Cheating Perceptions Notes
Cheating Perceptions Notes
Kelly Honz
Ralston High School, Ralston, NE
Kenneth A. Kiewra
University of Nebraska-Lincoln
Ya-Shu Yang
University of Connecticut
Abstract
This study investigated high school students’ perceptions of cheating and its prevalence. Students were
administered the Academic Honesty Survey to determine their perceptions and prevalence of cheating
across three academic settings: tests, homework, and report writing. Overall, students had traditional
perceptions of what constitutes cheating. Despite these perceptions, most students cheated. In addition,
cheating perceptions and prevalence varied across academic settings. Perceptions and prevalence declined
going from test to homework to report writing settings. Three other interesting patterns emerged. First,
cheating was tied to effort. Cheating actions that still required students to exert effort were viewed as
less dishonest than those that required little effort. Second, cheating was tied to giving versus receiving.
Giving information was viewed less harshly than receiving it. Last, cheating perceptions were tied to
environment. Cheating behaviors occurring outside the classroom were viewed less harshly than those
occurring inside the classroom.
“Just because you plagiarize, that’s not like cheating,” a The prevalence of cheating now seems particularly
freshman high school student informed his teacher, the first pervasive where digital technology is involved. The rapid
author of this article. The student could define plagiarism, and expansion and development of digital technology has trans-
he described various methods of cheating for tests and other formed academic cheating into “digital cheating.” In a recent
schoolwork, but he simply did not recognize that plagiarism New York Times article, educators and school administrators
is cheating. “Teachers don’t tell you plagiarism is cheating; spoke about how digital forms of academic dishonesty are on
they just tell you not to copy and paste,” he argued. This the rise (Glater, 2006). One possible reason for this increase
conversation led the teacher, in partnership with educational is the huge amount of information that is rapidly accessible
psychologists, to investigate high school students’ percep- via computers, personal digital assistants (PDAs), search
tions about and prevalence of cheating. How many other engines, instant messenger systems, cell phones, and MP3
students have misperceptions about academic dishonesty players/ iPods™. Students are now capable of using these
and how many students actually cheat? systems to plagiarize, take credit for work done by others,
Cheating is a pervasive problem within American high falsify data, and download articles to copy and paste on tests
schools. The topic of student cheating has been researched— and assignments.
both in terms of perceptions and prevalence. In terms of In terms of perceptions about what constitutes cheating
prevalence, most studies report that about 75 percent of and the origin of faulty perceptions, students often blame their
students cheat. Bruggeman (1996) compared prevalence cheating on teachers’ failure to explain cheating adequately
of cheating at secular and parochial schools. He found that (recall the student’s comments at the start) or to enforce aca-
cheating and lying were prevalent at both types of schools, demic honesty (McCabe 1999). Students might be right. Less
with between 70 percent and 80 percent of students engag- cheating occurs when students are taught ethical guidelines
ing in dishonest actions. Somewhat surprisingly, cheating (Ames & Eskridge, 1992; McCabe & Treviño, 1993). And,
was no more prevalent at one type of school than the other. McCabe and colleagues (McCabe, Treviño, & Buttefireld,
Similarly, Whitley (1998) reported that nearly three in four 2001) found that although teachers support academic honesty
students admitted to cheating on academic work. Moreover, policies, they are reluctant to punish cheating. As a result,
McCabe’s (2001) study of 4,500 U.S. schools reported that students witness their peers cheating and getting away with
74 percent of students admitted to cheating on exams and as it. Consequently, they come to perceive cheating as com-
many as 90 percent of students admitted to using the Internet monplace and acceptable.
to plagiarize. According to many commentators, educators, Educators, meanwhile, often attribute cheating to a
and researchers, the phenomenon of cheating has reached fault in students (Anderman & Midgley, 2004). Anderman
“epidemic” proportions (e.g., ABC News Productions, 2004; and Midgley conducted a longitudinal study investigating
McCabe & Stephens, 2006). changes in students’ perceptions of cheating behavior. Results
Participants Procedures
Participants were 100 high school juniors from four Participating students in four different English classes
25-student English classes in a large Midwestern high completed the Academic Honesty Survey at the start of the
school. This public high school enrolled students primarily class period in their respective classrooms on the same day.
from middle SES homes who, on average, had ACT com- The primary researcher administered the surveys to each
posite scores of 24. Juniors were included because they had class. The surveys were distributed and verbal instructions
sufficient opportunity to form perceptions about cheating for completing the survey were given. In particular, students
and to practice or counter those perceptions. On average, were asked to answer honestly, knowing that their answers
participants had a grade point average (GPA) of 3.0 on a would remain anonymous. Students had the opportunity
four-point scale, worked about eleven hours each week, and to ask questions before beginning the survey. All students
spent between one and ten hours per week participating in completed the survey within fifteen minutes.
extracurricular activities.
Items 4-8 pertain to test taking. 12. Doing individual homework with a partner
4. Glancing at someone’s answers during the test Yes No
Yes No 0 1-2 3-4 5 or more
0 1-2 3-4 5 or more 13. Giving a completed homework assignment to
5. Providing answers to someone during the test another student
Yes No Yes No
0 1-2 3-4 5 or more 0 1-2 3-4 5 or more
6. Using outside notes during the test 14. Submitting someone’s homework as your own
Yes No Yes No
0 1-2 3-4 5 or more 0 1-2 3-4 5 or more
7. Providing test questions to someone yet to take Items 15-18 pertain to writing reports.
the test 15. Writing a report based on the movie instead of
Yes No reading the book
0 1-2 3-4 5 or more Yes No
8. Providing test answers to someone yet to take 0 1-2 3-4 5 or more
the test 16. Using outside resources to write a report without
Yes No reading the book
0 1-2 3-4 5 or more Yes No
Items 9-13 pertain to homework completion. 0 1-2 3-4 5 or more
9. Doing an individual take-home test with a partner 17. Using Internet information as your own
Yes No Yes No
0 1-2 3-4 5 or more 0 1-2 3-4 5 or more
10. Copying someone’s answers for a take-home test 18. Writing a report without crediting others for their
ideas
Yes No
Yes No
0 1-2 3-4 5 or more
0 1-2 3-4 5 or more
11. Copying someone’s homework answers
Yes No
0 1-2 3-4 5 or more
Tests
1. Glancing at someone’s answers during the test 89 87
2. Providing answers to someone during the test 94 74
3. Using outside notes 95 54
4. Providing test questions to someone yet to take the test 47 68
5. Providing test answers to someone yet to take the test 84 59
Homework
6. Doing individual take-home test with a partner 62 51
7. Copying someone’s answers for a take-home test 88 45
8. Copying someone’s homework answers 75 90
9. Doing individual homework with a partner 23 91
10. Giving a completed assignment to another student 68 60
11. Submitting someone’s homework as your own 93 20
Writing Reports
12. Writing a report based on the movie instead of reading the book 39 53
13. Using outside resources to write a report without reading the book 53 43
14. Using Internet information as your own 83 46
15. Writing a report without crediting others for their ideas 66 34