Test 6
Test 6
Test 6
com/
B However, reports of its decline were not entirely correct. Interrupted brome has enjoyed a revival, one
that’s not due to science. Because of the work of one gardening enthusiast, interrupted brome is thriving as a
pot plant. The relaunching into the wild of Britain’s almost extinct plant has excited conservationists
everywhere.
C Originally, Philip Smith didn’t know that he had the very unusual grass at his own home. When he heard
about the grass becoming extinct, he wanted to do something surprising. He attended a meeting of the
British Botanical Society in Manchester in 1979, and seized His opporlunity. He said that it was so
disappointing to hear about the demise of the interrupted brome. “What a pity we didn’t research it further!”
he added. Then. all of a sudden he displayed his pots with so called “extinct grass” lot all to see.
D Smith had kept the seeds from the last stronghold of the grass, Pamisford in 1963. It was then when the
grass stalled to disappear from the wild. Smith cultivated the grass, year after year. Ultimately, it was his
curiosity in the plant that saved it. not scientific or technological projects that
E For now, the bromes future is guaranteed. The seeds front Smith’s plants have beet, securely stored in the
cutting edge facilities of Millennium Seed Bank at Wakehurst Place in Sussex. And living plants thrive at the
botanic gardens at Kew, Edinburgh and Cambridge. This year, seeds are also saved at sites all across the
country and the grass now nourishes at several public gardens too.
F The grass will now be reintroduced to the British countryside. As a part of the Species Recovery Project,
the organisation English Nature will re-introduce interrupted brome into the agricultural landscape,
provided willing farmers are found. Alas, the grass is neither beautiful not practical. it is undoubtedly a
weed, a weed that nobody cares for these days. The brome wax probably never widespread enough to annoy
farmers and today, no one would appreciate its productivity or nutritious qualities. As a grass, it leaves a lot
to be desired by agriculturalists.
G Smith’s research has attempted to answer the question of where the grass came from. His research points
to mutations from other weedy grasses as the most likely source. So close is the relationship that
interrupted brome was originally deemed to be a mere variety of soil brome by the great Victorian
taxonomist Professor Hackel. A botanist from the 19th century, Druce. had taken notes on the grass and
convinced his peers that the grass deserved its own status as a species. Despite Druce growing up in poverty
and his self-taught profession, he became the leading botanist of his time.
https://www.ieltspdf.com/ Page 1
H Where the grass came from may be clear, but the timing of its birth may be tougher to find out. A clue lies
in its penchant for growing as a weed in fields shared with a fodder crop, in particular nitrogen-fixing
legumes such as sainfoin, lucerne or clover. According to agricultural historian Joan Thirsk. the humble
sainfoin and its company were first noticed in Britain in the early 17th century. Seeds brought in from the
Continent were sown in pastures to feed horses and other livestock. However, back then, only a few
enthusiastic gentlemen were willing to use the new crops for their prized horses.
I Not before too long though, the need to feed the parliamentary armies in Scotland, England and behind was
more pressing than ever. farmers were forced to produce more bread, cheese and beer. And by 1650 the
legumes were increasingly introduced into arable rotations, to serve as green nature to boost grain yields. A
bestseller of its day, Nathaniel Fiennes’s Sainfoin Improved, published in 1671, helped to spread the word.
With the advent of sainfoin, clover and lucerne. Britain’s very own rogue grass had suddenly at rivet.
J Although the credit for the discovery of interrupted brome goes to a Miss A. M. Barnard, who collected the
first specimens at Odsey, Bedfordshire, in 1849, the grass had probably lurked undetected in the English
countryside for at least a hundred years. Smith thinks the plant- the world’s version of the Dodo probably
evolved in the late 17th or early 18th century, once sainfoin became established. Due mainly to the
development of the motor car and subsequent decline of fodder crops for horses, the brome declined rapidly
over the 20th century. Today, sainfoin has almost disappeared from the countryside, though occasionally its
colourful flowers are spotted in lowland nature reserves. More recently artificial fertilizers have made
legume rotations unnecessary.
