Beyond The Conscious Mind
Beyond The Conscious Mind
Beyond The Conscious Mind
Conscious
Mind
Unlocking the
Secrets of the Self
Beyond the
Conscious
Mind
Unlocking the
Secrets of the Self
THOMAS R. BLAKESLEE
10987654321
All rights reserved
No part of this book may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted
in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, microfilming,
recording, or otherwise, without written permission from the Publisher
Preface
v
vi PREFACE
ix
x ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
xi
xii CONTENTS
The Self-Organizing
Mind
The eyes are not responsible when the mind does the seeing.
- PubLUius Syrus. 1 st century Be
The billions of neurons that constitute the human brain
are richly interconnected, not according to a precise wiring
plan like a computer, but more like the chaotic tangle of
plants in a jungle. In an awesome demonstration of the
power of self-organization and evolution, the miracle of
human consciousness emerges spontaneously from this
tangle of neurons. In recent years a new understanding of
this self-organizing process has emerged that revolution-
izes our understanding of the very nature of conscious-
ness.
Although our internal mental world feels like an in-
herent part of our very being, it is really an imagined
illusion defined, not by a physical network of neurons, but
rather, by a collection of learned concepts that we will refer
to here as the self-concept. This self-concept is learned in
childhood and serves to define the very nature of our
consciousness and our sense of self. It provides a mental
framework and a model for making sense of the continu-
ous flood of inputs from our sensory organs. These learned
3
4 CHAPTER ONE
CONTEXT SWITCHING
31
32 CHAPTER TWO
GAP FILLING
FIGURE 2. Close your right eye and hold the book about 8 inches in front of you while your left eye
stares directly at the X. Adjust the book position until the hole in the text disappears. Your brain easily
fills in words to cover up for the blind spot in your vision (where your optic nerve enters the retina).
This tendency of the brain to imaginatively fill-in gaps to make the world seem normal is the cause of
much confusion in the world.
GETTING TO KNOW YOUR SELF MODULE 39
seems sharp and in full color over the entire field of vision.
Much of the color is also imagined because your color vision
works only in a 30 degree circle at the center ofyourfield ofvision.
Your brain takes this jittery image with a hole in it,
sharp only in the middle, and with no peripheral color and
uses it to imagine the consistently clear, steady, full-color
picture you have come to expect. Obviously, we must be
very cautious about accepting anything we perceive at face
value because the brain is so good at fabricating to satisfy
our expectations.
Most magic tricks depend on the fact that what we see
is largely imagined. The magician uses misdirection to
distract your detailed vision to something else while he
actually performs the trickery in the fuzzy area that you
only imagine you see clearly.
BLINDNESS DENIAL
One dramatic demonstration of just how convincing
the mind's fabrications can be is called Anton's syndrome
or blindness denial. Sometimes brain injury leaves the pa-
tient totally blind yet completely unaware of the blindness. To
quote a doctor's description: "Asked to describe the doc-
tor's tie the patient may say that it is a blue tie with red
spots when in fact the doctor is wearing no tie at all. When
pressed further the patient may volunteer the information
that the light in the room seems a little dim.,,6
How could a person be blind and not know it? You
should know since, as Figure 2 demonstrated, you have a
blind spot yourself that you don't even notice! When no visual
image presents itself, the brain simply accommodates and
42 CHAPTER TWO
OTHER FABRICATIONS
their behavior: If they had cheated, they now felt that cheat-
ing was not so bad; if they had been tolerant of cheating
before, but been honest on the test, they were now more
disapproving of cheating.
It appears that, in some cases, the decision to cheat
was made by a module other than the self, but the self, as
press secretary, modified the belief system so it would no
longer conflict with its own observed behavior. Of course,
a strong-willed student could have consciously exercised
self-control to override the temptation to cheat. However,
as the results showed, this often doesn't happen.
asked why they chose the stockings they did, nobody men-
tioned the position in the array, even though that was
usually the real, though unconscious, reason for their
choice. When the examiner suggested that it could be
related to display placement, virtually all subjects denied
it, "usually with a worried glance at the interviewer sug-
gesting that they felt either that they had misunderstood
the question or were dealing with a madman."
In another study, 81 subjects were asked to memorize
a list of word pairs. Some of the word pairs were included
to see if they would affect the results of a later, different
word association task. For example, when subjects memo-
rized the combination" ocean-moon," it might make them
more likely to think of "Tide" when they were later asked
to name a detergent. When the results were tabulated, the
semantic cuing was found to double the frequency of
target responses. Subjects were asked in an open-ended
way why they had given their responses, and, though they
could still remember the word pairs, they almost never
mentioned the word pair as a reason for giving a particular
response. Instead, they gave answers like "Tide is the
best-known detergent," or "My mother uses Tide," "I like
the Tide box." When the experimenter suggested that the
word pairs could have influenced the choice, only about
one-third of the subjects would admit that the words
probably had an effect.
