Clinical Reasoning Processes - Unravelling Complexity Through Graphical Representation
Clinical Reasoning Processes - Unravelling Complexity Through Graphical Representation
CONTEXT Clinical reasoning is a core skill in then refined through an iterative validation process
medical practice, but remains notoriously difficult involving the same group of doctors, after which
for students to grasp and teachers to nurture. To other groups of clinicians were asked to solve a
date, an accepted model that adequately captures clinical problem involving simulated patients.
the complexity of clinical reasoning processes does
not exist. Knowledge-modelling software such as RESULTS A hierarchical model depicting the
MOT Plus (Modelling using Typified Objects multifaceted processes of clinical reasoning was
[MOT]) may be exploited to generate models produced. Validation rounds suggested generalis-
capable of unravelling some of this complexity. ability across disciplines and situations.
OBJECTIVES This study was designed to create a CONCLUSIONS The MOT model of clinical
comprehensive generic model of clinical reasoning reasoning processes has potentially important
processes that is intended for use by teachers and applications for use within undergraduate and
learners, and to provide data on the validity of the graduate medical curricula to inform teaching,
model. learning and assessment. Specifically, it could be
used to support curricular development because it
METHODS Using a participatory action research can help to identify opportune moments for learn-
method and the established modelling software ing specific elements of clinical reasoning. It could
(MOT Plus), knowledge was extracted and entered also be used to precisely identify and remediate
into the model by a cognitician in a series of reasoning errors in students, residents and practis-
encounters with a group of experienced clinicians ing doctors with persistent difficulties in clinical
over more than 250 contact hours. The model was reasoning.
1 7
Centre for Applied Pedagogy in Health Sciences (Centre de Department of Oral Health, Faculty of Dental Medicine, University
Pédagogie Appliquée aux Sciences de la Santé [CPASS]), University of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
8
of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada Department of Medicine, CPASS, University of Montreal,
2
Centre for Medical Education, McGill University, Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
Quebec, Canada
3
Department of Neurology, Montreal General Hospital, Montreal,
Correspondence: Professor Bernard Charlin, Médecine Direction,
Quebec, Canada
4 Centre de Pédagogie Appliquée aux Sciences de la Santé (CPASS),
Department of Family and Emergency Medicine, Hôpital Cité de la
Université de Montréal, CP 6128, succ. Centre-Ville, Montreal,
Santé de Laval, Quebec, Canada
5 Quebec H3C 3J7, Canada. Tel: 00 1 514 343 7864; Fax: 00 1 514
Consultant, Educational Engineering, Faculty of Pharmacy,
343 7650; E-mail: [email protected]
University of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
6
Department of Family and Emergency Medicine, Faculty of
Medicine, University of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
454 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2012. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2012; 46: 454–463
Clinical reasoning: graphical representation
ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2012. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2012; 46: 454–463 455
B Charlin et al
456 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2012. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2012; 46: 454–463
Clinical reasoning: graphical representation
Round 3: CPs and the cognitician than ‘diagnosis’ better described the product of
initial phases in clinical reasoning.
In this final round of model validation, data obtained
in the second round were used by CPs and the Types of knowledge used
cognitician to make final adjustments to the MOT
model with the added aim of producing a model that There is growing agreement among researchers and
would be detailed enough to reflect the multi- medical educators that several types of knowledge
dimensional structure of clinical reasoning, but interact in a medical encounter.1,4,19,20 The CPs felt it
would not be prohibitively unwieldy. In other words, was necessary to specify these in order to produce a
CPs were requested to fine-tune the model such that model that would be useful for teaching and learn-
important processes and concepts that emerged in ing. The illness script theory18–23 assumes that
Rounds 1 and 2 were included, but excessive details knowledge networks adapted to clinical tasks develop
that might render the model cumbersome or through experience and operate autonomously
impractical to use as a teaching and learning tool beneath the level of conscious awareness. These
were avoided. networks consist of associative links among illnesses
and their attributes, consequences, investigation or
treatment, and links with memories of previously
RESULTS encountered exemplars of the illness (instances). In
ambiguous situations, clinicians search for a fit
The Modelling using Typified Objects modelling between the available information and appropriate
sessions confirmed that clinical reasoning is a highly scripts.23,24 The CPs reported that, in clinical situa-
complex and multifaceted process. The CPs were all tions, they effectively mobilise specific knowledge
seasoned educators, who were well versed in classical (scripts) relevant to the particular situation (e.g.
