Cross-Cultural and Cognitive-Affective Models of Suicide Risk
Cross-Cultural and Cognitive-Affective Models of Suicide Risk
Cross-Cultural and Cognitive-Affective Models of Suicide Risk
research-article2014
JBPXXX10.1177/0095798414525967Journal of Black PsychologyKlibert et al.
Article
Journal of Black Psychology
2015, Vol. 41(3) 272–295
Cross-Cultural and © The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permissions:
Cognitive-Affective sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0095798414525967
Models of Suicide Risk jbp.sagepub.com
Abstract
Only a handful of empirical investigations have identified culturally salient
markers of suicide risk within samples of African American young adults. To
address this gap, our study examined the intercorrelations among cultural
congruity, defectiveness schemas, and multiple indices of suicide risk for African
American (n = 207) and European American (n = 208) students attending a
primary White institution. Cultural congruity was negatively associated with
reports of interpersonal and behavioral suicide risk for both African and
European American students. However, ethnic differences in the magnitude of
these relationships emerged. Specifically, as predicted, for African Americans,
lower levels of cultural congruity were more strongly related to greater
interpersonal factors associated with a desire to die. Finally, the relationships
between cultural congruity and multiple indices of suicide risk were partially
mediated by defectiveness schemas for both African and European American
students, suggesting a useful intervention target for students. These results
also have implications for suicide screening, prevention, and intervention
strategies directed toward African American students.
Keywords
cultural congruity, defectiveness schemas, suicide risk
Corresponding Author:
Klibert, Georgia Southern University, Box 8041, Statesboro, GA 30460, USA.
Email: [email protected]
Although theorists strongly assert that the interaction between ethnicity and
environmental stress serves as a trigger for suicide behaviors, few researchers
offer cross-cultural comparisons that delineate how suicide risk varies across
ethnic groups. Instead, the bulk of the suicide literature considers risk from a
European American perspective with little thought directed toward identify-
ing suicide markers unique to predominant ethnic groups in the United States
(Langhinrichsen-Rohling, Friend, & Powell, 2009). Inattention to processes
that consider how the interactive effects of ethnicity and environmental stress
promote suicide has impeded the development of culturally sensitive screen-
ing, prevention, and intervention efforts (Chu, Goldblum, Floyd, & Bongar,
2010), especially in regard to African American youth aged 18 to 24 for
whom suicide is the third leading cause of death (Centers for Disease Control
and Prevention, 2012).
African American suicide remains inadequately understood (Walker,
Wingate, Obasi, & Joiner, 2008) perhaps due to misinterpretations of mortal-
ity statistics and self-report assessments that suggest that African Americans
(5-6/100,000) have a considerably lower rate of suicide compared to European
Americans (12/100,000; American Association of Suicidology, 2011).
Interpreting mortality statistics and self-report ratings as an assessment of
risk can be misleading in two important ways. Namely, the accuracy by
which suicides are recorded is subject to underreporting and misclassifying
errors (Chu et al., 2010; Rockett et al., 2010). For instance, ethnic minorities
are more likely to engage in “hidden ideation,” a concept coined by Chu et al.
(2010) to describe the tendency to conceal the desire and willingness to
engage in suicide behaviors on self-report questionnaires and interviews.
Mortality statistics also offer a restricted definition of suicide that considers
only one outcome, the act of taking one’s own life. Instead recent theory
recommends that suicidal behavior be defined along a continuum (Lewinsohn,
Langhinrichsen-Rohling, Langford, & Rohde, 1995). The continuum begins
with thoughts of death and dying and a desire to engage in risk-taking behav-
iors. It progresses to suicide ideation, plans to attempt suicide, and eventually
suicide attempts. Identifying individuals who are engaging in behaviors at
early points along the continuum (e.g., intervening with risk-taking and inter-
personal correlates of suicide behavior) has been identified as an important
screening and prevention strategy; especially since suicide completion is a
relatively low base rate behavior (Langhinrichsen-Rohling, Klibert, &
Williams, 2011).