K The close relationship with out-of-fashion crops spells trouble for those seeking to re-establish
interrupted brome in today’s countryside. Much like the once common arable weeds, such as the corncockle,
its seeds cannot survive long in the soil. Each spring, the brome relied on farmers to resow its seeds; in the
days before weed killers and advanced seed sieves, an ample supply would have contaminated supplies of
crop seed. However fragile seeds are not the brome’s only problem: this species is also unwilling to release
its seeds as they ripen. According to Smith. The grass will struggle to survive even in optimal conditions. It
would be very difficult to thrive amongst its more resilient competitors found in today’s improved
agricultural landscape
L Nonetheless, interrupted brome’s reluctance to thrive independently may have some benefits. Any farmer
willing to foster this unique contribution to the world’s flora can rest assured that the grass will never
become an invasive pest. Restoring interrupted brome to its rightful home could bring other benefits too,
particularly if this strange species is granted recognition as a national treasure. Thanks to British farmers,
interrupted brome was given the chance to evolve in the first place. Conservationists would like to see the
grass grow once again in its natural habitat and perhaps, one day, seeing the grass become a badge of
honour for a new generation of environmentally conscious farmers.
Questions 1-8
Do the following statements agree with the information given in Reading Passage 1? In boxes 1-8 on you
answer sheet, write
https://www.ieltspdf.com/ Page 2
TRUE if the statement agrees with the information
FALSE if the statement contradicts the information
NOT GIVEN If there is no information on this
1. The name of interrupted brome came from the unprepossessing grass disappeared from places in the
world for a period.
2. Interrupted brome seeds cannot sprout because they were kept accidentally at unsuitable temperature.
3. Philip Smith works at University of Manchester.
4. Kew Botanic Gardens will operate English Nature.
5. Interrupted brome grew unwantedly at the sides of sainfoin.
6. Legumes were used for feeding livestock and enriching the soil.
7. The spread of seeds of interrupted brome depends on the harvesting of the farmers.
8. Only the weed killers can stop interrupted brome from becoming an invasive pest.
Questions 9-13
Look at the following opinions or deeds (Questions 9-13) and the list of people below. Match each opinion or
deed with the correct person, A-F.
A A. M. Barnard
B Philip Smith
C George Claridge Druce
D Joan Thirsk
E Professor Hackel
F Nathaniel Fiennes
A Last winter’s floods on the rivers of central Europe were among the worst since the Middle Ages, and as
winter storms return, the spectre of floods is returning too. Just weeks ago, the river Rhone in south-east
France burst its banks, driving 15,000 people from their homes, and worse could be on the way.
Traditionally, river engineers have gone for Plan A: get rid of the water fast, draining it off the land and down
to the sea in tall-sided rivers re-engineered as high-performance drains. But however big they dug city
drains, however wide and straight they made the rivers, and however high they built the banks, the floods
kept coming back to taunt them, from the Mississippi to the Danube. Arid when the floods came, they
seemed to be worse than ever. No wonder engineers are turning to Plan B: sap the water’s destructive
strength by dispersing it into fields, forgotten lakes, flood plains and aquifers.
B Back in the days when rivers took a more tortuous path to the sea, flood waters lost impetus and volume
while meandering across flood plains and idling through wetlands and inland deltas. But today the water
tends to have an unimpeded journey to the sea. And this means that when it rains in the uplands, the water
https://www.ieltspdf.com/ Page 3
comes down all at once. Worse, whenever we close off more flood plains, the river’s flow farther
downstream becomes more violent and uncontrollable. Dykes are only as good as their weakest link—-and
the water will unerringly find it. By trying to turn the complex hydrology of rivers into the simple mechanics
of a water pipe, engineers have often created danger where they promised safety, and intensified the floods
they meant to end. Take the Rhine, Europe’s most engineered river. For two centuries, German engineers
have erased its backwaters and cut it off from its flood plain.
C Today, the river has lost 7 percent of its original length and runs up to a third faster. When it rains hard in
the Alps, the peak flows from several tributaries coincide in the main river, where once they arrived
separately. And with four-fifths of the lower Rhine’s flood plain barricaded off, the waters rise ever higher.