clinical reasoning concepts, such as hypothesis gen- knowledge related to vertigo and then knowledge
eration or hypothetico-deductive reasoning, but in linked to cervical masses when they see two successive
the early stages of modelling they realised that certain patients whose main complaints are, respectively,
concepts required operational definition to ensure a dizziness and progressive swelling of the upper neck).
common base of language and understanding among The group reported that clinical reasoning occa-
themselves during the construction and validation of sionally taps into sources of knowledge of a different
the model. Space limitations preclude the inclusion nature and that the concept of knowledge encapsu-
in this paper of all discussed concepts. The following lation20,25 describes that type of knowledge well. In
section defines concepts taken from the medical the model, these latter sources of knowledge are
education literature that prompted discussion among referred to as ‘biological, psychological and socio-
CPs and were incorporated within the model. logical knowledge’.
ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2012. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2012; 46: 454–463 457
B Charlin et al
transformed in ‘an acute, recurrent attack of abrupt, orient the process ‘Categorise for the purpose of
nocturnal and extremely severe pain in a single, large action’. This process has two possible outcomes:
joint’. Metacognition refers to an individual’s knowl- ‘Categorisation is suitable for purposeful action’ and
edge concerning his or her own cognitive pro- ‘Categorisation is NOT suitable for purposeful
cesses.2,29 In medical practice, the application of action’. The former outcome feeds another process,
metacognition implies that the clinician is aware of ‘Implement purposeful action’, which has ‘Investiga-
encounter goals and verifies that his or her cognitive tions’ and ‘Therapeutic interventions’ as outputs.
processes and actions are contributing to the Results of these actions are then evaluated. If
achievement of the goals of the clinical encounter. satisfactory, the episode of care is terminated. If not, the
categorisation process or the therapeutic interven-
The MOT model tions put in place are reconsidered.
The MOT model is hierarchical. It is depicted on six Within Fig. 1, the core clinical reasoning processes
screens that show, respectively, a main model and five are depicted mainly along the screen’s horizontal
sub-models. Four screens are presented here. axis. However, the vertical axis of the figure is also
The complete, updated model (with appendices) important. Doctors’ repertoires of knowledge are
can be accessed at www.medent.umontreal.ca/ represented beneath the series of core processes by
clinical-reasoning/. Figure 1 features the main mod- rectangles entitled ‘Clinical knowledge organised for
el, a graphical representation of the core clinical action (= illness scripts)’ and ‘Biological, psycholog-
reasoning processes that emerged during develop- ical, sociological knowledge’. Links show that the
ment of the model. Beginning from the left side of latter underpins the former and that each clinical
the screen, the concepts (rectangles) ‘Context’ and encounter modifies and enriches illness scripts.
‘Patient’ are rich input sources for the process (oval) Above the series of core processes, problem repre-
‘Identify early cues’. The product of this process, sentation is horizontally depicted by three rectangles
‘Initial data’, then feeds into the process ‘Determine designating, respectively, ‘Initial representation’,
the objectives of the encounter’. These objectives ‘Dynamic representation’ and ‘Final representation’.
R IP
IP
IP IP
IP IP
IP
Context IP Objective(s) of the
IP Categorisation is suitable IP
encounter for IP
IP Investigations
IP IP
purposeful action IP
IP
IP
Determine the Implement Evaluate the
Identify early cues Categorise for the purposeful action
objectives of the IP
results
purpose of action
encounter
IP
Categorisation is
NOT suitable
IP
IP IP for purposeful action Therapeutic IP
Find alternative
hypotheses
Each clinical encounter Clinical knowledge organised for action
modifies and enriches R
( = illness scripts)
IP
illness scripts.