Examining risk outcomes at earlier points on the suicidal trajectory is also
consistent with culturally sensitive practices of suicide assessment. According
to Chu et al.’s (2010) cultural model of suicide, consideration for alternative
forms of suicide thought and intent (i.e., social discord, risk-taking behavior)
to the necessary levels of pain tolerance and fearlessness to initiate and com-
plete a lethal suicide attempt. Painful behaviors directly affiliated to suicide,
such as self-injury and previous suicide attempts, are the most efficient means
of cultivating an acquired capability; however, indirect risky behaviors (e.g.,
violence, sensation seeking) that occur with greater frequency are also impor-
tant in the development of an acquired capability (Smith & Cukrowicz,
2010). For instance, substance use, often a same day correlate of suicide, may
facilitate the necessary fearlessness to attempt to end one’s life.
The latter position mirrors the work of Lewinsohn et al. (1995), who theo-
rized and empirically investigated an individual’s propensity to engage in
future suicide behavior. According to Rohde, Lewinsohn, Seeley, and
Langhinrichsen-Rohling (1996), most instruments assess suicide behaviors
that occur at a high threshold of severity (e.g., suicide attempts). Such focus
often neglects consideration of life-threatening/risky behaviors (e.g., sub-
stance abuse, self-injury, social deviance), all of which are salient factors in
the etiology of youth suicide (Esposito, Spirito, & Overholser, 2003). These
behaviors may also be important prevention targets. In addition, most assess-
ments possess strong associations with cognitive, affective, and interpersonal
components of suicide, overlooking research suggesting that these compo-
nents are neither necessary nor sufficient in predicting actual suicide attempts
(Holden, Mendonca, & Serin, 1989). In response to these limitations,
Lewinsohn et al. (1995) developed the Life Attitudes Schedule (LAS) and
Life Attitudes Schedule–Short Form (LAS-SF) as a means to capture unique
components of suicide behavior not otherwise accounted for by cognitive,
affective, and interpersonal components. Item content of the LAS and
LAS-SF was designed to extract clusters of subtle or covert life-threatening
behaviors, as well as behaviors that are overtly suicidal (Rohde, Seeley,
Langhinrichsen-Rohling, & Rohling, 2003). Included within these clusters
are a number of behaviors (e.g., smashing a fist into a window, going on
drinking sprees) that are known to promote associated features (e.g., greater
levels of pain tolerance and fearlessness; Joiner, 2005) of an acquired capa-
bility to enact self-injury. In support of this theory, suicide prone behaviors
have been shown to correlate with recent suicide ideation and a history of
suicide ideation and attempts (Langhinrichsen-Rohling & Lamis, 2008).
Traditional symptoms of suicide (e.g., hopelessness, depression, etc.) are
not as salient in the prediction of future suicide behaviors among African
Americans when compared to European Americans (Abe, Mertz, Powell, &
Hanzlick, 2006), making it difficult to use most existing assessment strate-
gies aimed at identifying risk. In addition, differing expressions of suicide
may be steeped in unique cultural dynamics, intentions, and motivations
(Klibert et al., 2011), which in turn, may contribute to complications in risk
Current Study
The purpose of the current study was to investigate gender and cross-cultural
differences (African American vs. European American students) in suicide
risk. Research citing gender and ethnic differences in suicide rates has been
hampered by underreporting errors (Rockett et al., 2010). To address this
limitation, researchers (i.e., Chu et al., 2010) have endorsed conducting stud-
ies to examine gender and ethnic differences on alternative measures of sui-
cide desire and intent that may more accurately capture culturally sensitive
expressions of risk. As a result, the first goal of the current study was to
examine gender and ethnic differences on two unique measures of suicide
risk (interpersonal factors associated with a desire to die and suicide prone-
ness). Additionally, an emerging line of research has focused on the identifi-
cation of culturally salient correlates of suicide risk across ethnic groups.
