals (effect size 0.56), Classroom
Discussion (effect size 0.82) with Peer Influences (effect size 0.53)
6.4 Designing and Facilitating Blended Learning 221
are purposefully active towards meaningful goals and have variety in the learning
experience. There are also more challenging questions for any group to tackle if they
finish the assigned questions earlier.
Google Doc, Google Slide and Padlet are used for student work submission in
a class, enabling the lecturer to review and give appropriate feedback in real-time.
Google Doc is used most frequently, predominant for the posting of group answers
for the activities mentioned above. Google Slide is used for activities requiring stu-
dents to arrange events in a particular sequence, for example, how a series of plant
modifications made out of good intentions, resulted in an unintended consequence
that proved fatal. The use of Padlet enabled individual students to pose questions
to lecturers on topics that they did not understand, as well as for posting individual
work in response to concept-type review questions posed by the lecturer.
The opportunity to view students’ work in real-time is perhaps one of the most
significant benefits of flipped classroom learning, as it afforded both the lecturer and
students to analyse and evaluate the learning experience in situ—so to speak. This
benefits the students as they get to clarify any doubts or uncertainty in understanding
on the spot. For the lecturer this means that he/she can get feedback on the effective-
ness of a particular classroom strategy employed, and depending on the situation,
can make adjustments in real-time as well.
To ascertain the effectiveness of the different high effect size strategies used,
survey questionnaires, focus group discussion, and student co-participants were
employed (as in the previous Supported Experiment). Overall, students find the learn-
ing experience enriching. On the more specific strategies, the students reported that
the use of advance organizers is useful in helping them to keep track of their learning
progress and how the various topics are connected to form the big picture, although
some found it difficult to understand at first. They also found that the use of self-
evaluation exercises after every topic (multiple choice and/or true/false questions in
Socrative) are useful. Likewise, students reported that the use of Google Doc and
Google Slide in the classroom is very useful in helping them to learn, especially from
each other. Students also informed that they found the use of mock assignments and
marking with rubrics is useful to help them understand the lecturer’s expectations
and how to improve on their work. They, however, did not find the use of checklist
very useful, partly due to the way the lecture used it – which is seen as more for the
lecturers own check rather than for the students to track their learning.
Besides feedback from student co-participants, the lecturer also engaged in
Evidence-Based Reflective Practice (Sale 2015). This involves a structured thinking
process (e.g. analysis and evaluation) using evidence-based teaching (EBT) princi-
ples to all valid evidence sources (e.g. students, peers, peer observers), rather than
personal reflections in isolation. As a holistic process, it enables a better under-
standing of the reality of classroom learning (e.g. what is happening, and how this
is affecting the learning process). Through this process, the lecturer can pinpoint
areas where strategy/EdTech tool blending can be improved, and also identifying
one’s blind spot in assuming that students can retain key learning points from ear-
lier modules. Furthermore, via this process, the lecturer can draw similarities in one
classroom activity that posed difficulty and adjust the delivery approach in a later
6.4 Designing and Facilitating Blended Learning 223
classroom activity, e.g. making better alignment of topics and making connections
to topics covered earlier.
Table 6.1 summarizes the findings on the impact of each strategy employed from
the students’ perspective (using broad categories of “Very Useful’, ‘Useful’ etc.,
based on students’ responses to the survey questions) alongside with the lecturer’s
own reflection based on his classroom observation.
In the following subsections, I will illustrate how selected EdTech tools can signif-
icantly enhance different aspects of the learning process. This will involve a consid-
eration of e-tools from the genres identified prior, focusing mainly on content devel-
opment and delivery, knowledge building for understanding, and supporting skill
acquisition. This provides the key underpinning components for the development of
competence. The e-tools selected are not meant to be comprehensive coverage of
the genres or prescriptive in any way, as many others can serve similar pedagogic
purposes. Also, I am focusing on those tools that I find user-friendly, both in terms of
the teacher/developers’ perspective as well as from the learner’s experience. Most of
the tools discussed below are either freeware or ‘affordable’, recognizing the latter
is always relative.
The importance of good subject content knowledge, the essential information that
needs to eventually end up as well as formulated mental schemata in the learner’s
long-term memory is a crucial element of effective learning. Hence, the selection,
organization and presentation of content are important considerations in all learn-
ing contexts, and especially so in the online context, which usually lacks immediate
opportunities for clarification and feedback. It’s important also to be highly sceptical
of the notion that content is all out there in cyberspace, just waiting to be downloaded
by a few clicks of the mouse. This is wildly over-optimistic. While there are numer-
ous and varied resources on the Internet, much may lack validity and usefulness.
Keen (2007), for example, makes a damning criticism of so-called internet expertise
knowledge contained in such sites as Wikipedia:
…the real consequence of the Web 2.0 revolution is less culture, less reliable news, and chaos
of useless information. One chilling reality in this brave new digital epoch is the blurring,
obfuscation, and even disappearance of truth. (p. 16)
• User-friendly functionality requiring only a short learning curve (in some cases
only a few hours) to master
• Teaching and learning resources can be produced and updated rapidly. This
depends on the number of resources produced but is significantly quicker than
the previous e-learning development software
• The content mix can include text, graphics, embedded videos and podcasts,
hyperlinks to more detailed and differentiated content, activities and assessments.
This enables faculty to quickly get up to speed in being able to produce and inte-
grate a variety of media-rich and interactive learning resources tailored to programme
learning outcomes and accommodating a range of student learning capabilities. When
guided by a strong pedagogic literacy and creative teaching competence, these tools
provide an enormous capability for enhancing the student learning experience at the
level of exposure to the content knowledge to be learned; these specifically include:
• Content structuring that ensures good chunking to reduce cognitive overload
• Multimodal presentation to enhance interest and application for the content to be
learned
• Ongoing formative assessment, enabling retrieval practice of key concepts through
short quizzes and immediate feedback.
From my experience, apart from PowerPoint which has been around for a long
while, I have found the following rapid content development software tools to be
particularly useful:
SoftChalk LessonBuilder enables the creation of interactive web pages for
e-learning courses. The software is easy to use (really) and it enables the quick
production of interactive lessons that have a professional look to them. Specific
features include pop-up text annotations, self-assessment quizzes, and interactive
learning games. After production, you can package the lessons for delivery via CD-
ROM, Intranet, Internet, or integrate with a Learning Management System. As their
homepage states:
If you can use a word-processing program, you can use LessonBuilder. Designed for teachers
and content-experts that don’t have time to learn complex software, LessonBuilder is simple,
yet powerful, with only the features you need to create exciting, interactive content for your
online course. It claims to enable you to:
Create custom lessons by combining your materials with interactive learning content. The
mixture of personalized content, embedded assessment, and interactivity will increase
student engagement and improve learning outcomes.
They offer free trial downloads and the software is well priced in terms of
comparative products on the market. The current website is https://softchalk.com/.
Camtasia Studio enables the creation of packaged lessons within a self-contained
video format that can be web-enabled. Its screen recording system that will capture a
prepared lesson (e.g., PowerPoint presentation) as well as your voice-over during the
recording. Using good pedagogic design, high-quality teaching videos can be shared
with students on the Web, CD-ROM, as well as on portable media players such as
the iPod. The live-action video component adds the human touch to the presentation
226 6 Creative Teaching Competence and EdTech: Total Pedagogy
material and enables both technical professionalisms as well as the use of informal
narrative and humour. The current website is: https://www.techsmith.com/store.
VoiceThread is a web-based application tool that facilitates the presentation of
an environment of integrated learning resources (e.g., images, video, documents)
in which participants can interact and contribute (e.g., voice, video upload) both
synchronously and asynchronously as part of collaborative discussion. It is a more
interactive collaboration tool than the other tools outlined. The application is easy to
use, provides a versatile learning environment that is easily modifiable and reasonably
priced. The current website is: https://voicethread.com/.
VideoScribe is an easy to use tool that enables the production of content, incorpo-
rating text, graphics and audio into a visually powerful and animated video format.
In terms of learning affordances, it enhances presentation impact, can highlight key
concepts, and works particularly well in terms of our memory systems. For creative
teaching and the development of one’s creative teaching competence, it’s a real play-
ground for such activity. The opportunities for creating novelty—those powerful Von
Restorff effects—into the content structuring are probably unlimited. It is both fun
and challenging to use creatively and, from what we know about human learning, it
will have positive impacts on learning, especially student attention and engagement.
The current website is: https://www.videoscribe.co/.
While these applications have quite different affordances, all have significant
capability to enhance learning effectiveness and efficiency in terms of content provi-
sion. Given the user-friendly nature of these e-tools, it will not be a time consuming
or frustrating experience to experiment with them or view some good exemplars.
From that basis, it should then be readily apparent which applications (and you can
use more than one) are best suited for particular parts of your curriculum and for the
students you teach.
The ability to engage with a wide range of relevant content in the context of open
communication and collaboration with peers and experts, where there is ease in
posing and answering questions to facilitate building understanding, has much by
way of learning affordance and cuts across many of the core principles of learning.
This is another area in which EdTech can have an increasing creative impact on how
learning is facilitated but requires, of course, creative teachers. Students themselves
will likely find creative ways in which to use these technologies as they are supposedly
the ‘Digital Natives’ (Prensky 2001). However, are they? There is much to challenge
such popular generalizations, as Hattie and Yates (2014), from reviewing the research,
pointed out:
The central problem with the digital native theory is that it is advanced in the absence of any
known database…In its raw form, the digital native theory has to be seen as considerably
overstated and incorrect. Human capabilities are not as flexible or tied to experience as this
theory might suggest. (p. 197)
6.4 Designing and Facilitating Blended Learning 227
He goes on to argue:
Because of the collapse of the core values of kindness, charity, love, being-nice, and respect,
people resort to meanness as a way of dealing with each other. (p. 76)
Most people are cursed with a combination of too much want and too much won’t. (p. 82)
How accurate Winget’s framing is, and how generalizable it is across countries
and cultures, is likely to be contested by many, and I am not going to pursue the
debate here. However, if there are increasing numbers of students who are lacking in
such values, this will make the job of teachers even more challenging in promoting
self-directed lifelong learning, which is based as much on values and volition as it
is on cognition. Such concerns are not just pertinent to this analysis and evaluation
on teaching with technology, nor of teaching per se, but of wider societal concern.
Anyway, back to the specific issue of technology for learning in this context. Of all
the EdTech features, perhaps the most prolific in terms of impact on young people’s
engagement is the ever-increasing range of Web 2.0 and social media e-tools that
enable communication, content sharing and collaboration. There is some confusion
between what exactly are the differences between Web 2.0 tools and social media
tools? It seems that it is as Beattie (2011) wrote:
It would be difficult to find two popular buzzwords that are in more of a quagmire than social
media and Web 2.0. (technopedia)
I am not too concerned about these fine differentiations, though I note the qual-
itative difference of social media being more focused on the ease and simplicity of
user-generated content sharing and reviewing. Collectively, these tools provide an
extensive platform for both asynchronous and synchronous communication, shar-
ing and collaboration. Learners can engage in multiple platforms of subject content
knowledge, share and collaboratively work with these knowledge sources. Such
activity has the potential to help learners to build understanding, join and participate
in learning environments and communities that focus on their specific interests and
learning goals, whilst in a psychological climate that works for them. Furthermore,
related to the capability for extensive resource connectivity, there is the specific con-
necting (both synchronously and asynchronously) of people globally. Learners can
now connect with their tutors, peers and other experts who give their time to enthu-
siastic learners, as well as pretty much anyone prepared to communicate with them.
In today’s internet society, we can readily go beyond this physical local community
of learners, to a global community of learners. In this context, there is the emergence
of “Communities of Inquiry”, which Garrison and Vaughan (2008) defined as:
…a formally constituted group of individuals whose connection is that of academic purpose
and interest who work collaboratively toward intended learning goals and outcomes. (p. 6)
However, some words of caution, as popular notions of students being able to build
new knowledge and deep understanding through their inherent creative capabilities
with various social media and Web 2.0 tools such as blogs and Wikis may also be
somewhat exaggerated. Indeed, Willingham (2009) argued that getting students to
create new knowledge should not be the main goal, rather it’s better to focus on
developing a deep understanding of existing knowledge. He noted:
…posing students challenges that demand the creation of something new is a task beyond
their reach - but that does not mean you should never pose such tasks. (p. 109)
The blog is a Web 2.0 e-tool I find particularly useful. It is easy to use and young
people are very familiar with it. Blogs are now an everyday part of communication
channels for most students, albeit with a more social rather than educational orien-
tation at present. However, a blogs capability to enhance learning and attainment is
high if we look at its potential impact through the lenses of the Core Principles of
Learning. For example, to learn a subject effectively, students need to connect new
information with what they already know (Core Principle 2: Learners’ prior knowl-
edge is activated and connected to new knowledge). The very nature and design
of a blog facilitate this principle very well, as blog posts typically appear in reverse
chronological order with the most recent post appearing at the top of the web browser.
If the blog is regularly updated, students will be able to see a progressive update of
the material covered each week and will be able to link what they are currently
learning with prior knowledge. Similarly, as different media types can enhance the
learning experience (Core Principle 5: Instructional methods and presentation medi-
ums engage the range of human of senses), blogs can enable the publishing of a
range of multimedia content on the web (e.g., video, audio, animation). They can
also make explicit and clarify learning outcomes, encourage good thinking, provide
rapid, clear and constructive feedback, as well as create a psychological climate that
is success-oriented and fun.
A further affordance of the online learning environment for promoting student
thinking is that the use of asynchronous text can provide certain significant advantages
over the typical face-to-face situation. In face-to-face learning, there is often too much
information to absorb and too little time for critical and creative thinking around
the content. As a result, knowledge may not be fully understood or even effectively
transferred into long-term memory. In contrast, the provision of enduring text, which
enables students to spend time revisiting this content, posing and answering questions
around its application, helps to build a solid understanding of topics over time. As
Hamilton and Zimmerman (2002) argued:
The medium supports iterative exchanges of information and opinions over an extended
period, so ideas are not merely “hatched” and delivered but rather allowed to evolve and be
refined in a manner that makes information more convincing, narrative deliveries richer in
detail, and learning more thorough. (p. 265)
The blog is certainly, from my experience, a high leverage e-tool. It is easy to use,
low cost and has a range of learning affordances that can be utilized with a sound
pedagogic literacy and some creativity.
Kahoot is a student response e-tool that works in an interactive game-based quiz
environment. It is free (at the time of writing) as user-friendly as an e-tool can get
and provides a platform in which student prior knowledge and understanding can be
readily assessed in a non-threatening and fun environment. The creation of questions
is simple, and the question types can be easily varied (i.e., the typical multiple-choice
question format). Also, when used creatively, it can do all the above, plus be used as
a catalyst for other parts of the instructional strategy (e.g., extended questioning and
discussion post the quiz questions; an advance organizer for new input). It’s also a
good Von Restorff effect—but don’t over-use this. Kahoot is, in terms of a previous
230 6 Creative Teaching Competence and EdTech: Total Pedagogy
analogy, an elegant ICT enhanced “Russian Doll” and, with good Presentation Style,
open to much creativity in the classroom. The current website is https://getkahoot.
com/.
While numerous e-tools are offering a wide range of communication options, it
is important to recognize, as with most things, that more is not necessarily better.
There is much online chat and sharing in these social media cyber-places, but we
may also question just how effective such online collaboration is in real learning
outcome terms. For example, Brown (2009) concluded that:
The internet helps move information around but has done little to bring people together. Cre-
ative teams need to be able to share their thoughts not only verbally but visually and physically
as well. I am not at my best writing memos…I haven’t heard of a remote collaboration tool
that can substitute for the give-and-take of sharing ideas in real-time. (p. 30)
Indeed, the development of good thinking is a major challenge in any medium and
we explored the reasons for this in some detail prior. Furthermore, we also explored
strategies which firmly established that student thinking can be developed through:
• The explicit modelling of the cognitive processes that are involved in good thinking
• Involvement in questioning processes that cues and reinforces specific types of
thinking
• Engagement in authentic real-world meaningful tasks (e.g., projects, case studies)
that are challenging but achievable and necessitate the use of the main types of
thinking (e.g., critical, creative, and metacognitive).
There are now many EdTech applications that can be used to facilitate and enhance
thinking, provided that they are employed thoughtfully within the context of the
pedagogic considerations and practices. Such applications include:
• Online tutorials involving active problem solving with feedback
• Hypermedia software integrating knowledge, multimedia, activities and feedback
• A range of communication tools (e.g., email, blogs, bulletin boards, forums)
• Constructing software (e.g., desktop publishing, spreadsheets, etc.) where learners
can produce, manipulate and change information
• Simulations and virtual reality programmes.
For example, in a chemical engineering module, in which students had to solve
a range of problems in a chemical reaction plant simulator it was noted that certain
key factors in the instructional strategy combined to enhance the quality of stu-
dents’ thinking. Most important was that the design of activities was a challenge but
achievable in the time allowed, coupled with appropriate questioning strategies such
as cueing such critical thinking skills as analysis, comparison & contrast, evaluation
and making inferences & interpretations (Sale and Cheah 2011). Where activities
were not experienced as challenging by students, the simulator lost this capability
6.4 Designing and Facilitating Blended Learning 231
for enhancing the development of such critical thinking skills. It was also noted that
in situations where both faculty and students had shared notions of what constitutes
good thinking, there was evidence of further enhancements in the quality of student
thinking.
Interactive Videos
We are primarily visual learners, and our visual system is so advanced as compared
with our other senses. While we struggle to keep several (at best) pieces of new
information in our working memory, we can immediately apprehend (not necessar-
ily understand) a new complex visual experience. That’s not to say we pay attention
to, or will remember, everything in that visual field, as other mental systems come
in to play, including memory. However, we don’t suffer from visual overload in
the same way we do with cognitive overload. It was long popular, but now largely
refuted (e.g. Hattie 2009), that people had distinct learning styles (e.g., visual, audi-
tory, kinaesthetic). I had long challenged such notions, based on reason rather than
the research findings that have surfaced since then. Visual dominance seemed obvi-
ous from experience, and captured in the saying, “a picture paints a thousand words”.
Words and auditory learning are of course also important, and these enhance visual
learning. Equally, kinaesthetic learning is important, as much learning, especially
skills, involves touching stuff—so that’s obvious. However, the implication of the
importance of our senses is not to seek to understand student’s learning styles (quite
the opposite); rather be creative in designing learning experiences that engage the
necessary range of senses relevant to supporting the learning outcome(s). This is also
the case for engaging emotions, as these can aid attention and memory encoding.
However, we may not need to engage smelling and tasting—or emotions—in every-
thing we teach. The same generic heuristics, as in the selection and use of e-tools,
applies—can it/they enhance an aspect(s) of the learning process (in cost-effective
ways)?
I have used video extensively as a key learning resource in teacher education, long
before they had the newer technological affordances of interactivity and augmenta-
tion in the form of quiz pop-ups, reflective cues, hyperlinks and other embedded
features that enable increasing differentiation. It makes sound pedagogic sense in
teacher education to use video of actual teachers, doing their work in authentic con-
texts and then exploring aspects of practice with them through skillful mediation.
Furthermore, short, focused videos can provide key conceptual understanding and
illustration of specific teaching methods and tools, which can be logically packaged
and customized (basic curriculum design stuff) and hey presto—you have some good
teaching and learning resources.
Previously, in micro-teaching or post teaching video-recorded sessions, I would
manually stop the video at those times that I felt were relevant to exploring a specific
teaching practice or principles of learning (professional judgment here) and facilitate
accordingly (e.g., open sharing of experience and connections with evidence-based
principles). I may also capture key points and/or questions on the whiteboard for
further or later reference. In many ways, pedagogically, I was doing then what now
can be done more efficiently and in varied ways with technology.
232 6 Creative Teaching Competence and EdTech: Total Pedagogy
Interactive videos offer many affordances in terms of supporting all the core
principles of learning as well as motivation generically. It is not surprising that
Treadwell (2017) offers such a bold assertion:
Over the next 25 years, we will experience the rise of video as the primary information
source that learners will use for research and inquiry. It will also increasingly become the
medium that educators use to demonstrate their comprehension and understanding. (p. 4)
Miller (2016), argues that one of the major directives from Mayer’s research is that:
…narrated animations work better than animations paired with text, most likely because the
latter arrangement requires the learner to shift visual attention back and forth between these
two visually presented sources of information. Auditory input can be processed somewhat
independently from visual input, a claim that makes sense given what we know about sep-
arate brain mechanisms used for visual and auditory processing. It follows that the two can
complement one another when presented in pair fashion.
6.4 Designing and Facilitating Blended Learning 233
Furthermore, narration works best when it uses conversational everyday language, compared
to when it’s formal and academic in tone. (p. 151)
Ilkka (2018), from a similar analytical standpoint, sees the current AI systems as
severely limited, suggesting that there are technical, social, scientific and conceptual
limits to what they can do (p. 3) and concurs with the view of Luckin (2018) that at
present AI lacks most of the human’s metacognitive regulatory capabilities.
In contrast, Harari (2018) paints a more sinister and worrying picture of the future
of AI. He suggests:
AI not only stands poised to hack humans and outperform them in what were hitherto uniquely
human skills. It also enjoys uniquely non-human abilities, which make the difference between
an AI and a human worker one of kind rather than merely of degree. Two particularly
important non-human abilities that AI possesses are connectivity and updateability. (pp. 22–
23)
6.4 Designing and Facilitating Blended Learning 235
Harari’s inference and interpretation of this scenario are not one of a replacement
of millions of human workers by millions of individual robots and computers—but
being replaced by an integrated network, that is immensely more powerful. What
we can infer and interpret from the consequences of this are unknown, and we may
easily fall into the realms of science fiction, envisioning something akin to that of
the Terminator film series, in which machines take control of the world. Is it possible
that an AI integrated network could possess a superordinate form of consciousness
that could turn on humans?—we simply don’t know. As Harari (2016) states:
The rise of AI and biotechnology will certainly transform the world but it does not mandate
a single deterministic outcome. (p. 461)
The report highlights several challenges, the most salient being a redefining of
what educational aims and goals educational institutions should best focus on; it
highlights:
As AI will be used to automate production processes, we may need to reinvent current
educational institutions. It is, for example, possible that formal educational institutions will
play a diminishing role in creating job-related competencies. This could mean that the future
role of education will increasingly be in supporting human development. (p. 34)
For example, Gallagher-Mackay and Steinhauer (2017) make the case for increasing
social and emotional skills in the school curriculum. They point out that:
Strong social-emotional skills, like self-regulation, growth mindset and peaceful problem-
solving, give students the tools to manage themselves and their relationships better and clear
the way for improved learning. (p. 103)
SEL programs do not merely impact behaviour; they are also correlated to higher academic
performance. (p. 75)
which range from the most common but highly effective asynchronous tool, the
email, to more interactive synchronous virtual classrooms, such as Lync. The choice
of particular e-tools is often a matter of preference, cost and access. Many can do the
same things, which are to provide communication through the various mediums of
text, audio and visual. What is most important is how communication is conducted,
and this is essentially a good Presentation Style. The creative challenge is how best
to apply this contextually for effective presence in the online environment. From an
evidence-based approach, we have a set of heuristics to ascertain what is likely to
work well, how, when and on what basis. As in the face-to-face situation, the initial
experience, the Primacy effect is very important. For example, faced with a new group
of students, the disorganized teacher with an uninspiring presentational style is likely
to experience a very quick downturn in levels of attention and engagement. In the
online environment, this negative experience is likely to be even more heightened for
participants, often leading to early attrition. In my online tutorials, I typically start
with a short video, which will have some carefully crafted and positioned supporting
text to make the best initial contact, without cognitive overload. A major goal at
the onset is to communicate my approachability and commitment to supporting the
learning group. I am very mindful of my voice tone and body language, and try
to work as much on the unconscious mind as the conscious. I am also seeking to
convey the best possible clarity on what the purpose of the programme is, how it
works, what to expect, and how to deal with any questions and concerns. I then focus
on establishing an open and trusting base for ongoing two-way feedback.
There is much that can be modelled and customized from the field of customer
service practices. Customer service professionals are particularly aware of important
touchpoints in shaping the relationship between the customer service provider (e.g., in
this case the online tutor) and the customer (e.g., in this case the online learners). For
example, first impressions are significant as we have outlined previously, but these
can quickly fade, if not maintained and developed to learner expectations. In the
hotel industry, other touchpoints include the contact with customers in their coming
and going from the hotel, making requests—no matter how small—and creating
nice surprises (delighters) such as leaving a favourite magazine on the table in the
customer’s room (previous researched by hotel staff). Of course, we expect a high
level of customer service when staying at the so-called top hotels, and usually (not
always) receive it. However, this can also be the experience anywhere, as it’s not that
difficult to do, when you know how and, most importantly, want to do it really well. In
my many travels, I have had some of the best customer service experiences in the most
modest of places in terms of pricing or ascribed status. The same touchpoints apply
in the online environment. For example, students will need to be given information
and assignments at different points, there will be times when they need clarification
and other support, and there will simply be times when they get a bit fed up of doing
the work. Hence, try to make these touchpoints less painful than they could otherwise
be. Even better, and this is where creativity can come into play, introduce novelty
and pleasure into the mix, a humorous caption of the present situation that provides
a reframe in which the ‘funny side’ can be seen or introduce a fun activity. Anyway,
here’s a few guiding frames that often (not always) work well:
238 6 Creative Teaching Competence and EdTech: Total Pedagogy
• Ensure clarity, access and ease of use of all designated feedback channels. People
don’t like being left in ‘limbo’, so to speak. It’s much worse than being given a
‘no’ in many cases
• Avoid overburdening learners with too much information at any one time, it’ll
cause cognitive overload and strain. Use the announcement board, and any other
programme organizer to provide a clear structure and bite-sized guidance on what
needs to be done, how and when
• Maintain regular contact, but don’t overdo this. Too much communication can
become boring and eat up participants’ valued time. Most importantly, identify
and deal with concerns quickly
• Work towards an informal communication style that fits the comfort zone of your
learners. You can find this out through experimenting with your use of language
and tone, and some safe humour. The more you can work in a friendly informal
manner, the better is the likelihood of rapport. Once you have this, coupled with
sound pedagogy, everything (ok, most things with most people) will work better,
especially retention rates and student attainment.
To be more creative, look for opportunities to incorporate an appropriate Von
Restorff effect, in the context of the learner group. I like to use humour and the
occasional poignant story to achieve this, as it supports rapport building and creating
a positive psychological climate—“Russian Doll” stuff. Invariably, be careful not
to overuse these strategies, and ensure contextualization to the learner profile. Also,
you may remember the Recency effect. At the end of any specific period of learning
(e.g., transitions and endpoints), check key understanding and provide supportive
feedback, key summaries of what’s been achieved and what’s coming next.