IP
IP
458 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2012. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2012; 46: 454–463
Clinical reasoning: graphical representation
Above all is the metacognition process. A regulating produces as outcome (another rectangle) the
principle, contained in a hexagon, describes the ‘Patient’s needs according to the patient’. ‘Request
importance during clinical reasoning of taking into from the referring doctor’ (an input) feeds the
account the patient’s perspective and the impact of process ‘Acknowledge the request’, which produces
the illness on the patient’s life. as outcome ‘Patient’s needs according to the refer-
ring doctor’. ‘Identify the patient’s needs’ is a
On the site (www.medent.umontreal.ca/clinical- process that results in ‘Patient’s needs according to
reasoning), clicking on red arrows allows the user to the doctor’. The four outcomes feed the fifth
navigate between the main model and the five sub- process, ‘Establish priorities’, which ultimately pro-
models that depict specific processes from the main duces the ‘Objective(s) of the encounter’. This
model in greater detail. Examples of these sub- process is regulated by a principle (contained in a
screens are shown in Figs 2–4. hexagon): ‘When establishing priorities, factors such
as urgency, appropriateness and efficiency must be
Figure 2 depicts the five processes (ovals) that considered.’
underlie the process ‘Determine the objective(s) of
the encounter’: (i) ‘Clarify the patient’s request(s)’; Figure 3 depicts the sub-model ‘Categorise for the
(ii) ‘Acknowledge the request’; (iii) ‘Acknowledge purpose of action’. This process is informed by
the information’; (iv) ‘Identify the patient’s needs’, patient data and the objectives judged to be relevant
and (v) ‘Establish priorities’. As shown, ‘Patient’s in the encounter. It bears upon repertoires of
request(s)’ (input, represented by a rectangle) feeds knowledge for action (illness scripts). It is composed
the process ‘Clarify the patient’s request(s)’, which of a succession of six sub-processes: (i) ‘Search for
IP R
IP IP IP
Clarify the
Acknowledge the Acknowledge the Identify the Establish Objective(s) of the
patient's IP
request information patient's needs priorities encounter
request(s)
IP IP
IP IP
IP
Patient's needs Patient's needs Clinical information Patient's needs
according according to the provided by the third according
to the patient referring doctor party to the doctor
IP
IP
IP
Objective(s) of Initial hypothesis and New set of activated scripts and Categorisation is suitable
Patient associated activated script their associated attributes for purposeful action
the encounter
IP
IP IP
IP IP IP
IP IP
IP IP IP IP
IP IP
IP
IP
ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2012. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2012; 46: 454–463 459
B Charlin et al
Investigations
Evidence-based Particularity
Categorization is suitable data of the situation
for purposeful action Therapeutic
interventions
C
Practical Particularity Clinician IP
IP
Address the Implement
Consider factors that might Evaluate the fit between Discuss the
INVESTIGATION and investigation and/or
influence the choice of the clinical data and the option with the
INTERVENTION therapeutic
particular investigative or attributes of the relevant patient
components of the intervention plan
management options options
activated script(s)
IP IP
IP IP IP
IP IP IP
IP
Investigation plan IP
Investigative and Relevant options Selected options
therapeutic options
Therapeutic
intervention plan
460 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2012. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2012; 46: 454–463
Clinical reasoning: graphical representation
but that the essence of the processes is similar. For reasoning processes even while they concentrate on
instance, categorisation ⁄ diagnosis is often central in specific aspects in isolation in dedicated teaching
medicine, whereas in dentistry a greater part of sessions.
reasoning may revolve around treatment planning.
Under the complex and ambiguous conditions that
Lessons drawn from the development of the MOT characterise daily practice, seasoned clinicians are
model attentive to a wide variety of dimensions of a clinical
encounter and reasoning cannot be successful if
The extraction and graphical representation of the some of these dimensions are neglected. A narrow
clinical reasoning processes of our participants raised view of the multidimensional nature of clinical
several important points. Firstly, it confirmed the reasoning may explain the paucity of methods avail-
dynamic nature of problem representation2,27 able to medical educators for fostering the acquisi-
throughout a clinical encounter. Secondly, CPs found tion of clinical reasoning skills, detecting learners
the processes they used when considering manage- with deficiencies in clinical reasoning, and offering
ment (i.e. investigation and treatment) options very appropriate remediation to those who have been
similar to those they employed during categorisation identified as learners in trouble.7–10 Although some
processes: both entailed a search for a fit between clinical reasoning difficulties may be linked to clearly
activated scripts and information derived during the delineated steps in the reasoning process (e.g. ‘data
clinical encounter. For instance, performance of a gathering’ or ‘hypothesis generation’), others may be
lumbar puncture (an investigative option) ‘fits’ with ascribed to less commonly identified factors, such as
the investigation script ‘measure the patient’s intra- ability to appreciate the clinical context, ability to
cranial pressure’ only if there is no clinical evidence deal with uncertainty, or ability to communicate
of elevated intracranial pressure (such as papilloe- effectively. The relationship between these factors
dema, which might be considered an unacceptable and clinical reasoning is rarely discussed in the
value for the ‘measure intracranial pressure’ script literature. Our model may be a useful tool for
prior to the acquisition of a computed tomography bringing these crucial yet often overlooked dimen-
scan of the head). Thirdly, the core reasoning sions of clinical reasoning to light for the benefit of
processes depicted on the horizontal axis of the main discussion and instruction among teachers and
screen of the model suggest that, from the moment a learners.