Classic correlates of suicide risk (e.g., depression, hopelessness) appear less
predictive of future suicide behavior for African American versus European
American students (Abe et al., 2006). As a result, it is important that research-
ers identify culturally salient correlates to African American risk so that more
culturally sensitive practices associated with suicide prevention and interven-
tion can be developed and refined. Given this need, the second goal of the
current study was to examine the relationships between cultural congruity
and two measures of suicide risk for African American as well as European
Americans students. Also considering the large percentage of African
Americans students who report experiencing a discriminating event (98.5%;
Prelow, Mosher, & Bowman, 2006) and the deleterious effects of accultura-
tive stress on mental health (Walker, 2007), there may be some ethnic varia-
tion by which cultural congruity is related to indices of suicide risk. As a
result, the third goal of the study was to examine ethnic differences in the
associations between cultural congruity and indices of suicide risk. Finally,
theorists suggest that models of suicide have not been framed from a multi-
cultural context (Chu et al., 2010), which again limits the development of
culturally sensitive prevention and assessment strategies to suicide.
Multicultural theories (i.e., Pyke, 2010) posit that culturally incongruent
environments may influence the development of cognitive-affective themes
of inferiority and insecurity, which in turn may promote negative psychologi-
cal outcomes. To date, no known study has examined the fit of this model to
predict suicide risk in a sample of ethnically diverse students. To address this
gap, the fourth goal of the current study was to examine cultural congruence-
suicide risk relationships in the context of a third mediating variable, defec-
tiveness schemas, in a sample of African American students.
Given the current literature, the hypotheses for the current study were the
following: (a) minimal differences in self-reported measures of suicide risk
(i.e., interpersonal factors associated with a desire to die, suicide proneness)
would be reported between African American and European American col-
lege students; (b) cultural congruity would be inversely related to reports of
interpersonal and behavioral risk for both African American and European
American students; (c) the relations between cultural incongruity and suicide
risk would be stronger for African Americans as compared to European
Americans; and (d) defectiveness schemas would at least partially mediate
the relationships between cultural congruity and the indices of suicide risk,
especially for African American students.
Method
Participants
Participants included 487 (293 women and 194 men) students from a Primary
White Institution (PWI) in the southeastern region of the United States. All
interested students were invited to participate in the study through the univer-
sity’s subject pool and received one course credit for their time. The age of
the participants ranged from 18 to 26, with an average age of 19.55 (SD =
1.46). In terms of ethnicity, only participants who self-identified as European
American and African American were included in the analyses. Two hundred
and eight participants self-identified as European American (57.5%), and 207
participants self-identified as African American (42.5%). These characteris-
tics, including the preponderance of women, are similar to the demographics
of this university’s psychology participation pool.
Measures
Life Attitudes Schedule–Short Form (LAS-SF). The LAS-SF (Lewinsohn, Lang-
hinrichsen-Rohling, Rohde, & Langford, 2004) is a 24-item true-false ques-
tionnaire designed to measure behaviors known to increase the likelihood of
future suicide ideation, attempts, and completions. Total LAS-SF scores
range from 0 to 24, with higher scores indicating greater suicide proneness.
The LAS-SF total score has demonstrated solid internal consistency (α = .78)
with college student samples (Lewinsohn et al., 2004) and high construct
validity as evidenced by moderately high relationships with measures of
risky behaviors and reports of previous suicide attempts among college stu-
dents (Rohde et al., 2003). Similarly, in the current study, the LAS-SF total
score (α = .74) demonstrated adequate internal consistency.
Interpersonal Needs Questionnaire (INQ). The INQ (Van Orden et al., 2008) is
a 12-item questionnaire. It was designed to measure an individual’s desire to
die as reflected by a perceived lack of connection to others as well as by
overwhelming feelings of burdensomeness. Participants were asked to indi-
cate the extent to which each statement was recently true for them using a
7-point Likert-type scale (1 = Not at all true for me to 7 = Completely true for
me). INQ total scores range from 0 to 84, with higher scores indicating greater
interpersonal factors associated with a desire for death by suicide. Consider-
ing that the most dangerous form of suicide desire arises from the simultane-
ous presence of thwarted belongingness and perceived burdensomeness (Van
Orden et al., 2012), an INQ total score was calculated and used within the
current study. Klibert et al. (2014) found high internal consistency (α = .92)
and test-retest reliability (α = .68) for the INQ total score in college student
samples. In addition, the INQ total score has demonstrated excellent con-
struct validity with measures of perceived stress and suicide proneness across
time (Klibert et al., 2014). In the current study, the INQ total score (α = .92)
demonstrated high internal consistency.