During other, similar experiments, researchers also
quizzed people who didn't participate in the experiment
about what they thought people would do in the situation
presented by the experiment. They found an excellent
correlation between what an average outsider would theo-
rize about behavior in that situation and what the experi-
48 CHAPTER TWO
Time and
Consciousness
53
54 CHAPTER THREE
BACK-DATED MEMORY
If you are driving a car and talking simultaneously
and a child darts into the road, you will immediately
slam on the brakes to avoid hitting him. Your conscious-
ness of the braking and your emotions will actually
come after the braking has already occurred. Your pas-
senger may even hear you finish saying a word while
your foot is hitting the brake, but your memory will
make it seem like you consciously hit the brake. Though
the braking was actually done by another unconscious
module of the brain or spinal reflex, your memory is
backdated to keep things tidy and maintain the illusion
of conscious control (Figure 5).
Memory Revision
Fast
Motor Conscious
Stimulus Reaction Perception
t t
o .1 .2 .3 .4 .5
TIME (Seconds)
IS CONSCIOUSNESS IN CONTROL?
IS CONSCIOUSNESS CONTINUOUS?
Memory Illusions
67
68 CHAPTER FOUR
MEMORY GAP-FILLING
than I had done ... I told him that all I had been
able to do was to contain the case and assist in
keeping it out of the White House. I also told him
there was a long way to go before this matter would
end and that I certainly could make no assurances
that the day would not come when this matter
would start to unravel.
When the actual White House tapes of the conversa-
tion are compared to Dean's recall, we can see the creativ-
ity of memory recall. To quote Neisser's article2 in a
medical journal:
Comparison with the transcript shows that hardly
a word of Dean's account is true. Nixon did not say
any of the things attributed to him here: He didn't
ask Dean to sit down, he didn't say Haldeman had
kept him posted, he didn't say Dean had done a
good job (at least not in that part of the conversa-
tion), he didn't say anything about Liddy or the
indictments. Nor had Dean himself said the things
he later describes himself as saying: that he
couldn't take credit, that the matter might unravel
someday, etc. (Indeed, he said just the opposite
later on: "Nothing is going to come crashing
down.") His account is plausible, but entirely in-
correct.
It is apparent that Dean recalled very little of what was
actually said. Yet he was confident enough of his recon-
struction of the conversation to repeat essentially the same
account in sworn verbal testimony. What he recalled was
not what was actually said but a fantasy of what should
have been said from his personal point of view. Again quot-
ing Neisser's article:
72 CHAPTER FOUR
CHANGING MEMORIES
MODULAR MEMORY
87
88 CHAPTER FIVE
TABLE I
Self-Concept Bellefs
My destiny is controlled by myself/gods.
I imagine reality/it exists and I simply experience it.
Time flies/is not significant.
I can/cannot use introspection to see the workings of
my mind.
I can/cannot see visual imagery, calling up pictures in
my mind.
I can think in words/gods speak to me.
All/ some of my thoughts are in words.
I am naturally lucky/unlucky.
I am naturally healthy/unhealthy.
I am basically a good/bad person.
Life is beautiful/horrible.
I am sensitive/insensitive to pain.
My mind is singular/a collection of modules.
I primarily strive for my self/nuclear family/extended
family/tribe / company/country.
practice, that one will seem certain. (Note that the hag's
nose can be the beauty's chin, her mouth a necklace.) This
false feeling of certainty is a basic characteristic of the mind.
A belief becomes a template for making sense of the world,
and, once we fill in properly to make an interpretation
work, it feels convincingly like the only possible one.
The basic assumptions of our self-concept can be dead
wrong and yet appear to be obviously right. The good
news is that, just like our interpretation of Figure 6, our
assumptions can be changed. With practice a new version
of consciousness will click into place and feel natural.
People used to regard of earth as the center of the universe.
Our concept of the self module as the center of our mental
universe is just as false. Learning to see the self module as
a member of a team of specialists instead of the whole team
can go a long way toward improving our understanding
of ourselves and others.
Many of the self-concept beliefs in Table I can be
considered modem "inventions" that have never been
tried before and still have not spread to primitive societies.