As the online tutor, do not contribute unnecessary confusion and complexity to
your learners’ already busy lives. Do the opposite—provide structure, be predictable
in supporting their learning, and enhance their lives with a bit of fun and humour.
And, finally, to the point of repetition, work not only with the conscious aspects of
the mind, but pay good service to the unconscious aspects of human psychological
functioning. You will find that this works well.
6.5 Summary
What became apparent, through a careful reading of the extensive literature, was the reali-
sation that such positive effects are achieved through applications of the same principles of
learning that apply in all other areas of human learning. (p. 199)
References
Abeysekera L, Dawson P (2015) Motivation and cognitive load in the flipped classroom: definition,
rationale and a call for research. In: Higher education research and development, vol 34(1)
Ash K (2012) Educators view ‘flipped’ model with a more critical eye, Education Week, S6–S7
Beattie A (2011) What is the difference between social media and web 2.0? Technopedia.com, Nov
29. https://www.techopedia.com/2/27884/internet/social-media/what-is-the-difference-between-
social-media-and-web-20
Bergmann J, Sams A (2012) Flipped your classroom: reach every student in every class every day.
International society for technology in education
Berrett D (2012) How ’flipping’ the classroom can improve the traditional lecture. The chronicle
of higher education
Bersin J (2004) The blended learning book. Wiley, San Francisco
Brown T (2009) Change by design. HarperCollins, New York
Clark R, Lyons C (2005) Graphics for learning: proven guidelines for planning, designing, and
evaluating visuals in training materials. Pfeiffer, San Francisco
Colvin Clark R, Mayer RE (2011) E-learning and the science of instruction; proven guidelines for
consumers and designers of multimedia learning (3rd edn). Wiley, San Francisco. Retrieved from
https://formulasi.googlecode.com/files/e-Learning.pdf
Educause Horizon Report (2019) Higher education edition. Louisville, CO
Gallagher-Mackay K, Steinhauer N (2017). Pushing the limits, how schools can prepare our children
today for the challenges of tomorrow. Penguin Doubleday, Canada
Garrison DR, Kanuka H (2004) Blended learning: uncovering its transformative potential in higher
education. Internet High Educ 7(20):95–105
240 6 Creative Teaching Competence and EdTech: Total Pedagogy
Garrison DR, Vaughan ND (2008) Blended learning in higher education: framework, principles,
and guideline. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Gee JP (2017) Teaching, learning, literacy in our high-risk high-tech world: a framework for
becoming human. Teachers College Press, New York
Greenberg AD, Zanetis J (2012) The impact of broadcast and streaming video in education.
Wainhouse Research, LLC, Cisco Systems Inc.
Hamilton S, Zimmerman S (2002) Breaking through zero-sum academics: two students’ per-
spectives on computer-mediated learning environment. In: Rudestam KE, Schoenholtz J (eds)
Handbook of online learning: innovations in higher education and corporate training. Sage,
London
Harari YN (2016) Homo deus: a brief history of tomorrow. Penguin, UK
Harari YN (2018) 21 lessons for the 21st century. Jonathan Cape, London
Harris S (2010) The moral landscape: how science can determine human values. Free Press, New
York
Hattie J (2009) Visible learning. Routledge, New York
Hattie J, Yates GCR (2014) Visible learning and the science of how we learn. Routledge, New York
Herreid CF, Schiller NA (2013) Case studies and the flipped classroom. J. of College Science
Teaching, National Science Teachers Association, pp 62–66
Hodges P, Saba L (2002) Teaching statistics online. In: Rudestam KE, Schoenholtz (eds) Handbook
of online learning: innovations in higher education and corporate training chap. 18. Sage, London,
pp 389–404
Horton W (2006) e-learning by design. Pfeiffer, San Francisco
Ilkka T (2018) JRC science for policy report: the impact of artificial intelligence on learning,
teaching, and education. Publications Office of the European Union, Luxembourg
Jensen JL, Kummer TA, Godoy PD (2015) Improvements from a flipped classroom may simply be
the fruits of active learning. CBE Life Sci Educ 14:1–12
Keen A (2007) The cult of the amateur: how today’s internet is killing our culture and assaulting
our economy. Nicholas Brealey Publishing, London
Lincoln YS (1990) The making of a constructivist: a remembrance of transformations past. In: Guba
EG (ed) The paradigm dialog. Sage, London
Littlejohn A, Pegler C (2007) Preparing for blended e-learning. Routledge, London
Luckin R (2018) Machine learning and human intelligence: the future of education for the 21st
century. UCL Institute of Education Press, London
Margulieux L, Majerich D, McCraken M (2013) Guide to flipping your classroom. Center for 21st
century universities, Georgia Tech
Mason GS, Shuman TR, Cook KE (2013) Comparing the effectiveness of an inverted classroom to
a traditional classroom in an upper-division engineering course. IEEE Trans Edu 56(4):430–434
McLaughlin JE et al (2013) Pharmacy student engagement, performance, and perception in a flipped
satellite classroom. Am J Phar Edu 77(9):1–8
Means B et al (2010) Evaluation of evidence-based practices. In: Online learning a meta-analysis
and review of online learning. U.S. Department of Education Office of Planning, Evaluation, and
Policy Development Policy and Program Studies Service, Washington, DC
Melchior T et al (1997) New technologies. In: Costa A, Liebmann RM (eds) Supporting the spirit
of learning: when process is content. Corwin Press, Thousand Oaks, California
Miller MD (2016) Minds online: teaching effectively with technology. Harvard University Press,
Cambridge, MA
Mlodinow L (2012) Subliminal: how your unconscious mind rules your behaviour. Vintage Books,
New York
Moffett J (2014) Twelve tips for “flipping” the classroom. Med Teach 37(4):331–336
Moroder K (2013) You tube video. Learning that technology is not a silver bullet: Ed tech chal-
lenge intro (1/4). Available at https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=pXkkoAVrmvo&index=4&
list=UUQHWPc4O7z68ZqZwMjgNznA. Last accessed 26 Nov 2019
References 241
Olbrish Pagano K (2013) Immersive learning: designing for authentic practice. ASTD Press,
Alexandria, VA
Oliver R (2007) Describing ICT-based learning designs that promote quality learning outcomes.
In: Beetham H, Sharpe R (eds) Rethinking pedagogy for a digital age: designing and delivering
e-Learning. Routledge, London
Pacansky-Brock M (2013) Best practices for teaching with emerging technologies. Routledge, New
York
Picciano AG et al (eds) (2014) Blended learning research perspective, vol 2. Routledge, New York
Pienta NJ (2016) A “flipped classroom” reality check. J Chem Educ 93:1–2
Prensky M (2001) Digital natives, digital immigrants. On the Horizon 9(5):1–6
Robinson DH, Schraw G (2008) A need for quality research in e-Learning. In: Robinson DH,
Schraw G (eds) Recent innovations in educational technology that facilitates student learning.
Information Age Publishing, Charlotte, NC
Sale D, Cheah SM (2011) Developing critical thinking skills through dynamic simulation using
an explicit model of thinking. Paper presented at the 7th international CDIO conference at DTU
Lyngby, Denmark, June 20–23
Sale D, Cheah SM, Wan M (2017) Symposia on creative flipped classroom learning: an evidence-
based approach. In: Redesigning pedagogy international conference, National institute of
education, Singapore, May 31–June 2 2017
Sale D (2015) Creative teaching: an evidence-based approach. Springer, New York
Shank P, Sitze A (2004) Making sense of online learning. Pfeiffer, San Francisco
Shea-Shultz H, Fogarty J (2002) Online Learning Today. Berret-Koehler Publishers Inc, San
Francisco
Strayer JF (2012) How learning in an inverted classroom influences cooperation innovation and
task orientation. Learn Environ Res 15:171–193
Stein J, Graham CR (2014) Essentials for blended learning a standards-based guide. Routledge,
New York
Sylwester R (1995) A celebration of neurons: an educator’s guide to the human brain. Association
for Supervision and Curriculum Development, Alexandria, VA
Treadwell M (2017) The future of learning. The Global Curriculum Project, Mount Maunganui,
NZ
Van Vliet EA, Winnips JC, Brouwer N (2015) Flipped-class pedagogy enhances student meta-
cognition and collaborative-learning strategies higher education but effect does not persist. Life
Sci Educ 14:1–10
Willingham DT (2009) Why don’t students like school: a cognitive scientist answers questions
about how the mind works and what it means for the classroom. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Wilson S (2014) The flipped class: a method to address the challenges of an undergraduate statistics
course. Teach Psychol 40(3):193–199
Winget L (2017) What’s wrong with damn near everything!: how the collapse of core values is
destroying us and how to fix it. Wiley, New York
Zemsky P, Massey WF (2004) Thwarted innovation: what happened to e-learning and why.
University of Pennsylvania, Learning Alliance Report
Chapter 7
Framing a Curriculum
for the Twenty-First Century
Competencies
Abstract This chapter explores and addresses the key considerations that need to
be thoughtfully negotiated in curriculum development. It provides a critical analysis
and evaluation of what constitutes twenty-first century competencies, how these are
best derived from a cognitive science perspective, and the implications for framing
educational aims and outcomes. A core valuation is that while we must frame and
enact curriculum to meet the demands of industry and provide employability, there
is also a need to accommodate competencies for wider issues of well-being and
citizenship. There are concerns that technology—especially Artificial Intelligence—
may make employability increasingly difficult for more people, and this will provide
a systemic new challenge to twenty-first century curriculum planning and teaching.
7.1 Introduction
In Chap. 1, the short tour of Educational Jurassic Park illustrated the contestation
about what constitutes the nature and practices of good teaching. Equally impor-
tant, in terms of framing educational quality (however defined), there are similar
parallel issues in terms of what is the good curriculum. A stark description of cur-
riculum divergence over the decades (centuries) is aptly captured by Kelly (1989)
who describes it as:
….the battlefield of many competing influences and ideologies. (p. 149)
Framing the essential curriculum aims and components for work and living in
the twenty-first century may indeed be a battle; it will certainly constitute a major
educational challenge. At present much of the curriculum focus is on defining, and of
course teaching and assessing the so-called twenty-first century competencies. These
competencies are deemed essential for success, progress and well-being—though it
often feels more like survival—in the face of rapid technological and social change.
In this chapter, I unpack and explore the key curriculum issues in terms of an EBT
approach, which has similarities with what Pinker (2019) refers to as reason and
science in decision-making, concerning what we need/want our workers and citizens
to learn, to be, and on what basis. This provides the framework for deciding how
best to achieve such educational aims and goals, based on the pedagogic framework
established in the previous chapters. It will involve analysing and evaluating prevalent
conceptions of twenty-first century competencies, how they align to an evidence-
based approach to learning and the implications for professional practice in teaching.
Notions of curriculum innovation and change imply that certain forms of knowledge,
skill sets and attributes (e.g., attitudes, dispositions) are more important than others in
terms of facilitating a better society and/or in response to perceptions of a challenge to
existing societal arrangements. In ‘nuts and bolts’ terms, it has become apparent, or it
is perceived by significant interest groups (stakeholders) that significant changes need
to be made in terms of what is taught in school curricula to meet changing societal
needs; often those of industry, but also for issues of citizenship and well-being.
Curriculum change, whether now or in yesteryear, is always based on consid-
erations of what is a Good Society and how do we prepare (socialize) people into
achieving it and making it work—as best as is possible, given the existential condi-
tions that prevail. The good society is typically framed, in Edu-speak, as Educational
Aims; the means to bring this about is the Curriculum; and Pedagogy is what we do
in the various delivery forms and arrangements (e.g., instructional approaches) to
bring about that socialization.
No matter how much we try to wriggle out of this problem, with vague notions
of ‘accelerated learning’, ‘clicking on the creativity switch’, and other quick-fix
solutions to achieve success, we cannot get away from certain existential facts. Firstly,
we are living in an age when the discrepancy between our brains’ evolutionary
7.2 Human Learning and Curriculum Framing 245
capacity for processing information and the amounts of information hitting it, (albeit
that most of it is probably of no real use) is becoming increasingly problematic and
untenable. Cognitive overload, probably of little concern for our primitive ancestors,
is now a nemesis and may underpin much of modern stress in human learning.
Learning in primitive societies would have been little in terms of content knowledge,
and the important bits probably taught (and learned) relatively quickly. Of course,
skills (e.g., hunting, building shelter) may have taken a long time, depending on how
long it took to become competent or expert at doing this. This fits squarely with our
lazy brain (e.g., Kahneman 2012) and our desire to avoid thinking (e.g., Willingham
2009). Also, as documented in Chap. 3, research suggests that human brains were
pretty much the same (morphologically) some 50,000 years ago (Neubauer et al.
2018). Hence, folk then probably had similar cognitive abilities and motivational
dispositions to us now—but they just played out in different contexts, with different
resources, and different contingencies.
The problem is stark and clear, we are stuck (in the present cognitive arrangements)
with a stone-age brain, perfectly equipped for living in savannah in Africa some
hundreds of thousands of years ago, but unequipped to deal with the exponential
knowledge explosion of modern times.
Hence, we have an existential problem, though there are some credible futurists,
such as Kurzweil (2005) who argues that Radical Evolution (e.g., the coming together
of genetics, robotics, information-communication technologies and nanotechnology
into a singularity) will, apart from significantly increasing the human lifespan, also
increase human capability through bionic enhancement, transforming physical health
and cognitive capability through the connectivity of IT and brain neural networks.
Just as map applications have largely made redundant the painful task of years of
learning (memorizing) the names and locations of all London’s roads (which was
the case to acquire a taxi license for driving the famous black taxi-cab in London),
downloading a full language system straight into an organized set of neural networks
in the brain would take the drudge out of learning a foreign language. The futurist
writer, Harari (2016) makes an interesting assertion:
In the twenty-first century, humans are likely to make a serious bid for immortality. Struggling
against old age and death will merely carry on the time-honoured fight against famine and
disease. (p. 24)
Well, what are the curriculum implications of such radical evolution events? Off
the bat I don’t think this will happen for the next 50 years or so and, in the meantime,
we are living with a stone-age brain and dealing with a VUCA world. Hence, I will
continue writing this book.
Animals do learn, and this has certain correlates with human learning. For exam-
ple, there are similarities in terms of learning through the senses, especially visual,
246 7 Framing a Curriculum for the Twenty-First Century Competencies
auditory and kinaesthetic. I had a budgerigar (who was called Joey) and he could con-
sistently orate-well, “Who’s a pretty boy then” (note: with an East London accent).
There’s nothing mystical about how Joey acquired such exquisite linguistic capabil-
ity. I must have repeated this to him hundreds of times, and as he picked up bits of the
sentence syntax, I would give him a tasty bit of millet (that’s a food treat for budgies,
apparently). He learned from hearing (we would not call this empathic listening),
and inevitably from practice (though we would also not call this deliberate practice).
However, animals, lacking such metacognitive capability do not build culture as
humans do; their knowledge production over time is limited and their basic survival
mechanisms, deeply rooted in instinct, remain largely untouched. Invariably, as the
environmental conditions around them change, they will evolve new responses and
can communicate this information to each other (maybe more effectively than us
humans). For example, foxes are now quite prevalent in some English cities, and
they are not there for aesthetic reasons; they are seeking food and/or shelter—no
more, no less. Without degradation to their previous environment, they would not be
foraging around the cities.
While primitive men (and women) had the capability for extensive learning as their
distant successors (us), there was little need for it. Curriculum, though it is unlikely
that the word existed in their vocabulary, would constitute locally constructed knowl-
edge and practices mainly relating to skills in finding food, building shelter, dealing
with threats, and human conduct among themselves. They would also have notions
about their existence and its meaning, as noted in the wide variety of beliefs in the
supernatural among primitives. Such knowledge would largely be passed on orally,
though many created cave drawings and other artefacts, from generation to gener-
ation, and that’s how we know about them. Socialization into this cultural milieu
would not have required decades of formal education and lifelong learning. The big
difference for us today, for sure, is that its damn more complex now.
The unique human capability of metacognition may well have been relatively
latent for primitive people, as they did not have to do much thinking, and if there is
little by way of change, even less need for ‘thinking about thinking’. However, in the
present situation of exponential knowledge growth, as well as rapid social and tech-
nological change, the need for good thinking has become somewhat of a ‘Pandora’s
Box’—in that once opened, the nemeses are let out and tough challenges ensue.
Pandora’s box is considered one of the most descriptive myths of human behaviour
in Greek mythology in that Ancient Greeks used it not only to instruct themselves
about the weaknesses of humans but also to explain certain human misfortunes. As
the legend goes, Pandora was given a box by Gods who told her that it contained
special gifts from them, but she was not allowed to open the box ever. Even if you
are not familiar with the myth, you have probably guessed the rest—curiosity got the
better of Pandora and she opened the box allowing all the illnesses and hardships that
gods had hidden in the box to come out. While Pandora quickly closed the box, it
was too late—only Hope was left inside. Perhaps this is the metaphor of these times.
As the old saying goes “Hope springs eternal”.
7.2 Human Learning and Curriculum Framing 247
However, while today’s world is more complex, rapidly changing and potentially
more dangerous at the macro-level, humans of yesteryear still faced similar existential
challenges to their survival, making meaning of life, and dealing with death. As
mentioned prior, humans then still needed to solve difficult problems such as finding
food, staving off intruders, etc. It’s just that in modern culture, technology has scaled
up certain problems exponentially—as threatening tribes can be world-wide and the
weapons of today offer the potential for extinction, not just local genocide.
Different times, different cultures, different views on the human condition, dif-
ferent needs—one could go on with this—but it illustrates the culturally constructed
nature of the curriculum, reflecting the power structures and aims of education at any
particular time in human history. The same framing can also be made for primitive
mankind, though it was simpler, less formalized (there were no books about it), and
less contested in the localized context. All contexts were largely localized; hence,
homogeneity would tend to persist until threatened by external agencies.
Fundamental to the challenges of the curriculum today, issues revolve around the
framing of our educational aims, which in turn reflect our perception (grounded in
beliefs) of the good society, as identified earlier. Wringe (1988) captured this nicely:
Human beings have the potential for developing in many directions and the problem of
educational aims is deciding which kinds of development should be fostered and which
discouraged. (p. 43)
As identified earlier, the basic questions are not new; it has always been prevalent (and
current) at any time since the formulation of knowledge into organized structures (i.e.,
curriculum) for deliberate transmission to a learner group (e.g., the whole society
or segments within it). Hirst (1974) argued that socialization into these organized
structures of knowledge—what he referred to as, “Forms of Knowledge”—are very
important as there is a close relationship between their acquisition and the growth
and development of the mind. Such an approach makes the following psychological
assumptions about the nature of the mind:
• Knowledge is a quality of mind. Failure to receive certain forms of knowledge is
a failure to achieve rational ways of thinking in those areas.
• The mind does not develop rationally—it needs organised forms of knowledge.
Hirst argued that there are seven distinct forms of knowledge, each with its unique
concepts, distinctive logical structure, testability against experience and unique meth-
ods of testing. These seven forms of knowledge are mathematics, the physical sci-
ences, the human sciences, history, religion, literature and the fine arts, and philos-
ophy and moral knowledge. The framing of traditional school subjects is consonant
with Hirst’s model of the intelligent mind. Latin for example, which was central to
elite education, was perceived to be a valuable tool for the development of the mind,
and Lowe (2017) suggests that there are reasons and evidence to support such a view.
She notes that Latin helps with SAT scores and makes learning a modern vocabu-
lary easier. True, but Lowe also argues that there are more important objectives that
Latin achieves better than any other subject: The first is mental development, and the
second is an understanding of English grammar.
Latin, like math, provides students with the experience of studying one subject to
a mastery level which, according to her, is missing in modern education where we
try to teach everything, and we cover too many subjects superficially. There are few
opportunities to use higher-order thinking skills when you are merely a novice. It is
only when the student has studied a subject enough to have some depth that his/her
mind can be stretched and challenged with higher-order thinking skills. This relates
to Hirst’s (1974) notion that in moving upwards through the structure of a form of
knowledge increasingly develops rationality in that form of knowledge. Latin and
math give students the invaluable experience of studying one systematic subject to a
mastery level over a long period. From this perspective, such learning is essential for
mental and character development and is the most valuable academic experience a
child can have in school. Latin and math, when taught to a mastery level, according to
Lowe (2017), requires perseverance, hard work, stamina, will, and grit. Students need
to plan, adopt a never-give-up attitude, and display flexibility in learning. Sounds very
much like good preparation for self-directed lifelong learning! No, I am not arguing
that Latin becomes a universal part of the curriculum for the twenty-first century—but
the ‘thinking behind the thinking’—so to speak—has merit.
250 7 Framing a Curriculum for the Twenty-First Century Competencies
The extent to which these forms of knowledge are distinct has been open to
criticism. Young et al. (1971), for example, argued that knowledge is less delineated
at the experience level and is best learned in a more integrated and holistic context.
From this perspective, a well-integrated curriculum is more consistent with how we
learn, the nature of knowledge in the real world, as well as making learning more
interesting for students (e.g., Fogarty 2009).
As cultures develop, and extended forms of knowledge become available, there are
inevitable choices concerning what forms of knowledge are most useful. Questions
concerning how much of each form, and to what level of competence are the essentials
of curriculum content decision making. Neary (2014) summarized the perennial
issue:
The structure and methods of education must help to sustain the traditional values of society,
but they must also respond adequately to current cultural, social, industrial and technological
issues, and to future change.
Lawton’s (1975) notion of cultural analysis identifies the need to consider the
wider context of a society in curriculum decision-making, though recognizing that
this was still value-laden as decisions need to be made based on current perceptions
of relevance, and this must reflect the dominant forces shaping curriculum at any
particular time. Many factors or forces shape the curriculum; these include dominant
political ideology, dominant educational perspectives, industry, educational institu-
tions, practitioners, and students. The present context is no different, but the problems
are perhaps more pressing than in yesteryears. For example, Collins (2017) argues
that:
We educators can’t go on adding things to the school curriculum as knowledge grows expo-
nentially. We can’t keep people in school longer and longer until everybody needs several
advanced degrees just to deal with the complexity they face in their lives. We can’t make
our textbooks much fatter than they already are and cover more and more topics in the same
amount of time…Our strategies for coping with the exponential growth of knowledge are
hitting a wall. (xv)
The school curriculum is filled with stuff that most people will never use and hence will
forget as soon as they leave school or move on to the next grade. (p. 1)
Our current model of universal schooling is an Industrial Age institution, and it is not at all
clear how well it can adapt to the Information Age, where thinking and creativity are prized.
(p. 3)
The message and the problems are clear. There is a significant need to reframe cur-
riculum, and this has led to the growth of frameworks attempting to define twenty-first
century competencies. However, the same fundamental curriculum questions remain
in terms of what the competencies should be, what range and depth of knowledge,
skills and attitudes are the key content constituents, how to structure them into viable
educational offerings, and how best to deliver them pedagogically to meet desired
educational aims and outcomes. Certainly, as detailed in Chap. 3, much is now focus-
ing on the need to develop self-sufficiency in our students so that they can become
Self-Directed Lifelong Learners. Collins (2017) sees this as a major curriculum aim
in that:
7.4 What’s Our Best Package of Competencies in a VUCA World? 251
Having a critical mindset is critical to navigating through today’s complex world. (p. 340)
Planning, monitoring, and reflection are the basic elements of the learning cycle that pervades
everything we do. (p. 35)
There is abundant evidence that self-regulatory skills are central to living a happy and
successful life. Recent studies have shown that it is possible to learn these new skills, but
they are seldom taught in school, except in extra-curricular activities. They need to become
central to the school curriculum. (p. 35)
Various groups, such as the OECD, the European Commission, the Partnership
for twenty-first century Skills, and the U.S. National Research Council have made
significant contributions to framing these competencies. While there are differences
in the approaches taken, there is broad agreement that twenty-first century compe-
tencies are associated with growth in the cognitive, interpersonal and intrapersonal
dimensions. For example, The National Research Council report titled Education for
Life and Work: Developing Transferable Knowledge and Skills in the twenty-first
century synthesizes psychological and social science research evidence on skills that
have demonstrated positive short or long-term consequences for individuals. The
report summarizes those skills in the following framework:
1. Cognitive Competencies
1.1 Cognitive Process and Strategies
Critical thinking; problem-solving; analysis; reasoning and argumentation;
interpretation; adaptive learning; executive function
252 7 Framing a Curriculum for the Twenty-First Century Competencies
1.2. Knowledge
Information literacy, including research using evidence and recognizing bias in
sources; information and communication technology literacy; oral and written
communication; active listening
1.3. Creativity
Creativity and innovation.
2. Intrapersonal Competencies
2.1. Intellectual Openness
Flexibility; adaptability; artistic and cultural appreciation; personal and social
responsibility; cultural awareness and competence; appreciation for diversity;
continuous learning; intellectual interest and curiosity
2.2. Work Ethic/Conscientiousness
Initiative; self-direction; responsibility; perseverance; grit; productivity; type 1
Self-regulation (metacognitive skills, including forethought, performance, and
self-regulation); professionalism/ethics; integrity; citizenship; career orientation
2.3. Positive Core Self-Evaluation
Type 2 self-regulation (self-monitoring, self-evaluation, self-reinforcement);
physical and psychological health.
3. Interpersonal Competencies
3.1. Teamwork and Collaboration
Communication.
To derive standards for these competencies that are sufficiently clear, valid, and
practical for teaching and assessment, it is necessary to clarify certain conceptual
confusions about what constitutes a competency, especially as the terms competency
and skills are often used in interchanged ways. Competencies involve much more than
skills as such. For example, in the game of tennis, and this applies to any sport, there
are discrete skills in terms of hitting the ball with the racket to gain speed, spin and
other ball behaviours that might be effective in winning points. We talk about serving
techniques, which typically involve some skill combinations. For example, there are
serving techniques that put spin and slice on the ball, but these are skill combinations.
The top players are top players because of their combined high level of skills in these
different techniques. However, one can be skilful, but not that effective, and that’s
because of other factors (e.g., aptitude, traits/disposition) impact performance in
real-world contexts—whether at work or in play, as explained in Chap. 5. Without
clear framing of what we want our students to learn in order to develop a deep
understanding and skill in using competencies (e.g., the specific performance criteria,
evidence of what constitutes competence and expectancy levels), teachers with be
7.4 What’s Our Best Package of Competencies in a VUCA World? 253
unsure on what to teach and assess, creating much by way of confusion for all
concerned, especially the students. As identified in Chap. 2, many teachers are unsure
of what critical thinking entails or how to teach it (e.g., Wagner 2010). While teachers
may be experts in their subject domains, many may lack the necessary level of
expertise in the emerging science of learning and what this means for pedagogic
practices (e.g., EBT)—as outlined in Chap. 1.