Cultural Congruity Scale (CCS). The CCS (Gloria & Robinson-Kurpius, 1996)
is a 13-item measure designed to assess for the perceived fit between an indi-
vidual’s cultural values and traditions and the cultural climate of the environ-
ment in which he/she operates. The CCS asks participants to rate their
perceptions of cultural fit with their campus climate (e.g., “I feel that my
language and/or appearance make it hard for me to fit in with other students”)
on a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = Not at all, 7 = A great deal). Total scores
on the CCS range from 13 to 91, with higher scores indicating greater per-
ceived cultural congruity. The CCS has been shown to have adequate reli-
ability (α =.82) and excellent construct validity with measures of help-seeking
behavior (Gloria et al., 2001; Gloria, Robinson Kurpius, Hamilton, & Will-
son, 1999) among diverse samples of college students. In the current sample,
the CSS total score demonstrated solid internal consistency (α = .79).
Procedure
Data were collected anonymously through an online survey. Undergraduates
voluntarily provided their informed consent before responding to the set of
questionnaires. Participants took approximately 50 minutes to complete the
online survey. On completion of the survey, all participants were taken to a
debriefing page where they were offered free to low cost mental health ser-
vices and thanked for their participation. Ethical guidelines were followed in
the collection of these data, and institutional review board approval was
obtained prior to data collection.
Results
Mean Difference and Correlational Analyses
Mean Differences on Suicide Risk. A 2 gender (women vs. men) × 2 ethnicity
(African American vs. European American) MANOVA was conducted to
investigate ethnic differences among the two suicide indices (i.e., interper-
sonal factors associated with a desire to die and suicide proneness). Results
yielded a significant overall main effect for ethnicity Wilks’s Λ(1, 480) =
3.31, p = .037, η2 = .02. However, nonsignificant effects for gender, Wilks’s
Λ(1, 480) = .92, p = .14, η2 = .01, and the gender by ethnicity interaction,
Wilks’s Λ(1, 480) = .99, p = .791, η2 = .01, were revealed. For ethnicity, there
was significant main effect on suicide proneness, F(1, 482) = 5.47, p< .05,
η2 = .01, such that African American students (M = 5.22, SD = 3.31) reported
engaging in less life-threatening behaviors than did European American stu-
dents (M = 5.98, SD = 3.65). Overall, ethnicity main effects were not obtained
for self-reported interpersonal factors associated with a desire to die.
Variable 1 2 3 4 M SD
1. Cultural Congruity — −.55** −.44** −.60** 75.75 12.64
2. Desire to Die −.37** — .55** .64** 20.34 11.46
3. Suicide Proneness −.32** .50** — .51** 5.22†† 3.31
4. Defectiveness Schemas −.36** .60** .45** — 28.29 12.42
M 75.28 20.33 5.98†† 28.98
SD 10.45 10.98 3.65 11.91
Note: Mean scores and correlations in bold represent data for African American students.
**Correlation is significant at the .01 level.
††Significant mean differences between African American and European American students at
.05 level.
European
African American American cultural
Correlations cultural congruity congruity r to z score p Value
Desire to Die −.55 (n = 203) −.37 (n = 277) 2.44 <.05
Suicide Proneness −.44 (n = 204) −.32 (n = 279) 1.49 >.05
Defectiveness Schemas −.60 (n = 204) −.36 (n = 279) 3.30 <.01
Mediation Models
All variables were associated to a significant degree, meeting Baron and
Kenny’s (1986) four conditions to construct mediation models. As a result,
four series of ordinary least square multiple regressions were conducted to
examine the potential mediating effects of defectiveness schemas on the
Figure 1. Results of indirect and direct paths for African Americans in Models 1(a)
and 1(b). Results of indirect and direct paths for European Americans in Model 2(a)
and 2(b).
Note: Defectiveness schema is the potential mediator in the association between cultural
congruity and two indices of suicide risk. All numbers reflect standardized beta weights
(**p< .01). The beta weight in the parentheses reflects the direct path between cultural
congruity and measures of suicide risk.