For example, our belief that we can directly control our
own destiny and the idea that we can look into our owh
thoughts with introspection are both probably less than
5000 years 01d. 2 Both strongly affect the very nature of the
conscious experience.
o
Modified
idealist
Indigenous
a ,
Externalized conceptualizations
,,-
o possiones
Indigenous
psychology
~Dinka
psychology '"
""""""
,,-,//' @Chewon g
"""'" """
Self in " / Self under
control ,I' "'.. control
" ....
"" ........ Homeric
Greece
"" ........
Tibetan,,' ....'"
Buddism " Our .....
(mystical £!
Indigenous @ ",~ Maori
level) W
psychology
Idealist
Indigenous
0 " ~I----
Kalahari
,
'0 possiones
Modified
psychology Indigenous
psychology
Internalized conceptualizations
6
FIGURE 7. Heelas and Lock's plot of two dimensions of the
self-concepts of eight different cultures studied: degree of self-
control and degree of internalization. (Reprinted with the per-
mission of Academic Press, San Diego, from Indigenous
Psychologies: The Anthropology of the Self by Paul Heelas and
Andrew Lock.)
Boundaries of Self
that is uniquely ours and stays with us for life, the Balinese
names ll change with changes of status. Infants aren't even
given a name until they are 105 days old. That name is used
only sporadically until adolescence, when it ceases to be
used. More commonly used are kinship names, which one
shares with all siblings and cousins within one's genera-
tion. There are also names based on birth order: Wayan for
the firstborn, Nioman for the second, and so on. An adult
who becomes a parent is called "Father of ... " or "Mother
of ... " followed by the child's name. When a grandchild
is born the name changes to "Grandmother of ... " etc.
and similarly when a great-grandchild is born. Public titles
are also used for people with jobs such as postman, politi-
cian, and so on. Social life in such a society also tends to be
less personal, more general and formaL Relationships tend
to be seen as links between representatives of different
groups or classes.
Property concepts are another way to view the
boundaries of self. The nuclear family in the United States
ideally is bounded by the property lines of their home. In
many parts of the world, these boundaries are not so clear.
The Japanese often live in a company compound that
includes recreational facilities and even company vacation
resorts. The American Indians, and many other tribal and
nomadic cultures, have no concept of individual property.
The Dogon people12 of Mali have permanent villages
with a population in the thousands, yet their definition of
house is the people living there, not the structure. The
Dogon house is never sold and is thought of as belonging
to the mythical descendants of the people who occupied
the village before the Dogon arrived. For each individual,
the village is "his" house; his family simply sleeps in a
102 CHAPTER FIVE
them no matter what they are. Once the beliefs that consti-
tute our self-concept are planted in our minds in early
childhood, they tend to grow stronger with every year
because they seem to be continually confirmed by experi-
ence. Only with great mental effort can we use our logic to
see past these mental illusions and find and correct the
errors in our self-concept.
CHAPTER SIX
False Beliefs
113
114 CHAPTER SIX
MENTAL ILLNESS
FINANCIAL BELIEFS
they used to, that the dollar can remain stable and free
from inflation.
The prices of homes also suffer from boom and bust
cycles as a result of changing beliefs and expectations. The
inflation of the 1970s caused home prices to grow dramati-
cally in many parts of the country. As people learned to
expect home prices to double every few years, a kind of
frenzy developed, driving prices to ridiculous heights. In
many areas, less than 30% of the families could afford the
median-priced home. Once this bubble burst, hundreds of
thousands of homes were foreclosed as their value fell
below the loan balance. The idea of reasonable value for a
home is still unreasonably high in some areas because the
drop in values is seen as an aberration rather than a return
to sanity. The original belief in the wisdom of home own-
ership is still alive in most people, despite some very
negative evidence.
URBAN LEGENDS
AN URBAN NIGHTMARE
BELIEF IN MAGIC
141
142 CHAPTER SEVEN
HYPNOTISM: BELIEVED-IN
IMAGININGS
hypnotist can suggest that the room is very hot and beads
of sweat have been known to form on the forehead of some
subjects. Suggestion seems to be able to work directly on
modules that cannot usually be controlled by the self.
People yawning in a crowd can make you yawn also. This
is the result of suggestion even though most people cannot
consciously initiate a real yawn. The placebo effect also
demonstrates the power of suggestion over processes not
under conscious control. Studies have shown that the
immune system can be energized by suggestion or the
placebo effect enough that it significantly changes the
outcome of real diseases, including cancer.
Hypnotic suggestions of paralysis of limbs, blindness to
certain objects, amnesia, insensitivity to pain, and even deaf-
ness demonstrate the power of the mind to distort our
perception and abilities. Experiments have shown that hyp-
notic subjects are actually in control of their own behavior but they
convince themselves that the behavior is involuntary to fulfill their
belief that they are hypnotized. This is not a conscious deception
any more than the one that we all do when we fail to see the
blind spot in our field of vision. It is simply another demon-
stration of how strongly basic beliefs can affect the experi-
ence of consciousness. When people believe that they are
deeply hypnotizable, their behavior and their conscious ex-
perience will confirm that belief.