Defining competencies has its roots in the functional analysis of work roles. Many
different methods of developing competency frameworks have evolved, but the most
effective ones share certain characteristics. All of them follow McClelland’s (1973)
dictate to determine what leads to superior performance and to identify top performers
and find out what they do. This can be broken down into two important principles:
1. focus on highly successful people without making assumptions about their role
2. pay attention to what they do.
From this, the range of behaviours can be identified from observation in various
contexts, then explored, clarified and made meaning of through interviewing, which
often involves skilled questioning to evoke latent or tacit knowledge (e.g., Polanyi
1966). They can then be validated through checking with high performers and other
stakeholders (e.g., supervisors; other workers who are part of the role-set for that
work area). In other words, if the high performers can relate well to the competence,
supervisors agree that this constitutes good competence, and those who engage with
persons performing this work competence also feel that this is the case, that’s probably
competence in this work function(s). To use an analogy: ‘If it looks like a bird, sounds
like a bird, behaves like a bird’—it’s probably a bird.
Hence, to reiterate, there are many components to competence, and some (i.e.,
knowledge and skills) are more conducive to training than others. Aptitude, Traits and
Dispositions certainly have a hereditary/neurological base and related to personality
configuration. Discussions on how many hereditary aspects impact competence, and
how much results from environmentally structured experiences, is largely irrelevant
in the world of mainstream teaching and training. Currently, we are not going to
impact genetics much in the classroom. However, we can through the systematic use
of metacognition and self-regulatory strategies help people to monitor, evaluate and
modify their behaviour more in line with the requirements of the competency areas (at
least in work contexts). Therefore, these are also subject to curriculum and pedagogic
interventions as explained previously. There is also evidence that for persons who
use such strategies, especially volition, in changing their actual behaviours, this can
change brain structuring at the neural level (i.e., neuroplasticity), which starts to
‘rewire’ the person, in this area—so to speak. However, in my experience, while
there is validity in this framing and it should be pursued as a regulatory ideal (i.e.,
254 7 Framing a Curriculum for the Twenty-First Century Competencies
give everybody a chance and be inclusive), there are some individuals who display
almost a complete non-alignment to certain competencies. In working with thousands
of teachers, many in training, despite much mentoring and coaching, for whatever
of the above reasons, some just don’t seem to have enough of the ‘competency
pack’ to be effective teachers. Keeping such persons in the profession is not useful
for the students or the folk themselves to continue. There is an adage that seems
tautological (but it isn’t), that it is easier and better, in many cases, to ‘change people
than to change people’.
It is also necessary to remember that competence, knowledge and understand-
ing are all constructs and not something we can observe directly. Of course, we
can observe behaviour, which is time-consuming and thereby make inferences and
interpretations about a person’s competence. We also know that:
• Knowledge and understanding contribute to competence
• Knowledge and understanding are best learned ‘in use’
• Competency, knowledge and understanding are highly contextualized.
A significant part of the present confusion about framing twenty-first-century
competencies has stemmed from a lack of clarity in framing educational outcomes,
particularly, though not exclusively, to higher education. Diamond (1998), from a
wide range of sources, argues that:
A serious problem that institutions of higher education face is the perception by business
leaders, governmental leaders, and the public at large that they have enthusiastically avoided
stating clearly what competencies graduates should have and that as a result, they have
provided little evidence that they are successful at what they are expected to do. (p. 4)
The creation of an educational framework that would encompass this wide range
of competencies, enabling students on graduation to be “ready to engineer” (Crawley
et al. 2007, p. 6) is the basis and rationale of CDIO. The aim is to ensure that graduates
will leave universities and colleges with both relevant practical competences as well
as a thorough understanding of the role of an engineer in the present and future work
context, and what this entails.
In The Challenge of Reframing Engineering Education (2014) I documented the
complete curriculum development process from an OBE perspective. This included:
• Customizing the general syllabus (very much in the genre of most twenty-first
century skills), but lacking specificity (which is a major challenge at present) to
more specific and measurable skill areas.
• Calibrating the assessment approaches and tools for assessing more complex real
work performances (outlined and illustrated in this chapter).
• Developing an instructional strategy using EBT generically, though with a spe-
cific focus on identifying the most effective pedagogic methods (e.g., signature
pedagogies, Shulman 2005) conducive to key skill development in the context of
engineering education (e.g., project-based learning, case-based learning, simulated
practice).
256 7 Framing a Curriculum for the Twenty-First Century Competencies
The Towards Defining 21st century Competencies, Foundation Document for Dis-
cussion, Ontario (2016), emphasizes that key-criteria for evaluating the worth of
twenty-first century competencies must have measurable benefits for multiple areas
of life and therefore critical for all students. The most prominent twenty-first century
competencies found in international frameworks that have shown measurable benefits
are associated with critical thinking, communication, collaboration, and creativity
and innovation (p. 12). Certainly, the area of thinking, most notably identified—
critical and creative thinking—has been extensively framed as essential in a society
that is rapidly changing from a workforce embedded in an industrial model to a
technology-driven, interconnected globalized knowledge-based economy:
Researchers acknowledge that the need to engage in problem-solving and critical and creative
thinking has “always been at the core of learning and innovation” (Trilling and Fadel 2009,
p. 50). What’s new in the 21st century is the call for education systems to emphasize and
develop these competencies in explicit and intentional ways through deliberate changes in
curriculum design and pedagogical practice. The goal of these changes is to prepare students
to solve messy, complex problems – including problems we don’t yet know about – associated
with living in a competitive, globally-connected, and technology-intensive world. (p. 3)
skills, knowledge, and attitudes that are relevant to new areas of learning. Simi-
larly, The Towards Defining 21st Century Competencies, Foundation Document for
Discussion, Ontario (2016) highlight:
Researchers and thought leaders see that metacognition and a growth mindset (including
self-regulation skills and ethical and emotional awareness), while always important, are
much more so in a connected, global context that requires an ability to communicate, work,
and learn with diverse groups of individuals and teams worldwide. (p. 16)
Fortunately, such natural limitations can be mitigated in large part by firstly, being
aware of them, and secondly, through developing MC—not just in schools—being
able to self-regulate in more positive and effective ways. As Pinker points out (which
is positive):
However, long it takes, we must not let the existence of cognitive and emotional biases
or the spasms of irrationality in the political arena discourage us from the Enlightenment
ideal of relentlessly pursuing reason and truth. If we can identify ways in which humans
are irrational, we must know what rationality is. Since there’s nothing special about us, our
fellows must have at least some capacity for rationality as well. And it’s in the very nature
of rationality that reasoners can always step back, consider their shortcomings, and reason
out ways to work around them. (p. 384)
I grew up in East London in the 1960s, when gaining employment was not a difficult
task; it was indeed the “Affluent Society”, so aptly described by Galbraith (1958). On
leaving university for the first time in 1974, companies contacted me to see if I would
like to be considered for employment with them. This is a far cry from the situation
facing many graduates today, where applicants are in thousands, and they must go
through a rigorous multi-stage recruitment process. It seemed a different world then, a
7.7 Learning for What Purpose for the Many 259
world of plenty, and England seemed a great place to live. England also won the soccer
world cup in 1966, a feat yet to be replicated. Of course, things were not like that for
everywhere in the world; I was simply in an affluent bubble, but it was a nice bubble.
I was not from a wealthy family, my parents were solid working-class folk, and we
lived in a council flat (i.e., a local government-owned apartment). However, as a kid
(twelve-something), on a Saturday and Sunday mornings, I earned ‘pocket-money’
doing car cleaning, edge cutting, and helping a local trader deliver paraffin. All was
good, and my needs (e.g., money for chocolate bars and the amusement arcade)
were adequately met—not that I had any concept of what a need was. However, it’s a
different frame now for many people. Of course, the wealthy are largely immune from
such considerations—as the famous pop group ABBA recited in the song “Money,
Money, Money”, “it’s a rich man’s world.” Of course, this applies equally to women.
The worrying feature now is as Ford (2015) warns:
…it is becoming increasingly clear that many people will do all the right things in terms of
pursuing advanced education, but fail to find a foothold in the economy of the future. (p. 27)
We are running up against a fundamental limit both in terms of the capabilities of the people
being herded into colleges, and the number of high-skill jobs that will be available for them
if they manage to graduate. The problem is that the skill ladder is not a ladder at all; it is a
pyramid, and there is only so much room at the top. (p. 52)
For those of us working in the higher educational sectors, the vulnerability should
be obvious, as Ford reminds us:
If the higher education industry ultimately succumbs to the digital onslaught, the transfor-
mation will very likely be a duel-edged sword. A college credential may well become less
expensive and more accessible to many students, but at the same time, technology could dev-
astate an industry that is itself a major nexus of employment for highly educated workers.
(p. 143)
Not everyone shares Fords’ more pessimist framing. For example, in contrast,
Brynjolfson and McAfee (2014) take a less pessimistic frame. They argue that:
Computers are not useless, but there are still machines for generating answers, not posing
interesting new questions. That ability still seems to be uniquely human, and still highly
valuable. We predict that people who are good at idea creation will continue to have com-
petitive advantage over digital labour for some time to come and will find themselves in
demand. (p. 192)
The impact of technology on learning and teaching was considered in some detail
in Chap. 6. It is certainly changing the educational landscape, and this will only
accelerate; how significant it will be in terms of employment and the subsequent
societal consequences is probably still conjecture.
7.8 Summary
The macro-sociological theories of the past (e.g., Comte, Durkheim, Spencer, Mark)
seem to have little generalizability in an era of post-modernity—this VUCA world.
As Costa et al. (2016) wrote:
Hypercomplexity – our world is changing at an unprecedented rate, becoming more complex
and globally interactive. Clear cut, unambiguous understandings are no longer an option.
Thus, the capacity for self-directed and continuous learning has become the most impor-
tant capabilities needed for survival in the future. Fortunately, the brain permits our ability
to change, to grow, and to continuously develop our intellectual capabilities throughout a
lifetime. (p. 3)
In a world of many potential threats and unpredictability, but equally much oppor-
tunity for greater understanding and progress e.g., Pinker (2019), we may just have
to become more comfortable with being uncomfortable about the future. It may seem
like a different mindset, but humanity has always had to deal with threats and uncer-
tainty; I don’t think the great plague was much fun for those who contracted it or
lived in such localities. What we do know is that there are modern challenges to us
as humans which, while different in form and require different solutions, do share
a similar existential genesis. We seek to maintain a notion of the good society and
well-being and stage off threats from the environment, albeit much of the real threats
are within ourselves—though most do not see this—it’s an evolutionary nemesis.
However, on the positive side, we have made much progress as a species and with
better thinking (i.e., MC) we may even call it ‘global intelligence’, and with better
values, we may call it a ‘universal morality’—and we have the capacity for both—
we can use the curriculum for the twenty-first century as a vehicle to develop such
7.8 Summary 261
capacities. In a sense, we must pitch aspects of our evolutionary human nature against
other aspects of our human nature. Harris (2010), drawing from the reflections of
Adam Smith captures this paradox well:
The truth about us is plain to see: most of us are powerfully absorbed by selfish desires
almost every moment of our lives; our attention to our pains could scarcely be more acute;
only the most piercing cries of anonymous suffering capture our interest; then fleetingly.
And yet, when we consciously reflect on what we should do, an angel of beneficence and
impartiality seems to spread its wings within us: we genuinely want fair and just societies;
we want others to have their hopes realized; we want to leave the world better than we found
it. (pp. 58–59)
References
Brynjolfson E, McAfee A (2014) The second machine age: progress and prosperity in a time of
brilliant technologies. Norton, New York
Collins A (2017) What’s worth teaching: rethinking curriculum in the age of technology. Teachers
College Press, New York
Costa AL et al (2016) Cognitive coaching: developing self-directed leaders and learners. Rowman
& Littlefield, New York
Crawley E et al (2007) Rethinking engineering education. Springer, New York
Darwin C (1859) The origin of species. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/On_the_Origin_of_Species#
Publication_and_subsequent_edition. Last accessed 3 Nov 2019
Diamond RM (1998) Designing & assessing courses & curricula: a practical guide. Jossey-Bass,
San Francisco
Dweck CS (2006) Mindset: the new psychology of success. Ballantine, New York
Fogarty R (2009) How to integrate the curricula. Sage, London
Ford M (2015) The rise of the robots: technology and the threat of a jobless future. Basic Books,
New York
Friedman TL (2006) The world is flat. Penguin, UK
Fullan M, Langworthy M (2014) A rich seam: how new pedagogies find deep learning. Pearson,
London
Galbraith JK (1958) The affluent society. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, New York
Harari YN (2016) Homo Deus: a brief history of tomorrow. Vintage, Penguin, London
Harris S (2010) The moral landscape: how science can determine human values. Free Press, New
York
Hattie J (2009) Visible learning. Routledge, New York
Hattie J (2012) visible learning for teachers: maximizing impact on learning. Routledge, London
Hirst P (1974) Knowledge and the curriculum. Routledge & Kegan Paul, London
Kahneman D (2012) Thinking fast and slow. Penguin Books, London
Kelly AV (1989) The curriculum: theory and practice. Paul Chapman, London
Kurzweil R (2005) The singularity is near: when humans transcend biology. Viking, New York
Lawton D (1975) Class, culture, and the curriculum. Routledge and Kegan Paul Ltd, London
Lowe C (2017) How Latin develops the mind. https://www.memoriapress.com/articles/latin-
develops-mind/. Last accessed 28 Nov 2009
McClelland DC (1973) Testing for competence rather than for intelligence. Am Psychol 28:1–14
Neary M (2014) Curriculum studies in post-compulsory and adult education: a teachers’ and student
teachers’ study guide. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Neubauer S et al (2018) The evolution of modern human brain shape. Sci Adv 4(1):waao5961
262 7 Framing a Curriculum for the Twenty-First Century Competencies
Pellegrino JW, Hilton ML (2012) Education for life and work: developing transferable knowledge
and skills in the 21st Century. National Academies Press, Washington, DC
Pinker S (2019) enlightenment now: the case for reason, science, humanism and progress. Penguin,
London
Polanyi M (1966) The tacit dimension. Peter Smith, Gloucester, Mass
Reimers FM, Chung CK (eds) (2016) Teaching and learning for the twenty-first century. Harvard
Education Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts
Rychen DS, Salganik LH (eds) (2003) Key competencies for a successful life and a well-functioning
society. Hogrefe & Huber Publishers, Gottingen
Shulman LS (2005) Signature pedagogies in the professions. On professions & professionals
(Summer, 2005). Daedalus 134(3):52–59
The National Research Council report (2012) Education for life and work: developing transferable
knowledge and skills in the 21st century.
The Towards Defining 21st Century Competencies. The Foundation Document for Dis-
cussion, Ontario (2016). https://edugains.ca/resources21CL/About21stCentury/21CL_
21stCenturyCompetencies.pdf. Last accessed 24 Nov 2019
Tough P (2014) How children succeed: grit, curiosity, and the hidden power of character. Arrow,
London
Trilling B, Fadel C (2009) 21st century skills: learning for life in our times. John Wiley & Sons,
San Francisco, CA
Wagner T (2010) The global achievement gap. Basic Books, New York
Willingham DT (2009) Why don’t students like school: a cognitive scientist answers questions
about how the mind works and what it means for the classroom. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Wringe C (1988) Understanding educational aims. HarperCollins, London
Young MFD et al (1971) Knowledge and control. Collier-Macmillan, London
Chapter 8
Assessing Twenty-First Century
Competencies
Abstract This chapter focuses on the challenging area of assessing the twenty-first
century competencies identified and analysed in Chap. 7. Different competencies, as
well as different aspects of competence, will require different assessment methods.
Assessing more complex competencies such as metacognition, critical thinking and
creativity, will involve higher levels of inference in making valid and reliable assess-
ment decisions (as greater subjectivity is inherent in assessing these skill areas), and
it is both time and resource intensive. Furthermore, there is increasing pressure to
ensure quality in the assessment process, to maintain standards and justify expendi-
ture. Various strategies are explored and evaluated, but there is no easy solution to
the issue of cost. Even the best combinations of methods and technology affordances
may not be sufficient to meet such high expectations in a context of limited funding
availability.
8.1 Introduction
The previous chapter explored key curriculum questions in the framing of twenty-
first century competencies, especially what knowledge, skills and attitudes these
competencies should encompass and on what basis. This chapter closes the cur-
riculum development cycle, focusing on the big issues, questions and challenges in
assessing these more complex and interrelated competencies. This is a thorny issue
in education, for the simple reason that Ramsden (1992) suggests, assessment:
…defines the actual curriculum…Assessment sends messages about the standard and amount
of work required, and what aspects of the syllabus are most important. (pp. 187–188)
Firstly, in the present context of breakneck competition for viable students, and
increasing pressure on educational institutions to be accountable for their products
in cost-effective ways, assessment quality is high profile. There is a need to be able
to justify public expenditure in terms of value for money outputs. The quality of
teaching and the cost-effective use of resources are rightly important issues in this
context. However, it is the assessment credentials that largely define the value of
educational programmes. If assessment practices are lacking in quality, what value
can be placed on the qualifications accredited? The present situation is, as Bloxham
and Boyd (2007) highlight:
Assessment is now expected to assess subject knowledge and a wide range of intellectual,
professional and generic skills in a quality assurance climate that stresses reliability with
robust marking and moderation methods. (p. 4)
Secondly, and most significantly for understanding and enhancing students learn-
ing, there is increasing recognition of the important role that assessment plays in the
learning process (e.g., Ramsden 1992; Boud 1995) as detailed in Chap. 2. Assess-
ment is not simply a means to measure learning that has already occurred, it is a
major facilitator in the learning process itself. As Boud (1988) illustrated:
There have been several notable studies over the years which have demonstrated that assess-
ment methods and requirements probably have a greater influence on how and what students
learn than any other single factor. This influence may well be of greater significance than
the impact of teaching or learning materials. (p. 35)
Furthermore, much research supports the view that students choose their
approaches to learning rather than these being the result of innate characteristics
or dispositions. For example, Prosser and Trigwell (1998) argue:
…approaches to learning are not stable characteristics of students. Student’s approaches to
learning do change with changes in perception of their learning situation and their perception
of it can be changed by…teachers. (p. 83)
What this means in practice is that how we design and conduct our assessment
is fundamental to how students approach their learning, and the usefulness of that
8.2 The Need for Good Assessment 265
Assessment is more than the measurement of what has been learned (e.g., knowledge,
skills, attitudes), but an essential component of the learning process, as reflected in
the Core Principle of Learning: Assessment practices are integrated into the learning
design to promote desired learning outcomes and provide quality feedback.
Irrespective of the framing of twenty-first century Competencies, this core prin-
ciple, and the methods and activities that facilitate it, is central to the instructional
strategies employed and applies to all contexts and modes of delivery. It is cer-
tainly the case that the use of technology will play an increasingly prominent role
266 8 Assessing Twenty-First Century Competencies
in doing this effectively and efficiently. I am now seeing most academic faculty
using free EdTech tools such as Kahoot and Socrative to integrate formative assess-
ment into their everyday teaching. Those who have strong pedagogic competence in
applying EBT practices and principles are creating effective ‘Russian Doll’ strate-
gies (remember the analogy?) in their teaching. They combine and integrate high
effect methods and appropriate e-tools to create impactful instructional strategies
and learning experiences tailored to student profiles and learning outcomes. For
example, at the beginning of a session, they may use Kahoot as an advance organizer
to check key conceptual understanding from a previous session (i.e., activating prior
knowledge and stimulating thinking). From such activities, they can then address
any knowledge gaps and misconceptions through two-way feedback. New knowl-
edge can then be introduced using appropriate methods/e-tool blends. Furthermore,
such technology-based assessments have the potential to provide immediate and
precise descriptive feedback relating to student performance, enabling both diag-
nostic capability and personalization/differentiation of formative assessment. It also
automates much of the assessment process, freeing up instructional time for teach-
ing faculty. Similarly, in terms of assessing more complex technical and cognitive
skills, computer-based simulations can increasingly provide data that facilitates more
complex real-world performance assessment. The Towards Defining 21st Century
Competencies, Foundation Document for Discussion, Ontario (2016) identified:
Technology can support assessment for, as, and of learning, providing real-time assessment
information that deepens our understanding of student learning gains and challenges. Tech-
nology can also support the tasks of gathering and analysing assessment information about
student learning, thereby facilitating instructional decision making. (p. 35)
From this perspective, teaching and assessment are simply two sides of the same
coin. Through collaborative formative assessment, we can get to know our students
better, their learning preferences, and forge the essential communication links that
foster a supportive learning relationship and build trust. However, assessment is not
an exact science and the assessment of complex real-world performances, which
8.3 Assessment from an Evidence-Based Approach 267
involves the integration of a range of knowledge and skill bases, provides real chal-
lenges in terms of achieving high validity and reliability of assessment in a realistic
efficiency context. As Gray (2007) pointed out:
Finding or creating reliable, valid and appropriate assessment methods and tools matched to
all learning outcomes remains a challenge. (p. 165)
The reality, therefore, will be one of working smarter with existing resources. To
achieve this teaching professionals will need to have a clear frame on what consti-
tutes good assessment, the assessment formats, methods and strategies available, as
well as the compromises that may need to be made and their impact on assessment
quality. The following sections consider three essential questions which are central
to achieving the quality of assessment practice:
1. What is quality assessment?
2. What specific strategies can be used to enhance the effectiveness and efficiency
of assessment practices?
3. How to develop valid and practical assessment instruments for twenty-first
century Competencies.
Firstly, it is important to recognize that there are many different purposes for assess-
ing student learning, and these reflect different stakeholder interests. For example,
as Rowntree (1987) pointed out, the assessment may serve any of the following
purposes:
• Selection and grading
• Maintaining standards
• Diagnosing learning difficulties
• Providing feedback to support the learning process
• As a source of information for evaluating the effectiveness of the teaching/learning
strategy.
Furthermore, these purposes are not necessarily complementary and may conflict
in practice. For example, while grades and standards may be of prime interest to
employers, grading may do little to help students learn more effectively in the qual-
itative sense. In this context, it is important that the assessment approach effectively
and efficiently addresses these stakeholder interests in a balanced manner. Courses,
268 8 Assessing Twenty-First Century Competencies
however, structured and delivered, must enable the validity of the summative outcome
standards as well as supporting the learning process through formative assessment.
Fairness
Fairness relates to a range of considerations in the assessment. However, they are all
concerned with ensuring that learners, when being assessed, are provided with appro-
priate access to the assessment activities and are not unfairly discriminated against in
8.3 Assessment from an Evidence-Based Approach 269
Essentially an assessment plan identifies the why, what, when, where and how of the
assessment process for a module or unit of study. When well-constructed, it provides
a concise guide of the whole assessment process and components for both assessing
faculty and the students involved. In congruence with an aligned curriculum frame-
work (e.g., Biggs 1996), as summarized in Fig. 8.1, it should result in assessment
methods, instruments and procedures that both effectively develop the defined student
learning outcome (formative assessment) and measure them (summative assessment)
for any specified programme.
The following criteria, which incorporate the principles of good assessment iden-
tified earlier, frame the key considerations that need to be addressed in producing a
well constituted assessment plan:
• The assessment plan specifies the assessment methods to be used, their purpose,
the marks to be allocated, and the timing of assessments
• The selected assessment methods are valid for assessing the knowledge, skills and
attitudinal components specified, and at the appropriate levels
• The assessment methods are well constructed and sufficiently varied to enable
learners to display understanding/competence through different mediums
• The assessment methods are planned to make effective use of time and resources
in producing sufficent evidence
• The assessment methods provide fair and reliable assessment opportunities
• The key aspects of the assessment plan are explained to learners
• Opportunities are provided for learners to seek clarification on assessment
requirements
• Ways to ensure the authenticity of assessment evidence are identified
• The assessment plan is reviewed at agreed times and modified where necessary.
Of note, it is important to recognize that in practice there is often a trade-off
in terms of meeting the various principles of good assessment. For example, suffi-
ciency is typically problematic in that what constitutes a ‘sufficient’ range and depth
of assessment evidence (derived from a variety of methods) is open to judgement,
8.3 Assessment from an Evidence-Based Approach 271
as noted prior. However, addressing the sufficiency questions are crucial in areas
involving safety issues. It is here that significant problems may be encountered,
especially with an emphasis on more complex and interrelated competencies. Assess-
ment is time-consuming, resource-intensive, especially for high inference assess-
ment—where there is increasing subjectively involved in making valid and reliable
assessment decisions (e.g., creativity, leadership). As Knight (2006) highlighted,
reliable judgement can only be made where there have been several observations
from multiple observers in a range of contexts, which is not very practical in terms
of resources.
(b) Judging evidence and making assessment decisions
Being an assessor is in many ways akin to that of a ‘caring’ detective. The assessor
is responsible for maintaining the standards and ensuring that assessment decisions
are based on the specified performance criteria and the range of evidence sources
required, but at the same time show empathy and make fair and thoughtful judgements
relating to flexibility where appropriate. Flexibility can be used to accommodate
individual student’s special needs and circumstances through providing alternative
assessment methods (e.g., oral rather than written responses) without compromising
the essential knowledge and skills embedded in the standards.
One of the most significant challenges in making valid assessment decisions
revolves around considerations of appropriate standard or level of proficiency. While
we would all like to have clear standards from which to base assessment decisions,
this is often difficult to achieve in practice. Certainly, the explicit and valid iden-
tification of performance criteria is important here. Failure to appropriately make
explicit the key constructs/elements that underpin the performance areas will seri-
ously undermine the validity of the assessment, as well as create difficulty for asses-
sors, affecting inter-rater reliability. This, in turn, inevitably results in disagreement
between assessors, as well as between assessors and those being assessed. In the
worse scenario, this can lead to appeal situations and legal scenarios. In the final
analysis, summative assessment involves making a judgement concerning a person’s
worth or capability in a certain performance area. Getting this wrong through a deci-
sion of non-competence or grading lower than the criteria indicates can have severe
consequences for a learner in terms of employment or educational career access. Of
note, rarely do students appeal against a top grading, suggesting that it should be
lowered.