Discussion
The current literature reports mixed findings regarding variation in self-
reported suicide risk between African American and European American col-
lege students. As a result, the current study examined mean differences
between African American and European American students on two highly
regarded indices of suicide risk. Overall, on a positive note, both African
American and European American students reported low levels of interper-
sonal factors associated with a desire to die and suicide proneness. Moreover,
only minimal differences in suicide risk emerged between African American
and European American students. For instance, African Americans expressed
similar levels of interpersonal factors (thwarted belongingness and perceived
burdensomeness) when compared to European Americans, confirming previ-
ous findings (Davidson & Wingate, 2011). In addition, African American
students (M = 5.22) reported significantly lower suicide proneness scores
compared to European American students (M = 5.98), though the difference
may not hold much practical significance (η2 = .01). Taken as a whole, these
findings suggest that African Americans express suicide risk almost as fre-
quently as European Americans among samples of college students and sup-
port Davidson and Wingate’s (2011) empirical results suggesting that few, if
any, meaningful differences in self-reported suicide risk exist between
African American and European American students.
However, these findings stand in contrast to a large body of literature sug-
gesting that African Americans are at a decreased risk for suicidal behavior
(e.g., attempts, completions; Wenzel et al., 2011). How researchers measure
suicide risk may be an important factor in explaining these discrepancies. It
is important to note that in our study and other similar studies (e.g., Davidson
& Wingate, 2011) suicide risk was measured by more subtle and indirect
assessments. This is an important contribution to the literature as most empir-
ical investigations of ethnic disparities in suicide assess risk examine more
direct behaviors (e.g., suicide ideation and attempts). According to Chu et al.
(2010), use of direct suicide assessments may underdetect risk for ethnic
minorities because such practices engender little consideration for cultural
sanctions (e.g., shame) that might influence willingness to disclose. Instead,
ethnic minorities may be more willing to express symptoms of suicide risk on
indirect and subtle measures because these assessment strategies may help
minimize potential stigma, shame, and/or embarrassment. In keeping with
this position, our findings provide support for diversifying the means by
which clinicians assess suicide in African Americans to include subtle and
indirect measures.
other studies investigating the link between cultural-related stressors and sui-
cide (Davidson, Wingate, Slish, & Rasmussen, 2010). Our results extend pre-
vious research on culturally relevant theories of suicide by highlighting the
role of cognitive-affective variables as a salient factor in explaining the rela-
tionships between cultural congruity and suicide risk, especially among
African American students. However, it is important to note that defective-
ness schemas only partially mediated the relationships between cultural con-
gruity and suicide risk indices; suggesting that other factors may be equally
important in understanding how perceptions of cultural congruity are related
to a diverse range of suicide markers for African American students.
Identifying other factors that mediate or moderate these relationships will be
important in future studies.
Our results also offer some unique insights into the assessment of suicide
risk and provision of therapeutic services to African American students. First,
screening and evaluation of suicide risk needs to consider the interactive
effects of unwelcoming social climates and resulting internalized self-attribu-
tions in the expression of suicide risk. Second, our results highlight the
importance of adopting culturally congruent practices to treatment. We cau-
tion against the exclusive use of traditional psychotherapeutic approaches in
treatment because they often fail to consider culturally salient experiences
associated with the presenting problem (Bernal & Sáez-Santiago, 2006).
Instead, it may be useful to implement postmodern practices as a culturally
sensitive method of reducing risk. For instance, narrative practice is a client-
led approach that is designed to examine the intersection between prominent
life experiences and the formation of identity conclusions. In this way,
African American students are offered opportunities to gain insights on how
oppressive experiences have altered their perceptions of self. Clinicians can
use the insights gained to flush out unique outcomes, neglected aspects of the
individuals’ cultural identity, and reinvigorate cultural supports that may
help buffer against the desire and/or ability to enact suicide behaviors.
Funding
The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publica-
tion of this article.