When experimenters change the expectations of hyp-
nosis by changing their comments and explanations before
they begin? they can totally change the outcome. For
example, if subjects are told that the ability to resist sug-
gestion is one indication of very deep hypnosis, they will
successfully resist having their arm raise during a sug-
gested visualization of an upward pull on their arm. If they
146 CHAPTER SEVEN
CREATING MULTIPLE
CONSCIOUSNESS
Hypnosis can be used to alter consciousness in ways
that split the mind9 into multiple entities. E. Hilgard of
Stanford University performed numerous experiments in
which he used hypnosis to set up a hidden observer in the
subject tha t communicated only through automatic writing.
In one such experiment he suggested a numbing of one
hand to consciousness but not to the hidden observer. He
could then dip the subject'S numbed hand into ice water
and the subject would claim to feel nothing though the
hidden observer would simultaneously write down a mes-
sage indicating pain. When the subjects were interviewed
after the experiment, one said he was annoyed at the superior
attitude of the hidden observer, which seemed amused at
150 CHAPTER SEVEN
Psychotherapy and
Multiple Personalities
The man who once cursed his Jate, now curses himself-and
pays his psychoanalyst.
- John W. Gardner; 1968
A mental illness called hysteria was extremely common
among women in Freud's time. Hysteria could cause pa-
ralysis, loss of consciousness, seizures, blindness, and
other symptoms of physical illness. Hysteric patients were
found to be mostly women who were very easily hypno-
tized and also needful of more attention and respect from
others. Hysteria is rare today, but people with this profile
now suffer from other syndromes that were rare in Freud's
day. Multiple personality disorder (MPD)l is one amazing
example.
Though it was previously considered quite rare, in the
past decade over 20,000 people 2 have been diagnosed as
having MPD. One of the catalysts for this explosion was
probably the best-selling fictionalized book Sybil,3 which
was published in 1973 and sold over four million copies. An
earlier book and movie The Three Faces of Eve also helped
define and popularize the idea of multiple personalities.
Once MPD had been defined in the popular imagina-
tion, positive feedback soon transformed it into an epi-
155
156 CHAPTER EIGHT
reading, or art, and you find that you have a gap in your
memory. This simply means that your self module was not
paying attention and therefore remembers nothing. This is
usually not a problem, but if you have no memory of a
shopping spree, a drinking binge, or other destructive
behavior it can cause serious problems.
Many children have imaginary playmates that develop
unique personalities. Children often talk to these playmates
by name and even answer in a different voice. Likewise,
many normal adults talk to themselves when alone. In fact,
normal variations in people include varying degrees of dis-
sociation between modules. When a questionnaire normally
used to screen mental patients for MPD was given to 415
students lO in a wide range of majors at the University of
Idaho, 8.9% of them scored above the threshold that usually
indicates MPD or other dissociative disorders. Clearly many
normally functioning people have dissociative experiences.
Usually, a module that is not in control is still in the
receptive mode, following and remembering what is hap-
pening. However, each module has its own mental habits.
Since the self module is the one that fills out questionnaires,
high dissociative scores could indicate that the self module
is in the habit of not being receptive at certain times. In
some cases this can be a good mental habit indicating that
the self module is standing back completely to avoid in-
terfering in areas where it is not qualified.
the front porch, and then on the sidewalk, and so forth until
eventually the patient has regained complete freedom of
movement. Homework assignments between therapist vis-
its help the patients learn to progress on their own. The
cognitive part of the treatment involves teaching the pa-
tient techniques for distracting their mind from negative
thoughts and also learning to interpret the feelings of panic
in a healthier way. The principle is to "feel the fear and do
it anyway." Cure rates as high as 80%13 have been reported
with very few relapses. A similar technique is very effective
for obsessive-compulsive ritualistic behavior. The impor-
tant thing is to do the work in the same real-life situations
that would normally trigger the behavior so that the mod-
ule with the problem will be engaged.
SATANIC MEMORIES
should be the goal of therapy. All parents try their best but
fail in varying degrees because of human imperfection.
The Norman Rockwell vision of the perfect family exists
only in fantasy. We are bound to find life disappointing if
we can't even learn to be tolerant of our own parents'
shortcomings, as we all must live in a world of imperfect
people.
CHAPTER NINE
But what am I?
An infant crying in the night:
An infant cryingfor the light:
And with no language but a cry.