It is also the case with high inference assessment areas, that even when criteria are
well derived and delineated, actual judgment in terms of how well students perform
requires interpretation, and this can vary across markers. Much of the problem is
identified by Knight (2006) who argues that complex learning cannot be reduced to
something simple enough to measure reliably: the more complex the learning, the
more we draw on connoisseurship (Eisner 1985) rather than measurement to make
our judgment (p. 38). Connoisseurship refers to the ‘art of appreciating something’,
and is typically used in ways to depict a person’s deep awareness of, especially the
subtle aspects, in an area of experience (e.g., music or art). In terms of assessment,
having deep knowledge and experiences over many situations (e.g., expertise) helps
272 8 Assessing Twenty-First Century Competencies
to get a better holistic view of the performance; rather than just aggregating discrete
elements into a final grade. In practice, breaking down complex performances into
highly detailed and specific criteria can result in a level of reductionism that both fail
to capture the holistic contextualized performance as well as encouraging students
to focus on these more atomistic components. In consequence, this can also mitigate
students adopting a deep approach to learning and the capability to transfer learning
across a range of similar performance situations.
A final important consideration in this context relates to the authenticity of assess-
ment evidence provided by students, as documented earlier. In today’s globally wired
world, plagiarism is a serious assessment concern.
Bearing in mind the considerations identified above, the following criteria identify
the key areas of practice for making the best judgments we can:
• Learners are provided with clear access to assessment
• The assessment evidence is judged accurately against the agreed assessment
criteria
• Only the criteria specified for the assessment are used to judge assessment evidence
• The assessment decisions are based on all relevant assessment evidence available
• Inconsistencies in assessment evidence are clarified and resolved
• The requirements to ensure authenticity are maintained.
important is that the methods used should be as closely calibrated to the types of
learning outcomes being assessed as is viable. The emphasis on real-world projects
and tasks, which require the integration of knowledge and skills across subject and
domain fields, will feature increasingly in the facilitation and assessment of twenty-
first century Competencies. While students learning of key content knowledge and
understanding can be effectively and efficiently assessed in written and other paper
and pencil tests, this needs to be increasingly augmented by integrated real-world
projects and authentic learning experiences.
All assessment methods have strengths and limitations in terms of the types of
assessment evidence they can generate, and their usefulness will be largely dependent
on the learning outcomes being assessed. For example, while multiple-choice items
can be very effective and efficient for assessing knowledge and understanding (e.g.,
specific types of thinking such as analysis, comparison and contrast, inference and
interpretation, and evaluation), they have little validity for assessing integrated skills
in complex problem-solving activities. Similarly, performance-based items, often the
most valid means for assessing more complex real-world performances, emphasized
in the twenty-first century skills frameworks, are much more time and resource
consuming, inevitably provide challenges both to the sufficiency of the evidence and,
as they involve higher levels of inference than traditional testing methods, reliability.
Within this context, it is suggested that combinations of the following strategies
can contribute to the effectiveness and efficiency of assessment practices in given
assessment situations. The strategies are not meant to be exhaustive or summative,
but represent practical frames from which assessment decisions can be thoughtfully
made and practically customized to the range of assessment situations:
(a) Produce assessment activities that are interesting and challenging
(b) Integrate a range of learning outcomes in assessment activities
(c) Provide as much transparency as possible in the assessment process
(d) Utilize student collaboration in formative assessment.
One of the central themes of enhancing learning is through learning experiences that
are more intrinsically motivating for students. A major means for achieving greater
engagement as well as aspects of metacognitive capability and other twenty-first
century competencies is through well contextualized real-world learning tasks. Just
as a well-constituted syllabus is of limited value in the hands of faculty who lack
competence in pedagogic practices and the ability to create interesting and engaging
learning experiences, the same logic applies to our assessment activities. If they
lack interest and purpose for students and encourage rote learning, we should not
be surprised to see them adopt the surface approaches to learning, as explained
prior. In these situations, students will learn what is necessary for assessment but
are unlikely to derive both a real understanding of the subject or a genuine interest
in it. Once the assessment process is finished, much of what was learned will soon
be forgotten. In contrast, where students find the assessment activities interesting
and sufficiently challenging, they are more likely to develop a genuine interest in
274 8 Assessing Twenty-First Century Competencies
the learning involved (Struyven et al. 2002). It is motivation for mastering the tasks
set that leads to a desire for understanding the important concepts and principles of
a subject and makes possible the transfer of learning (e.g., McTighe and Wiggins
2000).
(b) Integrate a range of learning outcomes in assessment activities
There is an old English metaphor, “kill two birds with one stone”. In the context
of assessment, this means getting the best efficiency from assessment activities and
situations. The more assessment activities enable coverage of a range of learning
outcomes, especially if they integrate knowledge and skills across topic and domain
areas, more efficient is the assessment task. This will be increasingly necessary for
the assessment of twenty-first century competencies, as they are typically employed
simultaneously in expert performance in many situations. For example, an expert
in almost every field will often need to combine good thinking with good commu-
nication skills and teamwork as part of a seamless performance. In doing so, MC
conducts the orchestration of skills needed for the complex work activities that we
now need to enact. As emphasized prior, many twenty-first century skills are mainly
‘souped-up’ first century skills, and they are certainly interrelated in real-life applica-
tions. However, MC, while evolutionary hatched as a potential capability for primate
mankind in terms of brain morphology, it would be far less required in yesteryear,
as compared to the information overload and breakneck change of today’s world.
For primitive man, though with milieu variation, most aspects of life were highly
predictable from year to year, and the need for thinking quite limited (at least in
terms of System 2 thinking, Kahneman 2012).
The interconnectivity of twenty-first century skills, from the main competencies,
will, as Lai and Viering (2012) suggests, create a need for:
…multiple measures that either 1) represent multiple assessment modes or 2) sample from
multiple content domains to permit triangulation of inferences. (p. 43)
There is little doubt that this will be problematic in these times of austerity. As I
remember it, quality was a big issue and focus in the 1980s and 1990s, whereas today,
this seems to be a given, and the focus is on cost-saving as the competitive advantage.
The question is how far does this high quality—low-cost paradigm go? This could
mean more and more people working longer, harder, for less and less. In terms of
assessment, we are wanting more and more quality and fidelity of measurement of
highly complex interrelated skills—but attained with fewer resources.
This is an area in which technology may play a significant role in future, as it
is presently providing the means for testing content knowledge and understanding
(which requires types of thinking) as well as increasingly personalized specific feed-
back and guidance for future learning pathways and strategy use. Interactive videos,
augmented reality, virtual reality, and the newer concept of mixed reality may offer
useful affordances and mitigate the costing involved in expert human observation,
as identified prior.
Metacognitive, cognitive and affective skills and attitudes are typically learned
within the context of domain-specific knowledge. Similarly, where issues of a value-
laden and ethical nature are involved, there is a need for both learning and assessment
to be contextualized to the subject domain. Teaching ethics and values separate
from real-life contexts is akin to teaching thinking without reference to a knowledge
domain. De-contextualized knowledge is difficult to transfer and may not even be
perceived as meaningful by learners. Nucci’s (2001) observation about the teaching
of values, is relevant in this context:
The greatest challenge for a teacher wishing to engage in domain appropriate practice is
to identify issues within the regular academic curriculum that will generate discussion and
reflection around a particular value. (pp. 178–79)
Students are firstly more likely to understand and internalize learning in the affec-
tive domain when it is contextualized to specific work-related contexts and issues.
Secondly, in terms of assessment, there is more likely to be authentic assessment
opportunities and greater validity in the assessment of this knowledge and skills in
such situations. For example, in the world of engineering practices, as in any pro-
fessional domain, there are ample opportunities to naturally infuse the full range
of types of thinking as well as areas of ethical concern—both in terms of personal
values and wider societal issues that involve professional ethics. I have developed
and conducted programmes on ethical reasoning through an exploration of specific
ethical dilemmas that can manifest themselves in different work contexts. These can
be structured in terms of variation of situation, emphasis, and levels of complexity.
Technology can be useful here, especially short role-play videos, as suggested in
Chap. 6. I produce my videos in this area, which seem to work well, and are easy to
produce with free (or very reasonably priced) video editing software tools. Also, this
avoids issues of plagiarism and seeking permission for reproduction, which is time-
consuming. One does not need to employ ‘National Geographic’ video production
quality—it’s all about good content, and make sure the audio is clear and focused.
You can also embed questions or reflection points into the video structure. When
276 8 Assessing Twenty-First Century Competencies
done with creativity and this is not so difficult, you are developing Creative Teaching
Competence, and as you improve, it gets easier and easier.
(c) Provide as much transparency as possible in the assessment process
Assessment should not be designed to mystify students about what they are expected
to learn; rather it should make explicit what is to be assessed, the specific criteria
involved in making the assessment decision, and the standard of the assessment
evidence required in behavioural terms—as far as is viable. For example, when doing
a driving test, you know exactly what is expected both in terms of knowledge (i.e., the
highway code for that country) and skills (e.g., parking the car, emergency stop, 3-
point turn). You are unlikely to be asked the capital of Bulgaria in doing your test, even
if you live there. There is full transparency as to what constitutes a competent driving
performance. It is also equally clear as to what gaps in knowledge and competence
will lead to failing the test. Other things being constant, such as assessor reliability
and fairness, etc., passing or failing will depend on the performances exhibited in the
test situation. However, there is disturbing evidence that factors such as mood, fatigue
and hunger can influence judgements. For example, Kahneman (2012), Referring to
research reported in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, in which
parole requests were studied in terms of the time of day they were granted or not
granted (the exact time of each decision was recorded, as well as the times of the
judges’ three food breaks), found that after each meal the proportion of requests
spiked upward with 65% of requests granted. However, during the two hours or so
until the judges’ next feeding, the approval rate dropped steadily, to about zero just
before the meal. As Kahneman concludes:
The best possible account of the data provides bad news: tired and hungry judges tend to fall
back on the easier default position of denying requests for parole. Both fatigue and hunger
probably play a role. (pp. 43–44)
Similarly, the Halo effect, which is the tendency for positive impressions of a
person in one area to positively influence one’s opinion or feelings in other areas.
For example, attractive-looking people are sometimes perceived as more outgoing,
socially competent and powerful, sexually responsive, intelligent, and healthy (e.g.,
Eagly et al. 1991; Zebrowitz and Rhodes 2004). Moreover, these trait impressions are
accompanied by preferential treatment of attractive people in a variety of domains,
including interpersonal relations, occupational settings and the judicial system (e.g.,
Langlois et al. 2000; Zebrowitz 1997). The Halo effect is a cognitive bias and explains
why many people try to make themselves as attractive as possible. It also explains
why people try hard to make good first impressions—the Primacy effect—as a good
Primacy effect, with a positive Halo effect (one can of course also suffer from a
negative Halo effect)—gets one off on a ‘good foot’—so to speak—with persons we
are trying to impress in some way. In the context of assessment, the Halo effect can
be influential in assessment decision making in 2 main ways. Firstly, for the students
that we like, there may be a tendency (and this typically occurs sub/unconsciously)
to be more generous or lenient in the assessment process. Secondly, if they generally
8.3 Assessment from an Evidence-Based Approach 277
produce high-quality work, and then on occasion submit lower standard work, we
might still give them a higher grade than the work merits.
Given that assessments typically involve human perception and mood, and other
attribution biases, you may simply just be lucky or unlucky in an assessment situation.
To mitigate such systemic biases, we need to establish robust checks on the assess-
ment process; this is typically referred to as verification. Verification, in the context
of assessment, is about ensuring that the agreed processes, procedures and practices
are carried out by designated personnel, as prescribed. It is not about duplicating the
assessment process—through a sampling of assessment evidence and assessment
decisions made is part of the overall verification role. An essential component of the
verification process in the Internal Verifier (IV) for an educational institution. This
person is typically an experienced and accredited assessor, who takes on the role of
developing, monitoring and reviewing assessment practice at an institutional level,
and will conduct internal quality assurance of the assessment process by:
• Carrying out and evaluating internal assessment and quality assurance systems
• Supporting assessors
• Monitoring the quality of assessors’ performance
• Meeting external quality assurance requirements.
This typically involves:
• Comparing own organizations requirements with those of the external awarding
body
• Identifying the outcomes needed by the agreed standards and their consequences
for internal auditing
• Auditing existing administrative and recording arrangements—modify/change if
necessary—to meet external audit requirements
• Carrying out assessment standardization arrangements (e.g., sampling strategy)
• Ensuring a procedure for complaints and appeals is in place (note: consider this
in the local context)
• Identifying problems and developing improvement plans.
There is a range of functions, though most important is ensuring the standard-
ization of assessment decisions and supporting teachers who have assessment roles.
Key areas include:
1. Ensure that assessors consistently make valid decisions
2. Ensure that assessors make the same decision on the same evidence base
3. Ensure that all candidates are assessed fairly.
This involves conducting moderation activities (e.g., different assessors judging
the same candidate’s evidence and comparing their perceptions and decisions). This
is an effective way to ensure that there is a common (as far as is possible) concep-
tion of standard in terms of the type, level of knowledge and skills that need to be
demonstrated for an element of competence. In terms of professional development
and support for assessors, the following are essential:
278 8 Assessing Twenty-First Century Competencies
Smart students have long worked out that the secret to success in assessment boils
down to basic logic, ‘know what needs to be learned, learn it and know that you have
learned it’. However, simply knowing this does not mean success either in learning
or assessment outcomes. Within this simple maxim is an implicit essential added
element, its called effort. Learning, at a high proficiency level typically involves
considerable effort and time on task, which many students may not be prepared to
do. Students who make the necessary effort and develop a high level of competence
deserve to be successful—don’t they? Metacognitive Capability is the best prepara-
tion we can offer our students, but, ultimately, they must take responsibility for their
learning—become agentic—which is a key part of being a Self-Directed Learner.
(d) Utilize student collaboration in formative assessment
As assessment is fundamentally linked to learning and teaching, it makes sense to uti-
lize the main stakeholders (faculty and students) to collaboratively make this ‘system’
work to the best advantages of both. For example, from a student’s perspective, the
ideal would be to use assessment to optimally support the learning process through
the various processes of formative assessment. Similarly, from a faculty perspective,
we would like to be able to identify student learning concerns/problems quickly and
be able to effectively and efficiently deal with them—whether through instructional
design or other learning support means. How then, might we create the kind of
symbiosis that makes possible the best collaboration between faculty and students,
without compromising the quality and credibility of final summative assessments?
8.3 Assessment from an Evidence-Based Approach 279
Ultimately, the most fundamental way to utilize this collaboration is to help stu-
dents to develop their own self-assessment capability, as emphasised prior. Students
who are able, in large part, to identify what they know and don’t know are already a
long way to becoming independent learners and reduce the load off faculty in terms
of instructional and remediation time. Time spent in developing student’s capabil-
ity to self-assess will result in better learning for students as well as making the
instructional process more efficient.
Secondly, having students involved in peer assessment is especially important, and
has a higher single Effect Size than self-assessment (0.63 as compared to 0.54, Hattie
2009). However, feedback effects from various sources (e.g., tutor–student; student–
student; other credible sources) will have synergistic impacts. In summary, good
feedback from good sources, and students taking an agentic approach to learning is
what we want to facilitate. It must be emphasized that students will initially need
direct instruction and plenty of deliberate practise to develop skills of assessment,
for example:
• Analysing goals, outcomes, and tasks to ascertain what knowledge and skills are
involved
• Analysing and deriving performance criteria for making judgements of worth
• Making inferences and interpretations from performance evidence to ascertain
competence or otherwise, and on what basis.
If the teaching of Good Thinking has been initiated and ongoing in the earlier
instruction—this should be a transfer activity for students, as this is good thinking
applied to the domain of assessing performance from evidence.
Good design is fundamental to all assessment items, whether a fixed response, essay
type or performance-tests. This is well documented in the literature (e.g., Osterlind
1989; Haladyna 1997). However, different assessment items, apart from offering
different assessment evidence on students’ learning, also provide different chal-
lenges in turns of making assessment decisions. For example, while multiple-choice
items require skill in design, and the production of a large bank of useful items
is time-consuming, marking is easy and efficient. Open response (essay-type) and
performance-based items, in contrast, require a more elaborate marking system and
are prone to subjectivity.
Performance-based assessments, however, are potentially the most valid forms
of assessment as they provide assessment opportunities where students can dis-
play key competences in the real world or simulated activities. Such tasks provide
more authentic and valid assessment opportunities, offering the following assessment
advantages over more traditional pencil and paper-based approaches:
• Greater validity as the focus is on real-life performance
• Measures a range of complex skills and processes in real-world or authentically
simulated contexts
280 8 Assessing Twenty-First Century Competencies
Will technology enable the assessment of types and levels of thinking and voli-
tional aspects of mind related to emotional regulation at the neural level through arti-
ficial intelligence networks connected to brain imaging technologies in the future? Is
‘Radical Evolution’ as Garreau (2005) and Kurzweil (2005) document possible, and
is it likely to happen soon? Well, as indicated prior, I don’t think it will happen in the
next couple of decades, and it may not be as radical as being prophesied—or desired.
In the absence of such technology-aided assessment tools, we must develop marking
systems for performance-based assessment tasks, especially where high inference
8.3 Assessment from an Evidence-Based Approach 281
Assessment areas constitute the main performances that are to be assessed in any
performance-based activity, and typically more than one assessment area can be
validly assessed. For example, in a project-based activity, there are usually opportu-
nities to assess critical and creative thinking, teamwork and communication, ethical
issues, as well as the technical subject content areas. However, just because a perfor-
mance test offers such opportunities for assessment, this does not automatically mean
that all possible performance areas must be assessed, especially in summative terms.
What is assessed from such an activity should be considered with other assessment
components for the module or unit of study. For example, if an area has been suffi-
ciently assessed elsewhere, it may be more practical to assess other important areas
that have not been previously assessed in the overall assessment scheme. However, it
is, of course, useful, whenever feasible, to provide appropriate formative assessment
in all the significant performance areas.
Once the summative assessment areas have been identified for the learning activ-
ity, it is then necessary to identify the marks allocation or weighting for each of
the designated areas. This should reflect the learning outcomes and their relative
importance within the module or unit context, as well with other components of the
assessment plan. Table 8.1 summarizes the assessment areas and mark weightings.
Performance Criteria
Performance criteria are the more specific and measurable elements/behaviours that
underpin the wider performance area to be assessed. For example, in assessing
Demonstrate Effective Written Communication (a component of Communication
Skills) the performance criteria may include ‘write with logical organization and
clear language flow’, ‘use concise and precise language’, ‘use correct grammar,
spelling and punctuation’, etc.
From my experience, the process of generating performance criteria for assess-
ing the performance areas of a course syllabus can be enhanced when educational
development specialists work with subject specialists, as this can ensure clarity and
validity, as well as faculty buy-in. Developing highly detailed lists that have a pristine
appearance but are seen as unnecessarily wieldy and obtrusive by the faculty that are
required to use them are rarely used in the ‘intended way’ in practice.
Performance criteria should provide the necessary guidance to enable assessing
faculty to make the most valid assessment decisions possible (in the context of time
and resource allocation) about the learning outcomes, based on the performance
evidence generated by the task activities. In designing performance-based assessment
tasks, I find it useful to ask the essential question below, and answer it as best as
possible:
Will this performance task offer the student a realistic opportunity to demonstrate that he/she
can meet these learning outcomes?
Table 8.2 shows how one school chose to organize their marking system for a third-
year capstone project. From an analysis of the range of activities that the students were
going to engage in throughout the project, and with the syllabus learning outcomes,
they derived the following performance criteria that would form the basis and focus
for the assessment areas.
Assessment Evidence refers to the range of performances and products that can
be validly and efficiently considered in making an assessment decision. In making
assessment decisions, it is necessary to consider what the range and types of evidence
are that can be generated by the various activities concerning the performance areas
and criteria. For example, in assessing teamwork, a wide range of evidence sources
can be generated and used to make a valid assessment decision. These could include
the following:
• Feedback from students (e.g., peer assessment)
• Lecturer observation of student interactions
• Questioning
• Meeting deadlines and objectives
• Students logs/journals.
8.5 Assessment Evidence 283
For projects, the following generic types of assessment evidence are typically
produced:
• Reports
• Progress reviews
• Logbook
• Scheduling documentation
• Engineering drawings
• Artefacts (e.g., models, prototypes, programmes, operating manuals, etc.)
• Presentations
• Responses to questions (e.g., oral, written).
Invariably, the greater the range of evidence sources that can be accessed (provid-
ing they are sufficiently valid and authentic), the more likely it is that we can make
accurate assessments of performance.
Assessment Rubrics
Assessment Rubrics are rating scales in which a prepared scoring system is used for
assessing learner performance for a specific task or assessment area across different
levels of that performance (usually 1–5, in which 1 denotes a very poor performance
and 5 denotes a very good performance). Assessment rubrics are most useful when
assessing complex activities where the assessment of performance is of variation,
and involving a high level of inference. For example, in assessing teamwork, it is
8.5 Assessment Evidence 285
often not a clear case of being either effective or ineffective in this performance
area, but rather variation along a continuum from very effective to very ineffective.
Furthermore, as there are many aspects and potentially different interpretations of
what constitutes effective teamwork, it is open to different inferences by different
assessors. The extent to which assessors are likely to differ in terms of assessment
decisions relating to an area of performance determines the level of inference.
In performances where all assessors, assuming expertise in the area, would consis-
tently agree on the level of performance, we can say that assessment is low inference.
This would be the case in most procedural aspects of a performance in which there
are clear and established, almost algorithmic, standards relating to effective perfor-
mance. In these assessment situations, a checklist is a more appropriate tool and
easier to use marking system. However, in areas such as creativity and aesthetics,
while certain features can be identified as criteria of quality, there is still a high level
of subjectivity in terms of personal interpretation of what this looks like overall. Such
areas constitute high inference assessment, where assessors may have quite diverse
perceptions of what is good and poor performance. In these assessment situations, the
descriptors of different levels of performance are useful in mitigating the variation
of assessment decisions and enhancing reliability. It only requires watching a few
episodes of popular singing competitions in which one judge may see a competitor’s
performance as ‘brilliant’ but another judge (and remember these are supposed to be
top experts in the field) see’s it ‘as self-indulgent rubbish’. I feel like it’s the latter
when subjected to listen (of course I try to avoid it) to much of modern music. Don
Mclean who famously sang American Pie, on a visit to Singapore a decade or so
back, was asked when he thought was the day the music died (an emphatic line in
the song). He replied, in the interview, and I hope I get this right, ‘some 40 years
ago’. I liked his song, and his judgement on musical quality is exemplary—here’s
subjectivity for you. Now, would you like me assessing you’re rap or techno song?
In using rubrics, decisions need to be made on whether to assess more holisti-
cally or analytically about performance areas and criteria. Essentially this relates to
whether to assess the performance area overall e.g., oral communication and give a
score; or break it down into key components/constructs, score these individually, then
derive the overall score. There are merits in both approaches (Biggs 2003; Gosling
and Moon 2003). Holistic rubrics enable a focus on the overall performance and are
more economical in terms of assessment time. They are typically used for summative
assessment and where some variation in reliability in parts of the assessment com-
ponents can be accepted, provided the overall assessment decision has justifiable
validity and reliability. In contrast, analytical rubrics enable a much greater focus
on the specific elements of the areas of learning involved and make possible a much
better utilization of formative assessment in the assessment process. One type of
rubric is not inherently better than another (Montgomery 2001); it depends on the
assessment purpose and context in which rubrics are used.
What is of primary importance is that the rubric does not make the assessment
decision. Rubrics provide a guiding framework for focusing attention on the key ele-
ments/constructs (performance criteria) of the assessment area and summary descrip-
tors of a range of performances. A good rubric offers sound heuristics, bringing the
286 8 Assessing Twenty-First Century Competencies
limited assessment value in highly detailed marking systems that are burdensome in
practice, as faculty won’t have (or make) the time for what they see as an unproductive
activity—they have enough to do already. For purposes of summative assessment, a
holistic rubric format usually works well, when used thoughtfully. Table 8.4 shows a
typical rubric design that has been used in a range of contexts for scoring performance
in Oral Presentation Skills.
This is a standard rubric design in which a performance area (in this case Oral
Presentation Skills) is broken down into key behavioural indicators relating to the
curriculum syllabus. These become the basis for the five levels of descriptive perfor-
mance. Marks can then be allocated to these broadband descriptors in terms of wider
assessment weightings and grading formats. Converting rubric scores into grades is
more a question of logic than any specific mathematical formula. What is essential is
that the marks allocation calibrates to what has been decided in the overall assessment
plan for the module or unit of study.
Note: the behavioural descriptors can both reduced or added to, or further refined,
depending on the learning outcome focus and level of elaboration required. Also,
range statements can be specified to meet the demands of different professional
contexts. In teaching, for example, I am reluctant to accredit teaching competence,
based only on teaching highly motivated adult learners, unless of course, this is the
specific range for a given teacher training programme.
8.6 Summary
While assessment is not an exact science, much is known about good assessment
practices in terms of the principles and standards as documented in this chapter.
Hence, we can explore, from a sound evidence base, the underpinning issues in
assessment quality and subsequently frame assessment approaches, strategies and
methods to address the challenges of assessing twenty-first century competencies.
However, as identified, developing assessment approaches and conducting a high-
quality assessment of more complex interrelated real-world performance-based tasks
for twenty-first century competencies, and making this cost-effective, will require
much by way of teacher expertise, leadership and resource provision.
Issues and practices relating to enhancing professional development in teaching,
as well as reframing aspects of educational policy and direction, will be explored in
the next and last chapter of this work.
References
Banta TW, Jones EA, Black KE (2009) Designing effective assessment: principles and profiles of
good practice. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Biggs JB (1996) Enhancing teaching through constructive alignment. High Educ 32(3):347–364
Biggs JB (2003) Teaching for quality learning at university. Open University Press/Society for
Research into Higher Education, Buckingham
Bloxham S, Boyd P (2007) Developing effective assessment in higher education. Open University
Press, Maidenhead
Boud D (ed) (1988) Developing student autonomy in learning. Kogan Page, London
Boud D (1995) Ensuring assessment fosters learning: meeting the challenges of more students with
diverse backgrounds. In: Armstrong J, Conrad L (eds) Subject evaluation. Griffith Institute for
Higher Education, Queensland
Boud D (2000) Sustainable assessment: rethinking assessment for the learning society. Stud Contin
Educ 22(2):151–167
References 289
Butler SM, McMunn ND (2006) A teacher’s guide to classroom assessment: understanding and
using assessment to improve student learning. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Eagly AH et al (1991) What is beautiful is good, but…: a meta-analytic review of research on the
physical attractiveness stereotype. Psychol Bull 10(1):109–128
Eisner EW (1985) The art of educational evaluation: a personal view. The Falmer Press, Basingstoke
Garreau J (2005) The promise and peril of enhancing our minds, our bodies—and what it means to
be human. Doubleday, New York
Gibbs G (2008) How assessment can support or undermine learning. In: TLHE, frontiers in higher
education. CDLT, Singapore
Gosling D, Moon J (2003) How to use learning outcomes and assessment criteria, 3rd edn. SEEC,
London
Gray P (2007) Student learning assessment. In: Crawley E, Malmqvist J, Ostlund S, Brodeur D
(2007) Rethinking engineering education. Springer, New York
Haladyna TM (1997) Writing test items to assess higher order thinking. Allyn & Bacon, Needham
Heights
Hattie J (2009) Visible learning. Routledge, New York
Kahneman D (2012) Thinking fast and slow. Penguin Books, London
Knight PT (ed) (1995) Assessment for learning in higher education. Kogan Page, London
Knight PT (2006) The local practices of assessment. Assess Eval High Educ 31(4):435–452
Kurzweil R (2005) The singularity is near: when humans transcend biology. Viking, New York
Lai ER, Viering M (2012) Assessing 21st century skills: integrating research findings. National
Council on Measurement in Education, Vancouver, B.C.