References
Abe, K., Mertz, K. J., Powell, K. E., & Hanzlick, R. L. (2006). Characteristics of Black
and White suicide decedents in Fulton County, Georgia, 1988-2002. American
Journal of Public Health, 96, 1794-1798. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2005.08213
American Association of Suicidology. (2011). U.S.A. suicide: 2005 official final
data. Retrieved from http://www.ct.gov/dmhas/lib/dmhas/prevention/cyspi/
aas2005data.pdf
Baron, R, & Kenny, D. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social
psychological research:Conceptual strategies and statistical considerations.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.51.6.1173
Bernal, G., & Sáez-Santiago, E. (2006). Culturally centered psychosocial interven-
tions. Journal of Community Psychology, 34, 121-132. doi:10.1002/jcop.20096
Cecero, J., Beitel, M., & Prout, T. (2008).Exploring the relationships among early
maladaptive schemas, psychological mindedness, and self-reported college
adjustment. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 81,
105-118.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1997). Web-based injury statistics query
and reporting systems (WISQARS). Atlanta, CA: National Center for Injury
Prevention and Control. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/ncipc/wisqars/
default.htm
Chao, R., Mallinckrodt, B., & Wei, M. (2012). Co-occurring problems in African
American college clients reporting racial discrimination distress. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice, 43, 199-207. doi:10.1037/a0027861
Christensen, H., Batterham, P. J., Soubelet, A., & Mackinnon, A. J. (2013). A test of
the interpersonal theory of suicide in a large community-based cohort. Journal of
Affective Disorders, 144, 225-234. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2012.07.002
Chu, J. P., Goldblum, P., Floyd, R., & Bongar, B. (2010). The cultural theory and
model of suicide. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 14, 25-40.
Constantine, M. G., Robinson, J. S., Wilton, L., & Caldwell, L. D. (2002). Collective
self-esteem and perceived social support as predictors of cultural congruity among
Black and Latino college students. Journal of College Student Development,43,
307-316.
Constantine, M. G., Smith, L., Redington, R. M., & Owens, D. (2008). Racial micro-
aggressions against Black counseling and counseling psychology faculty: A cen-
tral challenge in the multicultural counseling movement. Journal of Counseling
and Development, 86, 348-355.
Constantine, M. G., & Watt, S. K. (2002). Cultural congruity, womanist identity
attitudes, and life satisfaction among African American college women attend-
ing historically Black and predominantly White institutions. Journal of College
Student Development,43, 184-194.
Davidson, C. L., & Wingate, L. R. (2011). Racial disparities in risk and pro-
tective factors for suicide. Journal of Black Psychology, 37, 499-516.
doi:10.1177/0095798410397543
Davidson, C. L., Wingate, L. R., Slish, M. L., & Rasmussen, K. A. (2010). The great
black hope: Hope and its relation to suicide risk among African Americans. Suicide
and Life-Threatening Behavior, 40, 170-180. doi:10.1521/suli.2010.40.2.170
De Hoyos, L. R., & Ramirez, N. (2007). The relationship between shame and accul-
turation among Latino/Chicano students: Implication for social work education.
Journal of Ethnic & Cultural Diversity in Social Work: Innovation in Theory,
Research, & Practice, 15, 147-166. doi:10.1300/J051v15n01_07
Esposito, C., Spirito, A., & Overholser, J. (2003). Behavioral factors: Impulsive and
aggressive behavior. In A. Spirito & J. C. Overholser (Eds.), Evaluating and
treating adolescent suicide attempters: From research to practice (pp.113-145).
San Diego, CA: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/B978-012657951-2/5008-X
Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., Cleveland, M. J., Wills, T. A., & Brody, G. (2004).
Perceived discrimination and substance use in African American parents and
their children: A panel study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86,
517-529. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.4.517
Gibbons, F. X., O’Hara, R. E., Stock, M. L., Gerrard, M., Weng, C., & Wills, T. A.
(2012). The erosive effects of racism: Reduced self-control mediates the relation
between perceived racial discrimination and substance use in African American
adolescents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 1089-1104.
doi:10.1037/a0027404
Gloria, A. M., Hird, J. S., & Navarro, R. L. (2001). Relationships of cultural congruity
and perceptions of the university environment to help-seeking attitudes by socior-
ace and gender. Journal of College Student Development,42, 545-562.