- Tennyson. 1850
Months before a baby is born, alllD billion neurons that
will constitute its adult brain are already in place. Neurons
cannot divide as other cells do, but an infant's brain still
grows significantly in the first two years. Most of that
growth results from further development of the rich net-
work of interconnections (axons and dendrites) and sur-
rounding tissue that supports and insulates the neurons
(Figure 9).
Ultimately a neuron may have interconnections to as
many as a quarter of a million other neurons; the average,
however, is more like 10,000 connections. Though most of
the interconnections look like the wild tangle of plants in
a jungle, some sensory and motor connections are quite
regular and clearly follow a genetically determined plan.
Some of these interconnections seem to grow by a "sur-
vival of the fittest" process in which ineffective connec-
tions don't survive. This process works properly only if
there is sensory stimulation during the critical growth
period.
179
-~~~~'-.~.:-
FIGURE 9. All of the neurons you will ever have are present months before birth, but the connecting
dendrites and axons that join them continue to develop during the first two years of life. (From
Brain and Psyche by Jonathan Winson, Doubleday, New York, 1985.)
THE INFANT BRAIN 181
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
Reading is an important skill that sometimes gets
started down the wrong track, causing difficulties later.
For example, children who recognize words holistically, as
they would recognize other objects, do well reading at a
very simple level. As words start getting longer, com-
pound words appear, and spelling becomes a factor, this
initially successful strategy becomes a disaster. Reading
phonically requires a new module, not just adding some
words to a module that recognizes things by shape. Dys-
lexic children have extreme difficulty spelling and often
cannot distinguish between the backward spelling of a
word and the correct spelling. Whereas in recognizing an
object it is important to ignore its orientation (left to right
or vice versa), a word written backwards is simply wrong.
Once children start along the wrong path, the longer
they continue to use the wrong module, the more firmly
ingrained and reinforced it becomes. Remedial exercises
that force children to use the correct strategy must start
from scratch to train the proper module against strong
competition from the module that has the benefit of re-
peated practice.
Starting down a wrong path in reading has major
consequences, so corrective techniques have been devel-
oped, but consider all the other areas where a bad pattern
188 CHAPTER NINE
DEVELOPMENT OF THE
SELF-CONCEPT
SELF-CONTROL
cause they have the physical ability to get into trouble but
still lack self-control.
Self-control is learned from parents, friends, and
teachers. In a supportive environment, successes are
praised and failures are treated as an encouragement to
try harder. Some children learn a very negative attitude
of helplessness from their parents. This learned helpless-
ness can sometimes be undone by utilizing a strong role
model to retrain the child to believe in the power of
trying harder. It is important, however, for children also
to learn to live with their actual limitations, those that
no effort can overcome.
The self module undergoes a major upheaval in the
teen years as children try to define themselves as separate
beings from their parents. Rebellious behavior can often
result as a child tries to redefine boundaries and establish
an identity separate from the parents. In cultures where
the self boundary always includes the extended family the
teenage identity crisis, so common in this country, is rare.
The self module continues to evolve throughout life
with painful transitions often occurring at points where
the self boundary changes. In our society a midlife crisis is
often triggered when grown children move away to seek
employment, forcing the parents to redefine their self-
concept with narrower boundaries.
In cultures where the extended family boundary is a
stable lifetime concept these crises are not a problem. Our
new concept of the individual self is thus a double-edged
sword: It has produced great progress by providing a
mobile work force and freeing up individual creativity, but
it has also produced some painful side effects by breaking
up the unity of the family.
THE INFANT BRAIN 195
module that takes control during the first piece of pie and
effectively changes their personality. The cautious dieting
of their normal self-controlled behavior gives way to out
of control eating, which ends only when they are sick or
out of pie. Bulimics end this pattern by vomiting up what
they have eaten. The binge module has a life of its own and
can develop quite a different personality. Drunks, for ex-
ample, often have a mean brutal personality that is clearly
recognizable when they have lost control.
Work skills and hobbies also usually develop into
unique modules. A piano player can continue playing a
song while another module simultaneously holds a con-
versation about a request for another song. During many
skilled jobs other modules are free to have conversations,
thoughts, and daydreams while the work is in progress.
Almost all of us have a module for driving a car that
makes it possible to independently hold a conversation or
do intensive thinking while driving. Some people have a
driving personality that is obviously distinct from their
normal personality. Gentle people sometimes have an ag-
gressive personality that gets engaged during driving.
That personality may show anger by tailgating and cutting
off other cars, giving the finger, and even cursing. If some-
one cuts them off, a pleasant conversation may be inter-
rupted by a string of obscenities from the driving module,
which has a distinctly different sound and attitude.