Langlois JH et al (2000) Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review.
Psychol Bull 2000(126):390–423
Mazur E (1996) Peer instruction: a user’s manual. Pearson, London, U.K.
McTighe J, Wiggins G (2000) Understanding by design. Association for Supervision and Curriculum
Development, VA
Montgomery K (2001) Authentic assessment: a guide for elementary teachers. Longman, New York
Nucci IP (2001) Education in the moral domain. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Osterlind SJ (1989) Constructing test items: multiple-choice, constructed response, performance
and other formats. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, Netherlands
Price M, O’Donovan B (2006) A scholarly approach to solving the feedback dilemma in practice.
Assess & Eval High Educ, Volume 41, Issue 6, 2016
Prosser M, Trigwell K (1998) Understanding learning and teaching: the experience in higher
education. Open University Press, Buckingham
Ramsden P (1992) Learning to teach in higher education. Routledge, London
Rowntree D (1987) Assessing students: how shall we know them. Harper Row, London
Sale D (2014) The challenge of reframing engineering education. Springer, New York
Sibley J, Ostafichuk P (2014) Getting started with team-based learning. Stylus Publishing, USA
Stiggins R et al (2006) Classroom assessment for student learning. Pearson, New Jersey
Struyven K, Dochy F, Janssens S (2002) Students’ perceptions about assessment in higher educa-
tion: a review. Paper presented at the Joint Northumbria/EARLI SG assessment and evaluation
conference: learning communities and assessment cultures, University of Northumbria, 28–30
Aug
The Towards Defining 21st Century Competencies. The Foundation Document for Discussion,
Ontario (2016)
Zebrowitz LA (1997) Reading faces: window to the soul?. Westview Press, Boulder, Colo
Zebrowitz LA, Rhodes G (2004) Sensitivity to “bad genes” and the anomalous face overgener-
alization effect: cue validity, cue utilization, and accuracy in judging intelligence and health. J
Nonverbal Behav 28(3):167–185
Chapter 9
Framing Professional Development Now
9.1 Introduction
In the preceding chapters, I framed the context and challenges that many teachers are
increasingly facing now, and these will certainly dominate the educational landscape
for the next decade or so. Notions of accurately predicting teaching challenges and
context for the whole of the twenty-first century are highly speculative and probably
erroneous.
Debates about human learning are still prevalent in terms of framing cohesive
educational programmes for teacher professional development. Darling-Hammond
and Rothman (2015) captured the problem clearly:
While educators and policymakers agree that enabling teachers to improve student learning is
one of the most significant ways to raise student achievement, there are heated disagreements
about the most useful ways to do this. (p. 1)
Using an EBT approach I have argued that Pedagogic Literacy (PL), Metacog-
nitive Capability (MC) and Creative Teaching Competence (CTC) are three major
areas for framing teacher expertise and addressing the challenges of facilitating Self-
Directed Learning (SDL)—both for students and for teachers. Certainly, teachers
high in MC will have the capability to be self-directed in their professional devel-
opment as they will be effective evaluators of their ability to self-regulate—it goes
with the territory so to speak. As Costa and Garmston (2016) made explicit:
Self-directed evaluators use structures and skills that engage the professional’s thinking
about his/her performance. In self-directed evaluation, intentionally builds the capacity of
the professional for self-management, self-monitoring and self-modifying behaviors. (p. 82)
Furthermore, and equally important, they will also have the necessary competence
to teach these skills to students. As Powell and Kusuma-Powell (2015) point out:
Teachers who demonstrate self-directedness are much more likely to demonstrate emotion-
ally intelligent classroom behaviour. They look for cause and effect relationships between
their teaching and their students’ learning. Their emotional intelligence contributes to
the construction of powerful learning relationships with students as well as colleagues.
Self-regulation is all about controlling our impulses, particularly disruptive ones, delaying
gratification, thinking before acting, and suspending judgement. (p. 41)
Groups that have high collective intelligence are more innovative, more likely to find creative
solutions to problems, more likely to engage in reflection and therefore more likely to transfer
their learning to new and novel situations. (p. 15)
how the quantity and quality of interactions and social relations among people affects their
access to knowledge and information; their sense of expectation, obligation and trust; and
how far they are likely to adhere to the same norms or codes of behaviour…Social capital
increases your knowledge – it gives you access to other people’s human capital. (p. 90)
Invariable, these kinds of capital are mutually supporting and synergistic in that
as more and more faculty work collaboratively in the development of knowledge and
expertise in teaching, there are benefits to all participants, and this will have positive
impacts on other stakeholders, especially the student learners.
As the authors sum up:
collaborative cultures build social capital and therefore also professional capital in a school
community. (p. 115)
Firstly, here’s some context on what is still an area of contention in terms of human
learning, epitomised in a question I am frequently asked: “Do adults learn differ-
ently from children, and do we need to teach them differently? In Edu-speak this is
sometimes referred to as the ‘Pedagogy—Andragogy’ debate.
Pedagogy, as identified in Chap. 2, has been contrasted with the term Andragogy
(Knowles 1984), which focuses on the teaching of adult learners. However, knowl-
edge from cognitive science generally throws doubt on the notion that the underlying
learning processes of adults are structurally different from that of children who have
attained the stage of formal operational thought (Piaget 2001), typically around 12–
15 years of age. At this stage of brain maturation, children can reason logically and
use a range of thinking skills (e.g., analyzing, comparing & contrasting, making
inferences and interpretations, and evaluating). There is, however, evidence from
cognitive neuroscience that the prefrontal cortex which, apart from other things, is
responsible for the control of our impulses, complex actions, planning and organiza-
tion, doesn’t fully mature until a person is in his or her twenties (e.g., Swaab 2015).
9.3 The Pedagogy—Andragogy Debate 295
This means that mature adults are likely to be more organized and determined in
their learning efforts (e.g., higher MC) than younger learners who will need more
structure and support in these aspects of learning. This does not limit the usefulness
of developing MC in younger learners, but maybe not to expect self-directedness to
be fully achieved by all students on high school graduation day. There is much to
being self-directed, as Powell and Kusuma-Powell’s (2015) frame implies:
Self-direction is choosing one’s own way; increasingly becoming the author of who we are
coming to be; constructing an internal sense of personal meaningfulness that is founded on
examined beliefs and values. Self-direction is the process by which we continually become.
We perceive identity not so much as a static entity, but as a continual process of becoming.
(p. 32)
Furthermore, in teaching adults, there are certainly important areas of focus that
need to be attended to in qualitatively different ways. For example, there are signif-
icant differences in the level of prior experience of adults, as compared to children;
the former usually having more. Adults also choose what they want to learn, and this
is typically consciously directed to meet work or personal learning goals. In contrast,
pupils in a school are largely told what to learn, at least in the earlier years. Knowles
(1984) saw adult learners, as compared to younger learners, having the following
qualitative differences:
1. Concept of the learner. Adults need to be self-directing and be treated by others
as able to take responsibility. They resent others imposing their will and desire
participation in decisions that affect them
2. Role of the learner’s experience. Adults have a wide experience which serves to
determine who they are, to create their sense of self-identity. When this experience
is devalued or ignored by the teacher, this implies a rejection of the person, not
just the experience
3. Readiness to learn. Adults become ready to learn when they experience a need
to know or do something as a result of changes in their lives
4. Orientation to learning. Adults enter an educational activity with a life task or
problem-centred orientation to learning. They are less bothered by underpinning
knowledge or theory
5. Motivation to learn. Adults are motivated more by internal rather than exter-
nal agents (e.g., self-esteem, recognition by peers, a better quality of life,
self-actualization).
Based on these assumptions, it can be argued that there are certain key implications
for teaching adults:
• Adults want more involvement in the planning/decision-making and facilitation
of their learning. This may involve more flexibility in the learning arrangements
to accommodate the various demands of their life contexts.
• Adults are more interested in/focused on seeing relevance/purpose in what they
are learning (e.g., how it relates to a felt need or problem they are trying to address
in their professional and/or personal life).
296 9 Framing Professional Development Now
• Adults expect their adult status to be recognized and this has important human
conduct issues of equity and communication style in the process of teaching and
management of the learning environment.
These features of the Adult Learner, in contrast to younger learners, has been
aptly captured by Rogers (1998):
An instructor of adults is quite unlike a teacher of children or adolescents.
The person is an adult among adults. He or she cannot count on the customary
advantages of age, experience, and size … Many adults will have had experiences
that far surpass the background of the particular instructor. As a group, they have out-
travelled, out-parented, out-worked, and out-lived any of us as individual instructors.
Collectively, they have had more lovers, changed more jobs, survived more accidents,
moved more households, faced more debts, achieved more successes, and overcome
more failures. It is highly unlikely that we can simply impress them with our title,
whether it be a trainer or professor.
The dominant question and request of adult learners to anyone who instructs them is, “Can
you help me?” (p. 27)
However, it could be argued that many of these implications apply, at least in part,
to younger learners. It is unlikely than many of our students like to learn content
that they see no purpose or relevance in and would resent being denied choice and
some equity in human conduct issues. Similarly, the notion that adult learners prefer
to learn more independently and self- discover has also been challenged by recent
research. For example, Dickinson (2015) noted:
I find that time-pressed adult workers often just want someone to tell them what to do. They
have neither the time nor inclination to explore. (p. 157)
It is important therefore that teachers involved in adult education and training are
aware of these different orientations and apply EBT to the adult context. Teaching
adult learners requires much by way of contextualization, but the core principles
of learning still apply for their learning. For example, having taught adults from
many countries and cultural contexts for over 30 years, the differences in terms of
motivation and orientation identified above often apply, but I am more apt to agree
with Dickinson’s observation, and this is important. Adult learners, and increasingly
so, are doing busy workloads and often come to class tired, and their participation
may be more extrinsic than intrinsic (e.g., keeping their jobs, promotion, being told
they need this new accreditation as its now mandatory). Asking adult learners in the
classroom environment to spend their time doing online searching on a specific topic,
for purposes of later sharing and discussion may not go down well with many. Adults
do like to share and learn from each other, but they could do this online research at
home or in a coffee shop; they don’t need to rush to class for such experience. Also,
they expect, and rightly so, that you provide then with relevant new knowledge, skill
applications, and facilitate their thinking in ways to build the level of understanding
necessary to extend their work-related competencies. Yes, they do like spoon-feeding
of this key information, but also expect skilful facilitation in making it useful.
9.3 The Pedagogy—Andragogy Debate 297
The following sections will explore what this means for teacher professional
development, both at individual and collective levels, in terms of developing Creative
Teaching Competence and that superordinate competency—Metacognitive Capabil-
ity. The good news is that we are becoming increasingly aware of how best to achieve
this. Invariably, like the development of expertise in any domain or field, it involves
much learning, persistence and hard work—but what of real value does not?
There can be little argument that professional development must be central to enhanc-
ing professional practice, and much is made of the need for lifelong learning in
a world of exponential knowledge production and rapidly changing occupational
structures. Unfortunately, professional development in enhancing teaching quality
has typically been tempered by the strong, mostly anecdotal, evidence that much
professional development is not effective in terms of improving teacher practice and,
most significantly, in enhancing student attainment (Timperley et al. 2008). I recall
seeing a quote many years ago that is not inconsistent with the perception of many
teachers that I have worked with regarding the usefulness of attending professional
development workshops:
When I die, I hope it’s in a professional development workshop, as the difference will be
hardly noticeable (a teachers comment, anon).
I am guilty here, but fortunately, there will be many joining me should there
be a retrospective inquisition and such practices become indictable offences. I still
conduct professional development workshops in many aspects of curriculum, teach-
ing and assessment, but I am honest with participants about what is realistically
possible in such learning events. Sadly, there are still some who think that compe-
tence in a complex skill can be a realistic outcome from a short workshop involving
explanation and a little ‘hands-on’ simulated practice. Similarly, having spent many
years working out in gyms, I occasionally find it amusing when people express such
298 9 Framing Professional Development Now
disappointment that they have not achieved significant weight loss or enhancement
in muscle bulk or fitness when they have only spent a few hours over a couple of
weeks in the gym (and often doing very little in terms of actual exercise). There are
evidence-based reasons for both of these scenarios, as we know. While I have often
received positive feedback on many of my workshop programmes (and I am thankful
for this), I remain reticent in believing that I have significantly influenced teaching
practices to the extent that this has led to significant gains in student attainment. Over
the years I have received feedback from some participants, post-workshop, claiming
that they had changed aspects of practice, and this has transferred to better student
learning and attainment (e.g., better student feedback relating to their teaching and
improved student performance or grades). However, these are not the majority, and
it seems that such individuals are doing something else, which is not the typical
behaviour of participants who attend workshops without other supportive follow-up
arrangements. We will explore what this is and what the implications are later in the
chapter.
It seems that the most prevalent characteristic of educative professional develop-
ment is that it is often met with resistance (Duffy 1993). According to Duffy et al.
(2010), there are two reasons for this. First, educative professional development usu-
ally emphasizes teacher thoughtfulness, and teachers often resist being thoughtful.
They do so because it is easier to operate from routines, and because their “apprentice-
ship of observation” (Lorte 1975) as students themselves for 13 years causes them to
think that they already know how to teach (Kanfer and Kanfer 1991; Kennedy 1999).
The result is often what Windshitl (2002) called “additive” change or what Huber-
man (1990) called “tinkering”, in which teachers insert minor changes into their
existing practices. Second, research indicates that learning to be thoughtful occurs
in erratic spurts and not as steady growth (p. 9). Certainly, the issue of the ‘lazy
brain’ (e.g., Kahneman 2012) and thinking being something we don’t like to do too
often (e.g., Willingham 2009) feature here also, but these are not peculiar to teachers
per se. However, there are specific contextualized aspects to teachers in educational
institutions that are both causes and the manifestation of much educational thinking
(or lack of it) relating to what is good teaching and, by association, to what is good
professional development. Powell and Kusuma-Powell (2015) argue that this is part
of a wider systemic problem, arguing that:
The field of education is changing with lightening-speed; schools are changing at a snail’s
pace. (p. 18)
They quote Caine and Caine (2001) as capturing the irony of the present situation:
Unfortunately, many countries and cultures are employing a late 20th-century political pro-
cess in an attempt to perfect an early 20th century model of schools, based on 17th-century
beliefs about how people learn, to prepare children for the 21st century. (p. iv)
This may explain, at least in part, that while we are amassing strong evidence
concerning what practices do work well and on what basis, it is not being widely
synthesized and practised. From my experiences of working in this field for over
30 years, I would tend to agree with the findings of Timperley et al. (2008) relating
to two extremes, but often used approaches to professional development:
9.4 Professional Development that Does not Work Well 299
of the information may be lost, and this in itself may lead to it not ever being tried
out. Also, preparing a new instructional strategy will most likely make some extra
demands on time. Again, there may be an intention to use the new strategy, but in
a busy schedule it’s always easier to go with what you have done prior and it may
have worked ok anyway. However, even if the strategy is tried out with good intent
fairly soon after the workshop, there is every likelihood that it may not work out
particularly well, and may even feel counter-productive. This is the learning process
at the level of skill acquisition.
A few decades ago, I decided to learn to play the guitar as this was something I
wanted to do at school but did not have the opportunity. My music lessons at school, as
mentioned in Chap. 1, were far from motivating. In retrospect, the teacher probably
violated every core principle of learning. I had no idea of what we were supposed
to learn, saw no relevance in any of it and was occasionally caned for transgressions
in reciting the musical scales. Despite my loathing of the weekly music lesson, I
remained interested in playing the guitar. Sometimes in life, serendipity provides an
excellent opportunity to learn something and this was the case in my mid-twenties.
I met a young lady who played the guitar and sang in local venues where I lived.
She was also studying for an Open University degree in psychology and asked me if
I would review some of her assignments before formal submission. Jokingly I said
something like, “Sure, but you will have to teach me how to play the guitar in return.”
This was partly in jest, but only partly. She readily agreed and the arrangement worked
well. However, I soon learned why most people who are initially motivated to play
the guitar typically give up within a few weeks. Learning the ‘C’, ‘G’ & ‘F’ chords
is easy conceptually, but not at the level of skilful practice. Making a noise like
a cat encased in an aluminium dustbin rolling down a steep hill, and having very
sore fingers on one’s left hand (I am right-handed), as well as housemates smiling
quizzically as they walk past is hardly encouraging. Quite simply, knowing how to
do something is far from being able to do it, when skills are involved.
In the professional development of teachers, the same scenario plays out. The real
challenge is when teachers apply newly introduced instructional strategies into their
classroom practise at the skill level, as they are unlikely to work on the first time,
or the next time, and perhaps for many times. In this context, many teachers will
give up, and this makes perfect sense. Apart from time constraints, some teachers
may also be concerned about negative feedback from students in such situations, as
students may experience the changes as confusing and not see the value in terms of
better-quality learning at this point. Solving the gap between knowledge acquisition
and understanding to one of eventual expertise, that enables transfer to other related
learning contexts, is a challenging scenario for professional development. Hence, the
big question, given the constraints of teacher’s professional lives, is how best to do
this?
9.5 Unpacking the Components of Effective Professional Development 301
In essence, there are two interrelated black boxes, as depicted in Fig. 9.1. The
first concerns the necessary changes in teachers’ practice, but this must be of such
a nature that it brings about specific changes in student perception and behaviour
related to better learning.
The approach to the professional development of creative teaching professionals
outlined in this chapter is consistent with the evidence-based approach developed in
the preceding chapters. The core principles of learning are equally relevant to teacher
learning, as they are for student learning. While teachers, as adult learners, may have
certain advantages over younger learners in that they are likely to be more focused and
discerning in terms of identifying their learning goals and having greater expertise
to plan, monitor and evaluate their learning (e.g., greater MC), they are subject to
the same cognitive constraints of memory processing and cognitive overload as their
younger counterparts.
The notion of applying the same principles of learning for students to that of
teacher learning, invariably contextualized to the different tasks and contexts, is
fully supported by Powell and Kusuma-Powell (2015) who argue:
The answer to the quandary of improving learning for students lies squarely in improving
learning for teachers. As a result, teachers have a sacred obligation to become architects of
their own, on-going professional growth. (p. 18)
This is what we claim to want for students: independent critical thinkers who are enthu-
siastic life-long learners with the capacity for healthy and accurate self-assessment and
self-modification. If these are desirable outcomes for students, why would we not want then
for teachers as well? (p. 22)
In summary here, it is now apparent that there are many evidence-based princi-
ples and practices clearly aligned to improving teacher learning and expertise and,
therefore, enhancing student learning opportunities and outcomes. This sounds good
news in terms of being able to establish a solid professional development approach
for enhancing teaching competence and expertise. However, the kinds of profes-
sional development activities that are effective require considerable resource time
and effort, which may not be congruent with present practice in many educational
institutions. For example, Gulamhussein (2013) emphasized:
The duration of professional development must be significant and ongoing to allow time for
teachers to learn a new strategy and grapple with the implementation problem. (p. 3)
Similarly, Joyce and Showers (2002), from extensive research, suggest that staff
development focused on student achievement must include the following essential
elements:
• A community of professionals put into practice what they are learning, and share
the results
• The content of staff development develops around curricular and instructional
strategies selected because they have a high probability of affecting student
learning—and, as important, student’s ability to learn
• The magnitude of change generated is sufficient that the student’s gain in
knowledge and skills is palpable
• The processes of staff development enable educators to develop the skill to
implement what they are learning. (p. 4)
However, as Levin (2008) noted:
To get good at a new practice takes time and effort in schools – whether as a teacher or prin-
cipal or superintendent – no tradition or organization supports carefully supervised learning
of this kind. (pp. 80–81)
Core Principle 2: Learning goals, objectives and proficiency expectations are clearly
visible to learners is as applicable to teacher professional learning and development
as it is for the students we teach. Timperley (2008) posed the core question:
9.5 Unpacking the Components of Effective Professional … 303
What do teachers need to know in order to deepen their professional understandings (e.g.
pedagogical content knowledge) and extend their skills so as to have a positive impact on
student outcomes? (p. 27)
I think much of the answer lies in an EBT framing of teacher expertise, which
provides the underpinning Pedagogic Literacy as well as useful heuristics for guid-
ing effective professional development. Of course, we could argue over terminology,
emphasis, and valuations are always embedded. Education is essentially an experi-
ment; one could argue, so is life. Hence, in relation to expert teaching, we can and
need to make certain contextualizing statements about what Powell and Kusuma
Powell (2015) refer to as “goodness” in the profession of teaching. They assert:
As a profession, we need to work on the search for goodness. As a general rule, teachers
are not skilled at deconstructing and analysing exemplary teaching and learning. And as we
have seen researchers and politicians focused primarily on what isn’t working in education.
(p. 8)
Over the chapters, I have sought to establish much by way of what constitutes
‘goodness criteria’ relating to teaching and when fully articulated this may under-
pin much of Expert Teaching now—so to speak. For example, it would be good if
teachers had high MC—that ‘many-headed monster of obscure parentage’ and CTC
as organizing superordinate competencies. The functional and generic competencies
identified and discussed during the previous chapters would also be incorporated
within this ‘goodness framework’. Also, considerations of character traits, disposi-
tions and values, are important here. I think it’s good to have teachers with certain
broad trait configurations as identified prior, have values systems that include integrity
and fairness, and want to help people to learn and experience well-being.
The Gallup organization (2014), quoted in Powell and Kusama-Powell (2015),
studied the characteristics of exceptional teachers for over 40 years and have
identified three common attributes. Exceptionally effective teachers demonstrate:
1. Internal achievement motivation. These are teachers who are driven to reach
higher levels of mastery and learning. They enjoy setting challenging goals
for themselves, monitoring their progress and taking ownership of student
achievement.
2. Orchestration of classroom structure and flexibility. These are teachers who bal-
ance innovation with discipline. They are structured and deliberately organized
without sacrificing creativity and playfulness. They are risk-takers who view fail-
ure as an opportunity to learn. These teachers are constantly thinking about new
ways to present content and to engage students in learning and discovery.
304 9 Framing Professional Development Now
Duffy et al. (2010) suggest that observations of exemplary teachers strongly sug-
gest that effective teachers regulate and control their thinking as they teach. How-
ever, and worryingly, they point out that while it is assumed that teachers can learn
to be metacognitive and that both cognitive and dispositional aspects of professional
development can be designed to intentionally encourage teachers to be metacognitive
professionals, the fact is that:
current efforts are based primarily in common sense and theories rooted in constructivism
and sociocultural thinking, and no empirical data substantiate that one or other kind of
professional development results in teachers who are metacognitive. (p. 10)
What this means is that it is not just knowledge per se, but a deep understanding
of how these knowledge bases connect to the world of practice and what pedagogic
thinking and actions are necessary for situations to create and facilitate learning
experiences that result in better learner outcomes. Hence, there must be a high level
of teacher engagement in terms of good thinking, strategic application and ongoing
9.5 Unpacking the Components of Effective Professional … 305
A crucial component in this process is practice, but not ad hoc practice. As we saw
practice is most effective when it is both spaced and deliberate, which was explained
in detail in Chap. 2. The same principles and procedures are appropriate in the context
of the teacher’s professional development. As Willingham (2009) argued:
Teaching, like any complex skill, must be practised to be improved. (p. 147)
It is at this stage of the learning process that many institutional professional devel-
opment programmes typically experience the problems of implementation identified
earlier. Competence, let alone expertise, does not come easily in any area of life
that requires a high skill component. One may have a good understanding of a per-
formance area and speak very intelligently about it, but that’s not competence or
expertise. The world is full of expert ‘armchair pundits’, especially in the world of
sport. Most people can watch a sport and speak convincingly on what a team or player
should be doing or should stop doing. However, why are they (the actual players)
earning millions of dollars a year while the rest of us are paying television fees to
watch them? In all situations, early attempts at learning a new skill can be highly
disheartening especially when one is doing this publicly, and teaching is very much
a public situation.
It is for this reason that teachers need strong support systems if they are to do
the necessary professional learning to sufficiently master the range of strategies and
skills to eventually reach a stage of adaptive expertise. This is particularly the case
when teachers are grappling with the actual implementation of new practices as they
are in many ways like a novice in any performance situation, often experiencing their
performance as erratic and inconsistent, even anxiety-producing. In this context, it
is not surprising that many teachers stick within their comfort zone. However, in the
longer term, it is no comfort zone.
There is a substantial evidence base of what works and on what basis. Petty (2015),
for example, drawing from the research of Joyce and Showers (2002), suggests, it is
essential to first consider what the training needs should focus on. He suggests the
following approach:
• Teachers democratically ascertain “their most pressing needs” and pose the
question, “What do our results tell us?”
• A set of improvements is drawn up, combined and prioritized until a common goal
(e.g., relating to curriculum, teaching and assessment) is identified, so as to raise
attainment.
(The common goal needs to focus on a process, designed to produce better
outcomes etc. It must affect the student experience if it is going to have an effect.)
Similarly, Timperley et al. (2008) use the term ‘catalyst’ as often being the origi-
nator for driving the direction for professional learning. Catalysts can take the form
of:
• Information showing that current teaching practices are not achieving the desired
outcomes for a group of students
• A lack of shared understanding of an area of practice
• Challenging or ‘problematizing’ current definitions of specific curriculum out-
comes.
9.5 Unpacking the Components of Effective Professional … 307
Again, certain core principles of learning are essential here. Time must be spent
on the necessary retrieval, spaced and deliberate practice. Good understanding and
basic competence will enable motivated teaching professionals to then take a more
self-directed approach towards personal expertise. They suggest that the learning
processes engaged when developing new understandings and skills involve cycles of
(one or more of) the following:
• Process 1: Cueing and retrieving knowledge consolidated and/or examined.
