Girp Report
Girp Report
Health-Informative Video
There has been a lot of research proving placebo effects, including “the white coat effect” where
patients perceive doctors wearing white coats to be more attractive and trustworthy. But how does the
presence of a white coat in an information presentation affect information retention among viewers? We
tested subjects on their knowledge of a health topic before and after presenting them with an
informational video. The video was either presented by a doctor in a white coat or the same doctor in a
normal outfit, but was otherwise the same. We found that the groups who watched the non-white coat
video actually did better than the groups who watched the white coat video. This leads us to believe that
white coats do not improve information retention, but more studies on the impact of the placebo effect of
drug/treatment that are due to a patient’s belief in its efficacy rather than an actual property of the
drug/treatment. Placebo effects are a puzzling phenomenon considering they can often have a greater
impact on a patient recovering than a well-known surgery. One type of placebo effect studied by Gary
Brase and colleagues (2006) is the “white coat effect”, where patients seem to view people wearing doctor
coats as authority-figures and be more trusting of them, sometimes even seeing better results when treated
by them. However, as far as we know, there has not been a study analyzing the effect of white coats on
knowledge retention—does the placebo effect apply there? The question we sought to answer: how does
white coat presence in an informative presentation affect knowledge retention among viewers? We
hypothesize that knowledge retention will be higher with a white coat present than without because of the
placebo effect. A doctor’s white coat is a symbol of education, credibility, and prestige which would make
the patient more likely to listen to their words as truth and trust them with their health knowledge.
Trusting and learning from people with status symbols, rather than those without, would increase fitness
and be evolutionarily advantageous by ensuring information that informs the way you maintain your
health is legitimate and truthful. This behavioral adaptation where people follow “prestige cues” has been
To collect data for our study, we had a doctor film a short educational video about cholesterol and
statins, which are drugs that can help lower cholesterol. The doctor filmed the video two times: once
while wearing a white coat, and once in standard casual clothing. We then created a short quiz, which
asked questions about cholesterol that were answered in the educational video. We surveyed 71 friends,
family members, and UW students by having them take the quiz testing their knowledge before the video,
watch one of the two educational videos (randomly assigned by picking the number 1 or 2), and then take
the identical quiz again afterwards. We measured performance before and after as well as improvement.
Our control group consisted of the 37 participants who watched the educational video without the white
coat, and our experimental group consisted of the 34 participants who watched the educational video with
subtracted by pre quiz score), which will measure the participants’ knowledge retention. We will then
create two histograms, one each for the control and experimental group which display the change in quiz
scores. We will compare the distributions of each histogram to determine if the educational video with the
white coat had any affect on the knowledge retention of the participants. Additionally, we will control for
improvement obtained by the participants in the two groups according to their score on the pre-test.
If our hypothesis is correct, then the group that watched the white coat educational video’s
histogram distribution will lean further to the right, meaning they experienced a higher change in quiz
score on average, as they likely paid more attention due to the prestige the doctor had (F1). Moreover, we
should see that when we control for pre-test scores, participants in the white coat-viewing group will
consistently obtain a higher proportion of potential improvement after viewing the video (F2).
Consequently, under the null hypothesis there would be no substantial difference in the changes of quiz
scores nor the proportion of potential improvement obtained after controlling for pre-video knowledge for
For our results, on the pre-test, the participants assigned into the non-white coat group (n=34) and
the white coat group (n=37) scored roughly the same (F5). After watching their assigned videos and
taking the post-video quiz, the non-white coat participants scores are more heavily skewed towards the
right relative to the white coat group (F6). Analyzing the raw difference between pre- and post-test scores
reveals that on average, the participants in the control group experienced a greater improvement in their
score after watching their video than the experimental group (F7). Controlling for pre-test scores did not
change the trend. In every pre-test score control condition except for one, the non-white coat group on
average obtained a higher proportion of potential improvement than the white coat group (F8). This
contradicts our predictions and hypothesis as it suggests that the white coat video-viewing participants are
not retaining as much information relative to the non-white coat video group. The one pre-test control
condition that supported our hypothesis is pre-test=0 points. Under this condition, the experimental group
obtained on average 15% more of potential improvement relative to the control. It is important to note
however that the pre-test=0 condition had quite a small sample size relative to the other pre-test
conditions with n(white coat)=1 and n(non-white coat)=4, thus limiting how much significance we can
From the data collected, we conclude that individuals who watched the white coat video did not
retain knowledge any better than those who watched the non-white coat video. Contradictory to our
hypothesis, the mean change in pre vs post test scores were slightly higher for the control group (F7).
Additionally, the control group obtained higher percentages of potential improvement controlled for
pre-test scores (F9). Cumulatively, our results do not support our hypothesis, instead supporting the null.
Strengths of our study include that our experimental design minimized any potential bias about selecting
the two educational videos by blinding, as the participants chose which video to watch without seeing
them both. It also controlled for initial knowledge by having a pre-video quiz, which allowed us to record
changes in the pre vs post test scores as a function of knowledge retention from the shown videos. It was
also a novel question, not previously asked which is interesting and worthwhile. However, our study’s
weaknesses include that our educational videos were fast-paced and jargon-heavy, which may have
introduced learning fatigue as an unwanted factor. Some people may have not thoroughly watched the
video, skipping it and lackadaisical finishing the survey just to get it over with, which would’ve made our
data poor and unreliable. For example, one person guessed on the first quiz, but selected “I don’t know”
for all answers on the second quiz, making it possible to get points on the first quiz, but not the second.
Moreover, our sample size was very small and the participants were not representative of the population
as a whole, since they were mostly UW students and our friends, so that introduced bias into our sample.
A few questions remain after the completion of our experiment. First of all, could our hypothesis
still be correct? Our poor data and small sample size might be the only reason we see the trend we did.
Also, is it possible that the prestige attributed to white coats only does not occur in online settings? We
might see white coat’s positive impact on knowledge retention in an in-person setting. Additionally,
would showing the speaker’s whole body, making it a more obvious difference, in the white coat
distinguish them as more prestigious? Although the data collected does not indicate our hypothesis is
correct, answering these questions experimentally will provide a better understanding of the white coat
effect.
Looking ahead, repeating this experiment with a larger sample size and introducing a form of
monitoring to ensure the video is being watched would help us gain clarity on this study’s results. A larger
sample size provides data that more closely represents the population, and also accounts for any outliers.
If the white coat effect only applies for in person situations, then changing the format from online videos
to in-person presentations would provide data that supported our hypothesis. More studies would provide
useful information about what contexts the white coat effect applies, and if it could be a result of adaptive
behavior.
Figures
Literature Cited
Brase, Gary et al. The White-Coat Effect: Physician Attire and Perceived Authority, Friendliness, and
Attractiveness. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 34: 2469-2481.10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb01987.