Not all modules have the ability to control speech.
Some are able to work on a problem and pass the results
to other modules through intermodule connections. When
you suddenly realize the solution to a problem or remem-
ber a name without having been aware of thinking about
it, the work is clearly done by a nonverbal module working
198 CHAPI'ER NINE
Nonverbal Thinking
201
202 CHAPTER TEN
alter that belief to one that better correlates with the reality
of how the mind actually functions. With practice, you can
learn to modify your conscious experience so that it also
includes the richness of nonverbal thinking. This can sig-
nificantly increase your pleasure in life and allow you to
develop abilities you never thought possible.
Your brain's only job is to process sensory inputs and
respond by producing movement. Movement is, in fact,
the only useful result of the brain's activity: Thought by
itself is useless until it is expressed by some kind of move-
ment such as physical action, speech, writing, or keyboard
entry. In our evolution from lower animals, refinements,
such as language and verbal thought, were simply added
on top of the existing structures that generate movement.
Internal, verbal thought that evolved from speech was a
separate, late addition, not a replacement for sensory
thinking. Nonverbal thinking has continued to evolve and
still does the majority of the mind's work, using nonverbal
knowledge directly to solve problems and generate move-
ment. When you are proficient in sports or dance, your
thoughts are not in words but rather in the form of kines-
thetic images.
When we try to analyze and understand our thoughts,
the result is always in words. This one-sided view, which
ignores all of the nonverbal aspects, gives us a distorted
view of our thinking and knowledge. For example, if we
recall a conversation, we will probably retell only the
words even though a major part of human communica-
tions is nonverbal. It has been estimated that words carry
only 7%2 of the total feeling communicated, while 38% is
inflection and the remaining 55% is communicated in the
form of facial and body expression. Silent movies and
204 CHAPTER TEN
NONVERBAL CONSCIOUSNESS
The convenience of words can obscure an important
component of our own experience of consciousness.
Words are used to define the very meaning of conscious-
ness: in the dictionary, in philosophical and psychological
discussions, and even in this book. Our false belief in the
power of verbal introspection causes us to imagine a com-
plete mental world built on words.
H. D. Barlow3 of Cambridge University wrote:
It is argued that consciousness primarily arises in the
relation between one individual and another, and is
not a property of a brain in isolation. One can, of
course, be conscious when one is alone, but it is
suggested that on these occasions one is rehearsing
future discourse with an imagined individual.
... Thus the survival value of consciousness consists
of a peculiar form of gregarious behavior it generates
in man; it is nature's trick to chain him to the herd.
There is, however, another kind of consciousness that
uses no words. Stroke patients can have large parts of their
brain damaged by loss of blood circulation. Those who
completely lose their powers of logical speech provide a
clear demonstration of consciousness without words.
Though unable to speak about it, they are clearly still
conscious, with their nonverbal personality and knowl-
edge intact. Though their self module is essentially dead,
they defiantly remain conscious. The kind of conscious-
ness they feel is present in all of us, but it tends to get
upstaged by the easy verbalization of the self module.
Some brain tumor patients have even had the entire
left hemisphere surgically removed. Though they lost all
NONVERBAL THINKING 207
CREATIVE THINKING
NONVERBAL LOGIC
simply pain. The fine distinctions are all things you will
have to fabricate with words and the feelings surely will
suffer in the translation. If you want to theorize about the
reasons, do it as you would for a friend-with the full
realization that the words are coming from an observer.
Love
Merging the Self
229
230 CHAPTER ELEVEN
BODY LANGUAGE
SENSUOUS LOVE
247
248 CHAPTER TWELVE
tity. If the home team wins, fans are heard to yell "We won!"
and "We are the best!" even though most of the players are
not even from the city identified with the team. Teenage
gangs are on the increase because they provide an exten-
sion of the self and an identity that fills the void left by the
collapse of both the nuclear and the extended family. The
self-improvement industry is thriving with workshops on
such things as channeling, which puts your self in touch
with a spirit guide who is attached to you for life. Products
for losing weight, firming up, and eliminating wrinkles
fight for advertising space. One cable channel sells nothing
but exercise machines and self-improvement products. We
even have a popular magazine called Self. Overeating and
drug abuse may just be attempts to fill the empty self.
Shopaholics try to fill their emptiness by filling their closets.
The Cognitive
Revolution
In 1981 Dr. Roger Sperry of Caltech won a Nobel Prize for
his pioneering work on understanding the organization of
the brain. Since it was Sperry's work that led to the insights
we have been exploring in this book, a review of his
fascinating split-brain experiments is in order.