Outcome: Prior knowledge consolidated and/or examined.
• Process 2: Becoming aware of new information/skills and integrating them into
current values and belief system.
Outcome: New knowledge adopted or adapted.
• Process 3: Creating dissonance with current position (values and beliefs).
Outcome: Dissonance resolved (accepted/rejected), current values and belief
systems repositioned, reconstructed.
A wide range of methods, activities and resources are useful in helping teachers
to go through the learning processes outlined above (e.g., Timperley et al. 2008).
These include:
• Listening
• Watching
• Being observed and receiving feedback
• Reading
308 9 Framing Professional Development Now
Over some 30 years or so I have probably been involved in the appraisal of several
thousand teaching/training professionals in a range of educational, vocational and
cultural contexts, and many countries. It was once believed that goldfish, and maybe
other fish also, have a memory span of only a few seconds. However, new research
(not worth referencing) has challenged this and it may be more a question of months.
I sometimes feel like this in recalling my teaching activities. We know that memories
change in some way every time we evoke them, so I am beginning to worry about
the pristineness of some of my recollections. Now that’s over, catharsis not-excuses,
I feel that I can authentically recall a few useful reflections on Reflective Practice.
Many years back, in 1992, I completed a Master of Education degree at the University
of Exeter. During this programme, I remember being introduced to, as well as doing
considerable amounts of reading and thinking about reflective practice, especially
its usefulness as a professional development approach for improving one’s teaching.
Invariably, I read the definitive texts of the time, especially the work of Schön (1983,
1987). The notion that teachers should carefully reflect on their practice, how they
design, conduct and evaluate what they do in classrooms, on what basis, and the
310 9 Framing Professional Development Now
actual impact on student learning made perfect sense. After all, this is what we
would expect professionals to do in any field of practice. In the literature, there are
many definitions on what constitutes reflective practice, for example:
Schön (1983) defined Reflective practice as:
thoughtfully considering your own experiences in applying knowledge to practice.
Even a short analysis and evaluation of these different definitions will inevitably
lead one to make certain obvious inferences and interpretations, as common heuristics
are underpinning all. Learning to do reflective practice is the same as learning to
be competent at any activity, albeit this is a complex activity. One must have the
knowledge bases (e.g., what is reflective practice, how it works, be knowledgeable
and competent/expert in the area of practice—in this case, teaching), as well as being
able to use types of thinking; especially analysis, comparison, making inferences and
interpretations, and evaluation. It also helps if one can generate useful possibilities
for future improvement if needed.
Schön (1983, 1987), is perhaps the person most accredited to the initial framing
of Reflective Practice. Table 9.1, introduces two of his key concepts and what they
entail in actual practice.
Schön saw Reflection-on-action as the more powerful of the two methods, as the
practitioner can take some time recalling the various activities of the lesson, and what
was noticed about the student response (e.g., attention, engagement, task completion,
problems encountered). This coupled with other sources of feedback (e.g., student,
peer observer) can then be subjected to scrutiny, to identify what seemed to work,
and what may need some change in pedagogy for a future lesson in this area.
Reflection-in-action can also be very useful as the teacher can make situated
changes in the instructional strategy if he/she feels that the present method/activities
are not working as planned; hence making possible immediate improvements to the
student learning experience. Invariably, this requires a high level of sensory acuity,
quick thinking and pedagogic competence to do it consistently well.
In working with teachers in training I have always encouraged them to reflect on
their practice. It seemed a given method for self-evaluation and opening-up areas for
future improvement. Hence, asking teachers questions relating to what they thought
went well in the lesson and what areas were open to future development was standard
practice. Over many years of working with different teaching/training professionals,
there has been much variation in how they have responded to being asked (though
more often requested) to do reflective practice, usually as part of the professional
development curriculum they are undertaking. Some seem to find it useful; others
seem only to pay lip service to it. Being honest, in retrospect, in the early years, I
probably did not do much of it particularly well, and there were reasons for this.
Firstly, teachers are busy folk, and finding time for meaningful dialogue was often
difficult as they were more mindful of the next task to be done (teaching or otherwise)
rather than deep meditation and exploration about what has been done ( e.g., the
observed lesson), Often, unless there were serious issues, it was typically the case
previously that such meetings were more of an administrative activity rather than a
rigorous appraisal of different practices, challenges faced and exploration of options
for improvement. Furthermore, despite my background in psychology and many
years of teaching, I was quite frankly, until the past 20 years or so, pedagogically
illiterate in terms of possessing the kind of knowledge and understanding of human
learning that is prevalent across the research fields now. Earlier, we had our tour
in and out of Educational Jurassic Park, and the emergence of EBT as a means of
revolutionizing professionalism. As Powell and Kusuma-Powell (2015) summarized:
We have learned more about how the human brain learns in the last two decades than in the
rest of human history put together. (p. 19)
left quite early in their careers) did develop into excellent teaching professionals—
many winning teacher awards in their respective disciplines. I don’t take much credit
for this; they already had good communication skills and showed genuine care and
concern for students. I enjoyed my work with these teachers.
In contrast, the latter group of teachers, who seemed to have much less impact
in getting attention and engagement, as well as displaying a less organized and
contextualized instructional strategy, we’re often satisfied with what they were doing
and offered relatively little in terms of thoughtful analysis and evaluation. Many
examples capture the range of variation within this broad categorization, but one
lecturer was particularly striking in an almost archetypal framing of a ‘very poor
teacher’. In terms of the core principles of learning, he managed to violate most of
these in around 2-h of classroom observation. The student group comprised around
60+ students aged 16–17. Having observed this class with other teachers, I had
previously noted high levels of attention and engagement, and they seemed keen
to learn and fun to teach. In this situation, however, the majority were completely
disengaged from any participation in the lesson content. I was able to see most
of their laptops, and the screens bore no resemblance to the lesson content. The
boredom was tangible, I could almost feel it. Most worrying, however, was the
perceptions and interactions of the teacher in the post-observation tutorial. I always
invite teachers to offer their perceptions and feelings about the lesson, in the genre
of Cognitive Coaching (e.g., Costa and Garmston 2002). Much to my surprise, at
that time (though I am not surprised now), this teacher felt that the lesson went very
well and was pleased with the way he had taught it. When asked what he would
change if he taught the same lesson again, he responded very clearly—“nothing in
particular”. In such circumstances, emotional intelligence must come quickly in play,
as emotionally I felt somewhat annoyed at what seemed arrogance in that situation.
I asked questions to explore what he thought went well and how he knew this,
thinking this may open up an avenue for productive dialogue. This did not help the
communication process and I could see he was getting more agitated by the moment.
I quickly reassured him that the situation was not a problem, mentioning that this is
a learning experience, though we need to have a conversation about the teaching. I
did pass him my reflections and rationale on the standard teacher observation form
employed at the time and offered to go through this with him now or later. He curtly
replied, “not now”. In summary, I asked him to read through it, have a think, and
we’ll go from there.
I never saw that teacher again, and he may have left the institution soon afterwards.
Had I failed him in some way or was he just not suited to teaching?—I don’t know.
There are certainly people (for whatever reasons) who are just not suited for teaching,
and for the sake of both the students and themselves, are probably best outside of
the profession. In retrospect, I may have done better in terms of support. I am more
aware now that such wide variations in both competences, and reflective ability, can
be explained in large part by research into the differences between novice and expert
teachers (e.g., Timperley et al. 2008) and between experienced and expert teachers
(e.g., Hattie 2003). Timperley et al. (2008) make the following comparison between
novice and expert teachers:
9.7 Reflective Practice 313
The novice is someone who perceives the unfamiliar teaching situation in terms of discrete
elements and, in making use of new skills and knowledge, relies on rules rather than an
integrated vision of the practice. The primary focus is on the self and one’s performance.
As competence develops, the discrete elements become integrated into patterns, with some
aspects becoming automatic and the teacher less reliant on rules. In contrast, experts have
a more holistic grasp of relationships within a particular context and fluidly and efficiently
solve problems as they arise. The resources on which they can draw are much richer. (p. 11)
In summary terms, expert teachers see much more of relevance in the complex
dynamic situation of the classroom setting and the interactions that occur; make
better inferences and interpretations of what is occurring and what specifically needs
to be addressed if things don’t go as initially planned. Invariably, they are also better
at identifying the causes of classroom problems and, most importantly, can design
learning arrangements and experiences that foster better attainment. Many of such
teachers are creative teachers, within the framing heuristics of this book. Hattie
(2009) uses the term “with-it-ness” to summarize much of what such teachers do
differently and better than those less expert in the profession:
Teachers need to have the skills of ‘with-it-ness’ – that is, the ability to identify and quickly
act on potential problems and be aware of what is happening in the class (the proverbial
‘eyes in the back of the head’, or mindfulness). (p. 78)
Indeed, when exploring with the more open and critical faculty on the what and
how they perceive in the experience of teaching it seems to be the case that some
are seeing more and seeing this in better qualitative terms—more ‘with-it’, as Hattie
described. However, this is not surprising, as when I open the bonnet of a car, I see
metal, plastic and some wiring; nothing makes any sense at all. I can fondly recall
a neighbour of mine, who was adamant about fixing his car, even though he had
little knowledge of motor vehicle maintenance. He spent many a weekend in total
frustration, often with bloody fingers, trying to fix things that he had little idea as
to what was wrong and, inevitably, what to do to fix it. I have since learned not to
meddle with such things and ring a local mechanic.
While discovery learning may be fun, can lead to meaningful learning and has
pedagogic value for students in some learning situations, it is of limited value in a
typically busy working day. The mechanic always seems to be able to fix my car,
I guess he’s just lucky! However, I have a deep suspicion, that when he opens the
bonnet, it looks very different than what my neighbour and myself are seeing. Many
factors determine such differences in perceptual acuity and capability, but most stem
from prior learning and competence (or lack of it), as well as a human attribution
bias whereby we think we know more than we do. In most scenarios when there is
a lack of content knowledge and poor thinking, the result is a very limited framing
of that domain area. For many teachers, a lack of solid evidence-based pedagogic
knowledge and some ‘fuzzy’ thinking typically lead to both limited and inaccurate
perceptions and interpretations of what is occurring in their classrooms.
Asking many novice teachers (and some experienced ones also) to do reflective
practice is like asking students to do good thinking when they lack any prior useful
model of what this entails and how it works. Without such knowledge bases, this
314 9 Framing Professional Development Now
Earlier in the chapter, I referred to Powell and Kusuma Powell (2015) notion of
“goodness” in the profession of teaching. In this context, the authors further point
out that:
…when school people do witness exemplary teaching and learning, we often tend to respond
with immediate adulation and subsequent dismissal…We must learn to ‘look for goodness’,
deconstruct it and most importantly learn from it. (p. 26)
Unfortunately, I have only acquired this capability in the past two decades. As
Oscar Wilde said, sadly: “Youth is wasted on the young”, or the old adage,
“Hindsight is 20/20 vision”—if only!
3. A Disposition (e.g., motivation, mindset and integrity) for authentically helping
others achieve personal goals and develop the capability to be self-directed learn-
ers. This requires a high level of professional skills as well as genuine respect
and concern for learners.
Much of reflective practice has failed to recognize how this works, and that’s why
it hasn’t worked as intended. Furthermore, while institutional support and resources
help considerably, these are only—albeit important—support structures. As Levin
(2008) concluded:
…resources alone, however, will not change social practices. Teachers need to see not only
what they might do differently but how they could do it in the reality of their classrooms.
The key to developing this understanding is ongoing work with colleagues – seeing others
carry out new practices with students like yours and having others help you learn to do these
new practices. (p. 86)
Asking teachers to be more creative may be of little value if they don’t know
specifically what this entails in the context of actual teaching practices, and how to
do this effectively. Over the years I have been an observer in many workshops on
creativity. Sadly, while most have been interesting in part and well-intentioned, they
tend to offer generalities more than evidence-based heuristics. Having a passion and
a belief that one is naturally creative and it’s only a question of ‘switching on the
creative switch’ and hey presto ‘creative me’ emerges, is often the message. However,
this is akin to buying that expensive piece of exercise equipment, working out 10 min
a day, with no pain, and within a month or two you’re the ‘body beautiful’, or
whatever. Positive beliefs and thinking are an important aspect of effective learning,
but they don’t inevitably lead to successful learning. Also, while notions of ‘quick
fixes’ in terms of achieving rapid success are attractive, even seductive, it’s not how
successful learning and attainment of anything challenging works for some 99% of
the population. Creativity, as we saw in Chap. 5, is understandable, can be adequately
framed in the context of teaching, and it is a learnable capability. But it is subject to
the same core principles of learning as other human skills.
Reflection on, or in practice, then, without a clear evidence-based framework will
likely result only in partial and limited improvement at best. Willingham (2009)
made the key point concisely:
Education makes better minds, and knowledge of the mind can make better education.
(p. 165)
316 9 Framing Professional Development Now
I could equally pose the question—how do you fix cars well? Firstly, you must
understand how they work, know-how each part contributes to the working of the
sub-systems, and how these affect the functioning of the whole (we could call this
good analysis—of course, with the relevant content knowledge and understanding
of car workings). Secondly, it helps if you have experience across a range of motor
vehicle contexts, especially in solving mechanical problems (e.g., comparison and
contrast with previous experiences of similar problem scenarios) as this will draw out
prior knowledge in long-term memory, and ‘hey presto’, the solution comes to the
conscious mind. Experts often have the answer already nicely encoded neurologically
in those acquired mental models of motor vehicle defects. This helps to make correct
and quick inferences and interpretations of what is wrong and how to fix it. Finally, it
is just then an evaluative question—what are the ways to best fix it for the customer?
(e.g., effectiveness, efficiency, cost). There’s little need for creative thinking unless
the customer wants it to drive like a duck. Similarly, MC may not be a needed
capability in this context, providing the mechanic is well-disciplined to follow the
correct procedures and pay attention throughout—we could call this autonomous
learning and unconscious competence.
Hence, useful EBRP requires teaching professionals to possess Pedagogic Lit-
eracy, Metacognitive Capability and Creative Teaching Competence—to be able to
frame expertise, evaluate it accurately, and improve it. In the past 5 years or so, I have
developed and piloted a range of customized tools that facilitate effective reflective
practice. Do note that these tools are not algorithmic in that they unproblematically
capture all the components or constructs of effective teaching—or what Powell and
Kusuma-Powell (2015) referred to as, “Goodness Criteria”—noted prior.
Teaching is a complex activity, and classrooms are equally complex in terms of
a diverse range of student personalities, biographies and motivational status; the
latter having highly changeable features in the socio-psychological dynamics of
classroom interactions. My analogy with a car, whilst illustrative in context (he says
hopefully), has a major flaw in that the world of engineering is a closed system. It
can be highly complex, and when things go wrong at bad times, mechanical systems
(e.g., cars) can appear to ‘have minds of their own’, a bit like ‘Murphy’s Law’—but
it is still a closed system. John Cleese, a famous English comedian, who starred in
the television series Fawlty Towers, a British television sitcom broadcast on BBC2 in
1975 & 1979, cleverly and brutally demonstrates this non-rational perceived ability
of mechanical systems (in this case his car) to deliberately annoy him (i.e., break
down on the road) at the most inopportune time and circumstance. Cleese firstly
shouts at his car threatening it with punishment, but the car will not start on priming.
He eventually gets out of his car, finds a tree branch and beats the car with it. Did the
car subsequently decide to work? You may need to access the YouTube video to find
out. Similarly, when we see top tennis players smash their expensive rackets on the
court, it is as though they blame the racket for missing an easy shot. I have yet to meet
a metacognitive tennis racket. Mine made many errors, but I think it was all down to
9.8 How to Do Evidence-Based Reflective Practice (EBRP) 317
my lack of competence. Indeed, I did not possess a high-end expensive graphite racket
(or whatever they are now made of), so I have some leverage in terms of attribution
of blame. The same excuse may not work so well with the top professionals who,
without doubt, have access to the best tennis rackets that can be made. Teaching is
heuristic and it is an open system, which means that its components interact and create
synergistic effects, which are never completely predictable. Highly effective teams
are a good example of this, in that such teams are more than the sum of the parts, and
explains why teams, who are ‘on-paper’ not considered to be good enough to win
major championships, sometimes do win these. Leicester City winning the English
Premier League Championship in 2016 was a classic example. In the previous year,
they just avoided relegation from the Premier League in England and were 5000-1
with bookmakers for winning this title in 2016. However, as we know, real soccer
matches are not played on paper—its grass, right?
Useful heuristics are much better than fashion and fad, and the tools do not pre-
scribe a rigid method of application—they are flexible and designed to be used for
capturing interesting practice experiences—whether they are good, bad—even ugly.
They are certainly not form filling-in administrative activities—which turns teachers
off. However, they provide an essential structure and focus for capturing experience
that can subsequently be subjected to evidence-based pedagogic analysis—with good
thinking of course. Table 9.2, is an exemplar that focuses practitioners attention to
the employment of the core principles of learning as the organizing heuristics for
systematic reflection on practice.
The following example is an EBRP tool, used by 7 teacher-researchers over a
15-week module for a 2-year Ministry of Education, Singapore Tertiary Educa-
tion Research Fund project, which systematically applied Evidence-Based Teaching
(EBT) methods and learning principles to ascertain their impact on students’ intrinsic
motivation. The research used quantitative methods to measure levels of engagement
(e.g., behavioural, emotional, cognitive and agentic) and self-efficacy. Most impor-
tantly, it employed students as co-participants in the learning process to gain a deep
and ongoing insight into how students were experiencing their lecturers teaching over
an extended time duration (Sale et al. 2018). The research was able to make qualita-
tive comparisons between the teacher’s intent and pedagogic interventions with the
actual student learning experience. Table 9.3, is one used by a teacher-researcher for
2 weeks of the intervention.
The teacher-researchers in this project were all experienced teachers, including
4 who have the role of Academic Mentors, responsible for implementing pedagogic
initiatives at school level as well as collaboration in learning units across schools.
Most had received teaching awards and all were familiar with EBT. The following
are samples of their reflection on conducting EBRP and using the EBRP tool:
I have to admit that if not for the commitment to the research team, I would not be self-
disciplined enough to perform my weekly post-lesson reflective log. I can firmly state that
it was worth the effort and time. Because of the need to conduct ongoing reflective practice,
it was not just that I had to focus on my content delivery but at the same time constantly
trying to observe the responses of students at my interventions. The responses from students
are it in the positive or negative served as indicators for me to continue to adapt, modify
Table 9.2 Generic EBRP tool
318
Formative assessment of student learning and provision of quality two-way • Monitoring student learning through testing key concepts and skills
feedback? • Providing clear and specific feedback (e.g., task, process,
self-management)
• Encouraging two-way and peer feedback where possible and useful
(continued)
319
Table 9.2 (continued)
320
Opportunities for practice to enhance understanding and/or skill acquisition? • Retrieval practice to check key conceptual understanding
• Spaced practice across sessions to build understanding and competence
• Deliberate practice focused on specific skill development tailored to
student’s skill levels
Interactions/activities that foster a climate conducive for building rapport, • Use of growth mindset strategies (e.g., showing how effort impacts
encouraging success and a sense of fun? learning; sticking with students when they need help; mastery learning)
• Use of expressive language and supporting body language in
communication (e.g. expressive tone, smile, eye contact)
• Allowing/facilitating humour and fun to occur in the lesson
9 Framing Professional Development Now
Table 9.3 Example of a completed EBRP tool
Evidence-based reflective practice tool
Weeks 1 & 2: Oct 16–27, 2017
In the learning experience was there Evidence of effectiveness
What specific strategies, methods and/or resources were employed, and how
effective were they? (Based on your observation and any other feedback if
available (e.g., peer observation, student feedback)
Use of autonomy supportive style strategies? Use expressive facilitating language—this takes a bit of effort initially
• Clear expressive facilitating language However, did use plenty (at least in my own mind) of explanatory rationales
• Provides explanatory rationales Acknowledge students feeling—they certainly do not like to read, so I
• Acknowledge & accepts negative affect elaborated on the use of jigsaw approach (see later) to spread out the
• Displays patience workload. Also emphasized that in an increasingly complex word, it is ever
• Explores and allows student choice in the overall instructional strategy more important to read and think critically to make sense of available
• Two-way feedback to support understanding and skill development information
Patience—working on this! Set ground rules about talking in class… so far,
manageable
Giving students choices—did clarify that some choices are available, esp. in
terms of doing extra reading and optional exercises. Emphasised if they
make the effort to do, I will make the effort to mark and give them feedback
9.8 How to Do Evidence-Based Reflective Practice (EBRP)
2-way feedback: Informal chit-chat with those who came to class early on
how they feel about the module so far. Most said manageable—but this is
perhaps still early in the semester before the hardcore stuff kicks in
(continued)
321
Table 9.3 (continued)
322
Emphasis on key concepts and Principles that underpin understanding of this Week 2: Key concepts of inherently safer design (ISD) was emphasized
topic? repeatedly, in particular that it will be most effective when applied at the
R&D and process development stage before the chemical plant is built. This
is illustrated using the Bhopal Gas Disaster as a case study. Also the fact that
the chemical MIC was stored in excessive manner (as opposed to the ISD
strategy of MINIMIZE) appear to leave a strong impression among students,
especially when they were informed that the storage tanks measured 40-ft in
length and 8-ft in diameter—enough for an adult person to walk inside!
Some are able to recall the dimensions when asked!
(continued)
323
Table 9.3 (continued)
324
Use of activities that involved good thinking to facilitate understanding? “Slide show” of 10 real-world practices are shown one at a time, and
students are to discuss in their groups and provide an answer what inherently
safer design strategy is employed. In general, they did well. Some even
challenged—on 2 occasions—the answer given by lecturer. A lively
discussion ensued, which prompted the lecturer to consider an aspect of the
answer from the students’ viewpoint. Overall, they appear to develop a good
understanding of the subject
In the Google Slide mix-and-match case (how modifications made with
good intentions ended up introducing new hazards—intended consequences)
students need to identify the modifications made, link it to the rationale (at
the time decision was taken), and how it led to unintended consequences.
Overall, most of them got the linkages correct, with 1 or 2 groups with all
correct answers
Variation in the modes and methods of information presentation and Yes, first 2-h session: first students need to enter answers into Google Doc
interaction? (what went wrong at which stage of lifecycle, and what inherently safer
strategy can be used to address the issue), next the Google Slide
mix-and-match. Next 2-h, involve watching a short video to understand the
Amine Treating process, followed by more detailed “jigsaw” reading of more
detailed write-up of the process, and answer 14 TRUE/FALSE questions on
Socrative. Students are seen discussing among themselves in their respective
groups. All results are viewed in real-time; and it appears that most students
are able to answer most of them. Due to time running out, not able to do the
MCQs part in class, and students are asked to attempt them on their own
Application of practices consistent with human memory processes (e.g., Chunking done for online video of mini-lectures (each 3–5 min) focusing on
chunking of content to minimize cognitive overload; rehearsal/review one key concept per video. Lecturer first use a video to explain overall set-up
activities)? of Amine Treating process and the different units that made up the process.
Jigsaw reading was then carried out in-class where each student within a
group is given a unit (process description, and drawing) to understand, and
collectively they work together to answer the TRUE/FALSE Socrative
questions. Generally, results of class 04 better than class 03
(continued)
9 Framing Professional Development Now
Table 9.3 (continued)
Incorporation of formative assessment to provide quality two-way feedback? Feedback given on Google Doc, Google Slide activities mentioned earlier.
Selected items were clarified in class, others were added to Google Doc after
class, and informed students during the next class. Follow-up explains given
Tue (to class 2B/03), and Wed (to class 2B/04) on queries noted on Mon (for
2B/03 and 04 present)
Use of deliberate practice to enhance understanding and/or skill acquisition? Not able to cover a Google Doc open-ended questions in class—14 in total,
designed to test/ enhance understanding of the Amine Treating Process. This
was left for students to try on their own. Explained that these exercises are
optional, and the benefits of trying them, i.e. able to build a stronger
foundation that helps tackling later topics. Will monitor the Google Doc
entry for any attempt at answering them
Interactions/activities to foster a climate conducive for building rapport, I think so—students sit with their groups (formed by themselves) for all
encouraging success and a sense of fun? lessons. These classes tend to be noisy already to begin with. FUN I think the
students certainly have. Students tend to offer witty remarks all the time in
the 2 weeks with them. A few attempts (covered elsewhere, or everywhere)
in these submissions was aimed at encouraging success; but may not be able
to quantify these—how to measure ‘success’ besides scoring well for tests
An aspect(s) of creativity (e.g., story, humour, activity, presentation style, Jokes used, and funny pictures embedded into PPT slides
9.8 How to Do Evidence-Based Reflective Practice (EBRP)
example) that significantly enhanced motivation in this learning experience? Real world examples added as appropriate, either from case studies or from
own work experience
Overall class is lively, even boisterous. Thus far, able to manage ‘crowd
control’ and students do pipe down when important matters are discussed.
Not sure if these enhanced the learning experience though
325
326 9 Framing Professional Development Now
or even drop certain style features or methods of teaching during this 15-week programme.
It was very fulfilling and encouraging when I came to know that the students were able to
understand the reasons or purposes of something I have done or created for them during
lessons. I also feel much more capable and confident in my role as Academic Mentor, in
that I can better understand more specifically what teaching challenges some of our faculty
are personally facing in their teaching and can provide specific and useful feedback on what
might help them meet these. It enables me to go beyond personal experience and have a
sense of scientific backing to what I am doing in a coaching/mentoring context.
In using the Evidence-Based Reflective Practice Tool, this provided a clear structure for
evaluating my teaching in very specific terms and being able to change/modify aspects of
strategy on the spot in the lesson context, if I felt it needed such action. I noticed that when I
taught the same topic 2–3 times in a week or so to different groups, I felt that I am improving
session by session. It made me very metacognitive to what I was doing, and over time it
becomes somewhat automatic.
The EBRP tool, which involved a bit more work initially, helped to focus my planning and
post-lesson reflection. It will be useful for teachers who genuinely want to improve their
practice. After a few sessions, it became less time consuming, and as it is a flexible tool, not
a form-filling in exercise – it was not burdensome.
Reflective practice using the EBRP tool kept me checking and thoughtfully planning the
lesson structure and sequences and ensuring that I was both focused on the learning goals
and the students’ needs weekly. Over time, this became easier and easier, as I become
increasingly familiar with the design process that is the basis of EBT – although it is still a
challenge to make all lesson parts interesting for students.