The result is more frequent flooding that does ever-greater damage to the homes, offices and roads that sit
on the flood plain. Much the same has happened in the US on the mighty Mississippi, which drains the
world’s second largest river catchment into the Gulf of Mexico.
D The European Union is trying to improve rain forecasts and more accurately model how intense rains
swell rivers. That may help cities prepare, but it won’t stop the floods. To do that, say hydrologists, you need
a new approach to engineering not just rivers, but the whole landscape. The UK’s Environment Agency -
which has been granted an extra £150 million a year to spend in the wake of floods in 2000 that cost the
country £1 billion- puts it like this: “The focus is now on working with the forces of nature. Towering
concrete walks are out, and new wetlands : are in.” To help keep London’s feet dry, the agency is breaking
the Thames’s banks upstream and reflooding 10 square kilometres of ancient flood plain at Otmoor outside
Oxford. Nearer to London it has spent £100 million creating new wetlands and a relief channel across 16
kilometres of flood plain to protect the town of Maidenhead, as well as the ancient playing fields of Eton
College. And near the south coast, the agency is digging out channels to reconnect old meanders on the river
Cuckmere in East Sussex that were cut off by flood banks 150 years ago.
E The same is taking place on a much grander scale in Austria, in one of Europe’s largest river restorations to
date. Engineers are regenerating flood plains along 60 kilometres of the river Drava as it exits the Alps. They
are also widening the river bed and channelling it back into abandoned meanders, oxbow lakes and
backwaters overhung with willows. The engineers calculate that the restored flood plain can now store up to
10 million cubic metres of flood waters and slow storm surges coming out of the Alps by more than an hour,
protecting towns as far downstream as Slovenia and Croatia.
F “Rivers have to be allowed to take more space. They have to be turned from flood-chutes into flood-
foilers,” says Nienhuis. And the Dutch, for whom preventing floods is a matter of survival, have gone furthest.
A nation built largely on drained marshes and seabed had the fright of its life in 1993 when the Rhine almost
overwhelmed it. The same happened again in 1995, when a quarter of a million people were evacuated from
the Netherlands. But a new breed of “soft engineers” wants our cities to become porous, and Berlin is their
shining example. Since reunification, the city’s massive redevelopment has been governed by tough new
rules to prevent its drains becoming overloaded after heavy rains. Harald Kraft, an architect working in the
city, says: “We now see rainwater as a resource to be kept rather than got rid of at great cost.” A good
illustration is the giant Potsdamer Platz, a huge new commercial redevelopment by Daimler Chrysler in the
heart of the city.
https://www.ieltspdf.com/ Page 4
G Los Angeles has spent billions of dollars digging huge drains and concreting river beds to carry away the
water from occasional intense storms. The latest plan is to spend a cool $280 million raising the concrete
walls on the Los Angeles river by another 2 metres. Yet many communities still flood regularly. Meanwhile
this desert city is shipping in water from hundreds of kilometres away in northern California and from the
Colorado river in Arizona to fill its taps and swimming pools, and irrigate its green spaces. It all sounds like
bad planning. “In LA we receive half the water we need in rainfall, and we throw it away. Then we spend
hundreds of millions to import water,” says Andy Lipkis, an LA environmentalist, along with citizen groups
like Friends of the Los Angeles River and Unpaved LA, want to beat the urban flood hazard and fill the taps
by holding onto the city’s flood water. And it’s not just a pipe dream. The authorities this year launched a
$100 million scheme to road-test the porous city in one flood-hit community in Sun Valley. The plan is to
catch the rain that falls on thousands of driveways, parking lots and rooftops in the valley. Trees will soak up
water from parking lots. Homes and public buildings will capture roof water to irrigate gardens and parks.
And road drains will empty into old gravel pits and other leaky places that should recharge the city’s
underground water reserves. Result: less flooding and more water for the city. Plan B says every city should
be porous, every river should have room to flood naturally and every coastline should be left to build its own
defences. It sounds expensive and utopian, until you realise how much we spend trying to drain cities and
protect our watery margins -and how bad we are at it.