The brains of all mammals are divided into two dis-
tinctly separate halves, or hemispheres, which are con-
nected only by a narrow band of nerves called the corpus
callosum. Each half of the brain is directly connected only
to the nerves and muscles on the opposite side of the body.
The optic nerve connections to the retina of the eye are
likewise crossed so that the right half of the brain sees only
the left side of the visual field 1 and vice versa. This sepa-
ration of control has a survival value because during a
battle you have two independent channels at work:
Threats from the right can be dealt with by the left brain
while at the same time the right brain handles threats from
the left.
257
258 APPENDIX ONE
world problems: The whole is often much more than the sum
of its parts, and most important emergent properties in
nature are not at all predictable by studying the basic
components.
For example, though water is simply a combination
of hydrogen and oxygen atoms, studying the charac-
teristics of these atoms gives us no hint of what the mar-
velous properties of water will be. Likewise, studying an
individual bee gives us no inkling of the complex proper-
ties that will emerge when bees assemble together in a
colony. The miracle of human intelligence and conscious-
ness that emerges from a relatively random assemblage of
neurons would never be predicted by studying those neu-
rons in isolation.
Behaviorism was an attempt to make psychology "sci-
entific" by objectively studying the basic stimulus-response
elements of behavior. Consciousness and introspection were
completely rejected as factors because they appeared to be so
unreliable. Between 1915 and 1965, behaviorism was the
dominant school of psychology in the United States. It
failed to produce useful results because consciousness can-
not be ignored and the emergent properties of the mind are
simply not predictable from reductionist models. Though
introspection is inaccurate fantasy when applied to all but
the self module, it is nonetheless important because it often
affects our behavior since we think it is accurate. Conscious-
ness is often not in control, but it can and does drive our
behavior at some very crucial times.
The cognitive revolution was finally able to displace
behaviorism in the 1970s because a new paradigm was
developed that allowed a top-down approach to systems
analysis to be superimposed on the traditional bottom-up
266 APPENDIX ONE
A Summary of
Conclusions
1. The neurons of the brain spontaneously organ-
ize into hundreds of separate, specialized mod-
ules.
2. At any given moment the one module that has
been most reinforced in the current context takes
control of our speech.
3. A similar control-to-the-strongest mechanism al-
lows only one module to control body move-
ment at any given moment. This mayor may not
be the same module that controls speech.
4. All modules can monitor the inputs from the
senses at all times, though some do not because
some sense inputs are not relevant to their spe-
cialty.
5. Some modules produce an experience of con-
sciousness, but in modem Western culture, one
module, which we call the self, tends to hog the
spotlight and make us ignore other kinds of
consciousness.
269
270 APPENDIX TWO
273
274 NOTES
you see it. If your left eye doesn't see well, turn the book
upside down and use your right.
4. Dennett (1992, p. 34).
5. Eye movements plotted with special optical sensing
equipment. These eye motions generally occur at a rate of
about three to five movements per second, even when we
think our eyes are still (Kosslyn and Koenig, 1992, p. 100).
6. Quote from Crick (1994, p. 167).
7. Dennett (1991, p. 361).
8. Cheating experiment demonstrating Festinger's
theory of cognitive dissonance (Gazzaniga, 1985, p. 139).
9. In the stockings experiment the bias may have been
related to the natural habit of scanning left to right. Since
the last stocking was as good as the others, it got chosen
as best (Nesbit and Wilson, 1977).
CHAPTER FOUR:
MEMORY ILLUSIONS
CHAPTER FIVE:
OTHER CONCEPTS OF SELF
1. In the world of computer software the self-concept
would represent the bottom layer on which specific pro-
grams such as your individual personality are added. It is
a bit like a computer operating system: a graphical com-
puter interface versus text orientation. Specific programs
loaded on top of it will have drastically different charac-
teristics depending on the operating system. Likewise,
individual personalities will have drastically different
characteristics depending on the underlying self-concept.
2. For a detailed development of the idea of conscious-
ness as a recent invention, see The Origin of Consciousness
in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind by Julian Jaynes
(1976), and also Lyons (1978).
3. Indigenous Psychologies: The Anthropology of the Self,
edited by Heelas and Lock (1981, p. 40).
4. Heelas and Lock (1981, p. 9).
NOTES 279
CHAPTER TEN:
NONVERBAL THINKING
1. Austin (1974, p. 103).
2. Mehrabian (1972) cited in Masters and Johnson
(1982, p. 250).