Gloria, A. M., & Robinson-Kurpius, S. E. (1996). The validation of the Cultural
Congruity Scale and the University Environment Scale with Chicano/a students.
Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 18, 533-549.
Gloria, A. M., Robinson Kurpius, S. E., Hamilton, K. D., & Willson, M. S. (1999).
African American students’ persistence at a pre dominantly White university:
Pyke, K. D. (2010). What is internalized racial oppression and why don’t we study it?
Acknowledging racism’s hidden injuries. Sociological Perspectives, 53, 551-572.
doi:10.1525/sop.2010.53.4.551
Rockett, I. R. H., Wang, S., Stack, S., De Leo, D., Frost, J. L., Ducatman, A. M.,
. . .Kapusta, N. (2010). Race/ethnicity and potential suicide misclassification:
Window on a minority suicide paradox? BMC Psychiatry, 10, 35.
Rohde, P., Lewinsohn, P. M., Seeley, J. R., & Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J. (1996) The
Life Attitudes Schedule Short Form: An abbreviated measure of life-enhancing
and life-threatening behaviors in adolescents. Suicide and Life-Threatening
Behavior, 26, 272-281.
Rohde, P., Seeley, J. R., Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J., & Rohling, M. L. (2003). The
Life Attitudes Schedule-Short Form: Psychometric properties and correlates
of adolescent suicide proneness. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 33,
249-260.
Smith, P. N., & Cukrowicz, K. C. (2010). Capable of suicide: A functional model
of the acquired capability component of the interpersonal-psychological theory
of suicide. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behaviors, 40, 266-275. doi:10.1521/
suli.2010.40.3.266
Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic intervals for indirect effects in structural equation
models. Sociological Methodology, 13, 290-312.
Szymanski, D. M., Kashubeck-West, S., & Meyer, J. (2008). Internalized heterosex-
ism: A historical and theoretical overview. The Counseling Psychologist, 36,
510-524. doi:10.1177/0011000007309488
Van Orden, K. A., Cukrowicz, K. C., Witte, T. K., & Joiner, T. E. (2012). Thwarted
belongingness and perceived burdensomeness: Construct validity and psycho-
metric properties of the Interpersonal Needs Questionnaire. Psychological
Assessment, 24, 197-215. doi:10.1037/a0025358
Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., Gordon, K. H., Bender, T. W., & Joiner, T. E.
(2008). Suicidal desire and the capability for suicide: Tests of the interpersonal-
psychological theory of suicidal behavior among adults. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 76, 72-83. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.76.1.72
Venzant-Chambers, T. T. (2009). The “receivement gap”: School tracking policies and
the fallacy of the “achievement gap.” Journal of Negro Education, 78, 417-431.
Walker, R. L. (2007). Acculturation and acculturative stress as indicators for suicide
risk among African Americans. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 77, 386-
391. doi:10.1037/0002-9432.77.3.386
Walker, R. L., Wingate, L. R., Obasi, E. M., & Joiner, T. R. (2008). An empirical
investigation of acculturative stress and ethnic identity as moderators for depres-
sion and suicidal ideation in college students. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
Minority Psychology,14, 75-82. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.14.1.75
Warren, C. S. (2006). Incorporating multiculturalism into undergraduate psychology
courses: Three simple active learning activities. Teaching of Psychology, 33,
105-109. doi:10.1207/s15328023top3302_5
Wenzel, A., Berchick, E. R., Tenhave, T., Halberstadt, S., Brown, G. K., & Beck, A. T.
(2011). Predictors of suicide relative to other deaths in patients with suicide
attempts and suicide ideation: A 30-year prospective study. Journal of Affective
Disorders,132, 375-382. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2011.03.006
Young, J. E. (2005). The YSQ-S2 Young Schema Questionnaire, Short Form.
In J. E. Young(Ed.), Schema Therapy complete packet: Workshop handouts
(Unpublished manuscript). New York, NY: Schema Therapy Institute.
Young, J. E., Klosko, J. S., & Weishaar, M. E. (2003). Schema therapy: A
practitioner’guide. New York, NY: Guilford Press.