Doing reflective practice using the EBRPT was both easy to use and helped me to think
more carefully about the structure of my lessons and how I might make the learning both
easier for the students to deal with the content and get them more engaged and interested.
I also noticed that by using EBT the students were giving more comprehensive answers to
questions. Overall, I felt that I was able to get a good relationship with the students and that
they were learning better.
A little knowledge can save one’s life or it can be a perpetual nemesis. For folk
who had prior knowledge, and especially experience, when the signs of a tsunami
become apparent, could take the quick evasive action of going upland and probably
be safe. In contrast, the unfortunate folk who had no knowledge (or did not believe
those who told them to run and go upland), especially if they went down to the
beach to explore this strange occurrence, would perish for the lack of this small bit
of factual knowledge. Similarly, parenting, which is a much-contested issue, raises
questions about useful and not-so-useful—even dangerous—knowledge. The so-
called ‘snowflake’ generation (e.g., Winget 2017; Piper 2017) are not the result of
brain mutation; numerous factors are impactful here, but certain parenting practices
are likely to contribute to such dispositions. I won’t pursue this further here, but it
will have implications for teaching, learning and well-being, especially for teachers
seeking to develop self-directed learners. SDL requires much in the way of volition,
9.9 An Evidence-Based Frame on EBRP 327
persistence, grit, or whatever is the vogue term at the time. Snowflakes don’t have
much of this if I read the analogy correctly.
The use of the EBRP tool, as noted, cannot capture all the important interactional
features in a classroom lesson. The teacher, even with the help of a trained observer,
may miss certain events that may be significant for enhancing understanding of the
learning experience for students. However, there is much of merit in expert peer
appraisal, in doing reflective practice, especially from an EBT perspective. There
is always subjectively, and the teacher must recognize this. Systemic perceptual
and attribution biases, as reinforced extensively in this work, abound in the case of
human apprehension (e.g., Kahneman 2012). There is a tendency to attribute blame
to external factors in the environment, including other people. Let’s be honest, how
often do we see people, when something has gone wrong, immediately stand up and
say, “it’s my mistake, I am responsible for this mess.” Most people will acknowledge
some personal blame if the evidence is striking, but the tendency to attribute some
of it elsewhere is prevalent. In 3 decades of coaching underperforming teachers
(defined in terms of consistently low feedback scores), few acknowledged their role
in producing such results. Only a handful said something akin to ‘Maybe I need to
improve my teaching’. In most cases, a range of external factors was identified as
the basis of their feedback. These included the students they had to teach (e.g., these
are ‘poor’ students); the curriculum they had to teach (e.g., not well written, obsolete
content); and time constraints (e.g., too much other work to do), etc. Changing
their attributions, apart from building trust and some rapport, was always the initial
challenging task.
EBRP does provide an approach and a methodology to both diagnose lesson
effectiveness and predict teaching and learning possibilities through a systematic,
focused and more objective base for understanding the experience of learning. It
applies both Reason and Science (e.g. Pinker 2019), as best as we can presently
frame it. As Treadwell (2017) summarizes in the wider context:
Our ability to reflect on our practice underpins all professional associations. If the medical
profession stopped looking for best practice, then the role of technologies and the realign-
ment of how hospitals met their purpose in the 21st century would not have resulted in the
transformation of hospital services over the last 20 years.
Education has just begun this process and it will take the same passionate determination to
ensure that every school is developing each learner’s competencies, enabling their ability to
learn – anything, anywhere, with anyone, at any time. (p. 195)
In the context of teacher education, EBRP makes possible more meaningful and
productive dialogues that can authentically change teachers’ mindsets—essentially
beliefs and perception, and eventually feelings. In using the EBRP tools, it is essential
that teachers who are asked/told to do reflective practise, for whatever reason (e.g.,
part of a formal course on teaching; been referred for mentoring/coaching due to
low student feedback scores; a new institutional requirement relating to performance
management goals) are inducted into the main tenets and practices underpinning
EBT. Without a strong knowledge base relating to what constitutes effective practice
and the underpinning learning science, there is likely to be a very limited analysis
328 9 Framing Professional Development Now
Teachers using EBRP will be able to both better predict the likely outcomes from their
lesson planning, as well as make a more accurate diagnosis of them after the event.
They will be doing what Schön referred to as reflection on action; the difference now
is that such reflection will not be “post hoc Justification” as Hattie (2009) alluded.
This approach has certain similarities with Lesson Study (e.g., Stigler and Hiebert
1999), a Japanese innovation that has proved highly successful in terms of effective,
efficient and engaging lesson creation. The approach starts with identifying teaching
methods/activities that seem to work best with students (based on observations and
student feedback), followed by the design of instructional strategies incorporating
these proposed methods/activities. The lessons are then piloted with various teach-
ers over time with peer appraisal. The idea is that the best methods are scrutinized
over time, with changes/modifications, and improvements being made. This process
typically continues until the teachers involved feel that they have sufficiently vali-
dated the lesson in terms of its potential for being an effective, efficient and engaging
instructional strategy for that topic area, student profile, and the desired learning
outcomes to be met.
Even a basic pedagogic analysis of Lesson Study, reveals that it involves much
by way of EBT practices and principles of learning:
9.10 EBRP, Lesson Study and Active Schemes of Work 329
• Researching the behaviour, interests and experiences of the learning group, finding
out how well they are learning (as well as not learning), activating prior knowl-
edge, and asking their learning preferences in terms of instructional methods (e.g.,
Autonomy Supporting style)
• Identifying levels of attainment and engagement (e.g., behavioural, emotional,
cognitive and agentic) as a basis for instructional design
• Designing instructional strategies for topics/learning outcomes—based on this
research
• Using spaced and deliberate practice with expert feedback for evaluating the
various components of the lesson in terms of student attainment and engagement
• Encouraging metacognition through all stages of the implementation process
(e.g., setting clear challenging goals, planning strategy, monitoring and reviewing
strategy, as well as evaluation and extending learning).
A similar pedagogic structure is that of Active Schemes of Work where partic-
ipating teachers collectively produce a plan of a wider structure of learning for a
programme (e.g., module or unit) and continually improve the instructional design
of the various lesson components (e.g., method blends, activities, examples, tech-
nology supports) over time. Typically, a scheme of work is a structured summary
breakdown of the whole module or unit, focusing on:
• The specific learning outcomes relating to the key learning goals or topic areas
for the particular curriculum programme. These are organized in terms of the
best sequencing and appropriately allocated to each of the lessons comprising the
programme
• A preferred instructional strategy (e.g., methods, activities, resources) as well
as other supporting data for each of the lessons (e.g., details and timelines for
completion of assignments, notes relating to infusing related process skills such
as thinking, learning strategies)
• Details and necessary guidance on the assessment to be used at specific stages and
activities in the programme (both summative and formative).
Petty’s (2015) framing of Active Schemes of Work advocates that these essential
planning organizers should be used dynamically by all the teaching team not only to
initially identify best evidence-based instructional strategies but continually improve
them, similar to that of the Lesson Study approach. By explicitly making the schemes
of work dynamic and part of a collaborative improvement process, this energizes
all the teaching team to actively input into the lesson design, especially activities
likely to be most effective for enhancing engagement and attainment for the student
groups. This is an ongoing process, and when driven by an EBT approach, reviewed
by ongoing feedback and peer discussion, can significantly improve the learning
experience for students. Active Schemes of Work provide the perfect context for
faculty skill development and creativity, They also provide a great way to foster
relationship building and motivation for the faculty involved. Petty summarizes the
rationale and range of benefits as follows:
330 9 Framing Professional Development Now
• Active learning works. Research shows that active learning is by far the best for
recall, student enjoyment, deep learning (full understanding), and for correcting
the learners’ misunderstandings.
• It improves results. School improvement research shows that teachers have about
three times the effect on achievement as their managers. So, achievement and
students’ life chances can only be improved if teaching is improved.
• It is likely to get a commitment to improvement. Subject centred discussion on
how to teach well is at the heart of a teacher’s role, teachers usually enjoy being
involved in practical development in their subject area.
• Teams share best practice. So, the best teaching methods are available to all.
• It raises expectations of teaching quality. Active schemes of work can raise
expectations of what it means to teach well, as well as showing how this can be
done.
• It ‘stores’ best practice. Good teachers who leave the college leave behind their
methods for others to benefit from and enjoy.
• It supports beginning teachers. Novice teachers are given effective methods to
adopt and to learn from.
• It promotes professional development. Writing the scheme promotes subject
centred discussion on effective teaching and so develops staff.
9.11 Coaching
Teachers as learners are subject to the same learning principles and constraints as
their students, especially when they are learning new knowledge and skills. In this
situation they have to deal with acquiring new knowledge, building understanding,
and doing the necessary practice, as well as dealing with their existing beliefs and
emotional responses to change. While they may have greater experience and maturity
in self-regulation and metacognitive strategies than many of their students, there is
still much cognitive and emotional effort needed to build the necessary understanding
and competence. The importance of being in a learning relationship and context that
is congruent with the Core Principle of Learning: A psychological climate is created
which is both success-orientated and fun is equally applicable to teachers as learners.
Coaching has become an increasingly popular term in the language of training
and professional development. Previously, the term was typically used in other fields,
especially in sport. We rarely hear terms like a teacher of soccer; soccer coach seems
to be the norm. However, does not a soccer coach also teach soccer? Definitely.
I have been coached in soccer and have coached soccer for many years. In both
situations, there is an emphasis on improving performance, which entails aspects
of learning. Furthermore, this learning is not just knowledge and skill-based, but
often involves attitudinal components such as states of mind, dispositions, emotions
and how to manage them. These affective components, such as emotional and belief
management (e.g., mindset formation and maintenance) may be as important as the
9.11 Coaching 331
skill components. How many times have we seen professional sportspeople massively
underperform (e.g., appear to freeze) at the most crucial stages in major events (e.g.,
championship point—even breakpoints—in a tennis match; miskick a penalty in a
world cup soccer tournament). Hence, coaching is more than skill development but
developing competence/expertise, and this is more than knowledge and skills per se,
as we explored in some detail in Chap. 5.
There are many terms in the literature relating to ‘helping people to learn’ and
‘enhancing performance’, one could easily write a significant text on this alone—
there are probably some already. For example, there is sometimes confusion over
the terms of mentoring and coaching. In practice, mentoring is a wide-ranging term,
which while focusing on helping a person to understand themselves better, usually
in relation to their professional field (e.g., dealing with a significant challenge in a
present role or decision-making on changing roles or career), it can involve a range
of sub-roles (e.g., model, sponsor, adviser, teacher, counsellor)—even coach. There
are similar points of comparison and contrast with notions of teacher, trainer—even
facilitator. Does not a teacher train, and a trainer teach? Much is an issue of framing
preferences, though the historical ‘baggage’ seems to have perceived training as
focusing more on specific skills development, whereas teaching is wider and more
holistic. There is a present saying, and I have no idea of its source, but it has become
somewhat of a vogue term, “The teacher is no longer the sage on the stage but
the guide in the guide”. Simplistic, naïve, and certainly not useful. Expert teachers
can train people, facilitate learning and mentor and coach—that’s how I see it. For
example, in the modern context, the full range of facilitation skills should be part
of the skill repertoire of expert teachers. For example, they should be competent in
using such technical skills as:
• Listening actively
• Observing carefully
• Using active learning methods and process tools
• Asking and responding to questions
• Paraphrasing
• Giving & receiving feedback
• Staying neutral
• Testing assumptions
• Staying on track
• Synthesizing ideas
• Providing summaries
• Collecting and organizing ideas/knowledge generated.
There are many definitions of coaching in the literature. I particularly like that of
Costa and Garmston (2002) as it offers a process orientated frame and is consistent
with my experiences over a few decades:
Coaching serves as a foundation for continuous learning by mediating another’s capacity to
reflect before, during and after practice. (p. 23)
332 9 Framing Professional Development Now
then (call it their jargon if you like). Now don’t knock jargon, it may mean that the
operation your need will, in practice, be done properly. Somehow in teaching, even
using the term pedagogy scares some teachers, let alone that many-headed monster
‘metacognition’. Of course, we don’t want jargon for jargon-sake, but essential ter-
minology that captures key aspects of the learning process will facilitate rather than
hinder collaborative teacher learning and professional development.
For example, when I coach (train or mentor) teachers, I ensure that we have a com-
mon understanding of such terms as, activating prior knowledge, cognitive overload,
task and process feedback, retrieval, spaced and deliberate practice, interleaving,
mental models, specific types of thinking, etc.—and what is important about these
terms in the context of teaching practices and student learning.
The importance of an explicit content-specific language is what makes possible
the diagnosis and predictive capability of learning events from a more objective base.
In the context of coaching, as Downey (2003) summarized:
…language is what allows the client to be self-generating, and the practice that makes it
possible for the client to be a long-term excellent performer. (p. 9)
The goals of coaching can also be framed in either very specific terms or in wider
developmental terms, which depend on the goals/needs of the person being coached
and the context of learning. Using a personal example may illustrate the very specific
end of a continuum. As I do many overseas workshops often involving several days,
there is usually a closing ceremony that has a social/fun component to it—typically
a meal, speeches and some games, etc. I am often asked to do something as the
trainer/facilitator. Now doing a speech is not difficult, and I can tell jokes and stories
for some time—too long according to my wife. However, when asked to sing, and
this often happens in Asian countries, the alarm bells go off in my head—this scares
me. The reason is simple, I am a novice singer—ok, I’ll be honest—I am a dreadful
singer. Hence, I decided, over a decade ago, to have some singing lessons, but with
one specific objective: to sing one song not too badly. Let’s not get into what level of
competence this is, but I would not get through any first round of a singing contest
in this sector of the universe. In summary, I spent around $400 on being coached
to sing The Green, Green Grass of Home, a song made famous by Tom Jones. I
had no interest in developing self-directedness in learning or music appreciation,
but simply to get through any song without total embarrassment. Well, I did learn
to sing the song a bit better than prior, through some development in breathing and
voice control techniques, as well as spaced and deliberate practice. However, I have
yet to be offered a singing contract, and according to my wife, it still sounds “quite
dreadful”. I don’t lose any sleep over this, and my overall self-efficacy has not been
impaired. I accept that this is not a skill set that I have much by way of innate
aptitude. For me to develop even a foundational level competence (however defined)
would involve more practice and volition than I am prepared to give this goal. In
terms of intrinsic motivation and cost-benefit analysis—it’s not worth it to me. If
pushed into singing a song somewhere, I do my limited version of The Green, Green
Grass of Home, and generally, get away with a modicum of embarrassment. Positive
comparisons with Tom Jones are unusual.
334 9 Framing Professional Development Now
Before exploring the stages, strategies and techniques for achieving a productive
outcome in coaching relationships, it is useful to explore some fundamental under-
pinning assumptions about human interaction and communication. Coaching, like
teaching, involves an investment in terms of attaining a trusting relationship that
supports good rapport. All approaches to coaching place an important emphasis on
understanding the reality of the situation as perceived and experienced by the client.
This applies to teaching, mentoring and especially psychotherapy. The basic assump-
tion is that human behaviour is in no small part determined by a person’s perceptions
and that a change in perception and thought is a prerequisite to a change in behaviour.
Hence, in coaching teachers, it is essential to get a clear understanding of their per-
ceptions, thoughts and feelings about the learning situation they are involved in. This
is especially the case where teachers are referred to a coach for improvement, as this
can involve emotive aspects.
The field of neuro-linguistic programming (NLP) has provided much insight into
‘the structure of subjective experience’ (e.g., Bandler and Grinder 1990; Dilts 1980),
and how individuals make sense of their experience and interact with others, much of
this occurring subliminally. While neuroscience focuses on brain functioning at the
level of neuronal networks and neurotransmitters in the brain in response to different
environmental stimuli, NLP has sought to explain aspects of how the mind works at
the level of subjective experience. This is particularly interesting as there is often the
assumption that the mind is simply the result of what the brain is doing. This is far
from the case in terms of present understandings. Neuroscientists are increasingly
identifying correlations and causal links between electrical currents in the brain and
various subjective experiences, but how these billions of electrical signals result in
9.12 The Structure of Subjective Experience … 335
a mind that experiences a stream of consciousness that has specific feelings such as
I am angry or I am happy, is not known (Harari 2016). In his words:
As of 2016, we have absolutely no idea. (p. 128).
However, he goes on to argue that what we can be sure of is that the stream
of consciousness we directly experience in every moment is the surest thing in the
world. As he illustrates:
You cannot doubt its existence. Even when we are consumed by doubt and ask ourselves:
‘Do subjective experiences exist?’ we can be certain that we are experiencing doubt. (p. 123)
While each person has his/her individualized inner view of reality (Map in NLP
terms), the essential processes of the way that the map is constructed is generic and
understandable. Dilts went so far as to argue that:
When the confusions and complexities of life experiences are examined, sorted and untan-
gled, what remains is a set of behavioural elements and rules that aren’t too difficult to
understand at all. (p. 5)
336 9 Framing Professional Development Now
For NLP then, a major focus is on understanding how the brain structures the
inner world of subjective experience and, from such understanding, how we might
influence human perception and action in more productive ways. The structure of
subjective experience from an NLP perspective is summarized in Fig. 9.2.
For purposes of translation, The Territory represents the external world, which
is experienced through our senses. However, the external world (The territory) does
not get represented as it is in our internal world—The Map. The external world
cannot be understood separate from the interpretations we put on it, as each individ-
uals’ interpretation of the external world is always mediated by existential Filters,
which process the sensory information from the territory—the main ones being our
belief systems, language, memory and personality traits. The result of this process
of interpretation is that we construct our inner world (map) along with the deletions,
distortions and generalizations that result from this filtering process. What is most
significant here is that the way we experience the world, and our actions about that
experience, gives us the feeling of objectivity (e.g., this is the real world), but in fact,
it is a very personalized subjective framing of the world. In NLP terms, the Map is
not the Territory, only our representation of it.
From an NLP perspective, beliefs are the most significant neurological filter deter-
mining how we perceive and experience external reality. Furthermore, our percep-
tions of reality will determine both our thinking and behaviour. In Chap. 3, we consid-
ered in some detail how beliefs impact student learning, motivation and well-being.
The same theoretical perspective applies to the learning, motivation and well-being
of teachers in a coaching situation.
Indeed, differences in beliefs may represent man’s biggest threat to survival. In
the context of teaching and learning, a major barrier to successful learning for many
students (and some teachers) are limiting beliefs about intelligence, self and the
nature of learning itself. It is to be emphasized that maps are not just theoretical
constructs, they are the internal realities of all of us as we try to make sense of the
world we live in and find personal meaning for living a life. Furthermore, maps can
both assist us in our search for personal success and meaning as well as constitute
the biggest barrier to such fulfilment and well-being. In a nutshell, some maps are
better than others—much better. As Hall (2001) argues:
The richer our map, the more accurate, adequate, and useful our menu, the more choices.
The more impoverished our model, the fewer choices. The richer and fuller our linguistic
map, the richer our mind…Maps induce states, and states govern perception and behaviour.
(pp. 26–27)
9.12 The Structure of Subjective Experience … 337
Hence, the coach is involved in helping clients better understand how they are
thinking, feeling and acting, to help them to enhance the capacity for better think-
ing, decision-making and self-regulation. The effective coach is the mediator in this
process, helping to “promote behavioural changes towards more effective practice”
(Costa and Garmston 2016, p. 9).
It is not surprising that human communication, thinking and behaviour is often
messy, confusing and not productive to effective learning and well-being. Rational-
ity seems to be largely trumped by unconscious biological processes that challenge
notions of free will (e.g., Harris 2010). However, while humans are individually
unique in terms of their personality configurations, biology and experiences, there is
much similarity, which is to be expected given our universal brain morphology, human
needs, and the way the mind works in terms of the dynamics of subjective experience.
Hence, while the content of streams of consciousness can vary greatly, and they may
be framed as productive or unproductive for learning and well-being, there will be
streams of consciousness that can be understood and dealt with. For Costa and Garm-
ston (2002) a cognitive coach can play the important role of helping another person
to take appropriate action toward his or her goals, while simultaneously helping that
person to develop expertise in planning, reflecting, problem-solving, and decision
making. (p. 13).
In coaching, as in all human interaction situations, there are key touchpoints in
which perceptions and feelings can be most poignant. Given the many ways in which
biases can affect perception, feelings and behaviour, awareness of such touchpoints,
and skillful communication, can mediate perception more favourably towards build-
ing trust and rapport—essential for getting productive results with other people. For
example, in customer service, a key touchpoint is the first encounter—the Primacy
effect—and we know what can happen here. Being ignored or having a curt tone of
voice, or ‘that look’ (I can’t be bothered-look) can frame the nature and form of what
is to follow. I have lived and worked in Singapore for over 24 years, and I am still
amazed at the courtesy and friendliness of most locals. It is encouraged in schools
and the local community, and cynics may call it social engineering. All of culture
338 9 Framing Professional Development Now
and socialization is social engineering, and I would argue that some memes are better
than others—much, much better.
In coaching teachers, there are key touchpoints, which include the first meeting,
dialogues post-practice observations, helping clients solve problems and framing
future scenarios. How one conducts oneself is fundamental to how effective the
mediation of coaching is. The following are some reflections on key touchpoints
in the coaching process as well as key skills and techniques for building trust and
rapport in facilitating self-directedness in clients (teachers or others).
The terminology may vary, but the following represent broad interrelated stages that
may be reiterated over time, depending on outcomes and perceptions:
1. Planning Stage
2. Action Stage
3. Reflection Stage.
This can vary in how it arises. For example, in coaching clients, you may have
had some prior experience with them, which may have created some preconceptions
about you, which may or may not be favourable, and also based on very idiosyncratic
data. I still find it amazing how people talk about celebrities, that they have never
met, and make detailed evaluative judgements about what they are like as people—
whether good, bad or ugly. It’s as though they don’t know that many celebrities hire
public relation professionals to manage their media image presentation. Terms like
‘fake-news’ are now part of media-speak. On what basis would the general public
know what is true and false on things that they don’t have any direct contact with.
It’s hard enough to know what is true and false even in one’s territory, as much will
be determined by our neurological filters and the various cognitive biases that are
systemic to human brain functioning. To push the point, but not too far in this context,
how well do we know ourselves, and what exactly is the self? Kahneman (2012) puts
this in stark perspective when he asserts that:
The notion that we have limited access to the workings of our minds is difficult to accept
because, naturally it is alien to our experience, but it is true: you know far less about yourself
than you feel you do. (p. 52)
Equally, they may know very little about you and will go through the Primacy
effect of first impressions, and a myriad of factors—both conscious and uncon-
scious—will come in to play in terms of how they perceive you at this point—also
potentially positive or negative.
As a teacher, coach, or mentor, the same processes play out; hence I am especially
mindful of my presence, voice, tone and manner to mitigate negative perceptions,
9.13 The Stages of Coaching 339
It is very much an ‘allowing’ and receptive form of experiencing, such that when people are
more mindful, they are more accepting of what they experience without focusing, resisting,
or manipulating it. (p. 257)
There is an old saying, “people like people like themselves” and there is a reason for
this; it’s because they have a good rapport. They have things in common like interests,
views on key aspects of reality, what constitutes fun, similar rules of engagement—
in the language of NLP, ‘similar personal maps about the territory’. Neuroscience
is increasing showing how different interpersonal experience trigger physical and
emotional changes in the brain, either promoting or inhibiting open and trusting
interactions with others (e.g., Glaser 2014). Costa and Garmston (2016) go as far as
saying:
In rapport, brains synchronize, crossing the skull and skin barriers to beat as one. (p. 46)
In terms of friendships, rapport typically occurs (or does not occur) naturally and
largely unconsciously. I have not seen (or should I say heard) people say, “Hey we are
building rapport here, and that’s because we are doing x, y, & z”. Quite simply, they
are sharing streams of consciousness from their experiences and their maps about
things in the territory and are being stimulated in some positive way cognitively
and/or emotionally—the latter often being it is fun and includes some humour.
In a coaching situation, rapport needs to be coaxed somewhat, and this takes skill.
As coaches, we want to get rapport, as we know we can be more useful to clients’
needs. However, clients must perceive us as trustworthy and an ally in helping them
meet their goals. Building trust and rapport can take time, though losing it can be
immediate and based on what may appear a trivial act or a misperception. There is
an analogy, that it only takes one cockroach in a bowl of beautiful cherries to ruin its
appeal; however, one beautiful cherry does nothing to reduce the disgust of a bowel
of cockroaches.
Another tenet of NLP that I have found useful relates to linguistic preferences,
and how one can understand aspects of a person’s subjective experience to cre-
ate synchronicity that facilitates communication ease and rapport building sublimi-
nally. For example, it postulates that people tend to have preferred Representational
Systems (See Table 9.4) which incorporate specific Linguistic Predicates in their
conversational language to convey meaning.
Skill in recognising patterns in linguistic terminology and body language helps
to understand people’s maps and state of mind. For example, people with a strong
visual preference will tend to use terms like, “let’s image”, “we are drawing a blank
342 9 Framing Professional Development Now
here”, etc. In contrast, more kinesthetically orientated people are more likely to use
terms like, “let’s get a grip of”, “it strikes me”, etc. In a nutshell—am I revealing a
linguistic cue here? —people respond better to those who “speak their language”.
Also, this typically involves good sensory acuity and mindfulness.
It is through understanding clients’ representational system preferences and the
linguistic predicates used, that the coach can calibrate his or her communication
style in ways to facilitate better understanding. This is often referred to as Mirroring
the behaviour of the other person. By using similar linguistic predicates, tone of
voice, and calibrating body language (e.g., gestures and body language of the other)
providing it is not obvious imitation, helps in the rapport-building process. This has
neurological support in the notion that humans have certain types of neurons termed
Mirror Neurons (e.g., Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia 2008). The authors discovered unique
neurons in the frontal and premotor cortex while researching the neural representa-
tion of motor movements in monkeys. Unlike other motor neurons, these neurons
not only fired when engaged in planning a motor movement, but also through the
observation of a related movement in another person or other monkey. Such neurons
exist in humans, which can easily be understood in terms of how we respond to the
experiences of characters in films. Do you cry in sad movies? I do, even though I
know this is Hollywood fiction and the actors are being paid millions of dollars.
Whether we like it or not, our minds play over the actions we see around us, and we
feel the same or at least similar emotions. It’s also why laughter is often referred to as
contagious; once a person starts to laugh, others typically follow. In my experience
some join in who probably did not experience the basis for the original laughter, it’s
just how the mind works. Smiling works the same way, as we explored prior. It also
provides important feedback to others about your mood and approachability, both at
conscious and subconscious levels, and has contagion effects. For example, if you
smile at a student, he or she is likely to smile back at you and this can quickly spread
9.13 The Stages of Coaching 343
to his or her classmates. The skillful and natural use of the smile helps in building
rapport, and when used appropriately and calibrated to the communication style of
the client, it works especially well.