3. Barlow (1980, p. 81).
4. Gott (1973).
286 NOTES
CHAPTER ELEVEN:
LOVE-MERGING THE SELF
CHAPTER TWELVE:
THE EMPTY SELF
1. The suicide rate among teenagers has tripled in the
past 30 years. Victims tend to be solitary boys who hold
themselves to a high standard. Suicide is rare before the
age of 12, which is when awareness of self becomes acute
(Berk, 1989, box 11.1).
2. Population statistics and the concept of the empty
self from Cushman (1990, p. 603).
3. Sampson (1988, p. 19).
4. Of course, some black, gay, and women's groups
exist that have a positive attitude and can actually help
people to be accepted into mainstream society; however,
the victim approach seems to have more appeal in recent
NOTES 289
APPENDIX ONE:
THE COGNITIVE REVOLUTION
291
292 SELECTED BmLIOGRAPHY
Brain Sciences, 16, 1-14. A key article on how children learn the
concept of intentions.
Gott, Peggy S. (1973). Cognitive abilities following right and left
hemispherectomy. Cortex, 9, 266-273.
Greenwald, Anthony. (1992, June). Unconscious cognition re-
claimed. American Psychologist, pp. 766-777.
Guidano, Vittorio F. (1987). Complexity of the self: A developmental
approach to psychopathology and therapy. New York: Guilford
Press.
Hadamard, Jacques. (1945). The psychology of invention in the
mathematical field. New York: Dover Publications.
Hadley, & Strupp, Hans H. (1976). Contemporary views on
negative effects in psychotherapy. Psychiatry, 33, 1291-1294.
Hamachek, Don. (1971). Encounters with the self. New York: Holt,
Rinehart.
Harris, Harold. (Ed.). (1976). Astride the two cultures. New York:
Random House.
Harth, Erich. (1992). Windows on the mind. New York: Morrow.
Hecaen, H., & De Ajuriagueira, J. (1964). Left handedness: Manual
superiority and cerebral dominance (E. Ponder, transl.). New
York: Grune & Stratton.
Heelas, Paul, & Lock, Andrew. (Eds.). (1981). Indigenous psycholo-
gies: The anthropology of the self. New York: Academic Press.
Hilgard, Ernst R. (1977). Divided consciousness. New York: Wiley.
Humphry, Nicholas. (1992). A history of the mind. New York:
Simon & Schuster.
Hunt, Morton. (1982). The universe within. New York: Simon &
Schuster.
Ischlondsky, N. Dorin. (1955). The inhibitory process in the
cerebrophysicallaboratory and in the clinic. Journal ofNerv-
ous and Mental Disease, 121,5-18.
James, William. (1980). Principles of psychology, Vol 1. Fawcet
(1963, p. 294).
Jankowiak, W. R., & Fischer, E. F. (1992). Across-cultural perspec-
tive on romantic love. Ethnology, 31(2), 149-155.
SELECTED BmLlOGRAPHY 297
Ross, Colin. (1991). The dissociated self and the cultural disso-
ciation barrier. Dissociation, 4(1), 55-61.
Ross, Jerilyn. (1994). Triumph over fear. New York: Bantam Books.
An excellent book on contextual cognitive-behavioral therapy for
phobias.
Rowan, John. (1990). Subpersonalities: The people inside us. Lon-
don: Routledge.
Sacks, Oliver. (1970). The man who mistook his wife for a hat. New
York: Harper. A very entertaining account of several right hemi-
sphere damage cases.
Salkovskis, Paul M. (1989). Obsessions and compulsions. In Jan
Scott, Mark Williams, & Aaron Beck (Eds.), Cognitive therapy
and clinical practice. London: Routledge.
Sampson, Edward E. (1985). The decentralization of identity.
American Psychologist, 49(11), 1203-1211.
Sampson, Edward. (1988). The debate on individualism: Indige-
nous psychologies of the individual and their role in per-
sonal and societal functioning. American Psychologist, 43(1),
15-22.
Sanders, Barbara. (1992). The imaginary companion in multiple
personality disorder. Dissociation, 5(3),159-162.
Schacter, Daniel. (1992). Understanding implicit memory: Acog-
nitive neuroscience approach. American Psychologist, 47(4),
559-569.
Schieve, William c., & Allyn, Peter M. (Eds.). (1982). Self-organi-
zation and dissipative structures: Applications in the physical
and social sciences. Austin: University of Texas.
Schnabel, Jim. (1994). Chronic claims of alien abduction and
some other traumas as self victimization syndromes. Disso-
ciation, 6(1),51-59.
Scott, Jan, Williams, J. Mark, & Beck, Aaron. (Eds.). (1989).
Cognitive therapy in clinical practice. New York: Routledge.
Searle, John R. (1992). The rediscovery of the mind. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
302 SELECTED BmLIOGRAPHY
305
306 INDEX