Questions 14-19
Reading Passage 2 has seven paragraphs, A-G. Which paragraph contains the following information?
Questions 20-23
Do the following statements agree with the information given in Reading Passage 2? In boxes 20-23 on you
answer sheet, write
20. In the ancient times, the people in Europe made their efforts to improve the river banks, so the flood was
becoming less severe than before.
21. Flood makes river shorter than it used to be, which means faster speed and more damage to the
constructions on flood plain.
22. The new approach in the UK is better than that in Austria.
23. At least 300,000 people left from Netherlands in 1995.
Questions 24-26
Complete the sentences below. Choose NO MORE THAN TWO WORDS from the passage for each answer.
https://www.ieltspdf.com/ Page 5
UK’s Environment Agency carried out one innovative approach: a wetland is generated not far from the city
of (24)……………………………to protect it from flooding, (25)…………………………..suggested that cities should be
porous, and Berlin set a good example.
Another city devastated by heavy storms casually is (26)……………………….though government pours billions
of dollars each year in order to solve the problem.
As Daniel Haworth is settled into a high chair and wheeled behind a black screen, a sudden look of worry
furrows his 9-month-old brow. His dark blue eyes dart left and right in search of the familiar reassurance of
his mother’s face. She calls his name and makes soothing noises, but Daniel senses something unusual is
happening. He sucks his fingers for comfort, but, finding no solace, his month crumples, his body stiffens, and
he lets rip an almighty shriek of distress. This is the usual expression when babies are left alone or
abandoned. Mom picks him up, reassures him, and two minutes later, a chortling and alert Daniel returns to
the darkened booth behind the screen and submits himself to baby lab, a unit set up in 2005 at the
University of Manchester in northwest England to investigate how babies think.
Watching infants piece life together, seeing their senses, emotions and motor skills take shape, is a source of
mystery and endless fascination—at least to parents and developmental psychologists. We can decode their
signals of distress or read a million messages into their first smile. But how much do we really know about
what’s going on behind those wide, innocent eyes? How much of their understanding of and response to the
world comes preloaded at birth? How much is built from scratch by experience? Such are the questions
being explored at baby lab. Though the facility is just 18 months old and has tested only 100 infants, it’s
already challenging current thinking on what babies know and how they come to know it.
Daniel is now engrossed in watching video clips of a red toy train on a circular track. The train disappears
into a tunnel and emerges on the other side. A hidden device above the screen is tracking Daniel’s eyes as
they follow the train and measuring the diametre of his pupils 50 times a second. As the child gets bored—or
“habituated”, as psychologists call the process— his attention level steadily drops. But it picks up a little
whenever some novelty is introduced. The train might be green, or it might be blue. And sometimes an
impossible thing happens— the train goes into the tunnel one color and comes out another.
Variations of experiments like this one, examining infant attention, have been a standard tool of
developmental psychology ever since the Swiss pioneer of the field, Jean Piaget, started experimenting on
his children in the 1920s. Piaget’s work led him to conclude that infants younger than 9 months have no
innate knowledge of how the world works or any sense of “object permanence” (that people and things still
exist even when they’re not seen). Instead, babies must gradually construct this knowledge from experience.
Piaget’s “constructivist” theories were massively influential on postwar educators and psychologist, but over
the past 20 years or so they have been largely set aside by a new generation of “nativist” psychologists and
cognitive scientists whose more sophisticated experiments led them to theorise that infants arrive already
equipped with some knowledge of the physical world and even rudimentary programming for math and
language. Baby lab director Sylvain Sirois has been putting these smart-baby theories through a rigorous set
of tests. His conclusions so far tend to be more Piagetian: “Babies,” he says, “know nothing.”
https://www.ieltspdf.com/ Page 6
What Sirois and his postgraduate assistant Lain Jackson are challenging is the interpretation of a variety of
classic experiments begun in the mid-1980s in which babies were shown physical events that appeared to
violate such basic concepts as gravity, solidity and contiguity. In one such experiment, by University of
Illinois psychologist Renee Baillargeon, a hinged wooden panel appeared to pass right through a box.