clinical encounter begins, a whole series of concom-
itant cognitive actions occur: the clinical reservoir of Written assessments of clinical reasoning typically
knowledge is tapped; scripts are mobilised and provide a brief clinical context and then ask ques-
enriched, and from time to time encapsulated bio- tions such as ‘What is the correct or most probable
psycho-social knowledge is accessed. All these pro- diagnosis?’ or ‘What is the most relevant investiga-
cesses work in parallel and are under the control of tion or treatment option?’ Such formats assess the
metacognition, indicating that clinical reasoning is outcome of clinical reasoning, rather than its pro-
not a linear process consisting of a succession of cesses. As long as the right answer is provided by the
steps, but, rather, a complex deployment of numer- learner, reasoning is considered acceptable and no
ous cognitive processes. regard is paid to how the answer is obtained. In such
instances, educators who assess clinical reasoning do
Potential educational applications of the MOT model not actually have access to learners’ reasoning
of clinical reasoning processes and are therefore unable to identify
whether important elements are lacking or are being
The identification and graphical representation of misused. Educators are therefore generally good at
the complexity of clinical reasoning has potentially detecting ‘those who do not have it’, but are rarely
important applications for use within undergraduate able to point out precisely where in a learner’s
and graduate medical curricula to inform teaching, reasoning processes about a particular case the
learning and assessment. The MOT model could be problem lies, and thus have difficulty designing
presented early in these curricula as an advance appropriate remediation activities.10 The MOT
organiser, thus providing students with an overview of model can be used as a guide to test specific parts of
experts’ clinical reasoning processes. It could be used learners’ clinical reasoning processes, and therefore
to support curricular development because it can to help pinpoint flaws in reasoning as learners
help to identify opportune moments for students to reason through clinical encounters. Remediation
learn specific elements of clinical reasoning, and help activities can then be designed to target these
students gain a global appreciation of clinical specific areas of difficulty.
ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2012. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2012; 46: 454–463 461
B Charlin et al
462 ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2012. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2012; 46: 454–463
Clinical reasoning: graphical representation
Clinical Reasoning in the Health Professions, 3rd edn. 20 Schmidt H, Rickers R. How expertise develops in
Edinburgh: Elsevier 2008;223–34. medicine: knowledge encapsulation and illness script
4 Eva KW. What every teacher needs to know about formation. Med Educ 2007;41:1133–9.
clinical reasoning. Med Educ 2005;39:98–106. 21 Feltovich PJ, Barrows HS. Issues of generality in
5 Bowen J. Educational strategies to promote clinical medical problem solving. In: Schmidt HG, De Volder
diagnostic reasoning. N Engl J Med 2006;355:2217–25. ML, eds. Tutorials in Problem-Based Learning. Assen ⁄
6 Bargh JA, Chartrand TL. The unbearable automaticity Maastricht: Van Gorcum 1984;128–42.
of being. Am Psychol 1999;54:462–79. 22 Schmidt HG, Norman GR, Boshuizen HPA. A cognitive
7 Bordage G. Why did I miss the diagnosis? Some perspective on medical expertise: theory and implica-
cognitive explanations and educational implication. tions. Acad Med 1990;65:611–21.
Acad Med 1999;74 (Suppl):138–43. 23 Charlin B, Boshuizen HP, Custers EJ, Feltovich PJ. Scripts
8 Mamede S, Schmidt HG, Rikers R. Diagnostic errors and clinical reasoning. Med Educ 2007;41:1178–84.
and reflective practice in medicine. J Eval Clin Pract 24 Lurie S. Towards greater clarity in the role of ambiguity
2007;13:138–45. in clinical reasoning. Med Educ 2011;45:326–8.