Questioning
The skillful use of questioning in promoting good thinking has been illustrated prior,
though Robbins (2001) view on the importance of questions is worth restating in this
context:
Thinking itself is nothing but the process of asking and answering questions
Questions immediately change what we focus on and, therefore, how we feel (pp. 179–8)
Good questioning, using the other skills of sensory acuity, linguistic predicates,
voice tone and paralanguage calibration, facilitates rapport and the mediation of
experiences towards the goals of helping teachers to be self-directed and capable
of their evaluation and professional development directions. NLP also uses a Meta-
Model of Language, that helps to clarify meaning and reduce miscommunications,
which Dilts (1980) refers to as:
an explicit set of questions as well as a model for asking questions. (pp 77–79)
This specifically focuses on how language works in and affects our neurological
states of mind, emotions, perceptions, relationships, and skills. This makes us aware
of how the Words we use, our Tone of voice and supporting Body Language makes
the difference in building rapport with other people. I have found this a useful set of
heuristics in terms of framing and using questions to clarify the meaning, uncovering
distortions, deletions and generalizations that are typically embedded in peoples’
maps that may be seriously impacting the quality of their cognition, and subsequent
action in the territory.
By asking certain questions, we can gain insight—enter-into—another person’s
model of the world (Map) and understand the world from their point of view.
Questions focus on:
• What
• How
• Who.
Such questioning structures facilitate the other person’s active involvement in
examining his/her maps and the mapping process to run Quality Control checks on
the mapping—enabling them to reframe and change meaning in the light of new
evidence. In many ways, this process has similarities with the Gallwey’s (1987)
famous work on the Inner Game. His pioneering work was in understanding what
goes on in the heads of tennis players that enhance or inhibit their performance.
This applies to all people in situations where performance is involved (especially
where it is high stakes and under observation from others) and many factors can
come into play in affecting it. Invariably, this is very noticeable in top-level sport,
for example, epitomized by a world-class soccer player failing to get the ball on
344 9 Framing Professional Development Now
target when taking a penalty kick or a golfer missing a 6-inch putt. However, it can
also happen when actors/comedians get what is referred to as ‘stage fright’ or when
people doing a public speech get ‘lost for words’. Gallwey’s key point was that a
person’s performance at any time has what he referred to as ‘Potential’—their level
of actual capability/skills. Hence, if they truly played to their potential that would
be their best performance at this level of capability. For, example, have you ever
done something (e.g., taught a lesson, given a talk, played a sport) when you felt
that this was your best effort. Well, that would be near your potential. However,
as human beings, in our minds (hence the term inner game) there is much that can
significantly impact our thinking and feelings- both positively or negatively. Negative
aspects that can undermine performance he refers to as ‘Interference’. Interference is
anything that enters our mind (and sub/unconscious stuff is involved here) that creates
negative disturbance and, consequently, mitigate performance. Most documented
are feelings of anxiety and fear that seriously affect concentration and even bodily
control. However, it is our thinking and what beliefs and images flit through the mind
that are the typical causes of interference at the psychological level, which quickly
impact emotions, brain behaviour and psychomotor aspects, often epitomized in
the phrase ‘I feel like jelly’. For Gallwey, much of coaching is about improving
Potential and reducing Interference to increase Performance. In doing this, there is
also a synergistic effect, as increases in performance enhance belief and confidence,
which in turn builds a resource for managing interference.
I remember when Garbine Muguruza won the ladies Wimbledon Title in 1917,
referring back to her defeat in the same final two years prior, in the post-match
interview, saying something like she was a ‘different player now than then’, and this
wasn’t about the tennis, but her resources to manage the interference in her head
(Note: she did not use such terms, but the intended meaning seemed congruent with
such affect). Similarly, she reflected on how she played in this final, and her thinking
when faced with set-points that would have resulted in the loss of the first set:
When I had those set points against me, I’m like, ‘Hey, it’s normal. I’m playing Venus here.’
It’s so I just keep fighting. And I knew that if I was playing like I was playing during the two
weeks, I was going to have eventually an opportunity. So I was, like, calm. If I lose the first
set, I still have two more. Let’s not make a drama.
An important aspect of this process is generating the right questions for clients
needs at the right time, as they can use their metacognitive thinking to explore their
beliefs and feelings and get into a more productive state, focusing on what can be
done, rather than what negative outcomes could occur; this is using one’s inner voice,
so to speak (again no pun intended). Treadwell (2017) framed it succinctly:
Our unique capability to be able to talk to ourselves and use our ‘inner voice’ to question
and interrogate our world, is essential to our learning. (p. 39)
Muguruza executed it perfectly, as she won the final in straight sets, winning the
second set 6-0, against Venus Williams—which is no mean feat.
There is no use for the Why question in the meta-model. From an NLP perspective
why questions, at best, get justifications and do nothing to change the situation. I am
9.13 The Stages of Coaching 345
often amused when I see interviewers asking candidates why they want to teach. Do
they expect anyone to say, “I like the long holidays”? I have even heard of approaches
that advocate asking 5 why questions to probe deeply. Quite frankly after a second
or third why question, I think I might just become a tad annoyed. The skilful use
of what and how questions are particularly effective as it opens up the opportunity
to unpack aspects of practice and how it works or don’t work. Ask clients how they
felt things went, what specifically, how they know this, etc. The manner, tone and
calibration are of course crucial—I make it a conversation with a purpose, informal
but focused. We may even have a joke in between the dialogue. I usually do most
of the reflection stage in an informal setting over coffee if clients feel comfortable
with this. It has worked well for me over the years, though the cost of buying coffees
has put a few years on my working life. It has much in common with the open and
flexible approach of creative interviewing (Douglas 1984), which involves:
…the use of many strategies and tactics of interaction, largely based on an understanding
of friendly feelings and intimacy, to optimize cooperative, mutual disclosure and a creative
search for mutual understanding. (p. 24)
together to check our mutual understanding; I may say something like, “Let’s support
each other on this”. In other words, I make paraphrasing an explicit technique that we
can collaboratively utilize as we mediate the teaching experiences for better clarity
and outcomes. Once there is rapport and trust, this is not difficult to introduce in most
cases.
The importance of feedback in learning has been explored in detail. What is
important, apart from the communication manner, is that it is data-driven and focused
on the area(s) that may be high leverage in terms of improvement (e.g., task, process).
Furthermore, simply giving what you think is useful and clear feedback does not mean
that it will be interpreted in such terms. Bandler and Grinder (1990) make a poignant
statement in this context:
The meaning of your communication is the response you get. (p. 61)
What this means is action research is less likely to scare busy teachers away from
doing this essential activity, but at the same time necessitates the systematic applica-
tion of research methodologies used in qualitative research. As outlined above, it is
possible to start small in terms of specific practice in one’s own classroom, develop
strategy possibilities, try them out and share this with colleagues, and eventually
develop validated instructional approaches that impact beyond the classroom, the
institution, the local community, and even to the wider global educational community.
This potentially extending process of action research is summarized in Fig. 9.3.
Finally, I want to emphasize the motivated teacher’s capability to do this work
well. Given time and support, teachers with their specific professional training, MC
and CTC are well equipped to excel as researchers investigating their practices. We
know that setting challenging goals leads to better learning for students; hence why
do we not see this as important for our teachers? Equally of note, such an approach
involves both teachers and students as collaborators involved in examining ways to
improve learning—now that’s a real focus on learning, not a debate about what are
teacher-centred or student-centred methods. All methods are centred on learning for
both students and teachers, and this would enable, as Hattie (2009) claims:
The ultimate requirement is for teachers to develop the skill of evaluating the effect that they
have on their students. (p. 36)
The main components, activities and processes for effective professional develop-
ment have been identified and explained in the previous sections. This section sum-
marizes institutional and societal features that are likely to be powerful enablers in
both developing high impact professional development for teachers and, of course,
the best learning opportunities for our students in terms of attainment and well-being.
Valuing Good Teaching
Firstly, it is important to bear in mind that professional development approaches
will be largely ineffective without a strong motivational base and commitment from
teachers themselves. As Hargreaves and Evans (1997) stated:
…where educational change is concerned, if a teacher can’t or won’t do it, it simply can’t
be done. (p. 3)
They also quote Costa et al. (2014) in this context, who wrote:
While our media-rich culture places a high value on talent, the irony is that in most schools,
talent is underrated and often teachers remain silent about their own beliefs about talent.
(p. 75)
While educators and policymakers agree that enabling teachers to improve student learning
is one of the most significant ways to raise achievement, there are heated disagreements
about the most useful ways to do this. (p. 1)
This makes perfect sense from the perspective taken here. Firstly, the teaching
force is the most single important factor in terms of educational quality. After all,
they are the front line in the teaching and learning process, just as the team players are
the front line in professional team sports. Rarely do poor teams win major champi-
onships in any professional sporting arena. Also, it helps if there is good leadership,
and the same comparisons between school leaders and professional team coaches
equally apply. Having worked extensively in educational development in Singapore
for 24 years, as advisor, researcher and conducting workshops across the educa-
tional sectors, I was not surprised when the Economist Magazine (2018) referred to
the Singapore Education System as the “best in the world”, noting that the major
contributing factors included a focus on quality teaching and pedagogy being based
on educational research. What was increasing becoming ‘face-validity’ for me, has
taken on a wider empirical frame.
Darling-Hammond and Rothamn’s (2015) extensive analysis and evaluation of
high performing educational systems (e.g., Finland, Ontario and Singapore) have
spelt out the main features that seem to make the difference:
• Deep respect for the profession from the top levels of government and throughout
society
• Strong common training for all teachers and leaders around these shared goals
• Systemic mentoring and induction for new teachers by trained senior teachers
• Continual development of educational knowledge, skills, and talent through exten-
sive, governmentally subsidized professional development opportunities and a
career ladder offering roles that expand and share experience
• Significant scheduled time for teachers to collaborate and learn together through
lesson study, action research, and other reflections on practice.
globally noticed in terms of its educational system but also in its economy, recently
reported to be the most competitive in the world (Straits Times, October 2019).
Firstly, Singapore as long sought to develop a workforce that not only can learn
faster but also better (my interpretation). The development of personal attributes
relating to what are now referred to as twenty-first-century competencies, as long
been in the Singaporean educational and cultural landscape. For example, in 1995,
the Education Minister, Lee Yock Suan, highlighted the need for students to be able
to:
...learn to think independently and solve unexpected problems to survive and prosper in the
years ahead, when knowledge and skills will become obsolete faster than before. (Conference
to top civil servants on June 30)
A particularly salient landmark was the then Prime Minister of Singapore, Mr. Goh
Chok Tong’s (1997) framing of Singapore’s educational system in terms of “Thinking
Schools, Learning Nation” at the opening of the 7th International Conference on
Thinking. He stated:
We must get away from the idea that it is only the people at the top who should be thinking,
and the job of everybody else is to do as told. Instead, we want to bring about a spirit of
innovation, of learning by doing, of everybody each at his level all the time asking how he
can do his job better. (p. 1)
Educational systems, philosophy and practices inevitably reflect the societal con-
text in which they prevail. They are also likely to incorporate the interests and con-
cerns of dominant decision-making groups in that society. In Singapore, there is a
heavy reliance on the continuous development of its human resources to sustain and
enhance competitive advantage, which will only be possible in the future with a
workforce capable of responding to the enormity and complexity of economic and
technological change with both productiveness and creativity. As the Prime Minister
of Singapore, Mr. Goh Chok Tong (1997) stated:
The old formulae for success are unlikely to prepare our young for the new circumstances
and new problems they will face. We do not even know what these problems will be, let alone
be able to provide the answers and solutions to them. But we must ensure that our young
can think for themselves so that the next generation can find their solutions to whatever new
problems they may face. (p. 3)
The Thinking Schools, Learning Nation program was launched in 1997, with
quality in schools being highlighted as a key to ensuring student success. This is
often referred to as the Ability-Driven Phase, which focused on “the development of
every child to maximize his or her full potential through an education system tailored
to that purpose” (Tan and Low 2016, p. 31). The concept of thinking schools was
based on the notion that:
…the development of thinking and committed citizens would be crucial in ensuring that
future challenges would be confidently dealt with, ensuring the continued success of Singa-
pore. Learning was promoted as a national culture by encouraging creativity at every level
of society. The role of teachers was also redefined, so that each school would be perceived
as a model learning organisation. (Singapore Infopedia)
352 9 Framing Professional Development Now
A Learning Nation envisions a national culture and social environment that promotes lifelong
learning in our people. The capacity of Singaporeans to continually learn, both for profes-
sional development and for personal enrichment, will determine our collective tolerance for
change. (Singapore Ministry of Education 2014, paras. 2–4).
At that time, I was Education Advisor at Singapore Polytechnic and was tasked
with developing a whole curriculum approach to promoting thinking, which I
euphemistically referred to as “The Thinking Curriculum: A Response to Think-
ing Schools, Learning Nation” (Sale 2004). Furthermore, while Singapore maintains
a strong adherence to developing cognitive capabilities and high educational attain-
ment levels, as necessary goals for its educational direction, it is also committed to
a holistic education that incorporates interpersonal and intrapersonal competencies
in the wider educational aims and curriculum goals. In 2011, what is referred to as
the Values-Driven Phase, sent a clear signal that the holistic education of individuals
was essential to survive in the twenty-first-century workplace and society? Mr. Heng
Swee Keat, Minister for Education, explained that values and character development
had to be placed at the core of the education system because parents and educa-
tors alike called for schools to develop students holistically in response to changing
demands in the global environment. As Tan and Low (2016) summarize:
The goals of this phase are “every school a good school,” “every student an engaged learner,”
“every teacher a caring educator,” and “every parent a supportive partner.” (p. 31) This values-
driven phase goes hand in hand with the ability-driven phase, where schools not only teach
academic and life skills but also help instil values and build character in students. (p. 31)
In this student-centric phase, clear desired goals and outcomes of schooling and
education were spelt out. The goal of the Singaporean education system is to nurture
every child, regardless of his or her ability or achievement level. The ecology of
educational reform is seen as resting on a set of shared values, The Desired Outcomes
of Education, which are attributes that educators aspire for every Singaporean to have
by the completion of formal education. These outcomes establish a common purpose
for educators, drive policies and programmes, and provide a means of determining
how well the education system is doing. They seek to develop:
• a confident person who has a strong sense of right and wrong, is adaptable
and resilient, knows himself, is discerning in judgment, thinks independently and
critically, and communicates effectively;
9.15 Institutional and Societal Features that Facilitate Good … 353
• a self-directed learner who takes responsibility for his learning, who questions,
reflects and perseveres in the pursuit of learning;
• an active contributor who can work effectively in teams, exercises initiative, takes
calculated risks, is innovative and strives for excellence; and,
• a concerned citizen who is rooted to Singapore, has a strong civic consciousness,
is informed, and takes an active role in bettering the lives of others around him.
How these outcomes are developed through key educational stages is summarized
in Table 9.5.
Most significant, in Singapore, policy quickly becomes active in practice, and new
thrusts are supported with comprehensive professional development and support for
teachers. I was astonished and delighted to have so much opportunity for professional
learning. It bore no resemblance to what I had experienced prior, and this is not in
any way critical of my prior workplaces. Teachers here have about 20 h a week built
into their schedule for shared planning and learning, including visits to one another’s
classrooms, as well as 100 h per year of state-supported professional development
outside of their school time. Furthermore, to create the space for critical thinking
in the classroom, the content of all subjects has been significantly reduced (by up
to 30%). Testing and assessments are increasingly being redesigned to encompass
critical thinking skills. Darling-Hammond and Rothman (2015) point out that the
purpose of the curriculum reduction was to free up space and time to focus on
Similarly, Ryan and Deci (2017) concur with the above analysis of these systems
as well as capturing the essence and purpose of this chapter.
Despite massive differences in curricular approaches, they have one important thing in com-
mon: they treat and train their teachers as professionals. These nations have invested in
higher salaries and higher-quality training, to recruit the best and the brightest and help
them internalize and develop effective classroom practices. In turn, the more competent and
professional the population of teachers, the more they can be expected to benefit from, and
make good use of professional autonomy. (p. 378)
The primary focus is student flourishing – that is, not only growing in cognitive skills and
knowledge but also developing and strengthening personal and social skills and experiencing
psychological health and well-being in the process. (p. 380)
9.16 Epilogue
Teaching expertise (in terms of Martin’s (2009) Knowledge Funnel) is now less of a
Mystery and more understandable in terms of useful Heuristics. As a consequence,
we can now develop tools (e.g., highly effective, efficient and creative instructional
strategies) that will enhance competence and performance in teaching, of which
Drucker framed as only possessed by the ‘naturals’ who somehow know how to
teach. Invariably, teaching itself needs significant reframing. As Treadwell (2017)
points out:
The role of the educator is now far more dynamic and requires a deeper professionalism
and rigour in our understanding of the Learning Process, the concept frameworks within
the domains and the competencies in which we are operating, as well as the thoughtful and
reflective practices we are encouraging. (p. 156)
For me, the aim is to improve teaching quality globally, as I feel that’s something
worthwhile in terms of a moral landscape (e.g., Harris 2010). I also think that a
global teaching force, skilled in Metacognitive Capability and Creative Teaching
Competence, would help a generation of learners to be able to better understand the
9.16 Epilogue 355
world (the one inside their heads, and that external to self). It may also contribute to
better decision making for mankind’s future. This might represent a major existential
feature of human progress, akin to Pinker’s (2019) “Enlightenment Now”.
References
Bandler R, Grinder J (1990) Frogs into princes: the introduction to neuro-linguistic programming.
Eden Grove Editions, Middlesex
Berliner DC (2004) Describing the behavior and documenting the accomplishments of expert
teachers. Bull Sci Technol Soc 24(3):200–212
Caine G, Caine R (2001) The brain education and the competitive edge. Scarecrow Press, Lanham,
MD
Clouder DL (2000) Reflective practice: realising its potential. Physiotherapy 86(10):517–522
Costa AL, Garmston RJ (2002) Cognitive coaching: a foundation for renaissance schools.
Christopher-Gordon Publishers Inc, Massachusetts
Costa AL, Garmston RJ, Zimmerman DP (2014) Investing in teacher quality. Teacher College Press,
New York
Costa AL, Garmston RJ (2016) Cognitive coaching: developing self-directed leaders and learners.
Rowman & Littlefield, New York
Darling-Hammond L, Rothman R (2015) Teaching in the flat world: learning from high-performing
systems. Teachers College Press, New York
Dewey J (2016) The University of Hong Kong, e-learning blog. https://tl.hku.hk/2016/03/reflection-
how-do-i-do-it-john-dewey-we-do-not-learn-from-experience-we-learn-from-reflecting-on-
experience/. Last accessed 29 Nov 2019
Dickinson M (2015) Evolution of an e-learning developer guide instructional design and project
management. In: Waldrop JB, Bowden MA (eds) Best practices for flipping the college classroom.
Routledge, New York
Dilts R et al (1980) Neuro-linguistic programming Vol. 1: the study of the structure of subjective
experience. Meta Publications, California
Douglas JD (1984) Creative interviewing. Sage Publications, Beverly Hills
Downey M (2003) Effective coaching: lessons from the coaches’ coach. Texere Publishing, New
York
Duffy GG (1993) Teachers’ progress toward becoming expert strategy teachers. Elementary Sch J
94:109–220
Duffy GG et al (2010) Teacher as metacognitive professionals. In: Hacker D, Dunlosky J, Graesser
A (eds) Handbook of metacognition in education. Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates, New Jersey
Economist Magazine (2018) What other countries and learn from Singapore’s schools.
Retrieved from https://www.economist.com/leaders/2018/08/30/what-other-countries-can-learn-
from-singapores-schools
Gallup Pool Organization (2014) The state of American schools
Gallwey TW (1987) The inner game of tennis. Jonathan Cape, London
Glaser J (2014) Conversational intelligence: how great leaders build trust and get extraordinary
results. Bibliomotion, Brookline, MA
Goh CT (1997) Speech by Prime Minister Goh Chook Tong at the opening of the 7th international
conference on thinking on Monday, 2 June at The Suntec City Convention Centre, Singapore
Gulamhussein A (2013) Teaching the teachers: effective professional development in an era of high
stakes accountability. National School Boards Association, Centre for Public Education
Hall LM (2001) The spirit of NLP. Crown House Publishing, Bethel
Harari YN (2016) Homo Deus: a brief history of tomorrow. Vintage, Penguin, London
356 9 Framing Professional Development Now
Hargreaves A, Evans R (1997) Beyond educational reform: bringing teachers back in. Open
University Press, Buckingham
Hargreaves A, Fullan M (2012) Professional capital: transforming teaching in every school. Teachers
College Press, New York
Harris S (2010) The moral landscape: how science can determine human values. Free Press, New
York
Hatano G, Inagaki K (1986) Two courses of expertise. Child development and education in Japan,
pp 262–272
Hattie J (2003) Teachers make a difference: what is the research evidence? Distinguishing expert
teachers from novice and experienced teachers. Australian Council for Educational Research
Hattie J (2009) Visible learning. Routledge, New York
Huberman M (1990) Linkage between researchers and practitioners: a qualitative study. Am Educ
Res J 27:363–391
Joyce BR, Showers B (2002) Student achievement through staff development, 3rd edn. ASCD,
Alexandria, CA
Kabat-Zin J (2003) Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clin
Psychol Sci Pract 10(2):144–156
Kahneman D (2012) Thinking fast and slow. Penguin Books, London
Kanfer R, Kanfer F (1991) Goals and self-regulation: applications of theory to work settings. In:
Maehr M, Pintrich PR (eds) Advances in metacognition and achievement, vol 7. JAI Press,
Greenswich, CN, pp 287–326
Kennedy M (1999) The role of preservice teacher education. In Darling-Hammond L, Sykes G (eds)
Teaching as the learning profession: handbook of policy and practice. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Knowles M (1984) Andragogy in action. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Darling-Hammond et al. (2009) Professional learning in the learning profession: a status report on
teacher development in the United States and abroad. National Staff Development Council
Lave J, Wenger E (1993) Situated learning: legitimate peripheral engagement. Cambridge University
Press, Cambridge
Lee YS (1995) The Straits Times, July 1
Leithwood et al (2004) How leadership influences student learning. University of Minnesota Center
for Applied Research and Educational Improvement
Levin B (2008) How to change 5000 schools. Harvard Education Press, Cambridge
Lincoln YS (1990) The making of a constructivist: a remembrance of transformations past. In: Guba
EG (ed) The paradigm dialog. Sage, London
Lortie DL (1975) Schoolteacher. University of Chicago, Chicago, IL
Martin R (2009) The design of business. Harvard Business Press, Massachusetts
Pentland A (2014) Social physics: how good ideas spread—the lessons from a new science. Scribe
Publications, London
Petty G (2015) Resources provided on Geoff Petty Homepage. Can be accessed at https://geoffpetty.
com/
Piaget J (2001) The psychology of intelligence. Routledge, New York
Pinker S (2019) Enlightenment now. Penguin, U.K.
Piper E (2017) Not a day care: the devastating consequences of abandoning truth. Salem, New
Jersey
Powell W, Kusuma-Powell O (2013) The OIQ factor: raising your schools organizational
intelligence. John Catt Educational Ltd, Melton, Woodbridge
Powell W, Kusuma-Powell O (2015) Teacher self-supervision: why teacher evaluation has failed
and what we can do about it. John Catt, Suffolk, UK
Reimers FM, Chung CK (2016) Teaching and learning for the twenty-first century. Harvard
Education Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts
Research Alert (2014) Educational Leadership May 2014, vol 71, no 8. Professional
learning reimagined. (https://www.ascd.org/publications/educational-leadership/may14/vol71/
num08/Double-Take.aspx). Last accessed 15 Nov 2019
References 357
Rizzolatti G, Sinigaglia C (2008) Mirrors in the mind: how we share our actions and emotions.
Oxford University Press, New York
Robbins A (2001) Unlimited power. Pocket books, London
Rogers A (1998) Teaching adults. Open University Press, Buckingham
Ryan RM, Deci EL (2017) Self determination theory: basic needs in motivation, development, and
wellness. The Guilford Press, New York
Sale D (2004) Promoting thinking: a Singaporean response. SPGG J
Sale D (2014) The challenge of reframing engineering education. Springer, New York
Sale D, Cheah SM, Wan M (2018) 3 paper symposia on enhancing students’ intrinsic motivation:
an evidence-based approach. In: ERAS-APERA international conference, Singapore, 12–14 Nov
2018
Schön DA (1983) The reflective practitioner: how professionals think in action. Temple Smith,
London
Schön DA (1987) Educating the reflective practitioner: toward a new design for teaching and learning
in the professions. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Singapore Infopedia. https://eresources.nlb.gov.sg/infopedia/. Last accessed 7 Nov 2019
Singapore Ministry of Education (2014) https://www.moe.gov.sg/education/education-system/
desired-outcomes-of-education
Stigler JW, Hiebert J (1999) The teaching gap. Free Press, New York
Stringer E (2004) Action research in education. Pearson, New Jersey
Swaab D (2015) We are our brains. Penguin, London
Tan J, Gopinathan S (2000) Education reform in Singapore: towards greater creativity and
innovation. NIRA Rev 7(3):5–10
Tan OS, Low EL (2016). Singapore’s systemic approach to teaching and learning twenty-first
century competencies. In: Reimers FM, Chung CK (eds) Teaching and learning for the twenty-
first century: educational goals, policies, and curricula from six nations. Harvard Education Press,
Cambridge, Massachusetts
The Straits Times (October, 2019) https://www.straitstimes.com/business/economy/singapore-is-
worlds-most-competitive-economy-world-economic-forum. Last accessed 22 Nov 2019
Timperley H et al (2008) Best evidence synthesis on professional learning and development. Report
to the Ministry of Education. Wellington, New Zealand
Treadwell M (2017) The future of learning. The Global Curriculum Project, Mount Maunganui,
NZ
Willingham DT (2009) Why don’t students like school: a cognitive scientist answers questions
about how the mind works and what it means for the classroom. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco
Windschitl M (2002) Framing constructivism in practice as the negotiation of dilemmas: an analysis
of the conceptual, pedagogical, cultural, and political challenges facing teachers. Rev Educ Res
72:131–175
Winget L (2017) What’s wrong with damn near everything!: how the collapse of core values is
destroying us and how to fix it. Wiley, New Jersey