Baillargeon and M.I.T’s Elizabeth Spelke found that babies as young as 3 1/2 months would reliably look
longer at the impossible event than at the normal one. Their conclusion: babies have enough built-in
knowledge to recognise that something is wrong.
Sirois does not take issue with the way these experiments were conducted. “The methods are correct and
replicable,” he says, “it’s the interpretation that’s the problem.” In a critical review to be published in the
forthcoming issue of the European Journal of Developmental Psychology, he and Jackson pour cold water
over recent experiments that claim to have observed innate or precocious social cognition skills in infants.
His own experiments indicate that a baby’s fascination with physically impossible events merely reflects a
response to stimuli that are novel. Data from the eye tracker and the measurement of the pupils (which
widen in response to arousal or interest) show that impossible events involving familiar objects are no more
interesting than possible events involving novel objects. In other words, when Daniel had seen the red train
come out of the tunnel green a few times, he gets as bored as when it stays the same color. The mistake of
previous research, says Sirois, has been to leap to the conclusion that infants can understand the concept of
impossibility from the mere fact that they are able to perceive some novelty in it. “The real explanation is
boring,” he says.
So how do babies bridge the gap between knowing squat and drawing triangles—a task Daniel’s sister Lois,
2 1/2, is happily tackling as she waits for her brother? “Babies have to learn everything, but as Piaget was
saying, they start with a few primitive reflexes that get things going,” said Sirois. For example, hardwired in
the brain is an instinct that draws a baby’s eyes to a human face. From brain imaging studies we also know
that the brain has some sort of visual buffer that continues to represent objects after they have been
removed—a lingering perception rather than conceptual understanding. So when babies encounter novel or
unexpected events, Sirois explains, “there’s a mismatch between the buffer and the information they’re
getting at that moment. And what you do when you’ve got a mismatch is you try to clear the buffer. And that
takes attention.” So learning, says Sirois, is essentially the laborious business of resolving mismatches. “The
thing is, you can do a lot of it with this wet sticky thing called a brain. It’s a fantastic, statistical-learning
machine”. Daniel, exams ended, picks up a plastic tiger and, chewing thoughtfully upon its heat, smiles as if
to agree.
Questions 27-32
Do the following statements agree with the information given in Reading Passage 3? In boxes 27-32 on you
answer sheet, write
https://www.ieltspdf.com/ Page 7
30. Piaget’s theory was rejected by parents in 1920s.
31. Sylvain Sirois’s conclusion on infant’s cognition is similar to Piaget’s.
32. Sylvain Sirois found serious flaws in the experimental designs by Baillargeon and Elizabeth Spelke.
Questions 33-37
Complete each sentence with the correct ending, A-E, below. Write the correct letter, A-E, in boxes 33-37 on
your answer sheet.
33. Jean Piaget thinks infants younger than 9 months won’t know something existing
34. Jean Piaget thinks babies only get the knowledge
35. Some cognitive scientists think babies have the mechanism to learn a language
36. Sylvain Sirois thinks that babies can reflect a response to stimuli that are novel
37. Sylvain Sirois thinks babies’ attention level will drop
Questions 38-40
Choose the correct letter, A, B, C or D. Write the correct letter in boxes 38-40 on your answer sheet.
39. What can we know from the writer in the fourth paragraph?
A The theories about what baby knows changed over time.
B Why the experiments that had been done before were rejected.
C Infants have the innate knowledge to know the external environment.
D Piaget’s “constructivist” theories were massively influential on parents.
40. What can we know from the argument of the experiment about the baby in the sixth paragraph?
A Infants are attracted by various colours of the trains all the time.
B Sylvain Sirois accuses misleading approaches of current experiments.
C Sylvain Sirois indicates that only impossible events make children interested.
D Sylvain Sirois suggests that novel things attract baby’s attention.
https://www.ieltspdf.com/ Page 8