9 Hauer K, Ciccone A, Henzel T, Katsufrakis P, Miller S, 25 Boshuizen HPA, Schmidt HG. On the role of bio-
Norcross W, Papadakis M, Irby D. Remediation of the medical knowledge in clinical reasoning by experts,
deficiencies of physicians across the continuum from intermediates and novices. Cogn Sci 1992;16:
medical school to practice: a thematic review of the 153–84.
literature. Acad Med 2009;84:1822–32. 26 Anderson J. Problem solving. In: Anderson J, ed.
10 Audétat MC, Dory V, Nendaz M, Vanpee D, Pestiaux D, Cognitive Psychology and its Implications, 3rd edn. New
Junod Perron D, Charlin B. What is so difficult about York, NY: W H Freeman 1990;219–55.
managing clinical reasoning difficulties? Med Educ 27 Nendaz M, Bordage G. Promoting diagnostic problem
2012;46:216–27. representation. Med Educ 2002;36:760–6.
11 Basque J, Pudelko B. Intersubjective meaning-making 28 Bordage G. Prototypes and semantic qualifiers: from
in dyads using object-typed concept mapping. In: past to present. Med Educ 2007;41:1117–21.
Torres PL, Marriott RCV, eds. Handbook of Research on 29 Flavell JH. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring, a
Collaborative Learning using Concept Mapping. New York, new area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. Am
NY: Hershey 2009;180–206. Psychol 1979;34:906–11.
12 Paquette G. Mode´lisation des Connaissances et des Compe´- 30 Elstein AS, Shulman LS, Sprafka SA. Medical Problem
tences. Un Langage Graphique pour Concevoir et Apprendre. Solving: An Analysis of Clinical Reasoning. Cambridge,
Sainte-Foy, QC: University of Quebec Press 2002. MA: Harvard University Press 1978;273–302.
13 Novak JD, Cañas AJ. The Theory underlying Concept Maps 31 Balla JI. The Diagnostic Process: A Model for Clinical
and How to Construct and Use Them. Technical Report Teachers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
IHMC CmapTools. Florida Institute for Human and 1985;49–106.
Machine Cognition 2008. http://cmap.ihmc.us/ 32 Barrows HS, Feltovich PJ. The clinical reasoning
publications/researchpapers/theorycmaps/theory process. Med Educ 1987;21:86–91.
underlyingconceptmaps.htm. [Accessed 27 February 33 Grant J, Marsden P. Primary knowledge, medical
2012]. education and consultant expertise. Med Educ 1988;22:
14 Daley BJ, Torre DM. Concept maps in medical educa- 173–9.
tion: an analytical literature review. Med Educ
2010;44:440–8.
15 Kemmis S, McTaggart R. Participatory action research: SUPPORTING INFORMATION
communicative action and the public sphere. In:
Denzin N, Lincoln Y, eds. Handbook of Qualitative Additional supporting information may be found in the
Research, 3rd edn. Beverley Hills, CA: Sage Publications online version of this article.
2005;271–330.
16 Schön D. From technical rationality to reflection in Figure S1. Process: ‘Identify early cues’.
action. In: Dowie J, Elstein A, eds. Professional Judgment:
a Reader in Clinical Decision Making (1988). Cambridge:
Figure S2. Process: ‘Regulate cognitive processes’.
Cambridge University Press 1983;60–77.
17 Gilhooly KS. Cognitive psychology and medical
Please note: Wiley-Blackwell is not responsible for the
diagnosis. Appl Cogn Psychol 1990;4:261–72.
content or functionality of any supporting materials sup-
18 Charlin B, Tardif J, Boshuizen HP. Scripts and medical
plied by the authors. Any queries (other than for missing
diagnostic knowledge: theory and applications for
material) should be directed to the corresponding author
clinical reasoning instruction and research. Acad Med
for the article.
2000;75 (2):182–90.
19 Custers EJ, Regehr G, Norman GR. Mental represen-
tations of medical diagnostic knowledge: a review. Acad Received 18 July 2011; editorial comments to authors 12 October
Med 1996;71 (Suppl):55–61. 2011; accepted for publication 1 November 2011
ª Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2012. MEDICAL EDUCATION 2012; 46: 454–463 463